Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Nov 5.
Published in final edited form as: Neuroepidemiology. 2014 Nov 5;43(2):131–139. doi: 10.1159/000365733

Subsets of a Large Cognitive Battery Better Power Clinical Trials on Early Stage Alzheimer Disease

Chengjie Xiong 1,6, Hua Weng 1, David A Bennett 2, Patricia A Boyle 2, Raj C Shah 2, Scot Fague 1, Charles B Hall 3, Richard B Lipton 4, John C Morris 5,6
PMCID: PMC4237272  NIHMSID: NIHMS633839  PMID: 25376544

Abstract

Background/Aims

Cognitive batteries routinely used by the Alzheimer disease (AD) research community may contain items uninformative for tracking disease progression to power clinical trials on early stage AD. We aim to identify subsets of the most informative items from an existing cognitive battery for better powering clinical trials on early AD.

Methods

Longitudinal change in item scores from the battery was associated with the onset of Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) in 1513 elderly individuals. Items whose longitudinal changes were correlated with the onset of MCI were selected as informative for tracking the early cognitive progression.

Results

226 items in the battery were annually assessed over a follow-up of up to 13 years. Changes of item scores over time from 187 items were significantly correlated with the onset of MCI. For clinical trials on preclinical AD and on MCI, informative items permit smaller or similar sample sizes as compared to the entire battery, whereas uninformative items require much larger sample sizes.

Conclusions

Longitudinal changes in item scores from about 17% of items in the cognitive battery are uninformative for tracking early disease progression. Clinical trials on early AD can be better powered using informative items rather than the entire battery.

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is an age-related neurodegenerative disorder that results in progressive cognitive impairment and death. Accumulating research suggests that the neurodegenerative processes associated with AD begin years prior to the symptomatic onset of AD when the disease is clinically at the early prodromal stage or a latent stage.13 Many recent clinicopathologic studies also have demonstrated that asymptomatic individuals can manifest the neuropathological changes of AD, notably senile plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. 46 These observations, coupled with the fact that there are currently no pharmaceutical treatments that reverse the pathological processes of AD, have led to a major paradigm shift in the search of efficacious treatments of AD, that is, the focus of modern AD clinical trials now is on individuals at the earliest clinical stages, such as Mild Cognitive Impairment 7 (MCI) and/or very mild dementia (i.e., a Clinical Dementia Rating 8 (CDR) of 0.5), or even the preclinical stage 9 prior to the substantial development of clinical symptoms as these may be the groups of individuals in which targeted therapies may have the greatest chance of preserving brain function.

The paradigm shift in clinical trials on AD subsequently has led to three major inter-related biomedical decisions that must be made by investigators in designing modern clinical trials at the early stages of AD: the cognitive outcome measure, sample size, and disease duration. Because cognitive batteries routinely used by the AD research community have been traditionally designed to track the disease progression after symptomatic onset and to identify cases of fully developed AD dementia in comparison to normal controls, they often show significant ceiling and floor effects. Therefore, the current cognitive batteries may contain items that are neither sensitive nor specific for tracking early stage disease progression. As a result, they only exhibit subtle changes during the very early stage or the preclinical stage of AD. Several recent randomized clinical trials (RCTs) using existing instruments (e.g., the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive subscale10 ) failed to detect significant decline in placebo groups with MCI. Especially for RCTs on early stage or preclinical stage of AD, the lack of progression on existing cognitive outcomes has become an important challenge to the feasibility of such trials because of the need for a large number of individuals to be followed over many years to allow meaningful statistical conclusions to be drawn.1113 Large, long-duration RCTs are time-consuming and prohibitively costly. Although emerging cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers and neuroimaging markers1418 have been reported to show early changes in AD progression, recently revised FDA guidelines for RCTs on early stage AD mandate that treatments of AD be only approved if they demonstrate cognitive and functional benefits (FDA Guidance for Industry Alzheimer’s Disease: Developing Drugs for the Treatment of Early Stage Disease:19 http://www.fda.gov/downloads/Drugs/GuidanceComplianceRegulatoryInformation/Guidances/UCM338287.pdf).

The objective of this manuscript is to provide better cognitive outcomes for future RCTs on early stage AD. We posit that subsets of items, identified from a large cognitive battery that are shown to be most informative to early disease progression, can better power clinical trials on early stage AD than the other uninformative items and even the entire battery by reducing the sample sizes and improving the efficiency. In order to identify the most informative items, we focused on tracking the disease progression (not necessarily for the prediction of the disease), and analyzed the longitudinal changes of item scores of individual items from the cognitive battery administered to a large longitudinal cohort study, and correlated the time to the onset of MCI with the time of the item score changes (i.e., from endorsement to non-endorsement of the item over time). Finally, we estimated the sample sizes to adequately power future RCTs on MCI and on preclinical AD using the composite cognitive scores from the items identified as the most informative for early disease progression, and compared them to the composite scores derived from the entire battery as well as the items not identified as informative.

Materials and Methods

The longitudinal cognitive database of the Rush Memory and Aging Project (MAP), a longitudinal clinical-pathologic cohort study of aging and dementia, was analyzed first for identifying the most informative items to track cognitive progression of early stage AD and then for powering clinical trials on early stage AD.

Participants

Participants were individuals from the MAP, an ongoing longitudinal, community study of common chronic conditions of old age that began in 1997. Participants were recruited primarily from continuing-care retirement communities throughout the Chicago metropolitan area because the ability to maintain high rates of clinical follow-up and to obtain autopsy is key to the MAP mission. This was supplemented by recruitment at senior and subsidized housing, churches, and social service agencies to ensure a range of socioeconomic status, race, and ethnicity. A requirement for study entry is that participants understood what was involved in the study in order to sign an Informed Consent and agreed to donate their brains, spinal cords, nerves and muscles at the time of death. 20 A total of N=1513 elderly individuals who were either cognitively normal or with MCI or AD at baseline were available for our analyses as of September 21, 2012. 1037 were cognitively normal, 402 had MCI and 74 had AD at baseline. Baseline characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of the sample (Total n = 1513)

Normal, n=1037 MCI, n=402 AD, n=74
Age (mean, SD) 78.81 (7.47) 81.91 (7.44) 84.86 (6.07)
Gender (% of female) 75.70 69.90 54.05
Education (y, mean, SD) 14.45 (3.28) 14.43 (3.02) 14.09 (3.92)
Race: % for Caucasian 93.83 90.80 94.59
% for African American 5.40 7.96 5.41
% of others 0.68 0.75 0
APOE4 positive (%) 17.65 27.61 32.43
MMSE (mean, SD) 28.41 (1.70) 26.6 (2.5) 18.33 (6.73)
Global cognition (mean, SD) 0.26 (0.46) −0.41 (0.49) −1.51 (0.74)

Standard Protocol Approval and Patient Consents

Written informed consent was obtained from all study participants. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Rush University Medical Center and the Washington University School of Medicine.

Cognitive Assessment and Clinical Diagnosis

Cognition was assessed annually with a comprehensive battery testing cognitive domains commonly affected by aging and AD: episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perceptual speed, and visual–spatial ability. Details of the cognitive function tests have been reported previously. 2122 In brief, 20 cognitive tests were administered annually. Seven were episodic memory measures: Word List Memory, Recall, and Recognition 23 and immediate and delayed recall of Story A from Logical Memory of the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised 24 and of the East Boston Story 2526 . Semantic memory was assessed with a 15-item version 21 of the Boston Naming Test, 27 Verbal Fluency, 23,26 and a 15-item version 26 of the National Adult Reading Test . 28 Working memory tests included Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Backward 24 and digit ordering . 26,29 Four measures of perceptual speed were administered: the oral version of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test , 30 Number Comparison , 26,31 and two measures from a modified version 22 of the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test: 32 number of color names correctly read in 30s minus the number of errors and number of colors correctly named in 30s minus the number of errors. Visuospatial ability was assessed with a 15-item version of Judgment of Line Orientation 33 and a 16-item version of Standard Progressive Matrices . 34 In addition, the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE 35 ) also was used to serve as a brief measure of cognitive function. To minimize floor and ceiling artifacts and other sources of measurement error, a global composite measure of cognition that was previously reported 2122 and based on all 20 tests including MMSE and measures of episodic memory, semantic memory, working memory, perceptual speed, and visuospatial ability was created by converting raw scores to z scores, using the baseline mean and SD across the entire cohort, and averaging the z scores. Further information about the individual tests and the derivation of the composite measure was described elsewhere. 2122 Cognitive testing was scored by computer and reviewed by a neuropsychologist to determine cognitive impairment. Participants were then evaluated by a clinician for a medical history and a neurologic examination to diagnose AD using National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria. 36 The diagnosis of dementia required a history of cognitive decline and impairment in at least 2 cognitive domains, and MCI (all types) required the presence of cognitive impairment in the absence of dementia. 20

Types of Item Scores

Individual items were scored as binary for most cognitive tests except for five (Color Name, Logical Memory IA and IIA, Verbal Fluency, and Words Correctly Read) which could not be reasonably decomposed into meaningful binary items and therefore were treated as single-item tests with the original count score. Table 2 presents the number of individual items as well as baseline summary statistics for each of the 20 cognitive tests (including MMSE). A few items from several tests were not originally scored as binary (e.g., one item in MMSE has an original score of 0 to 5), such items were treated as binary in the item analyses using the perfect score vs. others. Sensitivity analyses were also done with other possible cutoffs for these items.

Table 2.

Item characteristics of the cognitive tests at baseline and the number of informative items identified (SD=Standard Deviation, Inf.=Informative)

Test name Number of items Score types Total score: Mean (SD) Number Inf. Items
Normal MCI AD
color name 1 count 19.61 (7.24) 14.74 (7.49) 6.83 (6.95) 1
Logical Memory IIA 1 count 10.34 (3.83) 5.35 (4.04) 1.53 (2.23) 1
Boston Naming Test 15 binary 14.11 (1.63) 13.85 (6.62) 17.8 (26.03) 13
Verbal Fluency 2 count 35.32 (8.38) 27.66 (8.33) 17.03 (9.35) 2
Digits Backward 12 binary 6.48 (2.11) 5.39 (1.88) 4.86 (3.87) 7
Digits Forward 12 binary 8.45 (2.02) 7.77 (2.11) 7.16 (3.38) 6
Digit Ordering 10 binary 7.29 (1.45) 6.34 (1.71) 3.32 (2.55) 8
Delayed Story Recall 12 binary 9.64 (2.95) 7.59 (3.85) 10.04 (21.12) 12
Immediate Story Recall 12 binary 9.91 (1.69) 8.57 (3.25) 12.15 (20.36) 12
Line Orientation 30 binary 25.11 (3.32) 22.84 (4.22) 19.63 (6.70) 23
MMSE 27 binary 25.60 (2.17) 23.88 (3.35) 18.93 (19.84) 24
Number Comparison 4 binary 1.81 (0.96) 1.44 (0.96) 0.86 (0.75) 2
Progressive Matrices 16 binary 11.74 (2.60) 9.9 (2.88) 8.36 (2.67) 13
Reading Test 15 binary 12.65 (3.42) 12.32 (7.40) 22.8 (33.89) 14
Symbol Digit Modalities 5 binary 3.09 (1.23) 2.18 (1.32) 0.70 (0.94) 4
Word List Memory 30 binary 18.31 (7.93) 14.55 (13.62) 28.59 (66.46) 23
Word List Recall 10 binary 6.03 (3.00) 3.59 (7.04) 7.70 (22.72) 10
Word List Recognition 10 binary 9.84 (0.49) 8.67 (4.56) 13.25 (22.94) 10
Logical Memory IA 1 count 11.95 (3.84) 7.70 (4.12) 3.19 (3.25) 1
Words correctly read 1 count 51.13 (13.02) 45.8 (14.15) 30.75 (16.07) 1

Other Covariates

Demographics such as age, sex and years of education were recorded at the study entry. APOE genotyping was done and individuals were dichotomized into those with at least 1 or more copies of the E4 allele (E4 positive) vs. those without an E4 copy (E4 negative).

Statistical Analysis

A two-step procedure was implemented to select the most informative items with binary score (i.e., endorsement and non-endorsement of the item, oriented in the way that non-endorsement always indicates problems with cognition) from the entire cognitive battery for better tracking the disease progression and designing future RCTs at the early stages. The first step examined the longitudinal changes of scores for each individual item and tested whether the item score changes over time were associated with the onset of MCI. Specifically, for each individual, we first computed the age at onset of MCI. If an individual never developed MCI or AD during the entire follow-up, the age at onset was considered as right-censored. If a subject was already classified as MCI or AD at the baseline, the age at onset of MCI was considered interval censored between 0 and the age at baseline. For each individual and each binary item, we then computed the age of non-endorsement defined as the age when the item was incorrectly endorsed. Because of the fluctuation of item level scores over time, the item-specific age of non-endorsement for each individual also was considered interval censored with the left side of the interval as the first age in the follow-up when the item was endorsed incorrectly and the right side as the age at the first occurrence of non-endorsement after which the item remained incorrectly endorsed over time. 37 If an individual already incorrectly endorsed the item at baseline, the left side of the age of non-endorsement was defined as 0. If an individual never incorrectly endorsed the item during the entire follow-up, the age of non-endorsement was considered as right-censored at the age of last assessment. For each item, the item-specific age of non-endorsement and the age at onset of MCI were then correlated across the entire cohort including subjects who were normal, MCI, or AD at baseline. The correlation was estimated by a Kendall's coefficient of concordance through a bivariate smooth estimate of the joint density on the logarithms of the two time scales that was obtained using a mixture of Gaussian densities fixed on a grid with weights determined by a penalized likelihood approach. 3738 Items with a significant correlation (p<0.05) were identified as informative for tracking early disease progression.

Five tests in the cognitive battery could not be reasonably decomposed as binary items because their scores are counts that can be any non-negative integers: Color Name, Logical Memory IA and IIA, Verbal Fluency (with 2 items, Animal and Fruit), and Words Correctly Read. Preliminary analyses similar to those used for the binary items were conducted using different cutoffs, suggesting that all five tests have reasonable sensitivity and specificity for tracking early disease changes. Thus, they (a total of 6 items) were always included as informative items, using their original scores (not the dichotomized scores).

Because for each item, the correlation between the age of non-endorsement and the age at onset of MCI does not indicate whether the age of non-endorsement occurred earlier or later than the age at onset of MCI, the second step of our item selection procedure compared the center of the interval censored age of non-endorsement and age at onset of MCI, and identified the items whose age of non-endorsement occurred later than the age at onset MCI for at least more than half (e.g., >51%) of individuals in the entire cohort. This subset of the informative items was used for tracking the disease progression and designing RCTs on individuals who were already MCI at baseline.

Power Analysis

In traditional prevention trials, it is common to randomize high-risk subjects to active drug or placebo and analyze time to the onset of the disease as the primary efficacy endpoint with a standard Cox proportional hazards model (PHM). 39 Recently, for modern RCTs either on 'preclinical AD' or on MCI, it has been suggested that the PHM approach is subject to a loss of power to detect a treatment effect, in comparison to the approach with a linear mixed model for repeated measures (MMRM 40 ) on a well defined cognitive composite. 41 Based on the informative items identified, three cognitive composite scores were computed similar to the way the global cognitive composite score was defined (see Cognitive Assessment and Clinical Diagnosis): one using all informative items, another using the subset of the informative items whose age of non-endorsement occurred later than the age at onset of MCI, and the other using the items that were not identified as informative (i.e., uninformative). For each composite, a mixed model for repeated measures (MMRM 40 ) was then implemented to estimate the mean 2-year, 3-year, and 4-year change from the baseline as well as the relevant variance and covariance parameters from subjects who were considered 'preclinical AD' (in the absence of biomarker data, operationally defined as those who were cognitively normal at baseline but with at least 1 copy of APOE E4 allele) or MCI at baseline. To examine how the newly formed cognitive composites with informative items influence the sample sizes required for adequately powering future RCTs at the early stages, these estimates were used to further estimate the sample sizes for future RCTs on 'preclinical AD' or MCI with either 2-year, 3-year, or 4-year annual follow-ups using a standard normal test. 42 A power of 80% was assumed for all power analyses. For comparison purpose, similar power analyses were also conducted using the same subjects sample but the global cognitive composite derived from the entire cognitive battery as well as the cognitive composite derived from items not selected as informative. All statistical analyses were implemented in SAS. 43

Results

A total of 226 items (including 220 items with binary data and 6 items from 5 tests with count data) from the cognitive battery were analyzed. Of the 1037 cognitively normal individuals, 404 developed MCI (all types) or AD during up to 12 years of follow-up. Individual items with very limited longitudinal data (i.e., those with less than 30% of the total annual assessments across the entire cohort for a total of 34 items) were excluded from the item analyses. The first step of the two-step procedure identified a total of 181 binary items whose age of non-endorsement is significantly correlated with the age at onset of MCI, resulting in a total of 187 informative items including the 6 items from 5 tests with count data. At the second step, a subset of 62 items were further identified for tracking disease progression on individuals who had MCI at baseline because their age of non-endorsement occurred later than the age at onset of MCI for at least more than half (e.g., >51%) of individuals in the entire cohort. The last column of Table 2 presents the number of informative items identified from each of the 20 cognitive tests in the battery. Table 2A in the Appendix lists all individual items that were found to be informative for tracking the early disease progression.

Using a consistent way of forming cognitive composites (i.e., averaging the z scores across multiple tests obtained by using the baseline mean and SD of each test), four cognitive composites were computed using the following: the 187 informative items as identified above, the 62 informative items whose age of non-endorsement occurred later than the age at onset of MCI, all 20 cognitive tests in the battery, and the items not identified as informative by our analyses. Table 3 presents the estimated mean change and associated standard error (SE) of the cognitive composites from baseline among subjects with 'preclinical AD' at baseline, as well as among those with MCI at baseline. Results in Table 3 suggest that, for both 'preclinical AD' and MCI, the rate of change on the cognitive composite from the informative items is larger than that on the cognitive composite based on the entire battery, which in turn is larger than that on the cognitive composite based on items not identified as informative.

Table 3.

Estimated mean change (standard error, SE) of cognitive composites from baseline on individuals with MCI and 'Preclinical AD'

Types of items used in the composite Clinical stage Years of follow-up (sample size) Mean change(SE) from baseline Ratio of Mean/SE
Informative Items Preclinical AD 2y (N=139) −0.085 (0.030) −2.833
3y (N=121) −0.093 (0.031) −3.000
4y (N=102) −0.197 (0.036) −5.472
MCI 2y (N=275) −0.261 (0.038) −6.868
3y (N=212) −0.416 (0.048) −8.667
4y (N=174) −0.558 (0.060) −9.300
Uninformative Items Preclinical AD 2y (N=139) −0.034 (0.030) −1.133
3y (N=121) −0.034 (0.035) −0.971
4y (N=102) −0.083 (0.040) −2.075
MCI 2y (N=275) −0.072 (0.028) −2.571
3y (N=212) −0.144 (0.040) −3.600
4y (N=174) −0.206 (0.045) −4.578
All Tests Preclinical AD 2y (N=139) −0.063 (0.024) −2.625
3y (N=121) −0.082 (0.028) −2.929
4y (N=102) −0.182 (0.033) −5.515
MCI 2y (N=275) −0.129 (0.022) −5.863
3y (N=212) −0.244 (0.030) −8.133
4y (N=174) −0.330 (0.036) −9.167

To assess the ability of the items identified as informative for tracking early disease progression to improve the design of modern RCTs on early stage AD, we considered two types of future two-arm RCT to test the cognitive efficacy of a novel therapeutic compound against a placebo on individuals who were 'preclinical AD' or MCI at baseline. The sample size ratio of the RCTs is assumed 1:1 between the two arms. The longitudinal follow-ups are assumed to be annual with a range of 2 to 4 years. The effect size (ES) of the novel treatment is assumed as a percentage of improvement on the change from baseline as compared to the placebo, the latter of which was estimated by a MMRM assuming a covariance structure of compound symmetry. We used change on the cognitive composite from the 187 informative items as the primary efficacy endpoint for the RCT on 'preclinical AD', and change on the cognitive composite from the subset of 62 informative items as the primary efficacy endpoint for the RCT on MCI because these items' age of non-endorsement occurred later than the age at onset of MCI. As indicated in Table 4, for the trial on 'preclinical AD' with a 2-years or 3-years annual follow-up, the use of 187 informative items provides smaller sample sizes than the use of entire cognitive battery, resulting in a reduction of sample size from 2% to 10% across a wide range of effect sizes. On the other hand, the sample size of the trial using items not identified as informative is at least 6 times of that using the informative items. For example, with a reasonable effect size of 40%, the RCT of 2-year follow-up can be adequately powered with a total of 3484 subjects using the informative items and 3887 subjects using the entire battery, and 22159 subjects using the items not identified as informative. For the RCT on 'preclinical AD' with a 4-years follow-up using informative items, the sample size is slightly lower than that of using the entire battery, but is only about one seventh of that using items not identified as informative. Very similar observations can be made for the RCTs on MCI. In comparison to the entire battery, the 62 informative items provide a sample size reduction of 11% to 30% in a RCT on MCI with a 2 or 3-years of follow-up, and only a slight increase (about 1%) in a RCT on MCI with a 4-years follow-up. On the other hand, the items not identified as informative require sample sizes that are 4 to 7 times that of the informative items.

Table 4.

Sample sizes of future RCTs on 'Preclinical AD' and MCI (Inf=cognitive composite using informative items alone, UnInf=cognitive composite using uninformative items, Glob=cognitive composite using all 20 tests)

Clinical Stages 2 Years follow-up 3 Years follow-up 4 Years follow-up
ES (%) Inf Glob UnInf Inf Glob UnInf Inf Glob UnInf
Pre-clinical AD 20 13933 15545 88635 10632 10896 102055 2706 2658 18637
25 8917 9949 56727 6805 6973 65316 1732 1701 11928
30 6193 6909 39394 4726 4843 45358 1203 1182 8283
35 4550 5076 28943 3472 3558 33325 884 868 6086
40 3484 3887 22159 2658 2724 25514 677 665 4660
45 2753 3071 17509 2101 2153 20160 535 525 3682
50 2230 2488 14182 1702 1744 16329 433 426 2982
55 1843 2056 11721 1406 1441 13495 358 352 2465
60 1549 1728 9849 1182 1211 11340 301 296 2071
65 1320 1472 8392 1007 1032 9663 257 252 1765
70 1138 1269 7236 868 890 8332 221 217 1522
75 991 1106 6303 757 775 7258 193 189 1326
80 871 972 5540 665 681 6379 170 167 1165
MCI: 20 4594 6522 32030 2173 2437 12610 1581 1560 6393
25 2940 4174 20499 1391 1560 8071 1012 999 4091
30 2042 2899 14236 966 1083 5605 703 694 2841
35 1500 2130 10459 710 796 4118 517 510 2088
40 1149 1631 8008 544 610 3153 396 390 1599
45 908 1289 6327 430 482 2491 313 309 1263
50 735 1044 5125 348 390 2018 253 250 1023
55 608 863 4236 288 323 1668 210 207 846
60 511 725 3559 242 271 1402 176 174 711
65 435 618 3033 206 231 1194 150 148 606
70 375 533 2615 178 199 1030 130 128 522
75 327 464 2278 155 174 897 113 111 455
80 288 408 2002 136 153 789 99 98 400

Discussion

Individual items are the foundation for cognitive outcome measures used in RCTs on AD, and their test scores are inherently noisy when used to longitudinally track cognitive progression, especially at the early stages of disease. To our best knowledge, our item level longitudinal analyses represent the first comprehensive effort to associate the onset of early cognitive symptoms (i.e., MCI) with longitudinal change in item level scores from a comprehensive cognitive battery. We found that, out of a total of 226 items from a large cognitive battery administered longitudinally in MAP on a large sample size of 1513 individuals, the longitudinal item score changes were associated with the onset of MCI for 187 items over an annual follow-up of up to 13 years. Of these, the item score changes for 62 items (i.e., from endorsement to non-endorsement of items) occurred after the onset of MCI. A total of 39 items (i.e., 17.26%) were found to be uninformative for tracking the cognitive progression at early disease stage, i.e., the longitudinal changes of item scores were not associated with the onset of MCI.

Although the conventional test scores from cognitive batteries used in many AD studies have been very successful in cross sectionally discriminating fully developed symptomatic AD from normal aging, they are less satisfactory in longitudinally tracking the early changes of AD when individuals are at the early or preclinical stage of the disease. In fact, when some of current cognitive tests are administered to individuals in the preclinical or early stage of the disease, the resulting data are subject to enormous ceiling and floor effects. Because cognitive items and tests with significant ceiling and floor effects have limited use in tracking longitudinal changes, they are unlikely to be correlated with the early disease progression, and therefore have limited power to predict whether or at what time point an individual will develop subtle sign of early changes which will eventually lead to the onset of MCI and AD.

There is currently a major conundrum in the search of effective treatments of AD. On the one hand, accumulating research evidence indicates that neurodegenerative processes associated with AD begin years prior to the symptomatic onset of AD, 46 suggesting that the optimum time window for treatment interventions is when the disease is clinically at the early prodromal stage or even the latent or preclinical stage. On the other hand, the lack of detection of progression by cognitive tests routinely used in current AD research makes the sample size for clinical trials on early disease stage or preclinical AD a formidable task to achieve. This bottleneck is primarily due to the lack of cognitive measures that can reliably detect the earliest possible cognitive changes of early stage AD in the presence of high inherent inter-individual variability during the progression of the disease. 44 This question is challenging because, by definition, longitudinal cognitive changes have to be subtle during the early stages of the disease.

In comparison to the entire battery, we found that the 187 informative items (in the form of a standard composite) provide smaller or comparable sample sizes to adequately power future RCTs on individuals with 'preclinical AD', operationally defined as, in the absence of biomarker data in this analysis, those who were cognitively normal at baseline but with at least one allele of APOE4. For future RCTs on MCI, a subset of 62 informative items whose item score changes occurred later than the onset of MCI also provides sample sizes smaller than or comparable to those from the entire battery. Importantly, we found several folds of increased sample sizes to power future RCTs on either 'preclinical AD' or MCI when items not identified as informative by our analyses were used. These results have 3 major implications in designing future RCTs on early stage AD and in tracking early disease progression. First, they suggest that the commonly used cognitive batteries with years of longitudinal data remain the most important pilot data to design future RCTs on early stage AD, as a majority of the items were informative to early disease progression, consistent with several reports (albeit cross-sectional). 4549 Second, given the current challenges facing modern RCTs on early stage AD in terms of choosing appropriate cognitive outcomes and determining the adequate sample sizes, our results suggest the feasibility of using subsets of informative items from an existing cognitive battery as the cognitive efficacy outcome in powering future RCTs on 'preclinical AD' or MCI. Third, when it comes to tracking early disease progression, data collected on a large number of uninformative items may represent less than optimal use of precious research resources as well as an increased burden to research participants. In fact, as demonstrated by our results, the uninformative items in the battery dramatically reduce the power of the informative items in tracking early progression and designing RCTs at the early stage, partly due to the decreased rate of change and the inflated variance because of the contamination from the items not informative to early disease changes.

It is important to point out that the items that were not identified as informative (i.e., uninformative items) by our analyses do not automatically become invalid items from the cognitive battery. These items all have face validity and may be useful in tracking the disease progression at other stages. Even at the early disease stage, their composite score also shows some cognitive decline on subjects with MCI or ‘preclinical AD’ as presented in Table 3, albeit at a much lesser degree when compared to the informative items. Our results should therefore not be interpreted as against the use of these items in AD research. For example, the parent MAP study that provided the data performs annual clinical evaluations on participants until death; thus, items that are sensitive to change among persons with moderate to severe dementia also are needed.

Our study has many strengths. The participants were community-dwelling and examined with annual home visits. Thus, many of the biases inherent in getting persons to be evaluated in a clinic setting are reduced. The overall participation rate exceeded 90% over the entire length of follow-up, reducing bias that results from attrition. The large pool of items from 20 cognitive performance tests is among the largest item pools currently available in community-based prospective cohort studies. This allowed us to identify a large number of items of potential utility in RCTs on preclinical AD and MCI.

Our study also has limitations. The study cohort is selected, the generalizability of the findings needs to be established through independent studies, especially those with a population-based longitudinal design. Further, although it is the most cost effective to utilize an existing longitudinal cognitive database to select most informative items to track early disease progression, neuropsychological theory-based development of outcome measures on prospectively designed longitudinal studies is needed to fully establish the validity and psychometric properties of cognitive outcomes that can serve as the primary efficacy endpoint of future RCTs on early stage AD. Next, our analyses were based on the cognitive data already collected according to a well established protocol. 20 Therefore, the effect of order, presentation, and possibly interference 5051 of the cognitive testing on our findings can not be adequately addressed. In addition, our analytic approach was based on the technique of survival analysis, which implicitly assumed that everyone will develop MCI/AD if he or she lives long enough. Whereas this assumption may not be entirely unreasonable, its impact on the analysis results warrants further investigation. Finally, biomarker data would be needed to more accurately define 'preclinical AD', and our results need to be further validated when biomarker data are available.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank RUSH MAP Clinical Core for subject assessments. This study was supported by National Institute on Aging (NIA) grant R01 AG034119 for Chengjie Xiong. This study was also partly supported by the NIA grant P50 AG05681, P01 AG03991, P01AG26276, and U01 AG032438 for Chengjie Xiong and John Morris. The data set used in this manuscript can be requested to RUSH MAP and is available from the corresponding author after appropriate approval.

Appendix

Table 2A.

Informative items for designing RCTs on early AD (187 on 'Preclinical AD', and 62 on MCI) and their description

Test name List of items Ways items are administered Items for RCTs on MCI (Y=Yes)
Boston Naming Test bed Participants are shown pictures of certain objects. Then they are requested to name the objects. Y
camel Y
canoe Y
domino N
flower Y
funnel N
hammock Y
harmon Y
house Y
mask Y
tongs N
volcano N
whistle Y
Color Naming cname Number of colors read correctly in 30 secs. Y
Delayed Story Recall Injuries A three sentence story is read to the participants. Then they are requested to recall the story after a distractor-filled delay of approximately 3 minutes. Item names are key words of the story. N
Everyone N
Well N
Three N
Children Y
House N
Fire Y
Fireman N
Climb N
Children N
Rescured N
Minor N
Digit Ordering DigitOrder-41 A series of numbers are read aloud to the participants. One series at a time. After each series, participants are requested to repeat the series starting with the smallest number and going to the largest number. Y
DigitOrder-98 Y
DigitOrder-104 Y
DigitOrder-263 Y
DigitOrder-2413 N
DigitOrder-4216 N
DigitOrder-37570 N
DigitOrder-79210 N
Digits Backward DigitBack-38 A series of number sequences of increasing length are read out to the participants. Participants are requested to repeat the numbers backwards. Y
DigitBack-493 N
DigitBack-526 N
DigitBack-3814 N
DigitBack-1795 N
DigitBack-62972 N
DigitBack-48527 N
Digits Forward DigitFor-8396 A series of number sequences of increasing length are read out to the participants. Participants are requested to repeat the numbers forwards. Y
DigitFor-36925 N
DigitFor-69471 N
DigitFor-918427 N
DigitFor-635482 N
DigitFor-2814975 N
Immediate Story Recall Injuries A three sentence story is read to the participants. Then they are requested to recall the story immediately. Item names are key words of the story. N
Everyone N
Well N
Three Y
Children Y
House N
Fire Y
Fireman Y
Climb N
Children Y
Rescured Y
Minor N
Line Orientation line10a Each item requires the participants to estimate the angle subtended by two lines in a match-to-sample format. Y
line10b N
line11a Y
line11b N
line12a N
line12b N
line13a N
line13b N
line14a N
line14b N
line15b N
line1a N
line2a N
line2b N
line4a N
line4b N
line5a Y
line5b N
line6b N
line7b N
line8a N
line8b N
line9b N
Logical Memory IA A brief story is read to the participants. Then they are asked to retell it from memory immediately Y
Logical Memory IIA A brief story is read to the participants. Then they are asked to retell it from Y
MMSE30 apple Repeat the 1st word of 3 words read before-- apple Y
folds Fold a piece of paper in half Y
paper Put a piece of paper in right hand N
penny Repeat the 3rd word of 3 words read before-- penny Y
places Place a piece of paper on lap N
StreetName Name the street number of this place N
StreetNumber Name the street name of this place N
WORLD-backward Spell the word 'world' backward N
apple-recall Recall the first word of 3 words given previously --apple N
table-recall Recall the second word of 3 words given previously --table N
penny-recall Recall the third word of 3 words given previously --apple N
RepeatPhrase Repeat a phrase N
ReadWords Read the words shown on a card Y
WriteSentence Write any complete sentence Y
Year Name the current year Y
Copy Copy the drawing on a piece of paper N
Season Name the current season N
Day Name the day of the week Y
Month Name the current month Y
State Name the State of this place Y
Country Name this country Y
City Name the city of this place Y
Room Name the room of this place Y
table Repeat the 2nd word of 3 words read before-- table Y
Number Comparison Comparison-3 Participants are presented with 48 pairs of numbers. Some of the numbers are exactly the same while others do not match. The participants are asked to identify pairs as “same” or “different”. N
Comparison-4 N
Progressive Matrices Pattern-a11 Participants are shown a series of pattern and asked to identify the pattern below which would complete the pattern on top. N
Pattern-a2 Y
Pattern-a5 Y
Pattern-a6 Y
Pattern-a7 N
Pattern-a8 N
Pattern-b10 N
Pattern-b2 N
Pattern-b3 N
Pattern-b4 N
Pattern-b5 N
Pattern-b6 N
Pattern-b8 N
Reading Test Ache Participants are shown a series of words and asked to pronounce these words the best they can. Y
Placebo N
Façade N
Impugn N
Blatant N
Reify N
Topiary N
Naïve N
Recipe Y
Heir Y
Indict N
Debt Y
Sieve N
Corps N
Symbol Digit Modalities Symbol-1 Participants are shown a series of symbol. Each symbol corresponds to a number from 1 to 9. They are asked to call out the numbers that match the symbols shown to them one at a time. N
Symbol-2 N
Symbol-3 N
Symbol-4 N
Verbal Fluency animal Participants are asked to generate exemplars from the category in successive 1 minute trials Y
fruit Participants are asked to generate exemplars from the category in successive 1 minute trials. Y
Word List Memory wordt1_1-butter A 10-word list is presented, three times (total of 30 words), with three immediate recall trials and delayed tests of recall and recognition. N
wordt1_2-Arm N
wordt1_3-Shore N
wordt1_7-Pole N
wordt1_9-Grass N
wordt2_2-Cabin N
wordt2_3-Butter N
wordt2_4-Shore N
wordt2_5-Engine N
wordt2_6-Arm N
wordt2_7-Queen N
wordt2_8-Letter N
wordt2_9-Pole N
wordt3_1-Queen N
wordt3_2-Grass N
wordt3_3-Arm N
wordt3_4-Cabin N
wordt3_5-Pole N
wordt3_6-Shore N
wordt3_7-Butter N
wordt3_8-Engine N
wordt3_9-Ticket N
wordt3_x-Letter N
Word List Recall recall_1-Butter Participants are asked to read a list of ten words one at a time. Few minutes later they are asked to identify as many words as they can recall. N
recall_2-Arm N
recall_3-Shore N
recall_4-Letter N
recall_5-Queen N
recall_6-Cabin N
recall_7-Pole N
recall_8-Ticket N
recall_9-Grass N
recall_x-Engine N
Word List Recognition wordrec1-LETTER Participants are shown ten sets of four words, one set at a time, and asked to select the words from each set that (s)he was shown previously. Y
wordrec2-POLE Y
wordrec3-ENGINE Y
wordrec4-ARM Y
wordrec5-QUEEN Y
wordrec6-CABIN Y
wordrec7-TICKET Y
wordrec8-BUTTER Y
wordrec9-GRASS Y
wordrecx-SHORE Y
Word Corrected Read WordRead Words correctly read. Part of Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Y

References

  • 1.Bateman RJ, Xiong C, Benzinger T, et al. Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer’s disease. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2012;367(9):795–804. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1202753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer's & Dement. 2011;7:280–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Katzman R. Editorial: The prevalence and malignancy of Alzheimer disease. A major killer. Arch Neurol. 1976;33:217–218. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1976.00500040001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Price JL, McKeel DW, Buckles VD, Roe CM, Xiong C, Grundman M, Hansen LA, Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW, Smith CD, Davis DG, Schmitt FA, Markesbery WR, Kaye J, Kurlan R, Hulette C, Kurland BF, Higdon R, Kukull W, Morris JC. Neuropathology of nondemented aging: presumptive evidence for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:1026–1036. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Kelly JF, Aggarwal NT, Shah RC, Wilson RS. Neuropathology of older persons without cognitive impairment from two community-based studies. Neurology. 2006;66:837–1844. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219668.47116.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Morris JC, Price JL. Pathologic correlates of nondemented aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early stage Alzheimer's disease. J Mol Neurosci. 2001;17:101–118. doi: 10.1385/jmn:17:2:101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal L, Winblad B. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, Iwatsubo T, Jack CR, Jr, Kaye J, Montine TJ, Park DC, Reiman EM, Rowe CC, Siemers E, Stern Y, Yaffe K, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Morrison-Bogorad M, Wagster MV, Phelps CH. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):280–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mohs RC, Knopman D, Petersen RC, Ferris SH, Ernesto C, Grundman M, Sano M, Bieliauskas L, Geldmacher D, Clark C, Thal LJ. Development of cognitive instruments for use in clinical trials of antidementia drugs: additions to the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale that broaden its scope. The Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997;11 (Suppl 2):S13–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ringman JM, Grill J, Rodriguez-Agudelo Y, Chavez M, Xiong C. Prevention trials in persons at-risk for dominantly-inherited Alzheimer's disease: opportunities and challenges. Alzheimer's & Dementia. 2009;5:166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.DeKosky ST, Fitzpatrick A, Ives DG, Saxton J, Williamson J, Lopez OL, Burke G, Fried L, Kuller LH, Robbins J, Tracy R, Woolard N, Dunn L, Kronmal R, Nahin R, Furberg C GEMS Investigators. The Ginkgo Evaluation of Memory (GEM) study: design and baseline data of a randomized trial of Ginkgo biloba extract in prevention of dementia. Contemp Clin Trials. 2006;27:238–253. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2006.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Launer LJ. Prevention of AD: the which, when, and on whom? Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20 (3 Suppl 2):S75–78. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200607001-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hampel H, Frank R, Broich K, Teipel SJ, Katz RG, Hardy J, Herholz K, Bokde ALW, Jessen F, Hoessler YC, Sanhai WR, Zetterberg H, Woodcock J, Blennow K. Biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease: academic, industry and regulatory perspectives. Nature Reviews|Drug Discovery. 2010;9:560–574. doi: 10.1038/nrd3115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fagan A, Roe C, Xiong C, Mintun M, Morris JC, Holtzman D. Cerebrospinal fluid tau/ A 42 ratio as a prediction of cognitive decline in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:343–349. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.3.noc60123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shaw LM, Vanderstichele H, Knapik-Czajka M, Clark CM, Aisen PS, Petersen RC, Blennow K, Soares H, Simon A, Lewczuk P, Dean R, Siemers E, Potter W, Lee VM-Y, Trojanowski JQ the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarker signature in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative subjects. Ann Neurol. 2009;65:403–413. doi: 10.1002/ana.21610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mintun MA, LaRossa GN, Sheline YI, Dence CS, Lee SY, Mach RH, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, DeKosky ST, Morris JC. [11C] PIB in a nondemented population: Potential antecedent marker of Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2006;67:446–452. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000228230.26044.a4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Chen K, Reiman EM, Alexander GE, Bandy D, Renaut R, Crum WR, Fox NC, Rossor MN. An automated algorithm for the computation of brain volume change from sequential MRIs using an iterative principal component analysis and its evaluation for the assessment of whole-brain atrophy rates in patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage. 2004;22:134–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.http://www.fda.gov/downloads/Drugs/GuidanceComplianceRegulatoryInformation/Guidances/UCM338287.pdf
  • 20.Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Boyle PA, Wilson RS. Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012;9:646–663. doi: 10.2174/156720512801322663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25:634–642. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.634.14572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Krueger KR, Hoganson G, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Early and late life cognitive activity and cognitive systems in old age. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:400–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1989;39:1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised manual. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Albert MS, Smith LA, Scherr PA, et al. Use of brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Neurosci. 1991;57:167–178. doi: 10.3109/00207459109150691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bach J, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17:179–193. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Lea & Febiger; Philadelphia: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nelson HE. National Adult Reading Test (NART): test manual. Windsor, UK: NFER Nelson; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cooper JA, Sagar HJ. Incidental and intentional recall in Parkinson’s disease: an account based on diminished attentional resources. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1993;15:713–731. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Smith A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test manual–revised. Los Angeles: Western Psychological; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Dermen D. Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Trenerry MR, Crosson B, DeBoe J, Leber WR. Psychological Assessment Resources. Odessa, FL: 1989. The Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Benton AL, Varney N, et al. Visuospatial judgment: a clinical test. Arch Neurol. 1978;35:364–367. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J. Raven manual: Section 3. 1992. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press; 1992. Standard progressive matrices. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the mental state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bogaerts Kris, Lesaffre Emmanuel. Estimating local and global measures of association for bivariate interval censored data with a smooth estimate of the density. Statist Med. 2008;27:5941–5955. doi: 10.1002/sim.3374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bogaerts Kris, Lesaffre Emmanuel. Modeling the association of bivariate interval-censored data using the copula approach. Statist Med. 2008;27:6379–6392. doi: 10.1002/sim.3438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lawless JF. Statistical models and methods for lifetime data. Wiley; New York: 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Diggle PJ, Heagerty P, Liang K-Y, Zeger SL. Analysis of Longitudinal Data. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Donohue MC, Gamst AC, Thomas RG, Xu R, Beckett L, Petersen RC, Weiner MW, Aisen P the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. The relative efficiency of time-to-threshold and rate of change in longitudinal data. Contemp Clin Trials. 2011;32(5):685–693. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2011.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Xiong C, Zhu K, Yu K, Miller JP. Statistical Modeling in Biomedical Research: Longitudinal Data Analysis. In: Rao CR, Philip Miller J, Rao DC, editors. Handbook of Statistics: Epidemiology and Medical Statistics. Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.SAS Institute, Inc. User’s Guide. Cary, NC: 2013. SAS/STAT® 13.1. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Stern Y, Jacobs DM. Preliminary findings from the Predictors Study: utility of clinical signs for predicting disease course. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Disorders. 1995;9(1):S14–S18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Grober E, Buschke H, Crystal H, Bang S, Dresner R. Screening for dementia by memory testing. Neurology. 1988;38:900–903. doi: 10.1212/wnl.38.6.900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Backman L, Jones S, Berger AK, et al. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(4):520–531. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hall CB, Derby C, LeValley A, Katz MJ, Verghese J, Lipton RB. Education delays accelerated decline on a memory test in persons who develop dementia. Neurology. 2007;69:1657–1664. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000278163.82636.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hall CB, Lipton RB, Sliwinski M, Katz MJ, Derby CA, Verghese J. Cognitive activities delay onset of memory decline in persons who develop dementia. Neurology. 2009;73:356–361. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b04ae3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Twamley EW, Legendre Ropacki SA, Bondi MW. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(5):707–735. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rowe G, Hasher L, Turcotte J. Interference, aging, and visuospatial working memory: the role of similarity. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(6):804–7. doi: 10.1037/a0020244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Beglinger LJ, Gaydos B, Tangphao-Daniels O, Duff K, Kareken DA, Crawford J, Fastenau PS, Siemers ER. Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial neuropsychological testing. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2005;20:517–529. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES