Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2014 Nov 1;23(4):679–695. doi: 10.1044/2014_AJSLP-14-0040

Nonword repetition in children with cochlear implants: A potential clinical marker of poor language acquisition

Susan Nittrouer 1, Amanda Caldwell-Tarr 1, Emily Sansom 1, Jill Twersky 1, Joanna H Lowenstein 1
PMCID: PMC4229442  NIHMSID: NIHMS627591  PMID: 25340675

Abstract

Purpose

Cochlear implants (CIs) can facilitate the acquisition of spoken language for deaf children, but challenges remain. Language skills dependent upon phonological sensitivity are most at risk for these children, so having an effective way to diagnose problems at this level would be of value for school speech-language pathologists. The goal of this study was to assess whether a nonword repetition (NWR) task could serve that purpose.

Method

104 second graders participated: 49 with NH and 55 with CIs. In addition to NWR, children were tested on ten measures involving phonological awareness/processing, serial recall of words, vocabulary, reading, and grammar.

Results

Children with CIs performed more poorly than children with NH on NWR, and sensitivity to phonological structure alone explained that performance for children in both groups. For children with CIs, two audiological factors positively influenced outcomes on NWR: being identified with hearing loss at younger ages and having experience wearing a hearing aid on the unimplanted ear at the time of receiving a first CI. NWR scores were better able to rule out language deficits than rule in such deficits.

Conclusions

Well-designed NWR tasks could have clinical utility in assessments of language acquisition for school-age children with CIs.

Keywords: Nonword repetition, children, cochlear implants, deaf, language acquisition


Within the scope of practice for speech-language pathologists in the schools is the task of conducting assessments on students suspected of having or already diagnosed as having language-learning problems. These assessments are often intended to benchmark performance, such as when multifactorial evaluations are completed on children with individualized educational plans. However, these assessments should also help identify delays or barriers to the acquisition of specific language skills, so intervention can be initiated on those skills. The best assessment tools are able to evaluate mechanisms underlying the acquisition of spoken language, without being unduly influenced by factors external to the child, such as cultural background or socioeconomic status. Test procedures should be highly reliable. One group of children for whom tests sensitive to an assortment of language delays is especially needed are deaf children who get cochlear implants (CIs). Although the arrival of CIs has permitted deaf children to attain previously unprecedented levels of spoken language abilities, problems persist because these devices deliver only degraded spectral representations to their users. Consequently, children who must develop spoken language systems through these devices continue to face challenges in doing so. It is essential that the speech-language pathologists who serve these children in school settings have sensitive measures of their language abilities.

An assessment tool that has been touted as fitting the bill for children at risk of language delay is nonword repetition (NWR). In this task a child hears a string of phonemes that do not form a real lexical item, and must immediately repeat it. The series of phonemic strings are one or two syllables long at the start of the task, and gradually increase in length to four or five syllables. When these tasks are used with children who have normal hearing, but who have been diagnosed with language impairments, they are found to be highly sensitive to that diagnosis. For example, Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) used a NWR task with two groups of children, matched on age and gender: one group of children diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI; N = 44) and one group of children learning language in typical fashion (N = 41). All children were school age, spanning a range from 5 years; 8 months to 12 years, 2 months. Using a cutpoint of roughly two standard deviations below the mean of the language-typical group, these authors found that 61% of the children in the SLI group scored below the cutpoint. Using the same NWR task, other investigators (Ellis Weismer et al., 2000) replicated the basic result showing that children diagnosed with SLI tended to score below the cutpoint established by Dollaghan and Campbell; however, the proportion was lower in this later study, at just 25% (20 of the 80 children diagnosed with SLI).

The difference in observed sensitivity of the NWR task to language impairment found for these two studies might arise due to differences in the profiles of deficit across children in the two studies. In both studies, broad, albeit slightly different, definitions of language impairment were applied. Neither study asked which specific language skills were predicted by scores on the NWR task. That question, however, has important clinical implications. Knowing what skills are predicted by the diagnostic task is critical to its application.

Language skills predicted by NWR

Generally speaking, a wide assortment of language skills can be linked to NWR. Children diagnosed with language impairment based on broad measures of ability (i.e., norm-referenced tests) have been found to perform poorly on NWR tasks (Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Ellis Weismer et al., 2000; Kamhi, Catts, Mauer, Apel, & Gentry, 1988). However, one study found that correlations between NWR and language measures were only significant when expressive (rather than receptive) language was being assessed (Edwards & Lahey, 1998), which prompted the authors to conclude that the major problem underlying poor NWR performance involves difficulty generating highly refined phonological representations. The basis of the argument hinged on the idea that more refined representations are required to produce speech than to comprehend language.

Because the psycholinguistic phenomenon most clearly assessed by NWR tasks concerns the nature of phonological representations, it is no surprise that these tasks are able to predict lexical knowledge (e.g., Avons, Wragg, Cupples, & Lovegrove, 1998; Gathercole, 1995; Michas & Henry, 1994); both NWR and lexical acquisition rely on having access to detailed phonological structure. Neither is it surprising that NWR can help predict reading ability (e.g., Kamhi et al., 1988), given the evidence that learning to read depends on having strong phonological sensitivity (e.g., Brady, Shankweiler, & Mann, 1983; Catts, 1989; Fox & Routh, 1980; Liberman & Shankweiler, 1985; Wagner & Torgesen, 1987). Turning to grammatical abilities, a study by Sáhlen, Reuterskiöld-Wagner, Nettelbradt, and Radeborg (1999) found a significant correlation between NWR scores and scores on a test of children's abilities to use various grammatical forms in their constructions. In summary, there is support for the suggestion that appropriate NWR tasks should be able to predict performance on measures of lexical acquisition, reading ability, and use of grammatical structures. That makes NWR tasks potentially powerful tools for use in the clinical assessment of school-age children.

Independence from unwanted influences

Another consideration in selecting instruments for clinical assessments is that they should be as free as possible from inherent bias arising from differences in children's environments. This qualification can be a difficult one for assessment instruments to meet. Many norm-referenced tests used in the schools exhibit bias based on cultural or socioeconomic factors because of the content of individual items (e.g., Campbell, Dollaghan, Needleman, & Janosky, 1997). However, Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) demonstrated that a well-designed NWR task can avoid cultural and socioeconomic biases, and that conclusion was subsequently reached by Ellis Weismer et al. (2000), as well.

Because NWR involves having children repeat the sequences that are presented to them, another concern that could be raised is that scores on these tasks might be unduly influenced by oral motor skills. However, several studies investigating potential relationships between NWR and SLI have been careful to assess oral motor skills. In none of these instances has a correlation between oral motor skills and scores on the NWR task been observed (Edwards & Lahey, 1998; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; Sáhlen et al., 1999). At least some of the explanation for a failure to find a relationship between oral motor and NWR skills could be that many of the phonological sequences devised for use in NWR tasks are deliberately selected to consist of early acquired segments and no clusters. Presumably the oral motor actions needed to produce these sequences are within the abilities of most, if not all children by the time they reach school age.

Finally, a characteristic of the NWR task that makes it an attractive assessment tool is that it provides a measure of children's phonological processing that is less influenced by the size of a child's existing vocabulary than other phonological short-term memory tasks, especially when low-probability phonological sequences are used (e.g., Edwards, Beckman, & Munson, 2004; Gathercole, 1995; Archibald & Gathercole, 2006). In typical serial recall tasks, including digit span, children are asked to recall strings of items that are likely components of their lexicons. Consequently, the strength of these representations within the lexicon can influence recall because detailed phonological representations may not be needed to code the items into a short-term memory buffer; instead, partial structures may activate word representations, which in turn are what get coded into a memory buffer. As a result, children with larger vocabularies will obtain more of a boost to their serial recall than children with smaller vocabularies. Support for that statement is provided by the finding that NWR is a more reliable marker of SLI than is serial recall of lexical items (e.g., Conti-Ramsden, 2003; Archibald & Gathercole, 2007).

The size of a child's vocabulary, however, may still have some effect on their performance in NWR tasks, through an indirect route. That effect arises because vocabulary size mediates sensitivity to phonotactic probabilities, such that children with larger vocabularies have presumably developed stronger sensitivities to these probabilities. As a result, if phonological sequences used in NWR tasks are ones that occur frequently within the native language, children with stronger vocabularies may be better able to code those sequences into storage (e.g., Archibald & Gathercole, 2006; Munson, Kurtz, & Windsor, 2005). However, the effect of lexical knowledge is mitigated when low-probability sequences are used.

On the other hand, when nonwords consisting of low-probability phonological sequences are used in tests of NWR, the results can predict children's abilities to acquire new lexical items. The reason for that association is that all novel words begin as unfamiliar phonological strings. The better an individual's ability is to store these unfamiliar strings, the easier word learning will be for that individual. This advantage extends even to adults. For example, individuals who are successful at mastering multiple languages have been found to be especially good at NWR (Papagno & Vallar, 1995). Thus, by selecting a NWR task consisting of low-probability phonological sequences it should be possible to assess children's word-learning potential.

Examining the psycholinguistic processes underlying NWR

NWR tasks primarily assess the abilities of children to recover detailed phonological representations from the speech they hear (i.e., phonological sensitivity), and encode those representations into a memory buffer. Gathercole (2006) referred to the output of this process as phonological storage quality. This phonological sensitivity, as assessed by NWR, has reliably been shown to be deficient in children with normal hearing (NH) who are diagnosed with SLI (e.g., Conti-Ramsden, 2003; Edwards & Lahey, 1998; Ellis Weismer et al., 2000; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; Sáhlen et al., 1999). However, an additional deficit – one involving short-term memory – seems to be at work in these tasks for children with SLI, a conclusion reached by the finding that the magnitude of the effect changes across syllable length. The group (language normal vs. SLI) × syllable-length interaction is typically found to be significant in experiments using NWR, such that performance of children with SLI degrades across length more rapidly than that of children with NH (e.g., Briscoe, Bishop, & Norbury, 2001; Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; Marton & Schwartz, 2003).

NWR and children with CIs

The interest in using NWR tasks with children who are deaf and get CIs arises because CIs provide only spectrally degraded signals to their users. Phonological representations are recovered largely from the spectral structure of the acoustic signals reaching listeners. Thus, it could be predicted that children who must rely on CIs for language learning would face serious challenges as they develop phonological representations, and several studies have found evidence to support that prediction (e.g., Ambrose, Fey, & Eisenberg, 2012; James et al., 2005; Johnson & Goswami, 2010; Nittrouer, Caldwell, Lowenstein, Tarr, & Holloman, 2012). As a result, it could further be predicted that children with CIs would perform more poorly on NWR tasks than children with NH, and that outcome has also been supported by empirical evidence (Carter, Dillon, & Pisoni, 2002; Dillon & Pisoni, 2006). Nonetheless, additional questions are sparked by these findings from children with CIs on tasks of NWR. One question concerns the extent to which these measures can shed light on the basic psycholinguistic processes perturbed by hearing loss and subsequent implantation. In particular, is the NWR performance of children with CIs explained primarily by the quality of their phonological representations? Or does short-term memory play a role as well, as is the case for children with SLI? A study designed to help make this determination was done by Briscoe et al. (2001), who compared performance on a number of language tasks for children with mild-moderate hearing loss and children with normal hearing, but with SLI. Although Briscoe et al. found poorer NWR for children with mild-to-moderate hearing loss than for children with normal hearing and no SLI, the magnitude of the effect was consistent across stimulus length: the group × syllable length interaction was not significant. That outcome contrasted with their own results for the group of children with SLI in that study.

Another question concerning the performance of children with CIs on tasks of NWR involves the language skills that might be predicted by these tasks, for these children. Dillon and Pisoni (2006) tried to answer that question, to some extent, by looking at reading skills. These investigators observed poorer performance on NWR for children with CIs than for NH peers, and scores were correlated with reading abilities for the children with CIs. This finding suggests that NWR could be an effective means of assessing language skills in children with CIs.

Current Study

The current experiment had four specific objectives. The first objective was to compare NWR performance for children who were born with severe-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss and got CIs to that of peers with normal hearing. For this comparison, the nonwords developed by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) were selected. Two sets of nonwords have been used more than any others in NWR tasks. One set is known as the Children's Test of Nonword Repetition (CNRep), and it was developed by Gathercole, Willis, Baddeley, and Emslie (1994). These stimuli were the ones used by Carter et al. (2002) and by Dillon and Pisoni (2006) in their studies involving children with CIs. These stimuli are two to five syllables long, and contain some consonant clusters and lax vowels. Pisoni and colleagues found that children with CIs performed quite poorly with these stimuli: A mean of only 5% of all nonwords were repeated correctly by the children with CIs (Carter et al., 2002).

The nonword stimuli developed by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) consist of one to four syllables (four stimuli at each syllable length). There are no clusters included. No late-acquired consonants, as defined by Shriberg and Kwiatkowski (1994), are incorporated into the stimuli. Only tense vowels are used because these vowels are more salient than lax vowels and less susceptible to being reduced to schwa on repetition, making scoring more straightforward. An example of a one-syllable stimulus is [tɑʊʤ] and an example of a four-syllable stimulus is [dævoʊnɔIʧig]. Based on these characteristics, it was anticipated that children with CIs would attain higher scores with these stimuli than they had with the CNRep.

The second objective of the current study was to examine the psycholinguistic underpinnings of NWR for children with CIs. Again, for children with SLI, short-term memory deficits appear to influence NWR performance, as well as the poor quality of phonological representations that they have. Here it was asked if a similar effect would be found for children with CIs. In earlier work, children with CIs have been found to demonstrate poor digit span, which suggests poor short-term recall (e.g., Pisoni & Cleary, 2003; Pisoni, Kronenberger, Roman, & Geers, 2011). However, outcomes of one study were able to pinpoint the basis of that poor recall as being impoverished phonological representations (Nittrouer, Caldwell-Tarr, & Lowenstein, 2013). In particular, results showed that even though serial recall for lists of words was poorer for children with CIs than for their peers with NH, recall took no longer. Because response time is generally considered to be an index of processing effort (e.g., Cooper-Martin, 1994), it can be suggested that the children with CIs experienced no more difficulty with the processing part of this task than the children with NH. These results can be considered in light of multiple-component models of working memory (e.g., Baddeley, 1992; 2007), which propose that there is one component – the phonological loop – responsible for recovering phonological structure from speech signals and encoding that structure into the short-term memory store, and another component – the central executive – responsible for performing operations such as recall with that stored structure. Accordingly, the finding of less accurate serial recall with no increase in response time observed for children with CIs (compared to children with NH) suggests that children with CIs have difficulty encoding linguistic materials into a memory buffer, but are able to operate on items placed in that buffer without increased effort. Thus the problems encountered by children with CIs in their recall of words could largely be explained by difficulty encoding clear phonological representations, precisely the phenomenon thought to underlie NWR. This account might explain the NWR outcomes of Briscoe et al. (2001), where a group × syllable length interaction was observed for typically developing children and those with SLI, but not for typically developing children and those with mild-moderate hearing loss: In addition to impoverished phonological representations, the children with SLI may have had deficits in the operations of the central executive, whereas the children with hearing loss apparently did not.

The third objective of the current study was to examine the extent to which NWR for children with CIs is influenced by factors other than psycholinguistic skills. In particular, the question was whether NWR is affected by demographic factors such as socioeconomic status or audiological factors such as age of receiving a first CI. If NWR is heavily influenced by these external factors for these children, then NWR scores themselves offer little additional power for predicting language skills over what is available from knowing the child's socioeconomic status and age of receiving a first implant. In this case, it would not serve as a valuable clinical tool.

The fourth objective of the current study was to examine the set of language abilities that might be predicted by NWR for children with CIs. The more language skills that are well-predicted by this task, the more useful NWR could be as an assessment tool with this population of children. In particular, skills in three categories were examined as potentially predictable from NWR scores: vocabulary knowledge, reading abilities, and grammar.

Method

Participants

There were 104 participants in this study, in two groups: 49 with NH and 55 with CIs. These sample sizes provide 71% power and 99% power for detecting differences between groups with Cohen's ds of 0.5 and 1.0, respectively, with alpha set to 0.05. All these children had participated in an ongoing longitudinal study (e.g., Caldwell & Nittrouer, 2013; Nittrouer, 2010; Nittrouer et al., 2012; Nittrouer et al., 2013), and based on outcomes from earlier analyses done as part of that study, effect sizes of that magnitude or larger were anticipated. Consequently, adequate power was available.

All participants had just completed second grade when the data described in the current report were collected. Mean ages (and SDs) at the time of data collection were 8 years; 5 months (4 months) and 8 years; 7 months (6 months) for children with NH and those with CIs, respectively. In the NH group, there were 27 girls and 22 boys; in the CI group, there were 28 girls and 27 boys.

Socioeconomic status (SES) was indexed using a two-factor scale on which both the highest educational level and the occupational status of the primary income earner in the home is considered (Nittrouer & Burton, 2005). Scores for each of these factors range from 1 to 8, with 8 being high. Values for the two factors are multiplied together resulting in a range of possible scores from 1 to 64. In general, a SES score of 30 represents a household in which the primary income earner has a four-year university degree and a job such as a mid-level manager or a teacher. Means (and SDs) for SES were 35 (13) and 33 (11) for children with NH and children with CIs, respectively.

Nonverbal cognitive abilities were assessed using the Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised (Roid & Miller, 2002). Standard scores (and SDs) on this test were 105 (14) and 101 (17) for children with NH and children with CIs, respectively. These differences between groups for SES and nonverbal abilities were not significant.

All children with CIs had their hearing loss identified, hearing aids fit and intervention initiated by 2 years of age: Mean age of identification was 7 months (7 months); mean age of receiving hearing aids was 8 months (6 months), and mean age of starting intervention was 9 months (7 months). The mean better-ear, pure-tone average threshold for the three speech frequencies of 0.5, 1.0, and 2.0 kHz before receiving a CI (hereafter termed pre-implant PTA) was 100 dB hearing level (17 dB hearing level). Although all of these children were fit with hearing aids before 2 years of age, most of them (80%) also received their first CI before turning 2 years, as well. Thirty-six children had bilateral CIs. Of the 19 children with one CI at the time of testing, six wore a hearing aid on the unimplanted ear.

A final audiological factor considered here was whether these children had some period of experience wearing a hearing aid on the ear opposite to the implanted ear. Previous work has shown that children with this experience, known as bimodal stimulation, display better language skills, at least during the preschool years (Nittrouer & Chapman, 2009). The question addressed here was whether that benefit would extend to second grade. A basis for predicting that it might concerns the development of phonological representations, precisely the phenomenon underlying NWR. Even if acoustic hearing is available in only a limited frequency range, that signal is likely more detailed than what is available through the electric signal of the CI. To the extent that phonological categories are acquired during the early years, having some time with those more detailed signals could facilitate the acquisition of more refined representations. In the current study, 26 of the children with CIs had at least one year of experience wearing a hearing aid on the ear opposite to their CI at the time that they received that CI. Six were still wearing a hearing aid on the unimplanted ear. The mean time that the other 20 children spent wearing a hearing aid on the unimplanted ear after receiving a first implant was 24 months (10 months).

Equipment

All testing took place in sound-attenuated rooms. All stimuli used in testing were presented via a computer with a Creative Labs Soundblaster soundcard using a 44.1-kHz sampling rate with 16-bit digitization and a Roland MA-12C powered speaker for audio presentation. No live-voice stimuli were used. For the NWR and phonological processing tasks, stimuli were presented in audio-visual format using a 1500-kbps data rate and 24-bit digitization for video presentation. This allowed children to use visual cues for speech recognition. For the serial recall task, presentation was audio only. Presentation level was always 68 dB SPL, regardless of whether it was audiovisual or audio only. A touchscreen monitor was used for the serial recall task.

All test sessions were video-audio recorded using a SONY HDR-XR550V video recorder so scoring could be done later. Children wore SONY FM transmitters in specially designed vests. The FM receivers provided direct line input to the video cameras to ensure good sound quality for all recordings.

General Procedures

All children came to the Ohio State University for testing. Children were tested individually in sessions lasting no more than one hour. Breaks of at least one hour were provided between those data-collection sessions. For the current study, data were collected over three sessions. In one session, three phonological processing tasks were administered. In another session, a 20-minute language sample was collected in order to obtain the grammatical measures. In that same session, the NWR and serial recall tasks were administered. In a third session, the reading and vocabulary measures were collected.

Task-specific Procedures

NWR

The sixteen nonwords developed by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) were used. To ensure that they were produced exactly as they had been in that earlier work, an audio recording from Dollaghan and Campbell was obtained and reviewed. Stimuli were video-audio recorded by the last author, who is both a trained phonetician and trained singer, which ensured that the stimuli would be recorded as described. Equal stress was placed on all syllables, for all stimuli. Fundamental frequency was kept consistent and flat. Amplitude of stimuli was constant. The same instructions as had been used by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) were recorded by the last author, and placed at the start of the video recording of stimuli.

For this task, the children saw and heard the talker saying each nonword, and needed to repeat each one immediately upon hearing it. Children were video recorded repeating the nonword stimuli. Scoring was completed later by the third author, who was involved more broadly in scoring tests requiring verbal responses from these children. That permitted her to become familiar with the speech patterns of individual children. For this NWR task, phonemes were scored wrong if they were omitted or were clear substitutions of modeled phonemes. However, distortions were not scored as wrong. Twenty percent of the nonword video recordings (20 children) were scored independently by the last author in order to obtain a measure of reliability. Ten of these children had NH and ten had CIs.

Phonological processing

Three tasks of varying difficulty were used to assess children's sensitivity to phonological, especially phonemic, structure. Work by Stanovich, Cunningham and Cramer (1984) primarily served as the basis for predictions of difficulty level for these tasks, although Yopp (1988) also demonstrated the developmental time course of acquisition for specific phonological abilities. The specific tasks used in this study have been used in other studies, and have been found to be reliable within the same children and across children of the same age (e.g., Nittrouer, 1999; Nittrouer, Shune & Lowenstein, 2011; Nittrouer & Burton, 2002; Nittrouer & Miller, 1999). The first two tasks are commonly described as investigating phonological awareness, rather than processing (e.g., Snider, 1997; Stanovich et al., 1984) because there are lighter processing demands in these tasks than in some tasks, such as the third one used in this study. Nonetheless, the broader term ‘processing’ is used in this report to encompass all three of these tasks, for simplicity's sake. Using this set of tasks varying in difficulty diminished the probability that significant differences across groups in abilities would fail to be identified, either because a selected task was so easy that even children with phonological delays were able to perform it or because it was so difficult that even typically developing children could not. All three tasks were presented with specially developed software. Video recordings of a male talker served as stimuli for all three tasks. The experimenter entered the child's responses, and the software kept track of those responses.

The first task, the initial consonant choice task, was considered the easiest going into testing because Stanovich et al. (1984) showed that children develop sensitivity to phonological structure at the starts of words before they develop sensitivity to structure at the ends of words. This initial consonant choice task consisted of 48 items and began with the child getting a target word to repeat. The child was given three opportunities to repeat this target word correctly. Were the target not to be repeated correctly within three attempts, testing would have advanced to the next trial and the missed trial would not have been included in the overall calculations of percent correct. However, all these children were able to repeat all the target words correctly with the audiovisual presentation. Following correct repetition of the target word, the child was presented with three more words and had to choose the one that had the same beginning sound as the target word. Test items can be found in Appendix A. The dependent measure was the percent of trials on which the child correctly chose the word with the same initial sound as the target. Although this task, as well as the second, involved some memory load, the ability to retain four words in a short-term memory buffer should have been well within the abilities of all these second graders: Test norms for the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children (Wechsler, 1999) show that 99.5% of 8-year-olds with NH can recall 4-digit strings, and Pisoni, Kronenberger, Roman, and Geers (2011) showed that children with (early) CIs could recall 4-digit strings by 8 years of age.

The second task, the final consonant choice task, was considered to be intermediate in difficulty for children of this age: although procedures were identical to those of the initial consonant choice task, Stanovich et al. (1984) showed that sensitivity to syllable-final structure is acquired later. This task consisted of 48 items, shown in Appendix B. The dependent measure was the percent of trials on which the child correctly chose the word with the same ending sound as the target.

The third task, the phoneme deletion task, was the most difficult because it involved the most processing: In this task, the child saw and heard the talker say a nonword in the context “Say _______.” The child needed to repeat that nonword correctly. Then the talker would say “Now say without the ____ sound.” Thus, in this task children not only needed to recognize phonemic structure in a nonword, they also needed to manipulate that nonword structure so that one segment was removed, and the remaining segments were blended. The segment to be removed could occur anywhere within the word. The task consisted of 32 items, which are found in Appendix C. Percent correct responses served as the dependent measure.

Serial recall of word lists

Children were asked to recall the order of strings of six monosyllabic nouns produced by a male talker, presented as auditory lists: ball, coat, dog, ham, pack, and rake. For children, lists six words in length result in scores that are neither too close to ceiling or floor. Ten of these word lists were presented, with words ordered randomly by the software on each presentation. The words were presented at a rate of one per second. Simple pictures of the words were shown at the top of the touchscreen monitor. After presentation of each list, the child's task was to touch the pictures in the order that the words were heard. Responses were recorded by the program.

Before testing, training on this task was provided using six non-rhyming and visually distinct letters: F, H, Q, R, S, and Y. After practice with the letters and prior to testing, the six words were introduced, one at a time, to familiarize the child with each word and its corresponding picture. Then recognition was checked for each child prior to testing by showing pictures of all six words and playing each word, one at a time. The child needed to touch each word immediately after it was played. After testing, this recognition check was done again. If a child had difficulty recognizing any word, testing would not have been conducted (if it happened during the pre-test) or data would have been removed from analysis (if it happened during the post-test). However, all children readily recognized these simple nouns when presented in this auditory-only format. There was a maximum possible correct of 60 (six items in 10 lists), and the dependent measure was the percentage of words recalled in the correct order.

This test procedure has been used often to examine short-term memory (e.g., Brady et al., 1983; Spring & Perry, 1983), and this particular task with these particular words has been shown to have good test-retest reliability (Nittrouer & Miller, 1999).

Vocabulary knowledge

For this study, expressive vocabulary was assessed with the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT; Brownell, 2000). This task requires the child to provide the words that label a series of pictured items shown one at a time on separate pages. In this task, children's verbal responses were video recorded. A laboratory staff member scored responses later, and a second staff member checked all scores by watching the video recording again and confirming those scores. If any discrepancies were found the staff members resolved them by consensus. The laboratory manager monitored all scoring procedures. Standard scores were used as dependent measures.

Expressive vocabulary was measured in this study, rather than receptive vocabulary, because a word must be retained in the lexicon more robustly in order for a child to be able to access it during an expressive vocabulary test. That difference is due to the fact that in expressive vocabulary tests only pictures are shown. The child must be able to retrieve matching words from their lexicons on their own. In contrast, when receptive vocabulary is measured, the child hears the lexical item, and need only select from a set of four the picture that best matches the word heard. That task can be accomplished with less robust lexical representations.

Word reading of isolated words

In order to assess children's abilities to read words presented in isolation, the Word Reading Subtest of the Wide Range Achievement Test 4 (WRAT; Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006) was used. In this test, the child reads a list of words presented on a card. Reading responses were video recorded, and scored later by a staff member. Again, a second staff member confirmed all scores by watching the video recording and checking the scores of the first staff member. Standard scores were used as dependent measures.

Reading of words in context and reading comprehension

The Qualitative Reading Inventory (QRI; Leslie & Caldwell, 2006) was used to assess reading of words in context and reading comprehension. The QRI has both narrative and expository passages written at various levels of reading ability. In this study, three passages were used. One passage was a narrative written at one level below grade level, one was a narrative at grade level, and one was an expository at grade level. Children were video recorded reading each story and responding to questions designed to assess their comprehension. There were ten comprehension questions per story. A staff member scored the number of words read correctly and the number of questions answered correctly from the video recording. A second staff member checked all scores by watching the video recordings and checking the scores of the first staff member. The mean number of words read correctly across the three passages was used as the dependent measure of word reading ability, and the total number of questions answered correctly across the three passages served as the measure of reading comprehension.

Grammatical measures

Two measures of grammatical abilities were obtained from a 20-min sample of narrative language for each child. To collect this narrative sample, each child entered the sound booth and the experimenter explained that she had been called away for a few minutes. The equipment was set up for the child to view and hear a video of the book The Day Jimmy's Boa Ate the Wash (Noble, 1980). This story was video recorded with a narrator reading the printed material, but with separate staff members saying the material that appeared in quotes in the book. Full images of each face were shown to provide maximum opportunity for speechreading. Illustrations from the book were shown when appropriate. The experimenter explained that she hadn't seen the video story yet, and asked the child to watch carefully so it could be told to the experimenter when she returned. After the story was finished, the experimenter re-entered the sound booth, and asked the child to tell her the story in as much detail as possible. If the story retelling did not take a full 20 minutes, the experimenter supplemented the time by asking questions about personal experiences the child had that paralleled some of those of the children in the story.

The story retelling was transcribed later by members of the laboratory staff, starting at the 5-minute mark, according to methods first described by Hewitt, Hammer, Yont, and Tomblin (2005). Similar methods have subsequently been used by Moeller et al. (2010) and Nittrouer et al. (2012). Two staff members and the laboratory manager trained together on transcription methods for Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT; Miller & Iglesias, 2010), and together practiced transcribing several story retellings of children not in this study. One of the two staff members watched each video and transcribed every utterance the child produced (intelligible and unintelligible) in the 15-min segment. After completing the transcript, that staff member went back and checked it by watching the video again while reading the transcript. Then the second staff member checked the same transcript for accuracy by reading through it while watching the video. Finally, the two of them resolved any discrepancies in how specific utterances should be transcribed by watching the video together and discussing it. The entire transcription process was monitored by the laboratory manager, who spot-checked progress and served as an arbitrator if the staff members were unable to reach consensus regarding how a specific utterance should be transcribed. In addition, the laboratory manager transcribed 5% of the samples herself (3 from children with NH and 3 from children with CIs). No discrepancies at all were noted between the transcripts of the laboratory manager and those of the other staff members. The transcripts for the entire 15 minute samples were submitted to SALT, but results from just the first 100 complete and intelligible utterances were used in the current study.

In this study, two measures were used. First, mean length of utterances in morphemes (MLU) was included. Although this measure has been criticized for possibly being insensitive to differences in syntactic abilities among children after kindergarten (e.g., Crain & Lillo-Martin, 1999), it has been found to distinguish between children with language deficits and children with normal language during the elementary grades (e.g., Condouris, Meyer, & Tager-Flusberg, 2003). Consequently it was considered appropriate to use with these second-grade children with CIs, who were predicted to have delayed syntactic development.

The second SALT measure used in this study was the number of bound morphemes produced during the 100-utterance set. These morphemes included verb-related –ed, –s, –ing; noun-related plural –s and possessive –s; and adjective-related –er and –est. The number of bound morphemes in each transcript was highly correlated with MLU (for children with NH, r = .547; for children with CIs, r = .660; p < .001 in both cases), but it was nonetheless considered valuable to include this additional measure. More than most morphemes, the acquisition of bound morphemes depends on the child being able to recognize word-internal phonological structure, and that sensitivity (to word internal structure) is exactly what is required for NWR. Consequently, this grammatical skill might be especially well predicted by NWR.

Results

Reliability of NWR scores

External reliability for these scores was obtained by comparing outcomes of this study to those of Dollaghan and Campbell (1998). To do that, the total percent phonemes correct (TOTPPC) scores across all 16 stimuli for the two studies were compared. In total, there were 96 phonemes included in the 16 nonwords. Dollaghan and Campbell found mean TOTPPC (and SDs) of 84 (7) and 66 (12) for the children in their study with typical language development and children with SLI, respectively. In the current study, mean TOTPPC (and SDs) for children with NH and children with CIs were 83 (7) and 67 (12), respectively. Those similarities were considered to represent very good external reliability.

An estimate of internal reliability was obtained for these scores from the proportion of phoneme-by-phoneme agreement measured for the 20 children whose responses were scored by a second staff member (the last author). This is the same procedure as that used by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) to estimate reliability. In that earlier study, mean percent agreement in scores for two scorers across children (20% of all children) was 95%, and agreements for individual samples ranged from 91% to 99%. In the current study, a mean agreement score of 95% was obtained, with agreement scores for individual samples ranging from 90% to 100%. These outcomes were considered to represent very good reliability, and the scores from the staff member who scored all 104 samples were used in further analysis.

Data screening

All measures to be used in analyses were screened for homogeneity of variance and normal distribution. All measures were found to meet criteria to be appropriate for use in inferential and regression analyses, so no transformations were performed. In reporting statistical outcomes, precise results are given when p < 0.10; when p > 0.10, outcomes are reported simply as not significant.

Group differences

Table 1 shows means and SDs for each dependent measure included in the current study, for children with NH and children with CIs separately. In addition, outcomes of independent samples t tests performed on scores for each dependent measure are shown. All differences in group means were significant, with p < 0.01. Finally, Cohen's ds for each measure are shown. As can be seen from these values, the difference in scores for NWR between children with NH and children with CIs was the largest difference of any measure.

Table 1.

Mean scores (and SDs), along with outcomes of t tests and Cohen's ds for the dependent measures. Degrees of freedom were 102.

NH CIs t p Cohen's d
M (SD) M (SD)
Non Word Repetition
 Total Phoneme Percent Correct 83.0 (7.1) 67.5 (12.3) 7.72 <.001 1.60
Phonological Processing
 Initial Consonant Choice 87.4 (13.2) 63.1 (25.9) 5.92 <.001 1.24
 Final Consonant Choice 69.8 (17.9) 35.8 (25.6) 7.73 <.001 1.56
 Phoneme Deletion 71.5 (21.5) 47.5 (32.6) 4.35 <.001 0.89
Serial Recall
 Percent Correct 56.1 (16.5) 43.3 (15.4) 4.06 <.001 0.80
Vocabulary Knowledge
 EOWPVT Standard Score 110.0 (13.7) 94.4 (18.1) 4.92 <.001 0.98
Reading Isolated Words
 WRAT Standard Score 110.0 (11.7) 101.0 (14.6) 3.44 .001 0.68
Reading in Context
 QRI Words Correct 200.3 (5.3) 190.5 (14.7) 4.39 <.001 0.98
 QRI Comprehension 20.8 (3.0) 16.6 (6.0) 4.41 <.001 0.93
Grammar
 Mean Length of Utterance 6.3 (1.5) 5.5 (1.4) 2.67 .009 0.55
 Bound Morphemes 109.8 (33.5) 82.5 (31.4) 4.28 <.001 0.84

Bases of NWR

Figure 1 shows mean percent correct phoneme repetition for each syllable, for each group. From this figure it appears that the pattern of decrement in correct repetition was similar across the two groups, with a slight disparity for the 3-syllable stimuli: For stimuli of that length, children with NH appear to have maintained performance slightly better than children with CIs. To test for differences between groups in the pattern of decrement, a two-way, repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on these data with syllable length as the repeated measure and group as the between-subjects measure. Both main effects were found to be significant: syllable length, F(3,306) = 228.05, p < .001, η2 = .691, and group, F(1,102) = 69.40, p < .01, η2 = .405. However, the syllable length × group interaction was not significant, F(3,306) = 2.54, p = .057. The finding that the outcome was close to significant likely reflects the small discrepancy in pattern of decrement observed for stimuli three syllables in length.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mean percent correct phonemes repeated at each syllable length by children with NH and children with CIs. Error bars are standard errors of the mean.

Next, forward step-wise regression was performed on the TOTPPC scores, using the three phonological processing measures and serial recall scores as predictor variables in a one-step analysis. This procedure was undertaken specifically to examine whether the degree of phonological sensitivity, as measured by the phonological processing tasks, primarily accounted for NWR abilities, or if an additional short-term memory factor – such as operations of the central executive - might explained some additional variability for one or the other group.

A separate regression analysis was done on scores from each group. For children with NH, scores on the phoneme deletion task accounted for a significant amount of variance in TOTPPC scores, with standardized β = .519, p < .001, R2 = .27. No other variable accounted for any significant amount of additional variance; in particular, scores for the serial recall task did not account for any additional variance. For children with CIs, the initial consonant choice task accounted for a significant amount of variance in the TOTPPC scores, with standardized β = .483, p < .001, R2 = .23. No other variable accounted for any significant amount of additional variance; in particular, scores for the serial recall task did not explain any additional variance. These findings suggest that phonological sensitivity explained the most variance in NWR scores, for both groups. It is of interest that scores from the phonological processing task viewed as being developmentally the easiest explained these scores for children with CIs, who performed poorest on NWR, and scores from the developmentally most difficult phonological processing task explained NWR scores for children with NH. This trend highlights the relationship between the developmental processes of progressive refinement in phonological representations and abilities to retain a string of phonological units.

Independence from external influences on scores for children with CIs

An important consideration in assessing whether NWR would be a sensitive metric of language performance for these children with CIs is how strongly influenced scores are by factors external to the children. In order to make that determination, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were computed between TOTPPC and each demographic and audiological factor of interest: SES, age of identification of hearing loss, age of first implant, and pre-implant PTA. Gender was examined as a possible predictor of scores, using t tests. Before discussing outcomes for children with CIs, however, it is noteworthy that the correlation coefficient between TOTPPC and SES was not significant for children with NH, and no significant difference in scores was found between girls and boys. Consequently it can be concluded that these two demographic factors, found to explain some proportion of variance on other language measures, do not influence NWR for children with NH.

Returning to children with CIs, of the four demographic and audiological factors examined with correlational analyses, only age of identification of hearing loss was found to have a significant relationship with TOTPPC, r(55) = -.304, p = 0.024. The negative coefficient indicates that the younger children were when they were identified with hearing loss, the higher they scored on NWR. The magnitude of the effect indicates that age of identification accounted for roughly 9% of variance in TOTPPC at second grade. No significant difference in scores was found between these girls and boys.

Differences between groups of children with CIs were also examined, based on whether they had one or two CIs and whether or not they had a period of bimodal stimulation near the time of receiving their first CIs. For these analyses, the six children who continued to wear a hearing aid on the unimplanted ear at the time of testing were not included because they did not fit cleanly into the groups of interest.

First, differences between children with one versus two CIs were evaluated. A comparison of demographic and audiological factors for these two groups of children with CIs revealed no differences between groups on those factors. Scores for TOTPPC were 65.9 (10.6) and 69.7 (12.2) for children with one and two CIs, respectively. Although age of identification of hearing loss did not differ for children with one versus two CIs, it was incorporated into the analysis as a covariate. Nonetheless, the Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) revealed that the effect of having one or two CIs was not significant. Consequently, it can be concluded that having two CIs did not provide an advantage over having just one CI on this NWR task. For the 36 children who had two CIs at the time of testing, no significant effect of the age of receiving a second CI was observed.

Next, potential differences between children with CIs were evaluated based on whether or not they had a period of bimodal experience around the time of receiving a first CI. On NWR performance, it was found that the 20 children with some bimodal experience obtained a mean score of 75.4 (7.7) for TOTPPC and the 29 children with no bimodal experience received a mean score of 64.0 (12.0). Although age of identification did not differ for children with some bimodal and no bimodal experience, it was again used as a covariate. In this case the ANCOVA revealed that the effect of having bimodal experience was significant, F(46) = 16.75, p < .001, with a Cohen's d of 1.13. When a Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was computed on NWR scores and the length of time that children had bimodal experience, the resulting coefficient was .377; however, with only 20 subjects in the analysis it was not significant. Nonetheless, based on the ANCOVA result it may be concluded that having a period of bimodal experience around the time of receiving a first implant facilitates NWR.

In spite of finding a significant difference between bimodal groups, audiological factors were examined to see if there might actually be any group differences that could account for this effect. In this case, three differences were observed in audiological factors between groups. First, children with some bimodal experience generally received their first CIs later than children with no bimodal experience: 22 months (15 months) for children with some bimodal experience versus 16 months (6 months) for children with no bimodal experience. This difference was significant, t(47) = 2.15, p = .037. Second, children with some bimodal experience who had two implants (N = 18) received their second CIs later than children with no bimodal experience who received a second implant (N = 21). In this case, the mean age of receiving a second CI was 52 months (24 months) for children with some bimodal experience and 35 months (14 months) for children with no bimodal experience. This difference was significant, t(37) = 2.66, p = .012. Finally, pre-implant PTAs differed for these two groups of children. For children with some bimodal experience, mean pre-implant PTA was 97 dB hearing level (16 dB hearing level). For children with no bimodal experience, mean pre-implant PTA was 107 dB hearing level (11 dB hearing level). This difference was significant, t(47) = 2.63, p = .011.

In considering whether or not these group differences in audiological factors could have evoked the advantage in NWR scores observed for children with some bimodal experience, the following points are pertinent: The only audiological factor that had been found to have a significant effect on performance across all children with CIs was age of identification, and that factor did not differ for these groups. Regarding age of receiving a CI, children with some bimodal experience received both first and second implants later, on average, than children with no bimodal experience. Although not found to correlate significantly with TOTPPC in this study, later ages of receiving a CI are generally considered to negatively impact language development. Consequently, this difference between groups would only be expected to benefit children with no bimodal experience. Pre-implant PTAs did differ significantly between these groups, and in such a way that might be expected to benefit children with some bimodal experience. Thus, this potential effect was investigated more thoroughly, even though pre-implant PTAs had not been found to correlate with TOTPPC across all children with CIs. Figure 2 shows TOTPPC scores, as a function of pre-implant PTA, with symbols marking group identity. This figure clearly illustrates that TOTPPC was not related to pre-implant PTA, for either group. Consequently, the advantage observed in NWR performance for children with some bimodal experience over those with no bimodal experience could not be attributed to these differences in audiological factors.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

TOTPPC for children with CIs as a function of pre-implant PTAs.

In summary, these analyses indicated that demographic and audiological factors explained little variance in outcomes on the NWR task for children with CIs. The only exceptions to this trend was that age of identification of hearing loss accounted for roughly 9% of the variance in scores, and having had a period of time with bimodal experience provided positive effects.

Language skills predicted by NWR

A final consideration in assessing whether NWR would be a good clinical tool for use with children with CIs is how well it predicts other language skills for this group. Two approaches were taken to this question. First, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were computed between TOTPPC and scores on the language measures that had been hypothesized to be well predicted by NWR: that is, vocabulary knowledge, reading, and grammar. Specifically, correlation coefficients were computed between TOTPPC and each of the measures listed in Table 1 under Vocabulary Knowledge, Reading Isolated Words, Reading in Context, and Grammar. Again, considering outcomes first for children with NH, it was observed that three of these six measures had significant correlations with TOTPPC: EOWPVT, r(49) = .291, p = .042; WRAT, r(49) = .464, p = .001, and QRI words correct, r(49) = .298, p = .037. These findings indicate that vocabulary acquisition and word reading – whether for words in isolation or in context – were all found to depend to some extent on children's abilities to recover and store strings of well-defined phonological segments. However, only the correlation coefficient for TOTPPC and WRAT scores reached a description of being moderate in magnitude (i.e., had more than 10% of their variance in common). Reading comprehension and grammatical skills were not correlated with NWR for these children with NH at all.

Returning to the children with CIs, all six measures were found to have significant correlation coefficients: EOWPVT, r(55) = .582, p < .001, WRAT, r(55) = .488, p < .001, QRI words correct, r(55) = .592, p < .001, QRI comprehension, r(55) = .539, p < .001, MLU, r(55) = .479, p < .001, and bound morphemes, r(55) = .479, p < .001. These were all moderately strong correlations, indicating that scores on the NWR task explained between 23% and 35% of the variance on these other measures.

The second set of analyses performed to assess how well scores on the NWR task could predict performance on these other measures of vocabulary knowledge, reading, and grammar involved computing metrics of sensitivity and specificity. In these analyses, the cutpoint specified by Dollaghan and Campbell (1998) as distinguishing between poor and normal performance on the NWR task was used, and it was a TOTPPC score of 70. The decision to use this cutpoint was based on the finding that outcomes in this study involving children with CIs were similar to those of Dollaghan and Campbell for children with SLI, and it meant that 29 of the 55 children with CIs (53%) in this study scored below this cutpoint. Two specific questions were addressed in these analyses. The first question concerned how well performance below this cutpoint “ruled in” poor performance on the other language measures made in this study, for these children with CIs. This was the question of sensitivity. The second question addressed with this analysis was how well performance above this cutpoint “ruled out” poor performance on the other language measures, for these children with CIs. This was the question of specificity.

For the language measures considered here, children were categorized as having poor skills when scores were 1 SD or more below the mean of children with NH. For the two standard scores used in this study (EOWPVT and WRAT), these cutpoints were established according to published norms: that is, standard scores of 85 or less meant a child was categorized as having poor skills on vocabulary or isolated word reading. For the other four measures (QRI words correct, QRI comprehension, MLU, and bound morphemes), cutpoints were determined by subtracting one SD from the mean scores of the children with NH in this study. These values meant that children categorized as being in the affected group (i.e., one or more SD below the NH mean) were in the lowest 15% of expected performance for children with NH, a range where intervention would reasonably be expected.

Table 2 provides an example of the computations performed in these analyses, using EOWPVT as an example. In this case, a metric of sensitivity was obtained by computing the proportion of children scoring at or below 70 on the NWR task (29) who were also categorized as having poor vocabulary (15). This computation resulted in a sensitivity metric of 0.52 (15/29), indicating that TOTPPC is effective in identifying children with CIs who have poor vocabulary skills 52 percent of the time. Specificity in this example was obtained by computing the proportion of children who scored above the cutoff on TOTPPC (26) who had vocabulary skills in the normal range (23). This computation resulted in a specificity metric of 0.88 (23/29), which means that scoring above the cutoff on TOTPPC correctly identified children with normal vocabulary skills 88 percent of the time.

Table 2.

Numbers of children with CIs scoring above or below criterion for TOTPPC and above or below the cutpoint on vocabulary (EOWPVT standard scores). These numbers were used in the computation of sensitivity and specificity.

TOTPPC
≤ 70 > 70
EOWPVT ≤ 85 15 3
> 85 14 23

TOTAL 29 26

Table 3 shows metrics of sensitivity and specificity for each of the other measures used in this study. In each case, one or the other metric is at least moderately strong, suggesting that this relatively simple task (i.e., NWR) provides some predictive power regarding a range of language skills. It is evident from this table, however, that this measure of NWR was more reliable at ruling out other language problems than it was at ruling in other language problems. This pattern of result suggests that having good NWR skills was largely sufficient to support other language skills. However, having poor NWR skills was not necessarily sufficient to preclude the acquisition of other language skills. Some children scoring below the cutpoint on TOTPPC were able to perform within normal limits on the other measure being considered, an effect that is illustrated in Table 2. Those children were apparently able to compensate for those deficient skills in some other way; another skill or skills must have mediated learning in these cases.

Table 3.

Metrics of sensitivity and specificity for each measure of vocabulary knowledge, reading, and grammar employed.

Sensitivity Specificity
Vocabulary Knowledge
 EOWPVT Standard Score 0.52 0.88
Reading Isolated Words
 WRAT Standard Score 0.14 0.96
Reading in Context
 QRI Words Correct 0.72 0.62
 QRI Comprehension 0.66 0.69
Grammar
 Mean Length of Utterance 0.38 0.88
 Bound Morphemes 0.48 0.73

Discussion

The current study was undertaken to measure the abilities of children with CIs to repeat nonwords, and compare their abilities to do so to those of children with NH. This examination was conducted both to enhance our understanding of the language abilities – and deficits – of children with CIs, and to evaluate whether NWR tasks could provide useful information in clinical assessments. Four specific objectives were addressed: (1) to compare performance of children with CIs on a NWR task to that of children with NH; (2) to examine the psycholinguistic underpinnings of performance on the NWR task; (3) to assess whether performance on this NWR task is in any way biased by demographic or audiological factors; and (4) to assess how well performance on the NWR task can predict performance on other tasks of vocabulary knowledge, reading ability, and grammar for children with CIs.

Results revealed that the second grade children with CIs tested in this study performed significantly more poorly on the NWR task than age-matched peers with NH. In fact, of the 11 dependent measures used in this study, effect size (i.e., Cohen's d) was largest for the NWR task. Furthermore, two kinds of analyses suggested that the quality (detail) of phonological representations available to children explained the largest amount of variance in performance on the NWR task, for children with NH and those with CIs alike. First, for each group, one of the phonological processing tasks explained a significant amount of variance in the NWR scores. And second, scores for the short-term memory task did not explain any additional variance, after controlling for phonological processing scores. These outcomes are interpretable in light of the primary sensory impairment faced by children with CIs. Although CIs can restore sensory input to the damaged auditory systems of deaf children, the signals provided by these devices are highly impoverished when it comes to spectral structure. Phonological structure at the linguistic level is strongly dependent on spectral structure in the acoustic speech signal. Without access to detailed spectral structure in the acoustic signal, it makes sense that children who have had to develop phonological systems through CIs would not have detailed representations.

Insight into the nature of language learning and pattern of deficit for children with CIs is extended by other outcomes of this study. For example, the combined findings that: (1) children with CIs showed the same pattern of decline in NWR performance across syllable length as children with NH; and (2) scores on the serial recall task explained no variance in NWR over that explained by the phonological processing tasks suggest that these children with CIs do not share the deficits in memory processing exhibited by children with SLI. Instead, the problems encountered by children with CIs appear to arise almost exclusively from having impoverished phonological representations, a problem that could be expected due to the spectrally degraded nature of the signals they receive. Any language-related skills that depend on having refined phonological representations would likely be affected for these children. Such skills include vocabulary acquisition and reading because both of these skills are facilitated when children have access to detailed phonological representations. Some syntactic skills, such as word order within sentences, would likely not be affected as strongly by poor phonological representations because children could conceivably learn how words need to be combined, even if those words are not organized in the lexicon with highly detailed phonological structures. Support for this suggestion is garnered from the finding that the language measure showing the smallest effect size was MLU, a measure of sentence length.

The finding that the quality of phonological representations largely accounts for outcomes in the NWR task satisfies one criterion for determining if a language measure could have clinical utility: This measure appears to be based on just one psycholinguistic phenomenon for these children. Another quality of a language measure that can help make it a useful clinical tool is that the measure is an unbiased indicator of linguistic function; that is, it is not strongly influenced by factors external to the child. The measure of NWR employed in this study meets that criterion. For children with CIs, it was mildly correlated with age of identification of hearing loss, but was not correlated with any other demographic or audiological factors.

However, one other audiological factor served to distinguish children with CIs who performed well on this NWR task from those who did not perform well, and that factor involved having a period of bimodal experience around the time of receiving a first CI. That outcome makes sense in light of what is known about the kinds of signals available through CIs and hearing aids, and the kind of signal structure required to develop detailed phonological representations. The children who had some period of bimodal experience likely had more refined spectral signals available to them, even if it was only in the low-frequency region of the signal. Apparently having just these very limited signals available during early childhood provided some facilitative effect when it came to developing phonological categories. Subsequently, having somewhat better defined phonological categories supported NWR at second grade.

Finally, a useful clinical tool should be able to assign children to either the affected or unaffected group, with a fair degree of reliability. For children with CIs, the NWR task used in this study was found to correlate with the other language measures collected; these included tasks assessing skill in vocabulary knowledge, reading in isolation and in context, and grammatical abilities. Roughly a quarter to a third of the variability in these other measures was associated with performance on the NWR task, for children with CIs. Furthermore, the specificity of this NWR measure was fairly good, for a number of the language skills. Sensitivity was not exceptionally strong for the language measures that were examined, but the other advantages of this instrument should nonetheless make it a valuable tool for use by school speech-language pathologists. This NWR task is easy and fast to administer. Scoring is simple and reliable. The task is strongly based on one psycholinguistic phenomenon that, in turn, is known to underlie the acquisition of many language skills: that is, having detailed phonological representations. The NWR task used in this study was not influenced by factors external to the child taking the test. All these factors, along with good specificity, make NWR a reasonable clinical tool for use with children with CIs.

The finding that sensitivity was not as high as specificity when it came to the NWR task is informative. This trend indicates that when these children performed well on the NWR task they were very likely to perform within the normal range on whatever other language measure was being considered. In other words, having refined phonological representations was apparently a sufficient condition to acquiring these other skills. For example, specificity was strong for reading scores for words in isolation (i.e., WRAT standard scores), indicating that children who performed well on the NWR task were very likely to perform well on this word reading task, as well. That finding makes sense because having highly detailed phonological representations should be strongly related to being able to read words, in the absence of context. Finding that sensitivity was not very good on this task indicates that having poor phonological representations was not sufficient to inhibit these children in their efforts to learn to read isolated words; there were other language skills that were apparent mediators. The current study is unable to shed much light on what those mediators might be, but studies by others are helpful in this regard. In the case of word reading (for isolated words), for example, Dillon and Pisoni (2006) showed that vocabulary skills might be mediators. Accordingly, to the extent that children can acquire lexical items without those items being represented with detailed phonological structure (i.e., be more holistic in nature), children could use this lexical knowledge to facilitate the reading of isolated words. Nonetheless, more research is warranted to provide a fuller understanding of the bases of language acquisition in children with CIs.

Summary

The current study examined NWR by children with CIs, and compared their performance to that of children with NH. The goals were to better understand the language acquisition of children with CIs, and to assess whether NWR could provide a clinically useful tool for these children. Two groups of second-grade children participated: 49 with NH and 55 with severe-to-profound hearing loss who wore CIs. In addition to NWR, children were tested on ten measures of phonological processing, serial recall of words, vocabulary knowledge, reading, and grammar. Four results were most significant: (1) Children with CIs performed more poorly than children with NH on NWR; (2) The quality of phonological representations alone explained NWR performance for children in both groups; (3) No demographic factor influenced outcomes on the NWR for children in either group, but for children with CIs, two audiological factors positively influenced outcomes on NWR: being identified with hearing loss at younger ages and having some experience wearing a hearing aid on the unimplanted ear at the time of receiving a first CI; and (4) NWR scores were better able to rule out deficits in other areas of language acquisition than rule in such deficits. These outcomes provide evidence that NWR would have clinical utility in assessments of school-age children with CIs.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Grant No. R01 DC006237 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, the National Institutes of Health. The authors thank Chelsea Bates, Keri Low, and Caitlin Rice for their help in scoring, and Aaron C. Moberly for his comments on an earlier draft.

Appendix A. Initial Consonant Choice

Practice Examples
1. pet fire pack night 4. ball book seed mouth
2. blue bag fox egg 5. face pig fur top
3. cake sheep note kite 6. seal can dog sun
**Discontinue after 6 consecutive errors.

Test Trials Test Trials

1. milk date moon bag 25. clean spoon free cry
2. pear pen tile mask 26. lamb lick Juice cage
3. stick Slide drum flag 27. dog dart fall girl
4. bone meat lace bud 28. rake pig root bike
5. soap King dime salt 29. meat mice new doll
6. claw prize crib stair 30. boot cat bus push
7. leg Pin lock boat 31. nail lay nut bye
8. duck door soup light 32. stop skirt train crawl
9. plum tree star price 33. top two gum big
10. key fist cap sap 34. hen save down have
11. zip zoo web man 35. keep rock bark kiss
12. gate sun bin gum 36. clap crab tree slip
13. rug can rag pit 37. queen wheel gift quit
14. sky sleep crumb drip 38. hot hill fence base
15. fun dark pet fan 39. jog jar dig cow
16. peel wash pat vine 40. zap game zoom bed
17. grape class glue swing 41. dot pink fish dime
18. leap lip note wheel 42. bat song barn fun
19. house rain heel kid 43. fly truck fruit skip
20. toes bit girl tip 44. need nose hop draw
21. win well foot pan 45. wall deer leaf web
22. met map day box 46. van vase part like
23. sled frog brush stick 47. town dip tick king
24. jeep lock pail jug 48. glow fry drop grass

Appendix B. Final Consonant Choice

Practice Examples
1. rib mob phone heat 4. lamp rock juice tip
2. stove hose stamp cave 5. fist hat knob stem
3. hoof shed tough cop 6. head hem rod fork
**Discontinue after 6 consecutive errors.

Test Trials Test Trials

1. truck wave bike trust 25. desk path lock tube
2. duck bath song rake 26. home drum prince mouth
3. mud crowd mug dot 27. leaf suit roof leak
4. sand sash kid flute 28. thumb cream tub jug
5. flag cook step rug 29. barn tag night pin
6. car foot stair can 30. doll pig beef wheel
7. comb Cob drip room 31. train grade van cape
8. boat skate frog bone 32. bear shore clown rat
9. house mall dream kiss 33. pan skin grass beach
10. cup lip trash plate 34. hand hail lid run
11. meat date sock camp 35. pole land poke mail
12. worm price team soup 36. ball clip steak pool
13. hook mop weed neck 37. park bed lake crown
14. rain thief yawn sled 38. gum shoe gust lamb
15. horse lunch bag ice 39. vest cat star mess
16. chair slide chain deer 40. cough knife log dough
17. kite bat mouse grape 41. wrist risk throat store
18. crib job Hair wish 42. bug bus leg rope
19. fish shop Gym brush 43. door pear dorm food
20. hill moon bowl hip 44. nose goose maze zoo
21. hive glove Light hike 45. nail voice chef bill
22. milk block mitt tail 46. dress tape noise rice
23. ant school gate fan 47. box face mask book
24. dime note broom cube 48. spoon cheese back fin

Appendix C. Phoneme Deletion

Practice examples
Nonword Response
1. pin(t) _______ _______
2. (t)ink _______ _______
3. bar(p) _______ _______
4. p(r)ot _______ _______
5. no(s)t _______ _______
6. s(k)elf _______ _______
**Discontinue after 6 consecutive errors.

Test Trials

Nonword Response
1. (b)is _______ _______
2. to(b) _______ _______
3. (p)at _______ _______
4. as(p) _______ _______
5. (b)arch _______ _______
6. te(p) _______ _______
7. (k)elm _______ _______
8. bloo(t) _______ _______
9. jar(l) _______ _______
10. s(k) ad _______ _______
11. hil(p) _______ _______
12. k(r)ol _______ _______
13. (g)lamp _______ _______
14. ma(k)t _______ _______
15. s(p)olt _______ _______
16. (p)ran _______ _______
17. s(t)ip _______ _______
18. fli(m)p _______ _______
19. k(l)art _______ _______
20. (b)rok _______ _______
21. krem(p) _______ _______
22. hi(f)t _______ _______
23. dril(k) _______ _______
24. me(s)t _______ _______
25. (s)wont _______ _______
26. p(l)ost _______ _______
27. her(m) _______ _______
28. (f)rip _______ _______
29. tri(s)k _______ _______
30. star(p) _______ _______
31. fla(k)t _______ _______
32. (s)part _______ _______

References

  1. Ambrose SE, Fey ME, Eisenberg LS. Phonological awareness and print knowledge of preschool children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2012;55:811–823. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0086). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Archibald LM, Gathercole SE. Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:970–983. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/070). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Archibald LM, Gathercole SE. Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2007;14:919–924. doi: 10.3758/bf03194122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avons SE, Wragg CA, Cupples L, Lovegrove WJ. Measures of phonological short-term memory and their relationship to vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1998;19:583–602. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley AD. Working memory. Science. 1992;255:556–559. doi: 10.1126/science.1736359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baddeley AD. Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brady S, Shankweiler D, Mann V. Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1983;35:345–367. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(83)90087-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Briscoe J, Bishop DV, Norbury CF. Phonological processing, language, and literacy: A comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:329–340. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brownell R. Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT) 3rd. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications, Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  10. Caldwell A, Nittrouer S. Speech perception in noise by children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2013;56:13–30. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0338). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Campbell T, Dollaghan C, Needleman H, Janosky J. Reducing bias in language assessment: Processing-dependent measures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:519–525. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4003.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carter AK, Dillon CM, Pisoni DB. Imitation of nonwords by hearing impaired children with cochlear implants: Suprasegmental analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2002;16:619–638. doi: 10.1080/02699200021000034958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Catts HW. Phonological processing deficits and reading disabilities. In: Kamhi AG, Catts HW, editors. Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Boston: College Hill Press; 1989. pp. 101–135. [Google Scholar]
  14. Condouris K, Meyer E, Tager-Flusberg H. The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology. 2003;12:349–358. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2003/080). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Conti-Ramsden G. Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment (SLI) Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46:1029–1037. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/082). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cooper-Martin E. Measures of cognitive effort. Marketing Letters. 1994;5:43–56. [Google Scholar]
  17. Crain S, Lillo-Martin D. An introduction to linguistic theory and language acquisition. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  18. Dillon CM, Pisoni DB. Nonword repetition and reading skills in children who are deaf and have cochlear implants. Volta Review. 2006;106:121–145. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dollaghan C, Campbell TF. Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:1136–1146. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Edwards J, Beckman ME, Munson B. The interaction between vocabulary size and phonotactic probability effects on children's production accuracy and fluency in nonword repetition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:421–436. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/034). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Edwards J, Lahey M. Nonword repetitions of children with specific language impairment: Exploration of some explanations for their inaccuracies. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1998;19:279–309. [Google Scholar]
  22. Ellis Weismer S, Tomblin JB, Zhang X, Buckwalter P, Chynoweth JG, Jones M. Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2000;43:865–878. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4304.865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fox B, Routh DK. Phonemic analysis and severe reading disability in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 1980;9:115–119. doi: 10.1007/BF01067466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gathercole SE. Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition. 1995;23:83–94. doi: 10.3758/bf03210559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gathercole SE. Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2006;27:513–543. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language. 1990;29:336–360. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gathercole SE, Willis CS, Baddeley AD, Emslie H. The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory. 1994;2:103–127. doi: 10.1080/09658219408258940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hewitt LE, Hammer CS, Yont KM, Tomblin JB. Language sampling for kindergarten children with and without SLI: Mean length of utterance, IPSYN, and NDW. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2005;38:197–213. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. James D, Rajput K, Brown T, Sirimanna T, Brinton J, Goswami U. Phonological awareness in deaf children who use cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2005;48:1511–1528. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/105). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Johnson C, Goswami U. Phonological awareness, vocabulary, and reading in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53:237–261. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0139). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kamhi AG, Catts HW, Mauer D, Apel K, Gentry BF. Phonological and spatial processing abilities in language-and reading-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1988;53:316. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5303.316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Leslie L, Caldwell J. Qualitative reading inventory. New York: Pearson; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  33. Liberman IY, Shankweiler D. Phonology and the problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special Education. 1985;6:8–17. [Google Scholar]
  34. Marton K, Schwartz RG. Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46:1138. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/089). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Michas IC, Henry LA. The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1994;12:147–163. [Google Scholar]
  36. Miller J, Iglesias A. Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) (Research Version 2010) [Computer software] Madison, WI: SALT Software, LLC; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  37. Moeller MP, McCleary E, Putman C, Tyler-Krings A, Hoover B, Stelmachowicz P. Longitudinal development of phonology and morphology in children with late-identified mild-moderate sensorineural hearing loss. Ear and Hearing. 2010;31:625–635. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181df5cc2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Munson B, Kurtz BA, Windsor J. The influence of vocabulary size, phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2005;48:1033–1047. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/072). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nittrouer S. Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:925–942. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4204.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Nittrouer S. Early development of children with hearing loss. San Diego: Plural Publishing; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  41. Nittrouer S, Burton L. The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and language processing abilities in children with hearing loss. Volta Review. 2002;103:5–37. [Google Scholar]
  42. Nittrouer S, Burton LT. The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and phonological processing abilities: Evidence from 5-year-olds with histories of otitis media with effusion and low socioeconomic status. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2005;38:29–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nittrouer S, Caldwell A, Lowenstein JH, Tarr E, Holloman C. Emergent literacy in kindergartners with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing. 2012;33:683–697. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e318258c98e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Nittrouer S, Caldwell-Tarr A, Lowenstein JH. Working memory in children with cochlear implants: problems are in storage, not processing. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology. 2013;77:1886–1898. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2013.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nittrouer S, Chapman C. The effects of bilateral electric and bimodal electricacoustic stimulation on language development. Trends in Amplification. 2009;13:190–205. doi: 10.1177/1084713809346160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Nittrouer S, Miller ME. The development of phonemic coding strategies for serial recall. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1999;20:563–588. [Google Scholar]
  47. Nittrouer S, Shune S, Lowenstein JH. What is the deficit in phonological processing deficits: Auditory sensitivity, masking, or category formation? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108:762–785. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Noble TH. The day Jimmy's boa ate the wash. New York: Dial Books for Young Readers; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  49. Papagno C, Vallar G. Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1995;48A:98–107. doi: 10.1080/14640749508401378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pisoni DB, Cleary M. Measures of working memory span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing. 2003;24:106S–120S. doi: 10.1097/01.AUD.0000051692.05140.8E. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pisoni DB, Kronenberger WG, Roman AS, Geers AE. Measures of digit span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after more than 10 years of cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing. 2011;32:60S–74S. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181ffd58e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter International Performance Scale – Revised (Leiter-R) Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  53. Sáhlen B, Reuterskiöld-Wagner C, Nettelbladt U, Radeborg K. Non-word repetition in children with language impairment--pitfalls and possibilities. International Journal of Language Communication Disorders. 1999;34:337–352. doi: 10.1080/136828299247441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shriberg LD, Kwiatkowski J. Developmental phonological disorders I: A clinical profile. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1994;37:1100–1126. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3705.1100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Snider VE. The relationship between phonemic awareness and later reading achievement. The Journal of Educational Research. 1997;90:203–211. [Google Scholar]
  56. Spring C, Perry L. Naming speed and serial recall in poor and adequate readers. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 1983;8:141–145. [Google Scholar]
  57. Stanovich KE, Cunningham AE, Cramer BB. Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1984;38:175–190. [Google Scholar]
  58. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin. 1987;101:192–212. [Google Scholar]
  59. Weschler D. Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children. Third. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  60. Wilkinson GS, Robertson GJ. The Wide Range Achievement Test. 4th. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  61. Yopp HK. The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly. 1988;23:159–177. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES