Abstract
Mentalizing, the ability to attribute mental states to others and oneself, is a cognitive function with high relevance for social interactions. Recent neuroscientific research has increasingly contributed to attempts to decompose this complex social cognitive function into constituting neurocognitive building blocks. Additionally, clinical research that focuses on social cognition to find links between impaired social functioning and neurophysiological deviations has accumulated evidence that mentalizing is affected in most psychiatric disorders. Recently, both lines of research have started to employ transcranial magnetic stimulation: the first to modulate mentalizing in order to specify its neurocognitive components, the latter to treat impaired mentalizing in clinical conditions. This review integrates findings of these two different approaches to draw a more detailed picture of the neurocognitive basis of mentalizing and its deviations in psychiatric disorders. Moreover, we evaluate the effectiveness of hitherto employed stimulation techniques and protocols, paradigms and outcome measures. Based on this overview we highlight new directions for future research on the neurocognitive basis of functional and dysfunctional social cognition.
Keywords: transcranial magnetic stimulation, social cognition, mentalizing, theory of mind, autism spectrum disorders, major depressive disorder, psychiatric disorders
Introduction
In the middle of the night your neighbor is desperately trying to open your door with his key. Why is he doing that and how will you react? Knowing that he just came back from a birthday party and being aware of his drinking habits, you are able to infer that he probably falsely believes it is his door he is trying to open. Instead of calling the police you might then help him to find his own apartment. This example illustrates how our ability to understand other people's behavior by attributing mental states like beliefs, desires or intentions, also known as Theory of Mind (ToM) reasoning or mentalizing, drives social interactions.
A fast growing body of evidence suggests that mentalizing is affected in most psychiatric disorders, including but not limited to major depressive disorder (MDD; see Schreiter et al., 2013), bipolar disorder (Bora et al., 2005; Van Rheenen and Rossell, 2013), social anxiety (Ribeiro and Fearon, 2010; Samson et al., 2012), borderline personality disorder (Ghiassi et al., 2010; Mier et al., 2013), eating disorders (Schulte-Rüther et al., 2012) and neurodegenerative diseases (Le Bouc et al., 2012; Poletti et al., 2012). Moreover, it has long been hypothesized that social cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and schizophrenia result from impaired mentalizing (Brüne and Brüne-Cohrs, 2006; Frith, 2012).
However, this research is still in an early stage. Inconclusive findings (e.g., Arntz et al., 2009; Schreiter et al., 2013) and a heterogeneous conceptualization of impaired social cognition do not yet allow for drawing firm conclusions about the role of impaired mentalizing in psychiatric disorders. To resolve this ambiguity, it has been suggested to focus on the neurocognitive building blocks of mentalizing in order to find links between symptomatic impairment of social interactions and neurophysiological deviations in psychiatric disorders (Frith, 2012; Kennedy and Adolphs, 2012; Happé and Frith, 2014).
Recently, cognitive neuroscientists started to specify these neurocognitive building blocks of mentalizing using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS, Hetu et al., 2012). A coil, placed on the skull over the brain area of interest, produces a focal magnetic field which passes through the skull largely undistorted and induces neuronal depolarization in superficial cortical areas. When TMS is applied over specific brain regions in the context of a cognitive task, the interference with behavioral performance enables the study of causal relations between brain activity, cognitive processes, and behavior (Walsh and Cowey, 2000; Robertson et al., 2003; Paus, 2005).
TMS studies on mentalizing almost exclusively use repetitive TMS (rTMS), which is why the current review focuses on this method. A detailed description of recent technical and methodological issues for the application of various TMS protocols in the study of cognition is provided elsewhere (Sandrini et al., 2011). Applying rTMS has both an immediate interrupting effect on neuronal processing in the stimulated area, and a modulatory after-effect, which outlasts the stimulation period by minutes to hours (cf., Eisenegger et al., 2008). The direction of this after-effect (inhibitory/excitatory) depends on stimulation parameters and baseline activity of the stimulated area. Accordingly, rTMS can be applied during the performance of a cognitive task (online) or before task performance (offline). A third possibility is the application of single pulses which interrupt neuronal activity for a short but well-defined period, useful for identifying temporal characteristics of neurocognitive processes.
Parallel to TMS research on functional mentalizing, clinical research began to employ TMS to treat impaired mentalizing in psychiatric disorders. For this purpose, repeated sessions of rTMS have been applied over periods of several weeks for the treatment of mentalizing deficits in ASD and MDD (Enticott et al., 2011, 2014; Berlim et al., 2012).
Here, we integrate and evaluate findings of these separately emerging lines of research to show how TMS can advance our understanding of the neurocognitive basis of mentalizing and its impairment in clinical conditions, specifically in ASD and MDD. Further, Table 1 provides methodological details of hitherto available brain stimulation studies on mentalizing. We suggest that TMS combined with sensitive experimental paradigms is a promising method to specify the neurocognitive architecture of functional and dysfunctional mentalizing, which can be the key to elucidate impaired social functioning in psychiatric disorders.
Table 1.
Study | Brain region | Technique | Targeting procedure | Stimulation and Control | Paradigm | Outcome measure | Effect size* | Key findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RTPJ | ||||||||
Costa et al., 2008 | LTPJ; RTPJ | rTMS | Neuronavigation to a priori defined coordinates | 1 Hz, 15 min, 90% RMT; sham | Short stories: false belief/faux pas/control | PCR; RT | n/a | rTMS over RTPJ impaired performance in false belief task and in faux pas test |
Giardina et al., 2011 | LTPJ; RTPJ | rTMS | Neuronavigation to a priori defined coordinates | 1 Hz, 600 stimuli, 90% RMT; control (10/20 EEG position Oz) | Social interaction scenarios requiring either hostile or non-hostile intentionality attributions | Number of respective intentionality attribution responses; RT | n/a | rTMS over RTPJ increased hostile intention interpretations and decreased non-hostile intention interpretations; rTMS over LTPJ tended to show the reverse pattern |
Kelly et al., 2014 | LTPJ; RTPJ | Single-pulse TMS | fMRI-guided neuronavigation | 70% MSO; control (2 cm ant. to stimulation site) | Social attribution task: attribution of awareness to others (fMRI experiment); Visual detection task (subsequent TMS experiment) | Degree of awareness; PCR | n/a | single-pulse TMS over LTPJ/RTPJ associated with attribution of awareness to others disrupted subjects' attribution of awareness to themselves |
Silani et al., 2013 | rSMG** | rTMS | MRI-guided neuronavigation | 1 Hz, 15 min, 110% RMT; Control (vertex); (between subj. design) | Judgments of pleasantness of self- or other-experienced visuo-tactile stimulation | Stimulation pleasantness rating | η2p = 0.098 | rTMS increased egocentric empathic judgments |
Sowden and Catmur, 2013 | RTPJ | online rTMS | MRI-guided neuronavigation | 10 Hz, 6 pulses, 110% RMT; control (mid-occipital) | Imitation-inhibition task: inhibition of other-imitation; | RT; percent error | η2p = 0.25 | rTMS decreased imitation inhibition tendency |
Young et al., 2010 | RTPJ | rTMS; online rTMS (Exp.2) | fMRI-guided neuronavigation | 1 Hz, 25 min, 70% MSO (Exp.2: 10 Hz, 500 ms, 60% MSO); control (5 cm post. to RTPJ) | Moral scenarios manipulating protagonists' beliefs and action outcomes | moral judgments; RT | η2p = 0.50–0.56 | rTMS disrupted participants' mentalizing in moral judgments, especially when actions had the goal to harm others |
MPFC | ||||||||
Enticott et al., 2011 | bilateral dmPFC | rTMS | 7 cm anterior to M1 along the midline | 5 Hz, 30 10 s rTMS trains (20 s inter-train interval), 100% RMT; 9 sessions in 11 days | Interpersonal Reactivity Index: self-report measure of empathy; Autism Spectrum Quotient: self-report measure of autistic traits; Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale; Non-standardized interviews | Self-report: pre, post, 1 MO, 6 MO follow up | n/a | high-frequency rTMS lessened autistic symptoms and symptom intensity and improved social functioning in a 20-year-old woman with Asperger's syndrome |
Enticott et al., 2014 | bilateral dmPFC | rTMS | 7 cm anterior to M1 along the midline | 5 Hz, 30 10 s rTMS trains (20 s inter-train interval), 100% RMT; Sham; (between subj./double-blind design) 10 sessions every consecutive weekday | Interpersonal Reactivity Index: self-report measure of empathy; Autism Spectrum Quotient: self-report measure of autistic traits; Ritvo Autism Asperger Diagnostic Scale; Non-standardized interviews; Reading the mind in the eyes test: decoding of mental states from facial expressions; Frith-Happé-animations: mental state attribution to geometric shapes; | Self-report: pre, post, 1 MO follow up; PCR; Amount and appropriateness of mentalizing | Cohen's d = 0.41–1.41 | high-frequency rTMS (compared to sham stimulation) reduced social relating impairments and anxiety in social situations in individuals with Asperger's syndrome or high-functioning autism; No effects on experimental tasks |
Krause et al., 2012 | bilateral dmPFC | rTMS | 7 cm anterior to M1 along the midline | 1 Hz, 15 min, 100% RMT; Sham | Yoni task: cognitive and affective mentalizing; Reading the mind in the eyes test: decoding of mental states from facial expressions | PCR; RT | Cohen's d = 0.66–0.81 | rTMS reduced affective ToM performance in subjects with high self-reported empathy and increased affective ToM performance in subjects with low self-reported empathy |
Lev-Ran et al., 2012 | Ventromedial MPFC | rTMS | MRI-guided neuronavigation (based on scans from 3 subjects) to BA11,12 | 1 Hz, 100 stimuli before each of 4 subtests, 100% RMT (of right foot); Sham (to a superior temporal region); (between subj. design) | Yoni task: affective mentalizing, judgment of character's gaze direction | RT | n/a | rTMS affected learning mechanisms in the affective mentalizing task |
Schuwerk et al., 2014b | pMPFC | rTMS | 1.5 cm anterior to 1/3 of the distance from nasion to inion | 1 Hz; 33 min, 100% RMT; sham | False belief task requiring the computation of another's and one's own belief | PCR; RT | η2p = 0.24 | rTMS impaired the ability to distinguish the other's from one's own perspective |
DLPFC/IFG | ||||||||
Berlim et al., 2012 | lDLPFC | rTMS | 10/20 EEG position F3 | 10 Hz, 75 4 s rTMS trains (26 s inter-train interval); 120% RMT; daily for 4 weeks | Reading the mind in the eyes test: decoding of mental states from facial expressions; HaM-d21 (Hamilton, 1960): clinician-rated measure of major depression intensity | PCR; Rating scores | n/a | Performance in Reading the mind in the eyes test improved in proportion to major depression symptoms improvement |
Costa et al., 2008 | lDLPFC; rDLPFC; | rTMS | Neuronavigation to a priori defined coordinates | 1 Hz, 15 min, 90% RMT; sham | Short stories: false belief/faux pas/control | PCR; RT | n/a | rTMS over rDLPFC impaired performance in false belief task; rTMS over rDLPFC and lDLPFC impaired performance in faux pas test |
Kalbe et al., 2010 | rDLPFC | rTMS | MRI-guided neuronavigation, combined with 10/20 EEG position F4 and 5 cm-rule | 1 Hz, 15 min; 100% RMT; control (vertex) | Yoni task: cognitive and affective mentalizing | PCR; RT | n/a | rTMS over rDLPFC accelerated RTs in cognitive mentalizing and had no effect on affective mentalizing |
Keuken et al., 2011 | lIFG | rTMS | 10/20 EEG position F7 | 1 Hz, 5 min; 45% MSO; control (vertex, 10/20 EEG position Cz); (between subj. design) | Modification of the Reading the mind in the eyes test: decoding of mental states from facial expressions; Modification of cartoon task Brunet et al., 2000: attribution of belief and intentions; reasoning about physical causations | PCR; RT | n/a | No effect of rTMS on task performance |
RTPJ, right temporoparietal junction; LTPJ, left temporoparietal junction; rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; RMT, resting motor threshold; RT, reaction time; PCR, percentage of correct responses; (f)MRI, (functional) magnetic resonance imaging; MSO, maximum stimulator output; rSMG, right supramarginal gyrus; dmPFC, dorsomedial PFC; M1, primary motor cortex; BA, Brodmann area; MPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; (r/l)DLPFC (right/left) dorsolateral PFC, lIFG, left inferior frontal gyrus;
effect size of statistical analysis/analyses which was/were central for the interpretation of TMS effects
the rSMG as targeted in this study is anterior to the RTPJ.
What TMS reveals about mentalizing
The neurocognitive basis of mentalizing
Based on a large corpus of findings about the neurophysiological basis of mentalizing (e.g., Van Overwalle, 2009; Mar, 2011), neuroscientific methods are increasingly employed to test specific hypotheses about the neurocognitive processes that constitute mentalizing. Here, we focus on brain regions that appear to be central for mentalizing and have been targeted in TMS studies thus far, namely the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC; Figure 1).
Right TPJ and mental models of self and other
Consistently observed in neuroimaging studies on mentalizing, but little understood, is functional activity of the right TPJ (RTPJ)1. Costa et al. (2008) were the first to show that stimulation of the RTPJ interferes with mentalizing. Applying 1 Hz-rTMS impaired subjects' performance in a false belief task (questions on stories describing false beliefs about an object's location) and in a Faux Pas Test (questions on stories describing protagonists' mental states that led to awkward behavior). Consistently, Giardina et al. (2011) and Young et al. (2010) found that 1 Hz (/10 Hz-online)-rTMS over the RTPJ influenced the use of mental states in reasoning about ethical or unethical behavior of others.
Crucially, TMS can not only tell us that the RTPJ is involved in mentalizing, it also advances our understanding of its underlying function. As outlined below, recent TMS studies provide converging evidence for the idea that RTPJ's function in mentalizing is to handle internal models of one's own and other's mental states and their relation to the environment (Decety and Sommerville, 2003).
Several lines of evidence from neuroimaging and brain stimulation in single cases suggested that the RTPJ integrates multisensory input with internally stored information to form a first-person perspective, i.e., a coherent sense of one's own body situated in and distinguishable from the rest of the world (cf., Blanke et al., 2005; De Ridder et al., 2007; Ionta et al., 2011). This notion has been supported by a study in which RTPJ activity has been disrupted by single-pulse TMS during the rubber hand illusion, resulting in an impaired ability to distinguish between self-relevant (“this is part of my body”) and self-irrelevant (“this is not part of my body”) sensory information (Tsakiris et al., 2008). It was concluded that the RTPJ maintains an internal model of one's own body as a reference for self-relevance evaluations of incoming sensory information.
Heinisch et al. (2011; cf., Heinisch et al., 2012) provided evidence that the RTPJ is also involved in distinguishing self-relevant from other-relevant information. In this study subjects were presented with a picture of another person's face that morphed gradually into a picture of their own face or vice versa. The participants indicated the moment they recognized their own face, and the moment they were sure that they were seeing the face of another person, respectively. The application of 1 Hz-rTMS over the RTPJ biased this self-other discrimination toward self-face recognition at the expense of other-face recognition. Further evidence comes from a control-of-imitation task, in which the participants moved a finger either congruently or incongruently to a simultaneously observed finger movement of another person. The ability to control the tendency to imitate the other's movement was impaired by 10-Hz-online-rTMS (Sowden and Catmur, 2013)2.
In sum, these findings confirm the role of the RTPJ in mentalizing and specify that the RTPJ is critically relevant for simultaneously maintaining mental models of the self and others and their relation to the environment (“how do I perceive the world vs. how does the other perceive the world?”). This may be achieved by integrating sensory information from the environment with internally stored information (Cabeza et al., 2012) and with expectations and predictions about self and other (Koster-Hale and Saxe, 2013). By this we are able to flexibly switch between perspectives, depending on what is required in a certain situation.
DLPFC, IFG, and perspective inhibition
Evidence on the role of the DLPFC in mentalizing is still too sparse to draw firm conclusions about its neurocognitive role in mentalizing (cf., Costa et al., 2008; Kalbe et al., 2010). Further, only little TMS research has focused on the more ventral IFG, a region with much larger evidence on its involvement in mentalizing (Mar, 2011). It has been proposed that the IFG's role in mentalizing is perspective inhibition (Ruby and Decety, 2004; Ramsey et al., 2013). For example, when adopting another's perspective, one's own perspective has to be inhibited and vice versa. While the RTPJ maintains mental models of one's own and another's perspective, the IFG inhibits one of these models during perspective selection. To our knowledge only one study investigated the effect of rTMS over the left IFG (Keuken et al., 2011). In this study a relatively short 1 Hz-rTMS of 5 min had no effect on subsequent performance in two standard mentalizing tasks as compared to control stimulation. More studies employing well-suited tasks and stimulation protocols are required to test the IFG's causal role in mentalizing.
MPFC and decoupling
It was proposed that the MPFC's role in mentalizing is to subserve the decoupling mechanism, i.e., processing another's perspective independently from one's own view on the world (Leslie, 1987, 1994; Frith and Frith, 2003; Gallagher and Frith, 2003; Döhnel et al., 2012). Recent neuroimaging findings showed that during the computation of one's own and another's perspective, the posterior MPFC (pMPFC) is involved in establishing a perspective difference through inhibitory influence on temporoparietal brain regions (Schuwerk et al., 2014a). It seems that while the RTPJ maintains mental models of self and other by integrating internal (memory-based/predicted) and external (sensory) information, inhibitory influence of the pMPFC suppresses processing of external information to enable the decoupled computation of one's own and another's perspective. Consistent with this idea, inhibiting the pMPFC by 1 Hz-rTMS with a double-cone coil impaired the participant's ability to distinguish between one's own and another's perspective in a false belief task (Schuwerk et al., 2014b). In another study, 1 Hz-stimulation of the pMPFC with so-called “deep rTMS” modulated affective ToM performance in dependence of baseline empathic abilities (Krause et al., 2012). Taken together, these findings indicate that the pMPFC can be targeted by specific rTMS techniques and encourage future rTMS research focusing on that area.
TMS to study and treat dysfunctional mentalizing?
Recently, researchers began to test the therapeutic use of high-frequency rTMS on dysfunctional mentalizing in ASD and MDD. However, little is known about specifically impaired underlying neurocognitive mechanisms: ASD is characterized by widespread structural and functional brain abnormalities (Philip et al., 2012; Mueller et al., 2013). Among these, a reduced functional connectivity between the MPFC and RTPJ during mentalizing has been observed in individuals with ASD (Castelli et al., 2002; Kana et al., 2009). It can be hypothesized that the decoupled processing of one's own and another's perspective, mediated by inhibitory influence of the pMPFC to the temporoparietal cortex, is impaired in ASD. This is supported by the specific difficulty to attribute false beliefs to other people (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Senju et al., 2009), and close to the early hypothesis that a “failure of decoupling” underlies ASD (Leslie, 1987). If this were the case, could high-frequency stimulation of the pMPFC alleviate this mentalizing deficit?
In a double-blind randomized sham-controlled trial, Enticott et al. (2014) tested if high-frequency rTMS over the pMPFC improves impaired social functioning in ASD. Participants with high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome received 5 Hz-rTMS of the pMPFC on consecutive days over about 2 weeks. After active treatment as compared to sham treatment, patients reported reduced social relating impairments and anxiety in social situations (Enticott et al., 2014), as well as improved social functioning, including an increased capacity for perspective taking and empathy (Enticott et al., 2011).
Taken together, this fits with the hypothesis that the mentalizing deficit in ASD is related to impaired inhibitory influence of the pMPFC on the RTPJ. However, to provide direct evidence for this idea, future TMS studies have to show that (1) high-frequency TMS increases the connectivity between the pMPFC and RTPJ during mentalizing and (2) improves the ability to establish a perspective difference in a sensitive experimental task.
Clinical research indicates that a mentalizing deficit also plays a role in MDD (Schreiter et al., 2013). Compared to non-depressed controls, individuals with a current depressive episode showed a weaker performance in decoding mental states from facial expressions (Lee et al., 2005). Patients with a currently remitted MDD were impaired in a second-order false belief task (inferring thoughts about thoughts; Inoue et al., 2004) and had a higher risk for relapse 1 year later (Inoue et al., 2006). Also mentalizing-associated brain regions show abnormal functional activity and connectivity in depression (Aan Het Rot et al., 2009; Price and Drevets, 2012).
Is the neurocognitive basis of mentalizing affected in MDD? A prominent symptom of depressed patients is a high self-focus, i.e., a high attentional focus on oneself compared to others (e.g., Flory et al., 2000). An impaired ability to efficiently switch between one's own and another's perspective might be one contributing factor to this predominant self-focus. One's own negatively biased perspective constitutes the reference-point not only in judgments of one's own current and future situation, but also affects how one perceives the rest of the world (cf. the negative triad; Beck, 1972).
Presently, this idea remains speculative. Although a large body of evidence suggests that rTMS of the DLPFC has an antidepressant effect (Lefaucheur et al., 2014), we currently lack evidence on possible links between impaired neurocognitive components of mentalizing and depressive symptoms. To our knowledge, only one study has addressed this issue and found a relation between 10 Hz-rTMS to the left DLPFC, improved performance in a ToM task, and the alleviation of depressive symptoms (Berlim et al., 2012). However, these preliminary findings do not allow for firm conclusions about causal associations of these factors.
In sum, TMS seems to be a valuable tool in the investigation of the relationship between impaired mentalizing, its neuronal correlates, and related psychiatric disorders. At the same time it is definitely premature to claim that TMS constitutes a therapeutic strategy to improve impaired mentalizing. But in the light of accumulating evidence that brain stimulation may enhance (1) cognitive functioning in psychiatric disorders (Demirtas-Tatlidede et al., 2013) and (2) mentalizing in healthy subjects (Santiesteban et al., 2012), it can be regarded as a promising method to tackle dysfunctional mentalizing.
Effectiveness of employed stimulation methods
All previously reported stimulation techniques (online and offline rTMS, single-pulse TMS), brain site localization procedures, and most stimulation protocols produced effects of interest (Table 1). Unfortunately, only half of the TMS studies on mentalizing reviewed here reported effect sizes. In these studies the effect sizes are medium to large. To improve the evaluation of observed findings and facilitate the design of future studies, comprehensive descriptions of all methodological aspects and detailed reporting of results, including effect sizes, are highly desirable.
A critical issue appears to be the employment of sensitive experimental paradigms. Particularly, adaptations of traditional ToM tests, such as the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), ToM cartoons (Brunet et al., 2000) or social animations (e.g., Abell et al., 2000), produced heterogeneous results. Several authors stated that their employed tasks might not be sensitive enough to measure TMS-induced effects on behavioral outcome measures (e.g., Krause et al., 2012; Enticott et al., 2014; c.f., Keuken et al., 2011; Lev-Ran et al., 2012). Future studies must carefully design paradigms that allow for detecting TMS effects on reaction times and accuracy rates, the two most prominent outcome measures.
The development of sensitive mentalizing tasks for TMS research will also be critical for the evaluation of rTMS as a therapeutic approach for dysfunctional mentalizing. In a review on the role of social cognition in MDD, Schreiter et al. (2013) pointed out that especially objective measures, i.e., laboratory tasks, seem to be more reliable and sensitive than self-reports, for example.
Future directions and conclusions
To date, available TMS studies on mentalizing show that the RTPJ, DLPFC, and MPFC, identified by previous neuroimaging research on ToM, are causally involved in mentalizing. Given the correlational nature of functional magnetic resonance imaging or electroencephalography, this is a critical finding that adds to neuropsychological evidence on the causal role of those brain regions in ToM (e.g., Samson and Michel, 2013). Further, TMS is particularly suited to specify the neurocognitive building blocks of mentalizing, a current issue in ToM research. A major challenge for future research will be to develop sensitive paradigms to detect TMS-induced effects on mentalizing.
Future research should focus on the connectivity between mentalizing-associated brain regions. Both brain functions and stimulation effects are not restricted to specific regions, but have to be conceptualized as network effects. Only if we understand the brain as a network can we learn more about how the interplay of its regions underpins functional and dysfunctional social cognition (e.g., Kennedy and Adolphs, 2012). Although a set of brain regions which were linked to mentalizing by previous research is labeled “ToM network,” little is known about the critical interactions of those regions. One avenue for future research will be the simultaneous stimulation of several brain regions associated with mentalizing while another approach is the combination of brain stimulation and neuroimaging for the assessment of stimulation-induced network effects.
Claiming that TMS can be used as a therapeutic intervention for dysfunctional mentalizing is clearly premature. But, preliminary evidence for its influence on mentalizing in ASD and MDD promises that TMS, combined with sensitive paradigms, will provide insights into the dysfunction of the neurocognitive basis of mentalizing in clinical conditions. In the future, it may be possible to combine TMS with psychotherapeutic interventions in order to tackle impaired social cognition in psychiatric disorders.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank April Moeller for her help with the table and for comments on a previous version of this article.
Footnotes
1Also the left TPJ is involved in mentalizing (e.g., Samson et al., 2004; Schurz et al., 2014). However, because recent theoretical discussions focus on the RTPJ and the TMS studies reviewed here also predominantly targeted the RTPJ, we concentrate on this region in the current article.
2A similar effect was previously shown by Santiesteban et al. (2012) using the same task but a different stimulation method, namely transcranial direct current stimulation. Intriguingly, anodal tDCS (which increases cortical excitability) enhanced the ability to control imitation. Moreover, Santiesteban and colleagues showed that anodal tDCS not only facilitated the self at the expense of the other, but also vice versa. In another task, anodal stimulation facilitated the subjects' ability to inhibit their own perspective in order to adopt the perspective of another person. We point at this finding because of its high theoretical relevance. However, it is difficult to directly compare tDCS and rTMS findings as those two methods substantially differ. We refrain from a detailed discussion of this tDCS study as this falls beyond the scope of this review. For a general overview of both noninvasive brain stimulation methods see Wagner et al. (2007).
References
- Aan Het Rot M., Mathew S. J., Charney D. S. (2009). Neurobiological mechanisms in major depressive disorder. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 180, 305–313. 10.1503/cmaj.080697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abell F., Happè F., Frith U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn. Dev. 15, 1–16 10.1016/s0885-2014(00)00014-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arntz A., Bernstein D., Oorschot M., Schobre P. (2009). Theory of mind in borderline and cluster-C personality disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 197, 801–807. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181be78fb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A. M., Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46. 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J., Raste Y., Plumb I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251. 10.1111/1469-7610.00715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A. T. (1972). Depression; Causes and Treatment. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
- Berlim M. T., McGirr A., Beaulieu M. M., Turecki G. (2012). Theory of mind in subjects with major depressive disorder: is it influenced by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation? World J. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 474–479. 10.3109/15622975.2011.615861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanke O., Mohr C., Michel C. M., Pascual-Leone A., Brugger P., Seeck M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. J. Neurosci. 25, 550–557. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bora E., Vahip S., Gonul A. S., Akdeniz F., Alkan M., Ogut M., et al. (2005). Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 112, 110–116. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00570.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brüne M., Brüne-Cohrs U. (2006). Theory of mind—evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 437–455. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunet E., Sarfati Y., Hardy-Bayle M.-C., Decety J. (2000). A PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 11, 157–166. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R., Ciaramelli E., Moscovitch M. (2012). Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 338–352. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castelli F., Frith C., Happé F., Frith U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain 125, 1839–1849. 10.1093/brain/awf189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa A., Torriero S., Oliveri M., Caltagirone C. (2008). Prefrontal and temporo-parietal involvement in taking other's perspective: TMS evidence. Behav. Neurol. 19, 71–74. 10.1155/2008/694632 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J., Sommerville J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 527–533. 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demirtas-Tatlidede A., Vahabzadeh-Hagh A. M., Pascual-Leone A. (2013). Can noninvasive brain stimulation enhance cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders? Neuropharmacology 64, 566–578. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.06.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Ridder D., Van Laere K., Dupont P., Menovsky T., Van De Heyning P. (2007). Visualizing out-of-body experience in the brain. N. Engl. J. Med. 357, 1829–1833. 10.1056/NEJMoa070010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Döhnel K., Schuwerk T., Meinhardt J., Sodian B., Hajak G., Sommer M. (2012). Functional activity of the right temporo-parietal junction and of the medial prefrontal cortex associated with true and false belief reasoning. Neuroimage 60, 1652–1661. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenegger C., Treyer V., Fehr E., Knoch D. (2008). Time-course of “off-line” prefrontal rTMS effects–a PET study. Neuroimage 42, 379–384. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enticott P. G., Fitzgibbon B. M., Kennedy H. A., Arnold S. L., Elliot D., Peachey A., et al. (2014). A double-blind, randomized trial of deep Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS) for autism spectrum disorder. Brain Stimul. 7, 206–211. 10.1016/j.brs.2013.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enticott P. G., Kennedy H. A., Zangen A., Fitzgerald P. B. (2011). Deep repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation associated with improved social functioning in a young woman with an autism spectrum disorder. J. ECT 27, 41–43. 10.1097/YCT.0b013e3181f07948 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flory J. D., Räikkönen K., Matthews K. A., Owens J. F. (2000). Self-focused attention and mood during everyday social interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 875–883 10.1177/0146167200269012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Frith U. (2012). Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 2073–2092. 10.1080/17470218.2012.697178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith U., Frith C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 358, 459–473. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher H. L., Frith C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of [‘]theory of mind’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 77–83. 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00025-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghiassi V., Dimaggio G., Brüne M. (2010). Dysfunctions in understanding other minds in borderline personality disorder: a study using cartoon picture stories. Psychother. Res. 20, 657–667. 10.1080/10503307.2010.501040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giardina A., Caltagirone C., Oliveri M. (2011). Temporo-parietal junction is involved in attribution of hostile intentionality in social interactions: an rTMS study. Neurosci. Lett. 495, 150–154. 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–62. 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Happé F., Frith U. (2014). Annual research review: towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 553–577. 10.1111/jcpp.12162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinisch C., Dinse H. R., Tegenthoff M., Juckel G., Brüne M. (2011). An rTMS study into self-face recognition using video-morphing technique. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 442–449. 10.1093/scan/nsq062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinisch C., Krüger M. C., Brüne M. (2012). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the temporoparietal junction influences distinction of self from famous but not unfamiliar others. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 792–796. 10.1037/a0030581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hetu S., Taschereau-Dumouchel V., Jackson P. L. (2012). Stimulating the brain to study social interactions and empathy. Brain Stimul. 5, 95–102. 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue Y., Tonooka Y., Yamada K., Kanba S. (2004). Deficiency of theory of mind in patients with remitted mood disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 82, 403–409. 10.1016/j.jad.2004.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inoue Y., Yamada K., Kanba S. (2006). Deficit in theory of mind is a risk for relapse of major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 95, 125–127. 10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ionta S., Heydrich L., Lenggenhager B., Mouthon M., Fornari E., Chapuis D., et al. (2011). Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron 70, 363–374. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalbe E., Schlegel M., Sack A. T., Nowak D. A., Dafotakis M., Bangard C., et al. (2010). Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: a TMS study. Cortex 46, 769–780. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kana R. K., Keller T. A., Cherkassky V. L., Minshew N. J., Just M. A. (2009). Atypical frontal-posterior synchronization of theory of mind regions in autism during mental state attribution. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 135–152. 10.1080/17470910802198510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly Y. T., Webb T. W., Meier J. D., Arcaro M. J., Graziano M. S. (2014). Attributing awareness to oneself and to others. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 11, 5012–5017. 10.1073/pnas.1401201111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy D. P., Adolphs R. (2012). The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 559–572. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keuken M. C., Hardie A., Dorn B. T., Dev S., Paulus M. P., Jonas K. J., et al. (2011). The role of the left inferior frontal gyrus in social perception: an rTMS study. Brain Res. 1383, 196–205. 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.01.073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koster-Hale J., Saxe R. (2013). Theory of mind: a neural prediction problem. Neuron 79, 836–848. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krause L., Enticott P. G., Zangen A., Fitzgerald P. B. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in theory of mind: a deep rTMS study. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 87–90. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Bouc R., Lenfant P., Delbeuck X., Ravasi L., Lebert F., Semah F., et al. (2012). My belief or yours? Differential theory of mind deficits in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Brain 135, 3026–3038. 10.1093/brain/aws237 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee L., Harkness K. L., Sabbagh M. A., Jacobson J. A. (2005). Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression. J. Affect. Disord. 86, 247–258. 10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefaucheur J.-P., André-Obadia N., Antal A., Ayache S. S., Baeken C., Benninger D. H., et al. (2014). Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 2150–2206. 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leslie A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of “Theory of mind.” Psychol. Rev. 94, 412–426. [Google Scholar]
- Leslie A. M. (1994). Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition 50, 211–238. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90029-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lev-Ran S., Shamay-Tsoory S. G., Zangen A., Levkovitz Y. (2012). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs theory of mind learning. Eur. Psychiatry 27, 285–289. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mar R. A. (2011). The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 103–134. 10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mier D., Lis S., Esslinger C., Sauer C., Hagenhoff M., Ulferts J., et al. (2013). Neuronal correlates of social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 531–537. 10.1093/scan/nss028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mueller S., Keeser D., Samson A. C., Kirsch V., Blautzik J., Grothe M., et al. (2013). Convergent findings of altered functional and structural brain connectivity in individuals with high functioning autism: a multimodal MRI study. PLoS ONE 8:e67329. 10.1371/journal.pone.0067329 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paus T. (2005). Inferring causality in brain images: a perturbation approach. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 360, 1109–1114. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philip R. C. M., Dauvermann M. R., Whalley H. C., Baynham K., Lawrie S. M., Stanfield A. C. (2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 901–942. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poletti M., Enrici I., Adenzato M. (2012). Cognitive and affective theory of mind in neurodegenerative diseases: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2147–2164. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price J. L., Drevets W. C. (2012). Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 61–71. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramsey R., Hansen P., Apperly I., Samson D. (2013). Seeing it my way or your way: frontoparietal brain areas sustain viewpoint-independent perspective selection processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 670–684. 10.1162/jocn_a_00345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ribeiro L. A., Fearon P. (2010). Theory of mind and attentional bias to facial emotional expressions: a preliminary study. Scand. J. Psychol. 51, 285–289. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00797.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson E. M., Théoret H., Pascual-Leone A. (2003). Studies in cognition: the problems solved and created by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 948–960. 10.1162/089892903770007344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruby P., Decety J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 988–999. 10.1162/0898929041502661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson A. C., Lackner H. K., Weiss E. M., Papousek I. (2012). Perception of other people's mental states affects humor in social anxiety. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 43, 625–631. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson D., Apperly I. A., Chiavarino C., Humphreys G. W. (2004). Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 499–500. 10.1038/nn1223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson D., Michel C. M. (2013). Theory of mind: insights from patients with acquired brain damage, in Understanding Other Minds, eds Baron-Cohen S., Lombardo M-, Tager-Flusberg H. (Oxford: University Press; ), 164–192. [Google Scholar]
- Sandrini M., Umilta C., Rusconi E. (2011). The use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a new synthesis of methodological issues. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 516–536. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santiesteban I., Banissy M. J., Catmur C., Bird G. (2012). Enhancing social ability by stimulating right temporoparietal junction. Curr. Biol. 22, 2274–2277. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiter S., Pijnenborg G. H., Aan Het Rot M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 1–16. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulte-Rüther M., Mainz V., Fink G. R., Herpertz-Dahlmann B., Konrad K. (2012). Theory of mind and the brain in Anorexia nervosa: relation to treatment outcome. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 832–841.e811. 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schurz M., Radua J., Aichhorn M., Richlan F., Perner J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuwerk T., Döhnel K., Sodian B., Keck I. R., Rupprecht R., Sommer M. (2014a). Functional activity and effective connectivity of the posterior medial prefrontal cortex during processing of incongruent mental states. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2950–2965. 10.1002/hbm.22377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuwerk T., Schecklmann M., Langguth B., Döhnel K., Sodian B., Sommer M. (2014b). Inhibiting the posterior medial prefrontal cortex by rTMS decreases the discrepancy between self and other in theory of mind reasoning. Behav. Brain Res. 274, 312–318. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.08.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senju A., Southgate V., White S., Frith U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: an absence of spontaneous theory of mind in asperger syndrome. Science 325, 883–885. 10.1126/science.1176170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silani G., Lamm C., Ruff C. C., Singer T. (2013). Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to overcome emotional egocentricity bias in social judgments. J. Neurosci. 33, 15466–15476. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1488-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowden S., Catmur C. (2013). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in the control of imitation. Cereb. Cortex. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1093/cercor/bht306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsakiris M., Costantini M., Haggard P. (2008). The role of the right temporo-parietal junction in maintaining a coherent sense of one's body. Neuropsychologia 46, 3014–3018. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858. 10.1002/hbm.20547 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Rheenen T. E., Rossell S. L. (2013). Picture sequencing task performance indicates theory of mind deficit in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 151, 1132–1134. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner T., Valero-Cabre A., Pascual-Leone A. (2007). Noninvasive human brain stimulation. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 9, 527–565. 10.1146/annurev.bioeng.9.061206.133100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh V., Cowey A. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 73–79. 10.1038/35036239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L., Camprodon J. A., Hauser M., Pascual-Leone A., Saxe R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 6753–6758. 10.1073/pnas.0914826107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]