Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Nov 18.
Published in final edited form as: J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2008 May;69(3):406–411. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.406

Normative Misperceptions of Drinking Among College Students: A Look at the Specific Contexts of Prepartying and Drinking Games*

Eric R Pedersen 1, Joseph W LaBrie 1,
PMCID: PMC4235610  NIHMSID: NIHMS637674  PMID: 18432383

Abstract

Objective

In the collegiate context, misperceptions of student drinking norms are among the most salient predictors of heavy drinking, Despite overall overestimations of peer alcohol use, misperceptions of context-specific behaviors have been infrequently studied. The present study examines students' perceptions of the high-risk behaviors of prepartying and drinking games and investigates the relationship between perceived and actual behaviors.

Method

A sample of 524 college students completed an online assessment of actual and perceived alcohol use related to prepartying and drinking games. Quantity and frequency of overall drinking, prepartying, and drinking games were assessed for perceptions of all students at the university, as well as for male and female students separately. Questions also assessed participants' overall drinking, prepartying, and drinking game behaviors.

Results

Participants significantly overestimated the prepartying and drinking game behaviors of all students, male students, and female students at their university. For men, perceptions of same-sex prepartying quantity and drinking game frequency and quantity were associated with actual behavior. For women, perceptions of both same-sex and other-sex prepartying quantity were associated with actual behavior.

Conclusions

These findings provide preliminary support for the association between context-specific perceived norms and actual prepartying and drinking game behaviors. Addressing these same-sex and opposite-sex norms during interventions may help students reduce their own engagement in these risky behaviors


Heavy drinking among college students continues to be a problem, with substantial proportions of students experiencing ensuing consequences from their own or others' drinking (Hingson et al., 2005; Wechsler et al., 2002). Research focusing on the social norms approach to college drinking (Perkins and Berkowitz, 1986) suggests that a student's perception of other students' drinking relates to his or her own drinking. Research consistently demonstrates that college students overestimate how much other students actually drink (see Berkowitz, 2004; Perkins, 2003), and the magnitude of overestimation has been prospectively linked with heavier drinking (Baer et al., 1991; Neighbors et al, 2006a). These perceived norms are among the strongest and most consistent predictors of drinking for college students (Borsari and Carey, 2003; Neighbors et al., 2007).

Although students hold misperceptions of campus drinking norms in general, there is evidence that situational and contextual drinking norms may also be important (Neighbors et al., 2006b). The college context as a whole provides many opportunities and social pressure for peers to influence alcohol use (Carey, 1993, 1995), but within the broad environment are specific situations in which pressure may be even greater. High-risk situations and contexts that may perpetuate heavy drinking include drinking games (Borsari, 2004; Pedersen and LaBrie, 2006), holidays such as Halloween and St. Patrick's Day (Martell et al., 2006), football games and tailgating (Martell et al., 2006; Neighbors et al., 2006b), spring break (Lee et al, 2006; Smeaton et al., 1998), prepartying (Borsari et al., 2007; Pedersen and LaBrie, 2007), and 21st birthday celebrations (Neighbors et al., 2005, 2006b). Students misperceive the percentage of students who drink during celebratory occasions and high-risk events (e.g., spring break, Halloween, St. Patrick's Day, home football games) (Martell et al,, 2006). Furthermore, Neighbors and colleagues (2006b) found significant overestimations of the quantity of drinks consumed during 21st birthday celebrations and during football tailgating. These perceptions were associated with greater drinking during these specific events.

Because of the influence of general, situation-, and context-specific perceived norms on actual behavior, we designed the present study to examine misperceptions of two context-specific behaviors that are emerging in the literature as high risk: prepartying and drinking games. Prepartying (also referenced by researchers and students as “pregaming,” “preloading,” and “prefunking”) is a behavior well known among many college students that involves drinking before going out to an intended destination (e.g., bar, party, concert). Prepartying predicts heavier drinking after prepartying and associates with greater alcohol-related consequences (Pedersen and LaBrie, 2007). Participation in drinking games, although a traditionally male-dominated activity (e.g., Engs and Hanson, 1993; Johnson et al, 1998), has recently sparked research attention as an increasing prevalent heavy drinking practice among female students (e.g., Pedersen and LaBrie, 2006; Zamboanga et al, 2006). Examining these context-specific perceptions and the influence they may have on actual behavior may help further understanding of the nature of college drinking and aid in the development of social norms interventions specifically addressing these misperceptions.

The present study examined a diverse sample of male and female college students to explore perceived norms of prepartying and drinking games. Based on previous research with context-specific perceptions of behavior (Martell et al, 2006; Neighbors et al, 2006b), we hypothesized that participants will overestimate the frequency of and the number of drinks consumed during these contexts and that perceptions will associate with actual behavior. Sex-specific and general student norms will be examined among both male and female students.

Method

Participants

Across two sequential semesters, 224 participants seeking credit through the university psychology subject pool completed an online assessment of actual and perceived alcohol use. Participants received one subject pool credit for completing the survey. To access a larger and more representative sample of college students, we used a modified, respondent-driven sampling design (Heckathoran, 1997). This approach has been shown to reveal a nonhomogenous sample representative of the population targeted (Heckathoran, 2007; Heckathorn et al, 2002). Each subject pool participant was offered the opportunity to recruit one or two college-age peers (the criterion was a college student at the participant's university between the ages of 18 and 25) to complete his or her own online assessment. For each recruited participant, the subject pool participant was awarded one extra credit. Of the 308 participants recruited by subject pool participants, only 10 of them failed to complete the assessment. The final sample consisted of 522 college students from one medium-sized university on the West Coast (mean [SD] age = 19.37 [1.31]). Female students made up 61% (n = 316) of the sample, Ethnicity varied, with 51% white, 18% Hispanic/Latino (a), 12% Asian/Pacific Islander, 11% mixed ethnicity, 5% black, and 2% other; 1% declined to state ethnicity. In the past month, 81% (n = 420) of the participants drank at least one time, of which 75% (n = 316) prepartied and 64% (n = 271) played drinking games. Participants were similar in demographics with a larger, university-wide sampling conducted 2 years prior (N = 1,277 [approximately one fourth of the student population]; 53% white; 63% female; mean age = 20.4 [2.04]). These two samples matched the demographics of the university as a whole. Seventy-eight percent of the students in the larger sample reported consuming alcohol at least once in the past month. Prepartying and drinking game behaviors were not assessed in the larger study.

Design and procedure

Using an online survey distribution program, all participants were emailed an online assessment survey. Data were not linked to participants' names or emails. All participants were prompted to read and electronically sign a local institutional review board-approved consent form before completing the survey and were assured before beginning the survey that their data would not be linked to their name, email, or recruited (or recruiter) friend. Participants were allowed to complete their survey at any point during the semester in which they received it.

The survey began with demographic questions of age, sex, and ethnicity. Next, participants endorsed responses to questions involving drinking behavior in the past month. Pictures and descriptions of standard drinks (i.e., one drink containing 0.5 oz of ethyl alcohol) accompanied all questions assessing drinking behavior. Participants responded to two questions assessing typical monthly frequency (“How many days in the past month did you drink alcohol?”) and quantity (“How many drinks on average did you drink during a typical drinking occasion in the past month?”). Participants were also asked about the frequency of their drinking days in the past month that involved prepartying (defined for participants as drinking before going out to a planned destination [e.g., party, bar, concert] at which more alcohol may or may not be consumed) and frequency of drinking games (defined as a game in which drinking alcohol is part of the known rules, with a goal of avoiding drinking, drinking a lot, or forcing others to drink a lot). Items assessing typical quantities consumed during prepartying and drinking games in the past month were also included. These six questions revealed a reliability estimate of α = .76 for all participants.

Finally, participants were prompted to answer a series of questions regarding their perceptions of student drinking behavior within the past month at their particular university. Two questions assessed general frequency and quantity of drinking: “How many days per month does a typical (X University} student drink alcohol?” and “How many drinks on average does a typical {X University} student drink during a typical drinking occasion?” Questions also assessed general perceived frequency and quantity of prepartying and drinking game behaviors: “How many days in the past 30 days do you think a typical {X University} student prepartied?” “How many days in the past 30 days do you think a typical {X University} student played drinking games?” “How many drinks on average does a typical {X University} student drink while prepartying?” “How many drinks on average does a typical {X University} student drink while playing drinking games?” These questions were repeated to assess sex-specific norms (replacing the word “student” with “male student” and “female student”). These 14 perception questions revealed a reliability estimate of α = .89. The means of participants' responses to questions assessing their own behavior were used as actual behavior variables and thus were used to compare with participants' responses regarding perceived norms. Only drinkers' responses to questions regarding actual prepartying and drinking game behaviors were included in the analyses, whereas perceived norms of all participants were included. This was to control for lower means of actual behavior because of zero responses from nondrinkers. All continuous variables were recoded within three SDs from the mean to reduce the influence of outliers (Tabachnick and Fidel, 2007).

Results

Normative misperceptions

Drinking behavior

Participants overestimated the frequency of drinking days in the past month among a typical student at their university. Men perceived ail students at the university to drink a mean of 10.78 (5.41) days pet-month, and women perceived all students as drinking 10.01 (4.71) days per month. Participants drank an actual mean of 5.42 (5.27) days in the past month, revealing significant overestimations of frequency of drinking for both men (t = 13.87, 193 df, p < .001) and women (t = 17.10, 307 df, p < .001). Although participants drank an actual mean of 3.72 (2.98) drinks during a typical drinking occasion, men believed all students typically drank a mean of 6.21 (2,74) drinks (t =12.65, 193 df, p < .001), whereas women believed all students typically drank a mean of 5.36 (2.36) drinks (t =12.41, 306 df, p < .001).

Prepartying and drinking games

Table 1 contains means and standard deviations of the actual prepartying and drinking game behaviors in the past month of the sample, as well as the mean perception of these behaviors reported by male and female participants. Actual behavioral norms presented in the first data column represent the mean behavior reported by all participants, male participants, and female participants. The columns labeled Males and Females contain the samples' perceived means of behavior for all students, male students, and female students. The two columns are divided by sex to present perceptions of behavior by male and female participants. Separate one-sample t tests were run to examine differences between participants' perceived behavior and the actual behavior of the sample. Each actual behavioral norm for all students, male students, and female students was entered as the population mean and tested for men's and women's perceived means for each group. Both male and female participants evidenced significant overestimations of frequency and quantity of both behaviors for all students, men, and women. In addition, men and women were similar in their estimations of all prepartying and drinking game variables, differing only in their perceptions of all student prepartying quantity (t = 2.33, 507 df, p < .05) and female drinking game quantity (t = 2.33, 507 df, p < .05).

Table 1.

Differences between means on actual behavioral norms and mean perception of behavioral norms

Males (n = 224) Females (n = 330)


Behavior, in the past month Actual
Mean (SD)
Median Range Mean (SD)
perception
of norm
t Mean (SD)
perception
of norm
t
Prepartying frequency, days in past month
  All students 2.71 (3.50) 1 0–14 8.39 (5.46) 13.03 8.40 (4.98) 20.14
    Males 3.07 (3.71) 2 0–12 9.08 (6.24) 13.65 9.47 (5.50) 20.47
    Females 2.48 (3.34) 1 0–8 7.53 (5.72) 12.52 7.47 (4.42) 19.82
Prepartying quantity, average drinks per occasion
  All students 3.91 (1.94) 4 1–12 4.98 (3.35) 4.52 4.40 (2.36) 3.63
    Males 4.76 (2.14) 5 1–12 5.89 (3.12) 5.13 5.58 (2.47) 5.88
    Females 3.29 (1.51) 3 1–8 3.93 (2.76) 3.29 3.64 (1.71) 3.57
Drinking game frequency, days in past month
  All participants 2.09 (3.14) 1 0–13 7.66 (6.31) 12.46 7.22 (5.012) 18.25
    Males 3.04 (3.80) 2 0–13 8.09 (S.95) 11.98 7.94 (5.28) 16.36
    Females 1.47 (2.45) 1 0–13 5.71 (4.94) 6.72 5.36 (4.24) 10.35
Drinking game quantity. average drinks per occasion
  All students 2.39 (1.97) 2 1–7 6.14 (3.31) 15.95 6.24 (3.74) 17.96
    Males 2.94 (2.04) 3 1–7 6.93 (3.35) 16.75 7.27 (4.37) 17.33
    Females 1.84 (1.74) 1.71 1–6 4.69 (2.81) 14.24 5.36 (4.24) 14.51

Notes: Values for actual means are based on the mean behavior of die sample reported separately by all participants, males, and females (actual norms). Values under Male and Female columns represent means of males' and females' perceptions of behavior for all students, males, and females (perceived norms). T values represent difference between actual norms and perceived norms across each row. Actual mean behavior frequency for prepartying and drinking games reported for drinkers only, Actual mean behavior quantity for prepartying and drinking games reported for prepattyers and game players only, respectively.

p < .01;

p < .001.

Association between perceived norms and behavior

Correlation analyses determined relationships between perceived prepartying and drinking game norms and actual behavior. Men evidenced significant positive correlations between actual prepartying quantity and perceived prepartying quantity norms of male students (r = .34, p < .001) and all students (r ~ .27, p < .001). For women, significant positive correlations were evident between actual prepartying quantity and their perceived prepartying quantity norms of both men (r = .29, p < .001) and women (r = .27, p < .001) at the university. Regarding drinking games, significant positive correlations were observed for men's actual game frequency and perceived frequency norms of male students (r = .14, p < .05) and between actual quantity consumed during games and perceived quantity norms of all students at the university (r = .28, p < .05). No significant positive correlations existed between women's perceived and actual behavior for drinking games.

Discussion

The present study examined the extent that college students overestimate the context-specific behaviors of prepartying and drinking games among their peers and explored how these perceptions relate to drinking behavior. Consistent with our hypotheses and as in previous research examining situation- and context-specific perceptions (Martell et at., 2006; Neighbors et al., 2006b), students overestimated the frequency of prepartying and drinking games among all students, male students, and female students at their university. Male and female participants were similar in nearly all of their perceptions of behavior. In addition, a relationship emerged between perceived behavior and actual behavior. Normative perceptions appeared to relate to behavior to a greater extent for prepartying than drinking games. A relationship between drinking games and behavior existed for men only.

The overestimations of prepartying and drinking game behaviors observed among participants may influence an individual's own behavior, which is revealed in the relationship between perceived and actual behaviors. Perhaps the perception that other students will already be drunk once arriving at the destination motivates students to drink beforehand themselves, avoiding being “the only one” at the party, bar, or event without a good buzz. Men may desire to arrive at similar intoxication levels as their male peers, perhaps to demonstrate their drinking ability or to loosen up to meet members of the opposite sex (e.g., West, 2001). For women, the overestimation of both male and female norms may have a similar interpretation, because subjective reports suggest that women may desire to be accepted by and held in high regard by male drinking partners and that women who can drink amounts of alcohol comparable to men receive more sexual attention from male peers (Young et al., 2005). Believing that intoxicated female peers will have an advantage over them in impressing or meeting men may influence prepartying consumption levels. Women also appear to be influenced by opposite-sex norms, suggesting that their anticipation that men may arrive at the party or event intoxicated may influence their prepartying consumption levels.

Regarding drinking games, the competitive nature of this behavior may permeate into mere involvement, because men may want to show other men their ability to “hold their liquor” or impress women with their drinking game talents (Green and Grider, 1990; West, 2001). It is noteworthy that women's overestimations of drinking game behavior did not associate with their own drinking game behavior. Presumably, because drinking games have been viewed as a traditionally male-dominated activity (Borsari, 2004) and research suggests that students are most affected by same-sex norms (Lewis and Neighbors, 2004), women may not have been influenced to play games based on their perceptions. For women, drinking games may be more of a function of availability or peer pressure within the moment than a function of perceived norms.

Limitations exist in the study. First, participants were asked retrospectively about their own past use and the perceived use of their peers. Although participants completed the survey at different times throughout two semesters, participants' memories or perceived norms may have been influenced by a period of the semester that they associated with heavy or light drinking (e.g., immediately after spring break or midterm exams). Collection of data via the online survey is also a potential limitation. There are several strengths to this novel approach, including convenience, flexibility, and direct data entry by participants. However, there are important drawbacks, including concerns about confidentiality and security of data provided over the Web. Although this cannot be completely eliminated, participants were informed of extensive provisions to ensure confidentiality and security of their data in the informed consent. Furthermore, recent research (Miller et al., 2002) indicates no differences in self-reported alcohol use and problems between individuals randomized to complete Web versus paper assessments.

In addition, results contained herein refer to associations between perceived norms and actual behavior and do not imply causation. It may be that students who engage in these behaviors to a large degree hold higher perceptions. Finally, perceived norms were compared with “actual norms,” based on the responses of the study's participants. Approximately half the sample consisted of participants recruited by. psychology subject pool students. Although we anticipated this to be a strong strategy to help reveal a more generalizable sample beyond only psychology students, students may have recruited friends similar in drinking behavior and drinking attitudes to themselves. However, we hope this extraneous factor was balanced owing to the apparent wide range of student drinkers (and nondrinkers) in the sample and therefore balanced each other out (Heckathorn, 2007). In the large, university-wide study distributed previously, students reported a mean of 3.36 (2,90) drinks per occasion—similar to the average drinks per occasion reported in this study (mean = 3.72 [2.98]), Likewise, students were similar in demographics and drinking status. Despite this, we were unable to determine if the participants in the present study demonstrated representative prepartying and drinking game behaviors, compared with the larger university. These behaviors were not assessed in the larger survey. Therefore, as the first study to address misperceptions of prepartying and drinking games, our findings are preliminary, and we encourage more advanced predictive models in future research, as well as reports of “actual norms” that may more accurately represent the college population as a whole.

Despite limitations, these results provide preliminary support for the association between perceived norms and actual prepartying and drinking game behaviors. The overt overestimations observed highlight the need to focus norms presentations and interventions on both same-sex and opposite-sex context-specific behaviors. Providing students with accurate context-specific norms, particularly of the risky and prevalent behaviors of prepartying and drinking games, may help reduce these risky behaviors.

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism grant U18AA015451-01, a Model Award from the U.S. Department o f Education (grant Q184NO5O003), and a grant from the Alcoholic Beverage Medical Research Foundation.

References

  1. Baer JS, Stacy A, Larimer M. Biases in the perception of drinking norms among college students. J. Stud. Alcohol. 1991;52:580–586. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1991.52.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berkowitz AD. The Social Norms Approach: Theory, Research, and Annotated Bibliography. Trumansburg, NY: Alan D. Berkowitz; 2004. (available at: www.alanberkowitz.com/articles/social_norms.pdf). [Google Scholar]
  3. Borsari B. Drinking games in the college environment: A review. J. Alcohol Drug Educ. 2004;48(2):29–51. [Google Scholar]
  4. Borsari B, Boyle KE, Hustad JTP, Barnett NP, Tevyaw TO, Kahler CW. Drinking before drinking: Pregaming and drinking games in mandated students. Addict Behav. 2007;32:2694–2705. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and injunctive norms in college drinking: A meta-analytic integration. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2003;64:331–341. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carey KB. Situational determinants of heavy drinking among college students. J. Counsel. Psychol. 1993;40:217–220. [Google Scholar]
  7. Carey KB. Heavy drinking contexts and indices of problem drinking among college students. J. Stud. Alcohol. 1995;56:287–292. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Engs RC, Hanson DJ. Drinking games and problems related to drinking among moderate and heavy drinkers. Psychol. Rep. 1993;73:115–120. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1993.73.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Green TA, Grider SA. Reversal of competence in college drinking games. Play Cult. 1990;3:117–132. [Google Scholar]
  10. Heckathorn DD. Respondent-driven sampling: A new approach to the study of hidden populations. Social Probl. 1997;44:174–199. [Google Scholar]
  11. Heckathorn DD. Extensions of respondent-driven sampling: Analyzing continuous variables and controlling for differential recruitment. Sociol. Methodol. 2007;37:151–207. [Google Scholar]
  12. Heckathorn DD, Semaan S, Broadhead RS, Hughes JJ. Extensions of respondent-driven sampling: A new approach to the study of injection drug users aged 18–25. AIDS Behav. 2002;6:55–67. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter M, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Rev. Publ. Hlth. 2005;26:259–279. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Johnson TJ, Wendel J, Hamilton S. Social anxiety, alcohol expectancies, and drinking-game participation. Addict. Behav. 1998;23:65–79. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(97)00033-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lee CM, Maggs JL, Rankin LA. Spring break trips as a risk factor for heavy alcohol use among first-year college students. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2006;67:911–916. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lewis MA, Neighbors C. Gender-specific misperceptions of college student drinking norms. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2004;18:334–339. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.4.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Martell D, Atkin C, Hembroff LA, Smith S, Baumer AJ, Greenamyer J. College students and “celebration drinking.”. Social Norms Rev. 2006;1:10–17. [Google Scholar]
  18. Miller ET, Neal DJ, Roberts LJ, Baer JS, Cressler SO, Metrik J, Marlatt GA. Test-retest reliability of alcohol measures: Is there a difference between Internet-based assessment and traditional methods? Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2002;16:56–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Neighbors C, Dillard AJ, Lewis MA, Bergstrom RL, Neil TA. Normative misperceptions and temporal precedence of perceived norms and drinking. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2006a;67:290–299. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, Larimer ME. Are social norms the best predictor of outcomes among heavy-drinking college students? J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68:556–565. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Neighbors C, Oster-Aaland L, Bergstrom RL, Lewis MA. Event- and context-specific normative misperceptions and high-risk drinking: 21st birthday celebrations and football tailgating. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2006b;67:282–289. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Neighbors C, Spieker CJ, Oster-Aaland L, Lewis MA, Bergstrom RL. Celebration intoxication: An evaluation of 21st birth-day alcohol consumption. J. Amer. Coll. Hlth. 2005;54:76–80. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.2.76-80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Pedersen ER, Labrie J. Drinking game participation among college students: Gender and ethnic implications. Addict. Behav. 2006;31:2105–2115. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Pedersen ER, Labrie J. Partying before the party: Examining prepartying behavior among college students. J. Amer. Coll. Hlth. 2007;56:237–245. doi: 10.3200/JACH.56.3.237-246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Perkins HW, editor. The Social Norms Approach to Preventing School and College Age Substance Abuse: A Handbook for Educators, Counselors, and Clinicians. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  26. Perkins HW, Berkowitz AD. Perceiving the community norms of alcohol use among students: Some research implications for campus alcohol education programming. Int. J. Addict. 1986;21:961–976. doi: 10.3109/10826088609077249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Smeaton GL, Josiam BM, Dietrich UC. College students' binge drinking at a beach-front destination during spring break. J. Amer. Coll. Hlth. 1998;46:247–254. doi: 10.1080/07448489809596000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 5th Edition. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wechsler H, Lee JE, Kuo M, Seibring M, Nelson TF, Lee H. Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts: Findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study surveys, 1993–2001. J. Amer. Coll. Hlth. 2002;50:203–217. doi: 10.1080/07448480209595713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. West LA. Negotiating masculinities in American drinking subcultures. J. Men's Stud. 2001;9:371–392. [Google Scholar]
  31. Young AM, Morales M, McCabe SE, Boyd CJ, D'Arcy H. Drinking like a guy: Frequent binge drinking among undergraduate women. Subst. Use Misuse. 2005;40:241–267. doi: 10.1081/ja-200048464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Zamboanga BL, Leitkowski LK, Rodriguez L, Cascio KA. Drinking games in female college students: More than just a game? Addict. Behav. 2006;31:1485–1489. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES