Abstract
More and more college students are using microblogs, with some excessive users demonstrating addiction-like symptoms. However, there is currently no published scale available for use in assessing excessive use of these microblogs, a significant impediment to advancing this area of research. We collected data from 3,047 college students in China and developed a Microblog Excessive Use Scale (MEUS) for Chinese college students, comparing it with criteria used for assessing Internet addiction. Our diagnostic scale featured three factors, two of which–“withdrawal and health problem” and “time management and performance”–are already included in Internet addiction assessment scales. The third factor, “social comfort,” does not appear in Internet addiction assessment scales. Our study found that females have significantly higher MEUS scores than males, and that total MEUS scores positively correlated with scores from “self-disclosure” and “real social interaction” scales. These findings differ from results obtained in previous investigations into Internet addiction. Our results indicate that some characteristics of the excessive use of microblogs are different to those of Internet addiction, suggesting that microblog overuse may not correspond exactly to the state of Internet addiction.
Introduction
Recent times have witnessed the growth of Internet-based communication technologies; creating a new field of academic study focusing on social networks [1]. Social networking sites like MySpace and Facebook have grown quickly and have had significant impact on young people [2]. However, the emergence of microblogging sites like Twitter has shifted online communication and interaction trends. Many people have embraced this new form of communication in that nearly 40% of social networking sites users have migrated to microblogs [3]. Microblogs are particularly popular in the United States (Twitter) and in China (Weibo; in particular, Sina Weibo) [4]. For instance, according to Twitter’s 2013 Initial Public Offerings (IPO) filing [5], [6], the company registered a growth rate of 1,400% per year, had more than 215 million registered accounts, over 100 million daily active users, and boastes of “approximately 500 million Tweets every day”. In 2012, the number of microblog users in China surpassed 300 million, making China the country with highest number of microblog users in the world [7]. Most of these microblog users are college students (72.3%) [8] and microblogging is now considered an indispensable part of a college student’s life.
Due to the increase in number of microblog users, microblogging has attracted scholarship from different academic disciplines. Mainly, scholars believe that microblog use or overuse may present symptoms of behavioral addiction (e.g., [1]). Already, many studies have been conducted on Internet addiction (e.g., [9], [10]), as well as on people’s engagement in other Internet activities including online gaming [11], [12], social networking [13], mobile emailing [14], instant messaging [15], and online auctioning [13]. Most researchers believe that these Internet-related behaviors may be some form of Internet addiction, and have hence measured them against the central thinking in addiction studies [16]. However, in contrast to other forms of addiction like drug addiction and addictive behaviors such as gambling, Internet-related behaviors are not homogenous and as such different applications and use of the Internet may exhibit different addictive characteristics [17], [18]. Only few studies have focused on the use of microblogs and for these few studies, researchers have used Internet addiction diagnostic criteria to assess the excessive use of microblogs [19]. For example, Ma (2012) states that individuals who use microblogs for more than six hours every day and whose behavior meets the diagnostic criterion of Internet addiction are excessive users of microblogs [20].
At present, several Internet addiction diagnostic criteria have been published (e.g., [21]–[28]). Although the standard for Internet addiction is not exactly the same across all of them, each includes the basic characteristics of Internet addiction namely: impulse, loss of control, and insistence of negative impact behaviors [29]. Researchers have also determined that Internet addiction can lead to a variety of negative consequences such as decreased involvement in real-life communities, worsening of academic performance, and an increase in relationship problems [30]. Young’s 20-item Internet Addiction Test (IAT) [31] was the first published diagnostic scale; we utilized it in our study to measure Internet addiction. Chang and Law’s confirmatory study [32] identified three factors from Young’s IAT: (1) “withdrawal and social problems,” (2) “time management and performance,” and (3) “reality substitute.”
As a behavior related to Internet-based applications, excessive use of microblogs comprises a few of the basic characteristics of Internet addiction associated with social networking. Excessive use of this kind of online media can lead to, for example, misuse, dependence, and addiction [33]. That is, the excessive use of microblogs may bring about symptoms or outcomes similar to those seen in Internet addiction [34], including withdrawal effects and time-management problems (e.g., diminished quality and/or quantity of sleep time due to all-day use of microblogs). This is consistent with outcomes associated with the problematic or compulsive use of Facebook, which may contribute to the severity of symptoms associated with Internet addiction [35], such as poor sleep quality [36]. However, in contrast to another characteristic of Internet addiction, that of using the Internet for escapism [31], [32], [37], the primary reason for using microblogs is self-presentation [38], [39]; that is, discussing shared-interest topics or hot news to broaden one’s social networking size [40].
Therefore, there are similarities as well as differences between excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction. As problematic use of microblogs may be a form of Internet addiction, and since the overall use of microblogs is increasing rapidly, there is need for a psychometrically sound procedure for assessing excessive use of microblogs. In the present study, we developed a scale, the Microblog Excessive Use Scale (MEUS) modeled after Young’s (1998) 20-item IAT [31], and reviewed its psychometric properties. We also explored the characteristics of microblog overuse among several samples of college students in China, and assessed the similarities and differences between IAT and MEUS.
Methods and Materials
Participants
The study targeted college students in China. The total number of participants was 3,047. Participants were recruited in and divided into five phases/samples. Participants in Sample 3 were recruited online while participants in the other samples were recruited from five colleges in China. The amount and quality of information collected varied across these samples (see Table 1). Participants were selected on the premise that they had had at least one microblog account and were college students. Initially, before the comprehensive study, 10 college students (5 males and 5 females) were recruited for an index test.
Table 1. Summary of the demographic data of participants.
Sample | N | Male (%) | Age (M ± SD) | N of collection (rate of availability) | Geographic regions |
Sample 1 | 408 | 52.5 | 21.2±2.0 | 480 (85.0%) | Eastern China |
Sample 2 | 200 | 38.5 | 22.9±1.7 | 240 (83.3%) | Eastern China |
Sample 3 | 953 | 36.4 | 22.0±3.0 | 953 (100%) | all of China’s regions except Eastern China |
Sample 4 | 381 | 75.6 | 18.2±1.5 | 480 (79.4%) | Eastern China |
Sample 5 | 996 | 76.6 | 18.3±1.8 | 1280 (77.9%) | Eastern China |
Norm | 1949 | 57.0 | 20.1±3.1 | 2233 (87.3%) | all of China’s regions |
Note. Norm comprises sample 3 and sample 5.
Ethics Statement
Each participant to the study provided written informed consent after receiving an explanation of the study’s purpose and procedure. The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Science and Technology of China (USTC) according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were undergraduate and postgraduate students. We did not obtain informed consent from the next of kin, caretakers, or guardians on behalf of the minors/children enrolled in our study. Informed consent was obtained in written form from all participants.
We did not obtain informed consent from guardians of participants whose age was under 18. These young college students were considered to have comparable intelligence and ability to adult students, and able to take charge of their behaviors. According to the General principles of the Civil Law of the People’s Republic of China; “A minor aged 10 or over shall be a person with limited capacity for civil conduct and may engage in civil activities appropriate to his age and intellect; in other civil activities, he shall be represented by his agent ad litem or participate with the consent of his agent ad litem” (Article 12, Chapter II).
Data Collection
Planning for data collection took place before the index test. The data in samples were collected a month after the index test in two-month intervals. The collection rate and availability rate is shown in Table 1. Participants who failed to complete the questionnaire were excluded from the analyses. In addition to our MEUS, Sample 4 also completed the IAT developed by Young [31], and Sample 5 also completed the Jourard Self-Disclosure Questionnaire [41] and our revised Social Interaction Scale [42].
Measures
Demographic data
Each participant answered five questions concerning their gender, age, academic background, and other relevant information; see Supporting Information S1.
Characteristics of microblog use
Characteristics of microblog use by the participants were examined using eight questions (see Supporting Information S1). This section of our study examined participants’ primary behavior in relation to microblog use, including their perception on the importance of microblogging, their perceived dependency on it, first time using it, average time spent microblogging each day, frequency of browsing microblogs, and the average time spent on microblogs in a single session.
MEUS
Our questionnaire comprised 27 statements. The first 20 were modeled after Young’s IAT [31]–we simply replaced “Internet” with “microblog.” In addition, we featured further seven questions, which were based on responses obtained through our microblog interviews (see Supporting Information S2). For every statement (item) on our questionnaire, participants reported the extent to which that statement was true for them using a rating scale ranging from “1” (never) to “6” (always).
IAT
As noted earlier, the IAT is a 20-item, self-report measure of excessive Internet use [31]. This widely applied measure showed adequate internal consistency (α = .950) in our study. In the test, participants rated statements (items) on a six-point scale from “1” (does not apply) to “6” (always). Item scores were added together to obtain an overall score, with higher total scores indicating greater addiction-like symptoms.
Social Interaction Scale
We used the Social Interaction Scale, as revised by Yan [42], first developed by Chen and colleagues [43]; adapting it for the present study, we replaced the term “Internet” with “microblog.” The Social Interaction Scale comprised two subscales-a Real Social Interaction Scale and a Microblog Social Interaction Scale. Each subscale featured 12 statements. The Real Social Interaction Scale measured interpersonal communication with classmates, friends, parents, and other people in real life; for example, “parental intimacy,” “intimacy with friends,” and “informational disclosure with friends.” The Microblog Social Interaction Scale measured an individual’s interaction with others on Microblog and included factors such as “intimacy revealed” and “informational disclosure”. Participants rated each of the Social Interaction Scale’s 24 statements on a four-point scale ranging from “1” (never) to “4” (always). Individual item scores were added together to obtain a total score, with higher total scores indicating greater real or microblog interpersonal communication. The two scales showed good internal consistency (α = .845, α = .909) in our samples.
Self-Disclosure Questionnaire
We employed the Jourard Self-Disclosure Questionnaire [41] in our study to measure the extent to which individuals disclose information in real life. The Jourard Self-Disclosure Questionnaire has 60 statements (items) presented across six sections, with each section featuring 10 items. The questionnaire measures the degree or extent of information exchange with parents or friends on attitudes/opinions, interests/hobbies, study/work, money, personality, body, and so on. Participants rated each of the 60 items on a four-point scale: “0” means “Have told the other person nothing about this aspect of me”; “1” means “Have talked in general terms about this. The other person has only a general idea about this aspect of me”; “2” means “Have talked in full and complete detail about this item to the other person. They know me fully in this respect and could describe me accurately”; and “X” means “Have lied or misrepresented myself to the other person so that they have a false picture of me.” (X was given the value of 0). Item scores were added together to obtain a total score, with higher total scores indicating a greater degree of disclosure in real life. This scale showed good internal consistency (α = 0.964) in our study.
Statistical analysis
We informed the participants that the purpose of this study was to examine the use of Microblogs. Five people read the index test and were masked to the results of the other tests. There were no adverse events from performing the index test.
Data were analyzed using the statistics software package SPSS 18.0 and exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed. The data from Sample 1 and Sample 2 were used for item discrimination analysis and factor analysis to determine the number of items. The data from Sample 3, collected online, were used to confirm the factor structure. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed using Amos 7.0 software. The model fit indices indicated adequate model fit; the comparative fit index (CFI) was .953; the normed fit index (NFI) was .944; the relative fit index (RFI) was .916; and the incremental fit index (IFI) was .954. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was .069. According to Steiger (2007) [44], a stringent upper limit of 0.07 for RMSEA seems to be the consensus amongst authorities. To compare and identify differences between the excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction, we asked participants in Sample 4 to complete both the MEUS and the IAT. To determine which psychological characteristics are associated with microblog use, we asked participants in Sample 5 to complete the Social Interaction Scale and the Self-Disclosure Questionnaire as well as our MEUS. Finally, we combined Sample 3 and Sample 5 data in order to calculate the norm of the MEUS, as well as to identify a standard for the assessment of excessive use of microblogs among college students. We calculated correlations between the scales using Spearman’s rho (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient). To test the differences of different levels in the total score, we used the Kruskal-Wallis test (Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance) and the Mann-Whitney U test. The significance level in this study was p<.01.
Results
The study was conducted from May 2012 to October 2013.
Psychometric Properties of the MEUS–Sample 1
Item discrimination analysis
We computed the difference of each item in our 27-item questionnaire between two groups using the scores of two statements (the average numbers of Tweets browsed per day and the frequency of browsing Tweets per day). With reference to previous studies [45], [46], the high-scoring group included the top 27% participants and the low-scoring group included the bottom 27% participants. Results showed that all items except for item 3 had significant differences (p<.01), suggesting that this scale could identify the reaction degree of the different subjects.
EFA
Our 27-item MEUS showed good internal consistency (α = .946). We used EFA followed by oblique rotation to explore the underlying structure of these items. Factor loadings equal to and above 0.50 were considered indicative of items belonging to a specific factor. We extracted three factors, which accounted for 60.08% of total variance. After EFA, 23 items remained from the original 27. The specific item loadings on these three factors are listed in Table 2.
Table 2. Factor loadings from EFA of the 27-item MEUS.a .
Items | Factor 1(withdrawal and health problem) | Factor 2(social comfort) | Factor 3(time management and performance) | Correcteditem–total correlation |
23. How often do you sleep less than four hours a day becauseof using microblogs? | 0.842 | 0.082 | 0.038 | 0.626** |
21. How is your health affected by using microblogs? | 0.821 | 0.025 | −0.057 | 0.690** |
20. How often do you feel depressed, moody, or nervous whenyou are off microblogs? | 0.789 | 0.112 | 0.015 | 0.696** |
22. How often do you continue to use microblogs, even thoughthis use has resulted in continuous or recurrent physical, social, or psychological problems? | 0.781 | 0.105 | 0.101 | 0.637** |
19. How often do you choose to spend more time on microblogsover going out with others? | 0.748 | 0.145 | −0.050 | 0.672** |
13. How often do you snap, yell, or act annoyed if someonebothers you while you are using microblogs? | 0.691 | 0.008 | −0.074 | 0.686** |
3. How often do you prefer the excitement of microblogs tointimacy with your partner? | 0.682 | −0.139 | −0.133 | 0.430** |
17. How often do you try to cut down the amount of timeyou spend on microblogs but fail to do so? | 0.578 | 0.027 | −0.340 | 0.726** |
15. How often do you feel preoccupied with microblogs when off-line,or fantasize about being on a microblog? | 0.566 | −0.048 | −0.209 | 0.654** |
18. How often do you try to hide how long you have been using microblogs? | 0.538 | 0.177 | −0.036 | 0.651** |
5. How often do others in your life complain to you about the amountof time you spend on microblogs? | 0.526 | 0.040 | −0.331 | 0.546** |
12. How often do you feel life is empty and boring whenyou are unable to use microblogs? | 0.393 | 0.216 | −0.298 | 0.733** |
25. How often do you feel excited when others Retweet your Tweetsor comment on your microblogs? | −0.255 | 0.744 | −0.251 | 0.564** |
24. How often do you change your mood because of a change in thenumber of fans? | 0.143 | 0.654 | −0.106 | 0.632** |
27. How often do you get more followers by increasing the number that you are following? | 0.330 | 0.642 | 0.130 | 0.579** |
26. How often would you try to increase your followersunconsciously by all means? | 0.488 | 0.541 | 0.137 | 0.581** |
11. How often do you find yourself anticipating when you will usemicroblogs again? | 0.051 | 0.535 | 0.094 | 0.589** |
1. How often do you find yourself spending more time on microblogsthan you anticipating? | −0.234 | 0.048 | −0.774 | 0.512** |
2. How often do you neglect household chores to spend moretime on microblogs? | 0.243 | −0.010 | −0.667 | 0.542** |
8. How often does your job performance or productivity sufferbecause of your use of microblogs? | 0.236 | 0.114 | −0.608 | 0.430** |
7. How often do you check your microblog before something else that you need to do? | 0.077 | 0.287 | −0.578 | 0.627** |
16. How often do you find yourself saying “just a few more minutes”when using microblogs? | 0.234 | 0.025 | −0.552 | 0.643** |
14. How often do you lose sleep due to late-night log-ins? | 0.268 | −0.085 | −0.527 | 0.621** |
6. How often do your grades or schoolwork suffer because of theamount of time you spend on microblogs? | 0.496 | −0.055 | −0.510 | 0.674** |
4. How often do you make new friends by microblogs? | 0.049 | 0.317 | −0.427 | 0.461** |
9. How often do you feel the need to guard against or confidentialwhen someone asks what you do in microblogs? | 0.299 | 0.033 | −0.034 | 0.468** |
10. How often do you feel microblogs can help you to comfort andresist the cares of life? | 0.053 | 0.366 | −0.080 | 0.500** |
Percentage of variance explained | 42.69% | 7.35% | 5.62% |
Item numbers correspond to the original item number on the MEUS. **P<.01. The same as below.
The first factor accounted for 42.69% of the shared variance. 11 items had high loading that were associated with experiencing withdrawal effects and health problems due to excessive microblog use. The factor was hence termed “withdrawal and health problem.” Cronbach’s alpha for these 11 items was 0.930, indicating adequate internal consistency.
The second factor had 5 items, which were associated with interacting with others. This factor was termed “social comfort,” and it accounted for 7.35% of the shared variance. Cronbach’s alpha for these 5 items was 0.779, indicating good internal consistency.
The third factor had 7 items with high loadings, which were associated with negative effects on work or academic performance and mismanagement of time due to microblogging. This factor was termed “time management and performance,” and it accounted for 5.62% of the shared variance. Cronbach’s alpha for these 7 items was 0.864, indicating adequate internal consistency.
Reduction of numbers of items
Subsequently, we reviewed each item (statement or question) based on its weighting within the scale, its corrected item–total correlation value, and its meaning. We also considered the frequency of “no” answers for each item or question. 12 items (4 for each factor) were selected in order to reduce items and create a clear-cut factor structure for convenient use (see Table 3).
Table 3. Factor structure of the 12-item MEUS.
Factor 1 (withdrawal and health problem) | Factor 2 (social comfort) | Factor 3 (time management and performance) |
Item 23 | Item 25 | Item 8 |
Item 21 | Item 24 | Item 7 |
Item 20 | Item 27 | Item 16 |
Item 19 | Item 26 | Item 6 |
Psychometric Properties of the MEUS–Sample 2
EFA
The 12-item MEUS demonstrated good internal consistency (α = 0.835). Using the same criterion discussed above, we conducted an EFA in which we extracted three factors, which accounted for 63.18% of total variance. The specific item loadings on these three factors are listed in Table 4.
Table 4. Factor loadings from EFA of the 12-item MEUS.
Items | Factor 1 (withdrawal and health problem) | Factor 2 (social comfort) | Factor 3 (time management and performance) |
21. | 0.872 | −0.071 | 0.026 |
23. | 0.849 | −0.112 | 0.045 |
6. | 0.821 | 0.010 | −0.017 |
19. | 0.709 | 0.150 | 0.063 |
20. | 0.525 | 0.330 | 0.077 |
25. | −0.271 | 0.822 | 0.032 |
27. | 0.265 | 0.645 | −0.024 |
26. | 0.345 | 0.642 | 0.012 |
7. | −0.076 | 0.017 | 0.839 |
16. | 0.013 | 0.219 | 0.797 |
8. | 0.062 | −0.173 | 0.761 |
24. | 0.398 | 0.471 | 0.048 |
Reduction of numbers of items
Item 6 did not belong to the same factor in the two EFAs, and the loading of item 24 was lower than 0.5. As such we deleted these two items (see Table 5). Finally, we developed a 10-item questionnaire (see Supporting Information S3).
Table 5. Factor loadings from EFA of the 10-item MEUS.
Factor 1(withdrawal and health problem) | Factor 2(social comfort) | Factor 3(time management and performance) |
Item 23 | Item 25 | Item 8 |
Item 21 | Item 27 | Item 7 |
Item 20 | Item 26 | Item 16 |
Item 19 |
Correlations between subscales
The correlation of subscales with each other was above .211 (p<.001), and the correlation of the total scale with each of the subscales was greater than .678 (p<.001) (see Table 6).
Table 6. Correlations between subscales of MEUS.
Factor 1(withdrawal and health problem) | Factor 2(social comfort) | Factor 3(time management and performance) | |
Factor 1 | 1 | – | – |
Factor 2 | 0.328** | 1 | – |
Factor 3 | 0.473** | 0.211** | 1 |
The total score of MEUS | 0.784** | 0.704** | 0.678** |
** P<.05.
Psychometric Properties of the MEUS–Sample 3
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)–internal consistency of the MEUS
We found high reliabilities for each of the three subscales in Sample 3: “withdrawal and health problem” (α = .721), “social comfort” (α = .628), and “time management and performance” (α = .830). Cronbach’s alpha for the overall MEUS was .803.
Structure validity
To verify the factor structure identified through EFA, CFA was performed on Sample 3. For the current CFA model, the ratio of chi-square to degrees of freedom was 5.5 (χ2 = 165.8; df = 30); the comparative fit index was .953; the normed fit index was .944; the root mean square error of approximation was .069; the relative fit index was .916; and the incremental fit index was .954, indicating adequate model fit. The model of factorial structure for the MEUS is shown in Fig. 1.
Criterion validity
To assess the criterion validity of these scales, the correlation between the total score from the MEUS and self-reporting of the degree of importance of and dependence on microblogs was calculated. It was significantly related to the self-report of the degree of the importance of (ρ = 0.504, p<.001) and dependence on (ρ = 0.624, p<.001) microblogs, suggesting that the scale has good criterion validity.
Characteristics of College Students’ Excessive Use of Microblogs-Sample 3
Gender difference
We performed Mann-Whitney U tests to examine gender differences between total scores in Sample 3. Our results showed that female participants had significantly higher total scores compared to male participants (U = 92395.5, p = .002; male: M = 21.1, SD = 6.4; female: M = 22.4, SD = 6.2).
Characteristics of Microblog Use and of Excessive Microblog Use-Sample 3
Sample 3 showed that the correlation between the total duration of microblogging (the total duration of microblogging means the period that a participant has been using Microblog up to the time of the study, e.g. how many months.), and the MEUS score is not significant (ρ = 0.077, p = .153).
We undertook analysis of variance to examine the difference between the average numbers of Tweets browsed per day in the total scores. Our results showed that there was a significant main effect among four levels of Tweets browsed (χ2 = 129.2, p<.001). Furthermore, the more Tweets a participant browsed, the higher the total scores (see Fig. 2a).
We applied the same analysis on the frequency of browsing Microblog per day and the average amount of time spent browsing Microblog on a single occasion. There was also significant main effect among five levels of browsing frequency (χ2 = 192.8, p<.001) and time levels (χ2 = 108.1, p<.001). The more frequently a participant browsed tweets a day, the higher the total score (see Fig. 2b), and the same applied to the average amount of time of spent browsing a microblog at any one time (see Fig. 2c).
In Sample 3, participants’ behavior in microblogs chiefly comprised posting new Tweets (84.1%), browsing the statuses and opinions of people they were following (91.7%), learning hot topics (77.2%), viewing Re-tweets, commenting or using the Twitter @ symbol (75.1%). By comparison, other behaviors, such as participating in discussion of hot topics (26.5%), proposing a topic (15.4%), making friends/expanding number of friends/communicating with famous people or senior personage (16.9%) represented less than 30%.
Characteristics of Microblog Use and of Excessive Microblog Use-Sample 4
Excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction
The total score from the MEUS significantly correlated with the score from Young’s IAT (ρ = 0.581, p<.001). Compared to Young’s IAT, two factors (“withdrawal and health problem” and “time management and performance”) of the excessive use of microblogs are similar to those of Internet addiction. To confirm this, we computed the partial correlation between the excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction while regressing out these two factors. Our results revealed that the original significant correlation between the score from the MEUS and that from the IAT vanished in this sample after controlling for these two factors (r = 0.087, p = .090).
Characteristics of Microblog Use and of Excessive Microblog Use-Sample 5
Excessive use of microblogs, social interaction, and self-disclosure
Our results showed that the score from the MEUS had a significant positive correlation, with small effect (based on Cohen’s criterion [47]), with the score from the Real Social Interaction Scale (ρ = 0.106, p = .001), and with the score from the Self-disclosure Scale in reality (ρ = 0.189, p<.001). Further, the score of MEUS significantly positively correlated with the score of the Self-disclosure Scale on microblogs (ρ = 0.343, p<.001) with moderate effect [47].
Norm of College Students’ Excessive Use of Microblogs
Norm sample distribution
We used Sample 3 and Sample 5 data to calculate the norm table including raw scores and standard scores of the MEUS (see Supporting Information S4). This norm comprised 1,949 participants; 1,110 males and 839 females (see Table 1). The subjects can find their positions in this norm table according to their scores on MEUS.
Standard of the Excessive Use of Microblogs in College Students
In the absence of a standard cutoff point, as the results could not be compared with either clinical research results or with other previously validated questionnaires, obtained score results were grouped according to their deviation from the mean (see Demetrovics et al., 2011) [48]. We created four groups from this norm. Participants with a score below the mean score (19.5) were assigned to the “non excessive use” (NEU) group. Those whose score was one standard deviation (6.6), at most, above the mean score were assigned to the “average excessive use” (AEU) group. Participants with a score that was more than one standard deviation above the mean score belonged either to the “excessive use” (EU) group (with a score less than two standard deviations above the mean) or to the “significant excessive use” (SEU) group (with a score more than two standard deviations above the mean). According to this standard, participants with addictive symptoms accounting for circa 4% fell into the SEU category (see Fig. 3).
Utilizing our proposed standards on the excessive use of microblogs and Young’s criteria for the IAT [31], we calculated the amount of overlap between microblog overuse and Internet addiction. We found that 11 participants in our study exhibited symptoms of both problematic use of microblogs and Internet addiction (see Supporting Information S5).
Discussion
Psychometric Properties of the MEUS
As previously noted, there is currently no scale available for assessing the excessive use of microblogs. In the present study, we developed a scale assessing microblog overuse in Chinese college students in four stages, encompassing interviews, preliminary tests, subject data collection, and reliability and validity data analysis. In addition, our psychometric examinations included an evaluation of the factor structure of the scale’s items (questions), its internal consistency, and its construct and criterion validity. Ultimately, we compiled a 10-item scale, composed of three factors termed “withdrawal and health problem,” “time management and performance,” and “social comfort.” Although some of the consistency coefficients of this 10-item scale were a little lower than .70, the total reliability of the scale reached .80. The EFA data found that all factor loadings in the project were over .50, and the cumulative rate of interpretation was more than 63%. Result of confirmatory factor analysis also met acceptable psychometric standards. Comparative fit index values of 0.90 and above conventionally represent good model fit. Overall, the MEUS showed adequate psychometric properties within the study’s sample of college students from China. With Twitter now the most popular microblog platform in the United States, in the future, this instrument should be assessed on Twitter using a larger and more representative sample. We envisage that relatively the same results would be obtained in Twitter.
How Excessive is the “Excessive” Use of Microblogs?
We collected 1,949 nationwide norm data, which had strong representation of college students in China. Our data showed that 4%–15% of the participants were excessive users of microblogs, indicating that a certain proportion of college students may have symptoms of problematic microblog use, which relates to recent estimates that indicate that 4.07%–14.08% of college students in China have a predominantly inattentive subtype of Internet addiction disorder [49]–[53].
In this norm, participants with a score below 19.4 belonged to the NEU group. Those whose score was 19.4–26.1 belonged to the AEU group, which was characterized by mild symptoms of microblog overuse. Participants with a score 26.1–32.7 belonged to the EU group, whose members met some criterion of problematic microblog use with regard to withdrawal, time management. Those participants with a score of more than 32.7 (i.e., ≥33) fell into the SEU group, and are known as “microblog freaks” in China. They spend a lot of time on microblogs, and have a sense of dependency on microblogs exhibited in their behavior and psychology.
Similarities between the Excessive Use of Microblogs and Internet addiction
Based on our data, we established that the excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction are similar in two factors: “withdrawal and health problem” and “time management and performance problem.” These two factors are core components of Internet addiction or Facebook addiction, reflecting salience, loss of control, and withdrawal [16]. To the Internet addict, computer usage significantly preoccupies their time and thoughts [54]. As an Internet-based behavior, microblog usage is significantly predicted by an individual’s online behavior [55]. Thus, excessive users of microblogs show similar symptoms as Internet addicts. The “withdrawal effect” can be an indication of addictive properties, and the “time management problem” can be described as a control disorder like Internet addiction disorder. In fact, any form of excessive engagement in particular behavioral has some common characteristics, such as desire or impulse and loss of control [56], [57].
In addition, we did not find significant correlation between microblog use duration and excessive use of microblogs. This finding is similar to results in the field of Internet addiction; that is, there is no predictable association between total use time of internet and Internet addiction [58]. For the Internet addict, although computer usage significantly preoccupies their time and thoughts, normal computer use, such as working with a computer, can also take on an addictive quality at times [54]. Hilgard et al. (2013) suggest that a more effective way to prove whether an individual is an Internet game addict or not is to observe whether he or she always plays Internet games in their free time, rather than an analysis of the total usage time [58]. We assert that there is little relation between the total duration of microblogging and addiction, which means the duration of microblog use is not an indicator of microblog overuse [59].
The withdrawal effect, time management problems, and a lack of correlation between usage time and Internet addiction are an indication of addictive tendencies [21]. This further suggests that both Internet addiction and excessive use of microblogs have similar psychological mechanisms; in this case, the relevant element of addiction.
Furthermore, since 11 participants (out of 15 IA participants and 74 MEU participants) in our study exhibited symptoms of both problematic use of microblogs and Internet addiction, we may conclude that the excessive use of microblogs has some significant similarities with Internet addiction.
Differences between the Excessive Use of Microblogs and Internet addiction
In the present study, we identified three differences between the excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction. First, comparing the factors of the MEUS and Young’s IAT, we found the “social comfort” factor to be unique in the MEUS (in the IAT, it is the “reality substitute” factor). After controlling for the “withdrawal and health problem” and “time management and performance” factors, the significant correlation between MEUS and IAT disappeared. These findings indicate that “social comfort” may be a vital factor in measuring the excessive use of microblogs. The motivation for people using social networks, be it Twitter or Facebook, is to seek social comfort and reduce loneliness [60], while individuals play Internet games more for realistic alternative [30]. Second, we found that females scored significantly higher in the MEUS than males. This is consistent with Smith and Rainie’s (2010) research showing that women are more likely to use Twitter than men [61]. However, this is contrary to the finding of Internet addiction. Numerous research results on Internet addiction show that males get higher scores than females [62]–[66]. Compared to traditional Internet surfing, writing and posting tweets by young adults is more about self-presentation [38]. In addition, several studies have shown that females do tend to disclose more information than males do [67]–[69]. Further, in a meta-analysis of 205 studies, Dindia and Allen examined gender differences in self-disclosure and found that females disclose more personal information than males [70]. In our study, the results showed significant positive association between the excessive use of microblogs and self-disclosure. This is consistent with other results (see [38], [39]) that show that self-presentation is a major drive for actively participating in social networking sites [39]. The difference in self-presentation between males and females suggests the essential role of social comfort, hence its importance in our MEUS.
The last difference between Internet addiction and the excessive use of microblogs concerns social interactions. There are different forms of motivation that could explain Internet-based behaviors, such as interpersonal interaction, social support, and self-fulfillment [71]–[73]. Internet addiction leads to significant reductions of social interaction in reality among college students [74], as well as to the impairment of real-life social relationships [75]–[78]. Individuals may spend a lot of time surfing the Internet in order to avoid relationships in real life. This indicates a lack of skills and energy to build relationships in real life [79]. However, our study reveals that excessive use of microblogs has positive correlation with real-life social interactions. This suggests that microblogs may contribute to or not hinder the development of personal relations in real life [5]. Further, the real-time personal updates featured in Twitter and other microblogs may help to sustain a virtual feeling of proximity facilitating social connectedness (i.e., being there, still there) [80]. Thus, as recent research has identified, “the line between the physical world and online social realities has been blurred by the new possibilities afforded by real-time communication and broadcasting, which appear to greatly enhance our social and cognitive capabilities in establishing and maintaining social relations” [81]. Arguably, this is further augmented by a combination of mobile communication devices with new tools such as Twitter [82]. Hence, individuals can obtain social comfort from improving relationships-a notable difference between microblog overuse and Internet addiction.
In summary, from the perspective of factor analysis, gender differences, self-disclosure, and interpersonal interactions, “social comfort” is the main distinguishing factor of the excessive use of microblogs from Internet addiction. This also indicates the difference between microblog overuse and Internet addiction.
On one hand, Internet addiction is not regarded as homogeneous and several subtypes have been identified, such as excessive gaming, online sexual pre-occupation, and e-mailing/texting [83]. On the other hand, some researchers have argued that excessive use of different types of Internet applications reveal a single form of Internet addiction [15]. At the same time, with the development of technology, increasing numbers of popular network applications, including microblogs, are being invented. MySpace and Facebook, representing online social networking, have gone through a period of high-speed growth, while the rapid development of microblogging, with its social function, has become a key factor to its replacement of the social networking sites [3]. Furthermore, even newer communication platforms and media such as micro-letters like Whatapp are becoming more and more popular in China today [84]. Results of the present study indicate that excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction are not exactly the same, suggesting that some symptoms of Internet addiction may change due to development of new technologies. Moreover, Internet addiction may be different from other forms of addiction, such as substance abuse and impulse control disorder. It is not a “fixed” disease awaiting our exploration, but rather a changing and evolving disorder.
This alteration of the concept of Internet addiction may also reflect changes in the Internet itself. For the past two to three decades, people have generally thought that Internet use could damage social relations in real life. Weiser’s (2001) longitudinal study of college students showed a significant negative effect of spending more hours using the Internet, in terms of personal loneliness, depression, and life satisfaction [85]. Researchers thought that online communication reduced real human engagement [86], communication time with family and friends [87], and real social circles [88]. For teenagers, it was thought that their relationships with family and friends could become weaker through increased levels of Internet communication [89]. Online relationships rarely developed into real social relations networks, and so, compared to the latter, online relationships were considered to be very superficial and unsustainable [90], [91]. Conversely, recent research, especially that following the rise of social network sites (Facebook in 2004 and Twitter in 2006), suggests just the opposite. For example, youngsters who actively participate in online creation and sharing are reportedly more involved in additional extracurricular activities in their real life [92]. Koutamanis and colleagues reported that instant messaging can increase teenagers’ capacity for friendship initiation [93]; young adults can practice their social skills online, and, with the passage of time, may have better friendships in real life. Our results suggest the same thing, with the change from “reality substitute” to “social comfort.” Both “reality substitute” and “social comfort” reflect the influence of internet use on real social interaction–the distinction to be drawn is whether this is a positive or a negative influence. Two further studies have shown that the size of an individual’s online social network is related to the structure of specific, socialization-related areas of their brain (e.g., the amygdala, the left middle temporal gyrus, the right posterior superior temporal sulcus, and the right entorhinal cortex) [94], [95]. Our results and those from previously cited studies suggest that Internet use is being incorporated into real life more and more, developing from being considered impairment to our social skills to being a tool for our social skills. Moreover, our study suggests that the effect of the Internet may continue to change and become more complex, and so requires further attention and study.
Limitations and Conclusion
The study had some limitations that merit consideration. Firstly, data for the study were collected through an online survey and geographically, participants were mainly from universities in Eastern China. The sample may not be a correct representation of colleges in China. However, most of the universities at which the study was undertaken are national colleges that attract students from all regions of China. As such, we are confident that the sample correctly represented the microblogging tendencies of college students in China.
Secondly, the study exclusively used self-report questionnaires in data collection. Self-reports are sometimes biased and not entirely reliable. Participants may have understated the extent of their internet and microblog use; presenting less severe symptoms of withdrawal, tolerance and/or social problems. As a result, our findings may not provide a complete picture of the extent of excessive microblogging in China. Future studies should incorporate information provided by peers, guardians and teachers as well as clinical data and employ cognitive behavior tasks.
Thirdly, at present, we just collected data of normal population, and the results showed that 11 participants in our study exhibited symptoms of both problematic use of microblogs and Internet addiction. So, we are planning to collect clinical samples to further verify the validity of the scale, and also to provide a reference threshold for clinical evaluation. And then, we will work on the neural basis of the microblog excessive use to provide a complete picture of the extent of excessive microblogging.
In summary, the three-factor MEUS developed in the current study appeared to be adequate with regard to this sample, which showed good internal consistency as well as construct and criterion validity. Although there are some similarities between excessive use of microblogs and Internet addiction, the MEUS has a unique factor–“social comfort”–, compared to Young’s IAT [28]. Further, the MEUS had significant positive correlation with self-disclosure and social interaction, which is the reverse of published findings on Internet addiction. Our study suggests that excessive use of microblogs is not exactly the same as Internet addiction; hence, it would be illogical to measure the two using the same scale. Finally, the pathogenesis and potential treatment of problematic use of microblogs and Internet addiction may not be fully employed in the same way, an area in which warrants further research in future.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Yamikani Ndasauka for correcting the English, and thank three anonymous reviewers’ comments on the revising process. This work was supported by grants from Anhui Provincial Key Science Foundation for Outstanding Young Talent (2013SQRW007ZD), Academic and Technology Leaders Introduction project of Anhui University (023032030063), and Social Science Planned Project of Anhui Province (AHSKQ2014D30). Also this work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31171083, 31230032), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities of China (WK2070000033), 100 Talents Program of The Chinese Academy of Sciences (BJ2070000047), and Natural Science Fund Project of Anhui Province (1208085MH179).
Funding Statement
This work was supported by grants from Anhui Provincial Key Science Foundation for Outstanding Young Talent (2013SQRW007ZD), Academic and Technology Leaders Introduction project of Anhui University (023032030063), and Social Science Planned Project of Anhui Province (AHSKQ2014D30). Also this work was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31171083, 31230032), the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities of China (WK2070000033), 100 Talents Program of The Chinese Academy of Sciences (BJ2070000047), and Natural Science Fund Project of Anhui Province (1208085MH179). The National Natural Science Foundation of China and Anhui Provincial Key Science Foundation for Outstanding Young Talent played a role in data collection and analysis. The Social Science Planned Project of Anhui Province and 100 Talents Program of The Chinese Academy played a role in the decision to publish. The Academic and Technology Leaders Introduction project of Anhui University, the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities of China, and Natural Science Fund Project of Anhui Province played a role in the preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Barbera LD, Paglia LF, Valsavoia R (2009) Social network and addiction. Stud Health Technol Inform 144: 33–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Mesquita G, Reimao R (2007) Nightly use of computer by adolescents: its effect on quality of sleep. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 65: 428–432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.The thirtieth Statistics Report of China Internet Development (2012) China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC).
- 4.Sagolla D (2009) How Twitter was born: 140 characters. January 30, 2009. Available: http://www.140characters.com/2009/01/30/how-twitter-was-born. Accessed January 22, 2014.
- 5. Xifra J, Graub F (2010) Nanoblogging PR: The discourse on public relations in Twitter. Public Relat Rev 36: 171–174. [Google Scholar]
- 6.EDGAR (2013) Form S-1 Registration Statement, Twitter, Inc. As filed with the U.S. Securities and Exchange Commission on October 3, 2013. Available: http://www.sec.gov/Archives/edgar/data/1418091/000119312513390321/d564001ds1.htm. Accessed January 22, 2014.
- 7.Xinhua (2011) China’s micro blog user population tops 300 million. People’s Daily Online (English edition), November 22, 2011. Available: http://english.peopledaily.com.cn/90882/7651844.html. Accessed January 22, 2014.
- 8.Research report on characteristics of Micro-blog media and user (2010) Beijing Association of Online Media.
- 9. Iskender M, Akin A (2011) Self-compassion and Internet Addiction. Turkish Online J Educ Technol 10: 215–221. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jia R, Hartke H, Pearson J (2007) Can computer playfulness and cognitive absorption lead to problematic technology usage? In: Proceedings of the 28th International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS), December 31, 2007, paper 22. Available: http://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2007/22. Accessed January 22, 2014.
- 11.Griffiths MD, King DL, Demetrovics Z (2014) DSM-5 internet gaming disorder needs a unified approach to assessment. Neuropsychiatry, 4(1), 1–4.
- 12. Charlton JP, Danforth DW (2007) Distinguishing addiction and high engagement in the context of online game playing. Comput Human Behav 23: 1531–1548. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Thadani DR, Cheung MK (2011) Exploring the role of online social network dependency in habit of formation. In: Proceedings of the 32nd International Conference on Information Systems (ICIS) 2011, Shanghai, China, paper 43.
- 14. Turel O, Serenko A, Giles P (2011) Integrating technology addiction and use: An empirical investigation of online auction users. MIS Quarterly 35: 1043–1061. [Google Scholar]
- 15. Huang H, Leung L (2009) Instant messaging addiction among teenagers in China: Shyness, alienation, and academic performance decrement. Cyberpsychol Behav 12: 675–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Andreassen CS, Torsheim T, Brunborg GS, Pallesen S (2012) Development of a Facebook Addiction Scale. Psychol Rep 110: 501–517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Demetrovics Z, Griffiths MD (2012) Behavioral addictions: Past, present and future. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(1), 1–2. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 18. Pies R (2009) Should DSM-V designate “Internet addiction” a mental disorder? Psychiatry 6: 31–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Xun Y (2012) Micro-blog complex: Be careful Micro-blog addiction. The Future is Now 13.
- 20. Ma XH (2012) The causes analysis of college students’ “Weibo Kong” phenomenon. Press Circles 3: 001. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Goldberg I (1995b). Internet addictive disorder (IAD) diagnostic criteria (Vol. 2010).
- 22. Demetrovics Z, Urbán R, Nagygyörgy K, Farkas J, Griffiths MD, et al. (2012) The development of the Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ). PLoS ONE 7(5): e36417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Young KS (1997) What makes the internet addictive: Potential explanations for pathological internet use. Paper presented at the 105th annual conference of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
- 24. Hall AS, Parsons J (2001) Internet addiction: college student case study using best practices in cognitive behavior therapy. J Ment Health Couns 23: 312–327. [Google Scholar]
- 25. Shapira NA, Lessig MC, Goldsmith TD, Szabo ST, Lazoritz M, et al. (2003) Problematic internet use: proposed classification and diagnostic criteria. Depress Anxiety 17: 207–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Widyanto L, Griffiths M (2006) “Internet addiction”: A critical review. eCOMMUNITY: Int J Ment Health Addict 4: 31–51. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tao R, Huang X, Wang J, Zhang H, Zhang Y, et al. (2010) Proposed diagnostic criteria for Internet addiction. Addiction 105: 556–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Dong GH, DeVito EE, Du XX, Cui ZY (2012) Impaired inhibitory control in “Internet Addiction Disorder”: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging 203: 153–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Shaffer HJ, Hall MN, Bilt JV (2000) “Computer addiction”: A critical consideration. Am J Orthopsychiatry 70: 162–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Kuss DJ, Griffiths M (2011) Online Social Networking and Addiction-A Review of the Psychological Literature. Int. J. Environ. Res: Public Health 8: 3528–3552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Young KS (1998) Caught in the Net: How to recognize the signs of Internet addiction and a winning strategy for recovery. New York: John Wiley.
- 32. Chang MK, Law SPM (2008) Factor structure for Young’s Internet Addiction Test: A confirmatory study. Comput Human Behav 24: 2597–2619. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Griffiths M, Hussain Z, Grüsser SM, Thalemann R, Cole H, et al. (2011) Social Interactions in Online Gaming. Int J Game-Based Learning (IJGBL) 1: 74–90. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Baike (2010) Weibo Kong. Available: http://www.baike.com/wiki/微博控 (Chinese). Accessed March, 14, 2014.
- 35. Kittinger R, Correia CJ, Irons JG (2012) Relationship between Facebook use and problematic Internet use among college students. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 15: 324–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Wolniczak I, Cáceres-DelAguila JA, Palma-Ardiles G, Arroyo KJ, Solís-Visscher R, et al. (2013) Association between Facebook dependence and poor sleep quality: a study in a sample of undergraduate students in Peru. PLoS One 8: e59087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Young KS (2004) Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon and its consequences. Am Behav Sci 48: 402–415. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Mazur E, Kozarian L (2010) Self-presentation and interaction in blogs of adolescents and young emerging adults. J Adolesc Res 25: 124–144. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Krämer NC, Winter S (2008) Impression Management 2.0: The relationship of self-esteem extraversion, self-efficacy, and self-presentation within social networking sites. J Media Psychol 20: 106–116. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wang JX, Yang YY (2011) 2011 China social psychology research report. Beijing, China: Social Science Academic Press.
- 41. Jourard SM, Lasakow P (1958) Some factors in self-disclosure. J Abnorm Psychol 56: 91–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Yan MY (2011) The research of interpersonal and Internet addiction in the undergraduate students. Master’s dissertation, Anhui Medical University.
- 43. Chen D, Zhang JM, Shen LL (2009) Internet addiction and its relationship with social anxiety of college students. China J Health Psychol 17: 151–152. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Steiger JH (2007) Understanding the limitations of global fit assessment in structural equation modeling. Pers Individ Dif 42: 893–898. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Kelley TL (1939) The selection of upper and lower groups for the validation of test items. J Educ Psychol 30: 17–24. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Dai HQ, Zhang F, Chen XF(2004) Psychological and Educational Measurement, Guang Zhou, China: Jinan University Press, pp, 118.
- 47. Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112: 155–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Demetrovics Z, Szeredi B, Rózsa S (2008) The three-factor model of Internet addiction: The development of the Problematic Internet Use Questionnaire. Behav Res Methods 40: 563–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Pan XL, Li Z, Zhang ZD (2009) An investigation into university students’ Internet use and Internet addiction. J Guangdong Baiyun Inst 16: 41–47. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Luo JH, Wan JJ, Liu XQ (2010) The relationship of Internet use, Internet special self-efficacy and Internet addiction in university students. Psychol Dev Educ 26: 618–626. [Google Scholar]
- 51. Ma J, Li R, Zhu CF (2008) A study on relationship between IAD, IAT and mental health among adolescent students. Chinese J Prevention & Control of Chronic Non-Communicable Diseases 16: 161–164. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Yu BL, Ji HP (2010) Analysis of internet addiction status and risk factors of some college students in Jiangsu Province. J Nanjing Medical University (Nat Sci) 30: 476–479, 501.
- 53. Zhang RP, Li QA (2011) 11 Research on Internet Addiction Disorder and its related factors in senior high school students. China J Health Psychol 19: 714–716. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Shaw M, Black DW (2008) Internet addiction: Definition, assessment, epidemiology and clinical management. CNS Drugs 22: 353–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Thelwall M, Buckley K, Paltoglou G (2011) Sentiment in Twitter events. J Am Soc Inf Sci Technol 62: 406–418. [Google Scholar]
- 56. Peters CS, Malesky LA Jr (2008) Problematic Usage Among Highly-Engaged Players of Massively Multiplayer Online Role Playing Games. CyberPsychol Behav 11: 481–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Shaffer HJ, Hall MN, Bilt JV (1997) Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States and Canada: A meta-analysis. Boston, MA: President and Fellows of Harvard College. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 58. Hilgard J, Engelhardt CR, Bartholow BD (2013) Individual differences in motives, preferences, and pathology in video games: The gaming attitudes, motives, and experiences scales (GAMES). Front Psychol 4: 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Lin XH, Yan GG (2001) Internet addiction disorder, online behavior and personality. Chinese J Mental Health 15: 281–283. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Deters FG, Mehl MR (2013) Does posting Facebook status updates increase or decrease loneliness? An online social networking experiment. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 4: 579–586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Smith A, Rainie L (2010) Who Tweets?, Pew Internet & American Life Project, Washington, DC.
- 62. Ng BD, Wiemer-Hastings P (2005) Addiction to the Internet and online gaming. Cyberpsychol Behav 8: 110–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Jiang X, Zhao H, Yu SW (2012) The status and influence factors of internet addiction for Yingkou middle school students. Chinese J School Health 33: 225–226. [Google Scholar]
- 64. Kang YH, Shen L, Peng XL (2012) Analysis of Internet addiction and personality factors of middle school students in Haikou. Chinese J School Health 33: 99–101. [Google Scholar]
- 65. Li YC, Liu BL, Li RJ (2012) Investigation and analysis of related factors of one junior middle school students with Internet addiction disorder in Jinan. Chinese J School Health 33: 199–201. [Google Scholar]
- 66. Zhang SZ, Li FH (2011) An investigation on the status quo of university students’ Internet addiction. Teach Educ Res 23: 44–48. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Consedine NS, Sabag-Cohen S, Krivoshekova YS (2007) Ethnic, gender, and socio-economic differences in young adults’ self-disclosure: who discloses what and to whom? Cult Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol 13: 254–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Hinson JA, Swanson JL (1993) Willingness to seek help as a function of self-disclosure and problem severity. J Couns Dev 71: 465–470. [Google Scholar]
- 69. Sprecher S, Hendrick SS (2004) Self-disclosure in intimate relationships: Associations with individual and relationship characteristics over time. J Soc Clin Psycholo 23: 857–877. [Google Scholar]
- 70. Dindia K, Allen M (1992) Sex differences in self disclosure: A meta analysis. Psycholo Bull 112: 106–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Parks MR, Floyd K (1996) Making friends in cyberspace. J Comput-Med Comm 1: 80–97. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Turkle S (1995) Life on the Screen. New York: Simon and Schuster.
- 73. Young KS (1996) Psychology of computer use: XL. Addictive use of the Internet: A case that breaks the stereotype. Psychol Rep 79: 899–902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Yellowlees PM, Marks S (2007) Problematic Internet use or Internet addiction? Comput Human Behav 23: 1447–1453. [Google Scholar]
- 75. Yi FR, Zhou SJ (2012) The physical, psychological and social consequences of adolescents with Internet addiction. China J Health Psychol 20: 233–236. [Google Scholar]
- 76. Liu CN, Li R, Song JD (2008) Study on influential factors for Internet addiction among adolescent students of Tianjin. Chinese J Prevention & Control of Chronic Non-Communicable Diseases 16: 159–161. [Google Scholar]
- 77. Yao YH, Hao QH, Liu GG (2007) Harmfulness and prevention countermeasures on adolescent network addiction. Chinese J School Health 28: 475–477. [Google Scholar]
- 78. Yu YW, Du YS (2007) An investigation on mental health status of Internet-addicted middle school students in Shanghai. Shanghai Archives Psychiatry 19: 1–3. [Google Scholar]
- 79.China Youth Association for Network Development (CYAND) (2009) The Data Report of China Youth Internet Addiction.
- 80.Hu XM, Chen Y (2010) Psychology and micro-blogging: Self-presentation, social interaction and social culture. IEEE 2nd Symposium on Web Society (SWS), 2010: August 16–17, 2010, Beijing, China. Piscataway, NJ: IEEE Press. 643–647.
- 81. Gonçalves B, Perra N, Vespignani A (2011) Modeling users’ activity on Twitter networks: Validation of Dunbar’s number. PLoS ONE 6: e22656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Goncalves B, Perra N, Vespignani A (2011) Modeling users’ activity on Twitter networks: Validation of Dunbar’s number. PLoS ONE 6: e22656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Young KS (2009) Internet addiction: diagnosis and treatment consideration. J Contemp Psychother 39: 241–246. [Google Scholar]
- 84.The number of users of instant messaging “Micro-letters” rapid growth in Latin America. People’s Daily Online, June 11, 2014. Available: http://tech.sina.com.cn/i/2014-06-11/09369430606.shtml. Accessed June 15, 2014.
- 85. Weiser EB (2001) The functions of Internet use and their social and psychological consequences. Cyberpsychol Behav 4: 723–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Nie NH, Hillygus DS, Erbring L (2002) Internet use, interpersonal relations, and sociability: A time diary study. In Wellman B, Haythornthwaite C, editors. The Internet in everyday life. Oxford, England: Blackwell. 215–243.
- 87.Gershuny J (2000) Changing times: Work and leisure in postindustrial society. New York: Oxford University Press.
- 88. Mesch G (2001) Social relationships and Internet use among adolescents in Israel. Soc Sci Q 82: 329–340. [Google Scholar]
- 89. Sanders CE, Field TM, Diego M, Kaplan M (2000) The relationship of Internet use to depression and social isolation. Adolesc 35: 237–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Parks MR, Roberts LD (1998) “Making Moosic”: The development of personal relationships on-line and a comparison to their off-line counterparts. J Soc Pers Relat 15: 517–537. [Google Scholar]
- 91. Cornwell B, Lundgren DC (2001) “Love on the Internet: involvement and misrepresentation in romantic relationships in cyberspace vs. realspace”. Comput Human Behav 17: 197–211. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Lenhart A, Madden M (2007) Social networking websites and teens: An overview. Washington, D.C.: Pew Internet & American Life Project.
- 93. Koutamanis M, Vossen Helen GM, Peter J, Valkenburg PM (2013) Practice makes perfect: The longitudinal effect of adolescents’ instant messaging on their ability to initiate offline friendships. Comput Human Behav 29: 2265–2272. [Google Scholar]
- 94. Kanai R, Bahrami B, Roylance R, Rees G (2012) Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proc Biol Sci 279: 1327–1334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Bickart KC, Wright CI, Dautoff RJ, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF (2011) Amygdala volume and social network size in humans. Nat Neurosci 14: 163–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.