Skip to main content
Taylor & Francis Open Select logoLink to Taylor & Francis Open Select
. 2014 Jun 11;19(3):279–292. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2014.919599

Risk perception, motives and behaviours in university students

P Salameh a,*, J Salamé b, M Waked c, B Barbour d, N Zeidan d, I Baldi e
PMCID: PMC4235506  PMID: 25431509

Abstract

Risky behaviours among young people are relatively frequent, with several motives and attitudes lying behind. Our objective was to evaluate the role of risk perception, attractiveness and motives for risk behaviour taking among university students in Lebanon. A cross-sectional study was carried out using a proportionate cluster sample of Lebanese students in public and private universities. Items of risk intake and perception scale, attractiveness of risky behaviours, and motives for risky behaviours were assessed, in addition to cigarette and waterpipe smoking and dependence, alcohol problematic consumption and mental distress scale. After verifying the validity of scales and reliability in the university students' population, we found that risk perception was associated with lower risk intake, while risk attractiveness was a driver for it. Moreover, motives differed in their driving of risky behaviour, a particular point was that women indicated more goal achievement objectives, the latter concept was associated with lower risk taking. University students in Lebanon, women in particular, demonstrated wiser behaviour and may benefit from heath education programme to increase their awareness about risky behaviours. Identifying other personal, environmental, social and psychological predictors may also be important to improve effectiveness of these programmes.

Keywords: risk perception, risk benefit, motives, risk behaviour, university student

Introduction

Risky behaviours among young people are relatively frequent, and risk taking has been defined from several perspectives: from the decision-making perspective, Irwin defines it as ‘a volitional behavior whose outcome is uncertain and probably the reason of negative consequences’ (Irwin, 1990; Irwin & Millstein, 1991). Moore and Gullone (1996) defined risk taking behaviour as ‘behavior which involves potential negative consequences but is balanced in some way by perceived positive consequences’. Moreover, the reasons behind their risk taking have been explained by different theories and models (Igra & Irwin, 1996): the cognitive theory of risk taking is among the most popular and has investigated the development of decision-making capacities that potentially underlie risk-taking development, including sensitivity to risk, probability estimation and perceptions of vulnerability (Boyer, 2006). According to some researchers, adolescents often make rational decisions based on an appreciation of risk, and misjudgements are more likely to be the result of inexperience than of irrational decision-making, undeveloped cognitive abilities or a perception of personal invulnerability (Rodham, Brewer, Mistral, & Stallard, 2006).

Yet, risk taking has been presented as a precursor of problematic behaviour (Jessor & Jessor, 1977), leading to self destruction, psychological, social and health compromising situations (Ingersoll & Orr, 1989). However, risk taking has also been shown to be an important part of development into adulthood, particularly if it is goal directed (Jessor, 1991); risk taking is not merely for sensation seeking, but it sometimes has aims on a personal level (Shapiro, Siegel, Scovill, & Hays, 1998) or relational level (Engels & ter Bogt, 2001). A modest degree of risk-taking in adolescence seems to be normative and associated with some positive psychological characteristics (Shedler & Block, 1990).

To our knowledge, the majority of undertaken works on youth risk perception, motives and behaviours took place in developed countries; very few were published in the Middle Eastern Arab countries, Lebanon in particular. Because university students in Lebanon take risks frequently (Karam, Ghandour, Maalouf, Yamout, & Salamoun, 2010; Salameh et al., 2012a), and as risk taking is a culturally sensitive issue (Kloep, Guney, Cok, & Simsek, 2009), our objective was to evaluate the role of risk perception, attractiveness and motives for risk taking behaviour among university students in Lebanon.

Methods

Population and sampling

A cross-sectional study was carried out using a proportionate cluster sample of Lebanese students in public and private universities. A list of universities in Lebanon, provided by the Center for Pedagogic Researches, was used to adjust the sample size (Center for Educational Research and Development, 2010). A sample size of at least 3000 individuals was targeted to allow for adequate power for bivariate and multivariate analysis to be carried out.

Although ethical approval was granted by all Internal Review Boards of respective institutions where the study was conducted, most universities' administrative offices in Lebanon that we approached did not allow drawing a random sample of their enrolled students to participate in the study: they did not provide us with the lists of students and permission was not granted to enter classrooms and search for students nominatively. Thus, our research group had to work with a non-random sample of students outside their classes. Students were approached on campus during break times between courses by a field worker.

The latter explained the study objectives to the student, and after obtaining oral consent, the student was handed the anonymous and self-administered questionnaire. On average, the questionnaire was completed by participants within approximately 20 minutes. At the end of the process, the completed questionnaires were placed in closed boxes and sent for data entry. During the data collection process, the anonymity of the students was guaranteed. Out of 4900 distributed questionnaires, 3384 (69.1%) were returned to the field worker. Further methodological specifics are presented in more detail elsewhere (Salameh et al., 2012a; Salameh, Khayat, & Waked, 2012b; Salameh, Khayat, Waked, & Dramaix 2012c).

Questionnaires

The questionnaire used in this study was composed of several parts, including the socio-demographic part, and a detailed active and passive smoking history. For risk perception assessment, risk involvement and perception scale (RIPS) score items were used (Mantzouranis & Zimmermann, 2010; Shapiro et al., 1998; Siegel et al., 1994); additional items that were deemed adapted to the local culture by the research group were also included: for every item, a Likert scale question was asked about how ‘dangerous’ the behaviour was considered by the university student, ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Moreover, questions about perceived benefit from risky behaviours and how ‘attractive’ the same items were found by university students were asked. A scale of ‘attractiveness of risky behaviours’ (ARB) was thus constructed, using Likert scale evaluation ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Furthermore, ‘motives for risk taking’ (MRT) were evaluated using items from a cross-cultural scale (Kloep et al., 2009).

Socio-economic status was evaluated using the ratio of mean income per household over the number of persons in the family. Mental distress was measured using the Beirut Distress Scale-22 (BDS22) (Salameh & Barbour, 2011). Current cigarette and waterpipe consumption were evaluated. For cigarette dependence, we assessed the Young Adults Cigarette Dependence (YACD) scale (Salameh et al., 2013) and for waterpipe dependence, the Lebanese Waterpipe Dependence Scale-11 (LWDS-11) (Salameh, Waked, & Aoun, 2008), both of which were developed by our team for the Lebanese population. The YACD was developed for university students; it is composed of 16 items, loading over six factors: nicotine dependence, craving intensity, positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement (Salameh et al., 2013).

Statistical analysis

Data were entered and analysed using SPSS, version 18.0. Spearman correlation coefficients were calculated for scales and subscales correlations.

Factor analyses using the principal component analysis method were performed to allow for items loading over subscales of respective scales: ‘RIPS’, ‘ARB’ and ‘MRT’. For each factor analysis, Kaiser–Meyer–Oklin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy was calculated, along with Bartlett's test of sphericity. Items with communality < 0.3 or loading on factors < 0.4 were removed from the scales. Pattern matrix after Promax rotation was reported because factors were correlated. Reliability of scales and subscales was also evaluated using Cronbach's α measurement.

Afterwards, the scales were face validated using several risky behaviours as dependent variables (current smoking cigarettes, current cigarette dependence, current waterpipe smoking, current waterpipe dependence and problematic use of alcohol), the validated scales (RIPS, ARB and MRT) as major independent variables and studying in a private university, age, sex, field of studies, region, mental distress (BDS22) and socio-economic status as covariates. For these purposes, multivariate stepwise descending likelihood ratio logistic regressions (in case of dichotomous dependent variables) and stepwise multiple regressions (in case of continuous dependent variables) were used. Application conditions were verified before the final models were accepted.

Results

Risk involvement and perception scale

For the RIPS, the KMO measure of sampling adequacy was 0.922, Bartlett's test of sphericity was p < 0.001 and 54.43% of variance were explained by the items. Out of 38 risky behaviours, 31 items were considered dangerous by university students and loaded on seven scales, explaining 54% of the total variance (Table 1). The first factor included most dangerous defying authorities or law-breaking behaviours, such as stealing, damaging properties or gambling for money. The second factor included drinking and driving behaviours, the third factor encompassed items about calculated risk, such as defending an unpopular idea and volunteering to speak in front of a group. The fourth factor was about minor academic misconduct such as skipping courses and cheating during an exam. The fifth factor was about exaggeration behaviours such as binge eating and having sex with more than one person, the sixth factor about speeding in a car and driving without a seatbelt. Finally, the seventh factor was about smoking cigarettes and waterpipes. Reliability measure for the total scale was 0.894, while those of subscale ranged between 0.921 for factor 1 (most dangerous behaviours) and 0.440 for the fifth factor (exaggeration behaviours) (Table 1).

Table 1 . Risk intake and danger perception (RIPS) items (n = 31 items).

Items considered ‘dangerous’ Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 6 Factor 7 Cronbach's α
Nicking something from a shop 0.865             0.921
Stealing something in a restaurant 0.852              
Damaging something such as a public phone 0.827              
Cheating in a school project 0.713              
Gambling for money 0.688              
Defying authorities 0.640              
Carrying weapons 0.605              
Getting involved in a fight 0.575              
Accepting ride from a stranger 0.507              
Hitchhiking 0.491              
Deliberately annoying somebody 0.462              
Consuming cocaine 0.427              
Taking chances in street traffic 0.409              
Getting drunk   0.751           0.739
Driving after alcohol drinking   0.745            
Being in a car with a drunk driver   0.634            
Drinking alcohol   0.606            
Defending an unpopular idea     0.841         0.700
Volunteering to speak in front of a group     0.798          
Organising an activity for friends     0.730          
Skipping courses       0.725       0.592
Cheating on an exam       0.608        
Not studying on an exam       0.500        
Taking a sunbath       0.481        
Binge eating         0.731     0.440
Having sex with more than one person         0.651      
Driving a car           0.821   0.521
Speeding in a car           0.622    
Driving without a seatbelt           0.559    
Smoking waterpipe             0.851 0.760
Smoking cigarettes             0.796  
Correlation with total scale 0.896 0.538 0.263 0.695 0.517 0.444 0.487 p < 0.001 for all

Attractiveness of risky behaviours

For the ARB scale, the KMO measure of sampling adequacy was 0.932, Bartlett's test of sphericity was p < 0.001 and 57.74% of variance were explained by the items. Out of 38 items, 34 items were considered attractive by university students and loaded over eight factors, explaining 57.74% of the total variance of ARB (Table 2): the first factor involved ‘getting away’ behaviours such as stealing, damaging a public apparatus or cheating in a school work; the second factor was about ‘facing danger’ behaviours, such as contact sports, carrying weapons, fight involvements and so on. The third factor mainly involved licit and illicit substances consumption (alcohol, drugs, medications, …). The fourth factor included calculated responsible behaviours, the fifth factor was about sexual activity, the sixth factor was about relaxing behaviours (smoking and sunbathing), the seventh was about minor academic misconduct and the final eighth factor was about driving and speeding. Reliability α was 0.91 for the full scale, ranging from 0.877 for getting away behaviours to 0.479 for minor academic misconduct (Table 2).

Table 2 . ARB items (n = 34 items).

Items considered ‘attractive’ Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 6 Factor 7 Factor 8 Cronbach's α
Nicking something from a shop 0.857               0.877
Stealing in a restaurant 0.801                
Damaging something such as a public phone 0.778                
Hitchhiking 0.656                
Accepting ride from a stranger 0.651                
Cheating in a school work 0.447                
Contact sport participation   0.716             0.840
Carrying a weapon   0.701              
Getting involved in a fight   0.681              
Defying authorities   0.674              
Riding a motorcycle   0.635              
Taking chances in street traffic   0.630              
Deliberately annoying somebody   0.618              
Driving after alcohol drinking     0.720           0.825
Being in a car with a drunk driver     0.639            
Getting drunk     0.589            
Misusing prescription drugs     0.571            
Consuming cocaine     0.554            
Binge eating     0.540            
Crash dieting or taking diet pills     0.528            
Volunteering to speak in front of a group       0.812         0.745
Organising an activity for friends       0.750          
Defending an unpopular idea       0.741          
Having sex with a condom         0.871       0.635
Having sex without a condom         0.776        
Having sex with more than one person         0.523        
Smoking waterpipe           0.698     0.585
Smoking cigarettes           0.678      
Taking a sunbath           0.458      
Skipping courses             0.854   0.479
Cheating on exams             0.510    
Driving a car               0.769 0.550
Speeding in a car               0.551  
Driving without a seatbelt               0.434  
Correlation with total scale 0.786 0.834 0.741 0.241 0.598 0.607 0.561 0.612 p < 0.001 for all

Motives for risk taking

For the MRT scale, the KMO measure of sampling adequacy was 0.917, Bartlett's test of sphericity was p < 0.001 and 61.81% of variance were explained by the items. Twenty-six items were loaded on six factors as follows: the first factor includes motives of irresponsibility (not thinking about the consequences and recklessness); the second factor includes motives of novelty and sensation seeking and the third factor includes hedonistic motives (living in the moment). The fourth factor includes motives of social desirability (getting the attention and care of others, impressing others, etc.), the fifth factor includes achieving future goals and success and the sixth factor includes popularity seeking. Reliability α was 0.909 for the full scale, ranging from 0.874 for searching novelty by risky behaviours to 0.509 for seeking popularity (Table 3).

Table 3 . MRT items (n = 26 items).

Items considered motives for risky behaviours Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Factor 6 Cronbach's α
When taking risks, nothing can go wrong 0.794           0.837
I do not care if I have to regret it later 0.775            
Most of the times I do not believe I will get hurt 0.770            
I believe risky behaviours will not hurt me at all 0.760            
I care less about consequences of my behaviours 0.699            
I hate being careful 0.599            
I cannot stay away from these behaviours 0.522            
These behaviours make my heart beat faster   0.974         0.874
These behaviours are thrilling   0.948          
To feel the excitement is wonderful   0.914          
These behaviours give me a kick   0.615          
I do not want to miss enjoying the experience   0.496          
These behaviours give me courage to try new things   0.473          
Trying new things make me happy   0.423          
I love to live the moment     0.887       0.703
Even though I would have to pay, it is important for me to live the moment     0.830        
It is important to enjoy the moment     0.717        
I only live once, and I want to try everything     0.430        
With these behaviours, I get the attention of others       0.940     0.787
These behaviours let other people care for me       0.916      
These behaviours let me impress others       0.734      
Others expect me to behave like this       0.420      
I want to achieve goals that lead to future success         0.930   0.674
I think it is important to achieve goals in the future         0.839    
It is important for me to be popular           0.666 0.509
I enjoy acting ‘cool’           0.541  
Correlation with total scale 0.767 0.885 0.676 0.667 0.279 0.510 p < 0.001 for all

Sample description

In Table 4, we describe the risk perception, attractiveness and motives among boys and girls of Lebanese universities. Boys have more motives and attractiveness for risky behaviours, while girls have a higher perception of danger (p < 0.001 for all scales and subscales). There is one exception that is worth noting: girls have higher motives for goals' achievement (p = 0.010). This is why in multivariate analysis we kept the RIPS and ARB scales as is, while motives for risky behaviours were analysed separately.

Table 4 . Risk perception, attractiveness and motives among university students.

Scales and factors Boys, n = 1399 (41.3%) Girls, n = 1980 (58.5%) p-Value Total, n = 3384 (100%)
Has motives for risky behaviours (MRT) 48.10 (17.54) 40.55 (15.63)  < 0.001 43.59 (16.84)
Risk perception (RIPS) 72.49 (19.83) 83.91 (15.57)  < 0.001 79.24 (18.33)
ARB 57.79 (22.44) 47.77 (25.83)  < 0.001 51.96 (24.96)
Irresponsibility (Motive 1) 8.52 (5.68) 6.54 (5.20)  < 0.001 7.34 (5.49)
Novelty seeking (Motive 2) 15.35 (6.60) 11.64 (6.24)  < 0.001 13.14 (6.64)
Seizing the moment (Motive 3) 9.84 (3.81) 9.24 (3.64)  < 0.001 9.48 (3.72)
Social desirability (Motive 4) 4.07 (3.30) 3.00 (2.86)  < 0.001 3.43 (3.09)
Goals achievement (Motive 5) 6.17 (2.09) 6.37 (1.89) 0.010 6.29 (1.98)
Popularity seeking (Motive 6) 3.87 (2.03) 3.61 (2.00)  < 0.001 3.72 (2.02)

Multivariate analyses for selected risky behaviours

In Table 5, we show the multivariate analyses for selected risky behaviours. A higher RIPS was associated with lower cigarette smoking, cigarette dependence, lower waterpipe dependence and lower problematic alcohol drinking. ARB was associated with higher cigarette smoking, waterpipe smoking and problematic alcohol drinking.

Table 5 . Multivariate analyses of risky behaviours.

Dependent Independent variables Adjusted OR [95% CI] p-Value Model
Current cigarette smoking Studying in a private university 1.52 [1.10; 2.10] 0.011 Nagelkerke R2 = 0.376
  Field of studies (health vs. others) 0.29 [0.18; 0.47]  < 0.001  
  Older age in years 1.08 [1.01; 1.15] 0.036  
  South Lebanon versus others 0.34 [0.17; 0.68] 0.002  
  Mental distress (BDS22) 10.01 [1.00; 1.02] 0.010  
  Female sex versus male 0.26 [0.19; 0.36]  < 0.001  
  Higher socio-economic level 1.22 [1.07; 1.39] 0.003  
  ARB 1.01 [1.00; 1.02]  < 0.001  
  Risk perception (RIPS) 0.99 [0.98; 0.996] 0.003  
  Irresponsibility (Motive 1) 1.08 [1.05; 1.10]  < 0.001  
  Seizing the moment (Motive 3) 1.11 [1.06; 1.16]  < 0.001  
  Goals achievement (Motive 5) 0.93 [0.87; 1.00] 0.052  
Current waterpipe smoking Field of studies (health vs. others) 0.59 [0.41; 0.85] 0.004 Nagelkerke R2 = 0.126
  Female sex versus male 0.78 [0.60; 0.96] 0.023  
  Northern Lebanon versus others 0.53 [0.34; 0.83] 0.005  
  ARB 1.01 [1.005; 1.014]  < 0.001  
  Seizing the moment (Motive 3) 1.11 [1.07; 1.15]  < 0.001  
  Social desirability (Motive 4) 1.06 [1.02; 1.10] 0.001  
  Goals achievement (Motive 5) 1.06 [1.00; 1.13] 0.072  
    Adjusted β [95%CI]    
Current cigarette dependence (YACD) Risk perception (RIPS)  − 0.07 [ − 0.10; − 0.04]  < 0.001 Adjusted R2 = 0.241
  Irresponsibility (Motive 1) 0.16 [0.05; 0.26] 0.003  
  Social desirability (Motive 4) 0.21 [0.02; 0.40] 0.027  
  Goals achievement (Motive 5)  − 0.38 [ − 0.67; − 0.10] 0.009  
  Popularity seeking (Motive 6) 0.43 [0.17; 0.70] 0.001  
Current waterpipe dependence (LWDS11) Older age in years 0.65 [0.35; 0.94]  < 0.001 Adjusted R2 = 0.189
  Female sex versus male 1.36 [0.26; 2.46] 0.016  
  Risk perception (RIPS)  − 0.06 [ − 0.09; − 0.03]  < 0.001  
  Novelty seeking (Motive 2) 0.30 [0.27; 0.38]  < 0.001  
  Goals achievement (Motive 5)  − 0.32 [ − 0.61; − 0.03] 0.031  
Problematic use of alcohol (ASSIST) Female sex versus male  − 3.50 [ − 4.11; − 2.88]  < 0.001 Adjusted R2 = 0.260
  Older age in years  − 0.26 [ − 0.40; − 0.11]  < 0.001  
  Socio-economic status 0.81 [0.56; 1.07]  < 0.001  
  Studying in a private university 1.72 [1.13; 2.32]  < 0.001  
  ARB 0.02 [0.01; 0.03] 0.007  
  Risk perception (RIPS)  − 0.02 [ − 0.04; − 0.01] 0.006  
  Irresponsibility (Motive 1) 0.07 [0.003; 0.13] 0.041  
  Novelty seeking (Motive 2) 0.08 [0.01; 0.14] 0.019  
  Seizing the moment (Motive 3) 0.23 [0.13; 0.32]  < 0.001  
  Social desirability (Motive 4) 0.19 [0.09; 0.30]  < 0.001  
  Goals achievement (Motive 5)  − 0.15 [ − 0.30; − 0.04] 0.044  
  Popularity seeking (Motive 6)  − 0.19 [ − 0.33; − 0.04] 0.014  
  Mental distress (BDS22) 0.03 [0.01; 0.05] 0.015  

As for motives for risky behaviours, irresponsibility was associated with more cigarette smoking and dependence and problematic alcohol drinking. Novelty seeking was associated with waterpipe dependence and problematic use of alcohol, while seizing the moment was associated with cigarette smoking, waterpipe smoking and problematic alcohol drinking. Social desirability was associated with waterpipe smoking, cigarette dependence and problematic alcohol drinking. Goals achievement was inversely related to all risky behaviours, while popularity seeking was correlated to cigarette dependence and inversely correlated with problematic alcohol drinking (Table 5).

Discussion

In this study, we were able to verify the validity and reliability of three scales among Lebanese university students: RIPS, ARB and MRT. Their structures and psychometric properties were not different from the ones found in initial development articles (Kloep et al., 2009; Mantzouranis & Zimmermann, 2010; Shapiro et al., 1998; Siegel et al., 1994).

Moreover, we found that a higher RIPS was associated with lower cigarette smoking, cigarette dependence, lower waterpipe dependence and lower problematic alcohol drinking; risk perception was particularly more common among females. Thus, perceiving the danger of risky behaviours is a driver towards cautiousness among young Lebanese people, similar what is reported by Arab (Al-Kaabba, Saeed, Abdalla, Hassan, & Mustafa, 2011; Shafig et al., 2006) and Chinese researchers (Chueh, Ding, Yao, Huang, & Hung, 2013). These results go in favour of a rational decision-making theory of risk in our population; thus, health education programmes to increase awareness among university students are important to decrease health risk taking behaviours in Lebanon, females in particular. However, our results contradict those of Siegel et al. (1994) and Mantzouranis and Zimmermann (2010), who found that risk perception was not associated with less risky behaviours; moreover, health literacy was not found to be sufficient to decrease risky behaviour (Dermota et al., 2013). This discrepancy may be due to a cultural issue that needs to be addressed in specific studies.

In contrast, ARB was associated with higher cigarette smoking, waterpipe smoking and problematic alcohol drinking: thinking that risky behaviours were attractive was a driver towards adopting these behaviours. These results parallel those of Siegel et al. (1994) and Mantzouranis and Zimmermann (2010), where risky behaviour-associated benefits drive risk intake. However, ARB does not seem to interfere with dependence to cigarettes and waterpipe, which is a concept that includes nicotine addiction and other dimensions that lie beyond individual decision making in young people (Salameh et al., 2008, 2013).

As for motives for risky behaviours, irresponsibility and not thinking about consequences were associated with more cigarette smoking and dependence and problematic alcohol drinking for some individuals; this type of motive was particularly prevalent among males. This goes in line with others' findings (Ben-Zur & Reshef-Kfir, 2003), where risk taking was associated with its perceived benefit without taking into account the negative consequences. Similarly, novelty and sensation seeking was associated with waterpipe dependence and problematic use of alcohol, while seizing the moment was associated with cigarette smoking, waterpipe smoking and problematic alcohol drinking. These types of hedonistic motives have been shown to be associated with cigarette and waterpipe smoking (Hampson, Tildesley, Andrews, Barckley, & Peterson, 2013), alcohol consumption (Weiland et al., 2013), but also with more dangerous behaviours, such as substance misuse (Chakroun, Doron, & Swendsen, 2004) and delinquency (Peach & Gaultney, 2013). Our population, males in particular, could be at risk for this type of behaviour; preventive measures should be taken to avoid these dangerous consequences.

For some individuals, social desirability was associated with waterpipe smoking, cigarette dependence and problematic alcohol drinking, while popularity seeking was correlated to cigarette dependence and inversely correlated with problematic alcohol drinking. The fact that risk taking is associated with making friends and sociability has been shown by several researchers (Amin, Amr, Zaza, & Kaliyadan, 2012; Bonino, Cattelino, & Ciairano, 2005; Dworkin, 2005; Engels & ter Bogt, 2001). Popularity seeking was also shown by Shapiro et al. (1998), in addition to relieving stress perspectives; in fact, we found that mental distress was correlated with higher probability of cigarette smoking and problematic use of alcohol. This association has also been found by Curry and Youngblade (2006), where anger and negative affect directly predicted risky behaviours; other researchers also found similar results (Tavolacci et al., 2013). The role of mental distress remains to be more thoroughly studied in specific studies, perhaps in clinical settings.

In contrast to all risky behaviour motives, goals achievement was significantly more common among females and was inversely related to all assessed risky behaviours. This also goes in favour of a cognitive decision-making theory of risk taking in our population, females in particular. Several studies carried out in the Western world did not find such results: females are currently tending to adopt health-related behaviours similar to males in these societies (Bucksch, Finne, Glücks, Kolip, & HBSC-Team Deutschland, 2012; MacArthur et al., 2012), or even worse (Pritchard & Cox, 2007). In our population, this seems to be another culturally sensitive issue, affected by religiosity and social norms acceptability that still differ between males and females in Arab countries (Sika, 2011).

There is one particular finding in our study that deserves our attention: waterpipe smoking was neither associated with risk perception nor with irresponsibility motive. This corroborates the fact that waterpipe is misconceived as less harmful than cigarettes (Akl et al., 2013; Heinz et al., 2013), and it is frequently consumed among young people (Waked, Salameh, & Aoun, 2009) and women (Salameh et al., 2012b). Similar results were found by Primack et al. (2008). Waterpipe smoking seems to be positioned by university students out of the commonly known risky behaviours. Because waterpipe smoking and dependence have been shown to have negative health consequences (Akl et al., 2010; Salameh et al., 2012c), additional efforts should be made by concerned authorities to increase awareness on health-associated consequences and dangers of waterpipe smoking and dependence.

Our study, as with any, has its limitations: a selection bias could have been possible because the sample is not a random sample and may not be representative of the young adults and students' population in Lebanon. This non-random sampling could lead to an overrepresentation of students who skip classes and may have higher risky behaviours; however, on the other hand, the length of the questionnaire may also lead to an underrepresentation of this students' category, leading to compensation of the latter phenomenon. There could also be a possibility of respondent and information bias because the results of our study are based on a self-administered and completed questionnaire. The cross-sectional nature of the study also precludes causality assessment due to temporality issues.

Despite the fact that we ensured anonymity and confidentiality of all data that have been collected, respondents may have underreported some of their behaviours that lead to missing values. Thus, the assessment of health risk behaviours may have been underestimated in our study. We suggest that further research be carried taking into account these limitations; we also suggest qualitative research that can explore the knowledge, attitudes and values behind the risky behaviours in order to identify their personal, environmental, social and psychological predictors.

Conclusion

University students in Lebanon, women in particular, demonstrated better perception of risks and wiser associated behaviour than males; all may benefit from heath education programmes to increase their awareness about risky behaviours. A focus on dangers of waterpipe smoking is also suggested because knowledge about its dangers seems deficient. Identifying additional personal, environmental, social and psychological predictors of risky behaviours may also be important to improve effectiveness of these programmes.

Funding Statement

This work was granted by CEDRE program for Franco-Lebanese cooperation in research [grant number 118/2009].

Footnotes

1. The Lebanese National Conference on Health in University research group also includes: L. Jomaa, C. Issa, G. Farhat, H. Zeghondi, N. Gerges, M.T. Sabbagh, and M. Chaaya.

for the Lebanese National Conference on Health in University Research Group1

References

  1. Akl E. A., Gaddam S., Gunukula S. K., Honeine R., Jaoude P. A. Irani J. The effects of waterpipe tobacco smoking on health outcomes: A systematic review. International Journal Epidemiology. 2010;39:834–857. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akl E. A., Jawad M., Lam W. Y., Co C. N., Obeid R. Irani J. Motives, beliefs and attitudes towards waterpipe tobacco smoking: A systematic review. Harm Reduction Journal. 2013;10:12. doi: 10.1186/1477-7517-10-12. doi:10.1186/1477-7517-10-12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Al-Kaabba A. F., Saeed A. A., Abdalla A. M., Hassan H. A. Mustafa A. A. Prevalence and associated factors of cigarette smoking among medical students at King Fahad Medical City in Riyadh of Saudi Arabia. Journal of Family and Community Medicine. 2011;18:8–12. doi: 10.4103/1319-1683.78631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amin T. T., Amr M. A., Zaza B. O. Kaliyadan F. Predictors of waterpipe smoking among secondary school adolescents in Al Hassa, Saudi Arabia. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2012;19:324–335. doi: 10.1007/s12529-011-9169-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ben-Zur H. Reshef-Kfir Y. Risk taking and coping strategies among Israeli adolescents. Journal of Adolescence. 2003;26:255–265. doi: 10.1016/s0140-1971(03)00016-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bonino S., Cattelino E. Ciairano S. Milan: Springer-Verlag; 2005. Adolescents and risk; Behaviors, functions and protective factors; pp. 99–142. [Google Scholar]
  7. Boyer T. W. The development of risk-taking: A multi-perspective review. Developmental Review. 2006;26:291–345. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bucksch J., Finne E., Glücks S., Kolip P. HBSC-Team Deutschland Time trends in sex differences in adolescents' health behaviour from 2001 to 2010. Gesundheitswesen. 2012;74:S56–S62. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1312635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Center for Educational research and Development, Beirut, Lebanon . List of universities in Lebanon. Retrieved from http://www.crdp.org/CRDP. [Google Scholar]
  10. Chakroun N., Doron J. Swendsen J. Substance use, affective problems and personality traits: Test of two association models. Encephale. 2004;30:564–569. doi: 10.1016/s0013-7006(04)95471-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chueh K. H., Ding G. Y., Yao K. W., Huang Y. J. Hung C. C. Relationships among risk knowledge, attitudes and ability to resist substance abuse in adolescents. Hu Li Za Zhi. 2013;60:60–68. doi: 10.6224/JN.60.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Curry L. A. Youngblade L. M. Negative affect, risk perception, and adolescent risk behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2006;27:468–485. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dermota P., Wang J., Dey M., Gmel G., Studer J. Mohler-Kuo M. Health literacy and substance use in young Swiss men. International Journal of Public Health. 2013, July 11;58:939–948. doi: 10.1007/s00038-013-0487-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dworkin J. Risk taking as developmentally appropriate experimentation for college students. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2005;20:219–241. [Google Scholar]
  15. Engels R. C. M. ter Bogt T. Influences of risk behaviors on the quality of peer relations in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2001;30:675–695. [Google Scholar]
  16. Hampson S. E., Tildesley E., Andrews J. A., Barckley M. Peterson M. Smoking trajectories across high school: Sensation seeking and hookah use. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2013;15:1400–1408. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Heinz A. J., Giedgowd G. E., Crane N. A., Veilleux J. C., Conrad M., Braun A. R. Kassel J. D. A comprehensive examination of hookah smoking in college students: Use patterns and contexts, social norms and attitudes, harm perception, psychological correlates and co-occurring substance use. Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38:2751–2760. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Igra V. Irwin C. E. Theories of adolescent risk-taking behavior. Handbook of adolescent health risk behavior. In: DiClement R. J., Hansen W. B., Ponton L. E., editors. New York, NY: Plenum; 1996. pp. 1–420. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ingersoll G. M. Orr D. P. Behavioral and emotional risk in early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1989;9:392–408. [Google Scholar]
  20. Irwin C. E., Jr. The theoretical concept of at-risk adolescents. Adolescent Medicine. 1990;1:1–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Irwin C. E., Jr. Millstein S. G. Correlates and predictors of risk-taking behavior during adolescence. Self- regulatory behavior and risk-taking: Causes and consequences. In: Lipsit L. P., Mitnick L. L., editors. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing; 1991. pp. 3–21. [Google Scholar]
  22. Jessor R. Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action. Journal of Adolescence Health. 1991;12:597–605. doi: 10.1016/1054-139x(91)90007-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jessor R. Jessor S. L. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1977. Problem-behavior and psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of youth. [Google Scholar]
  24. Karam E. G., Ghandour L. A., Maalouf W. E., Yamout K. Salamoun M. M. A rapid situation assessment (RSA) study of alcohol and drug use in Lebanon. The Lebanese Medical Journal. 2010;58:76–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kloep M., Guney N., Cok F. Simsek O. F. Motives for risk taking in adolescence: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Adolescence. 2009;32:135–151. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. MacArthur G. J., Smith M. C., Melotti R., Heron J., Macleod J., Hickman M. Lewis G. Patterns of alcohol use and multiple risk behaviour by gender during early and late adolescence: The ALSPAC cohort. Journal of Public Health (Oxford) 2012;34(Suppl 1):i20–i30. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fds006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mantzouranis G. Zimmermann G. Taking risks, worth it? Risk behaviours and perceiving their risks among adolescents. Neuropsychiatrie de L'enfance et de L'adolescence. 2010;58:488–494. [Google Scholar]
  28. Moore S. Gullone E. Predicting adolescent risk behavior using a personalized cost-benefit analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1996;25:343–359. [Google Scholar]
  29. Peach H. D. Gaultney J. F. Sleep, impulse control, and sensation-seeking predict delinquent behavior in adolescents, emerging adults, and adults. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013;53:293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Primack B. A., Sidani J., Agarwal A. A., Shadel W. G., Donny E. C. Eissenberg T. E. Prevalence of and associations with waterpipe tobacco smoking among U.S. university students. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;36:81–86. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9047-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pritchard C. Cox M. Comparison of problematic behaviours of 10th and 11th year Southern English adolescents in 1985 and 2005. Part 1: Trends in gender behaviour. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health. 2007;19:127–140. doi: 10.1515/ijamh.2007.19.2.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rodham K., Brewer H., Mistral W. Stallard P. Adolescents' perception of risk and challenge: A qualitative study. Journal of Adolescence. 2006;29:261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Salameh P., Jomaa L., Farhat G., Zeghondi H., Gerges N., Issa C. The Lebanese National Conference for Health in Universities Study Group The Young Adults' Cigarette Dependence (YACD) score: An improved tool for cigarette dependence assessment in university students. Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38:2174–2179. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Salameh P. Barbour B. Psychological distress in Lebanese young adults: Construction and validation of the BDS-22 scale. International Journal of Culture and Mental health. 2011;5:94–108. [Google Scholar]
  35. Salameh P., Jomaa L., Issa C., Farhat G., Zeghondi H., Gerges N. The Lebanese National Conference for Health in University (LNCHU) Study Group Assessment of health risk behaviours among university students: A cross-sectional study in Lebanon. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth. 2012a;19:203–216. [Google Scholar]
  36. Salameh P., Khayat G. Waked M. Smoking in Lebanese women: A lower prevalence but a higher risk of waterpipe dependence. Women & Health. 2012b;52:1–16. doi: 10.1080/03630242.2012.656885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Salameh P., Khayat G., Waked M. Dramaix M. Waterpipe smoking and dependence are associated with Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease: A case-control study. The Open Epidemiology Journal. 2012c;5:36–44. [Google Scholar]
  38. Salameh P., Waked M. Aoun Z. Narguileh smoking: Construction and validation of the LWDS-11 dependence scale. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2008;10:148–159. doi: 10.1080/14622200701767753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shafiq M., Shah Z., Saleem A., Siddiqi M. T., Shaikh K. S., Salahuddin F. F. Naqvi H. Perceptions of Pakistani medical students about drugs and alcohol: A questionnaire-based survey. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention and Policy. 2006;1:31. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-1-31. doi:10.1186/1747-597X-1-31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Shapiro R., Siegel A., Scovill L. Hays J. Risk-taking patterns of female adolescents: What they do and why. Journal of Adolescence. 1998;21:143–159. doi: 10.1006/jado.1997.0138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Shedler J. Block J. Adolescent drug use and psychological health: A longitudinal inquiry. American Psychologist. 1990;45:612–630. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.5.612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Siegel A. W., Cousins J. H., Rubovits P., Parsons J. T., Lavery B. Crowley C. L. Adolescent perceptions of the benefits and risks of their own risk taking. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 1994;2:89–98. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sika N. The millennium development goals: Prospects for gender equality in the Arab world. Journal of International Women's Studies. 2011;12:27–42. [Google Scholar]
  44. Tavolacci M. P., Ladner J., Grigioni S., Richard L., Villet H. Dechelotte P. Prevalence and association of perceived stress, substance use and behavioral addictions: A cross-sectional study among university students in France, 2009–2011. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:724. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-724. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-13-724 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Waked M., Salameh P. Aoun Z. Water-pipe (Narguile) smokers in Lebanon: A pilot study. Eastern Mediterranean Health Journal. 2009;15:432–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Weiland B. J., Welsh R. C., Yau W. Y., Zucker R. A., Zubieta J. K. Heitzeg M. M. Accumbens functional connectivity during reward mediates sensation-seeking and alcohol use in high-risk youth. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2013;128:130–139. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.08.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Adolescence and Youth are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES