Skip to main content
Brain logoLink to Brain
. 2014 Oct 28;137(12):3284–3299. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu293

Prominent effects and neural correlates of visual crowding in a neurodegenerative disease population

Keir X X Yong 1,, Timothy J Shakespeare 1, Dave Cash 1,2, Susie M D Henley 1,3, Jennifer M Nicholas 1,4, Gerard R Ridgway 5,6, Hannah L Golden 1, Elizabeth K Warrington 1, Amelia M Carton 1, Diego Kaski 7, Jonathan M Schott 1, Jason D Warren 1, Sebastian J Crutch 1
PMCID: PMC4240300  PMID: 25351740

Visual crowding is a perceptual phenomenon whereby recognition of a stimulus is disrupted by the presence of flanker stimuli. Yong et al. observe excessive crowding in individuals with a neurodegenerative condition (posterior cortical atrophy) and identify associations between prominent crowding and lower grey matter volume in the right collateral sulcus.

Keywords: crowding, lateral masking, Alzheimer’s disease, posterior cortical atrophy, acquired dyslexia

Abstract

Crowding is a breakdown in the ability to identify objects in clutter, and is a major constraint on object recognition. Crowding particularly impairs object perception in peripheral, amblyopic and possibly developing vision. Here we argue that crowding is also a critical factor limiting object perception in central vision of individuals with neurodegeneration of the occipital cortices. In the current study, individuals with posterior cortical atrophy (n = 26), typical Alzheimer’s disease (n = 17) and healthy control subjects (n = 14) completed centrally-presented tests of letter identification under six different flanking conditions (unflanked, and with letter, shape, number, same polarity and reverse polarity flankers) with two different target-flanker spacings (condensed, spaced). Patients with posterior cortical atrophy were significantly less accurate and slower to identify targets in the condensed than spaced condition even when the target letters were surrounded by flankers of a different category. Importantly, this spacing effect was observed for same, but not reverse, polarity flankers. The difference in accuracy between spaced and condensed stimuli was significantly associated with lower grey matter volume in the right collateral sulcus, in a region lying between the fusiform and lingual gyri. Detailed error analysis also revealed that similarity between the error response and the averaged target and flanker stimuli (but not individual target or flanker stimuli) was a significant predictor of error rate, more consistent with averaging than substitution accounts of crowding. Our findings suggest that crowding in posterior cortical atrophy can be regarded as a pre-attentive process that uses averaging to regularize the pathologically noisy representation of letter feature position in central vision. These results also help to clarify the cortical localization of feature integration components of crowding. More broadly, we suggest that posterior cortical atrophy provides a neurodegenerative disease model for exploring the basis of crowding. These data have significant implications for patients with, or who will go on to develop, dementia-related visual impairment, in whom acquired excessive crowding likely contributes to deficits in word, object, face and scene perception.

Introduction

Crowding is a form of inhibitory interaction that is present in spatial vision, involving the diminishing effect of nearby stimuli (‘flankers’) on the identification of a target stimulus (Levi, 2008). The occurrence of crowding when target stimuli and flankers are separately presented to different eyes indicates a cortical locus (Flom et al., 1963; Tripathy and Levi, 1994). Crowding is dependent on eccentricity (distance from fixation), which determines the critical spacing between target and flankers (Pelli et al., 2007). The critical spacing is the distance at which flankers inhibit identification of the target stimulus, and has been roughly localized as being half the eccentricity of the target from fixation in peripheral vision (Bouma, 1970). While crowding is independent of stimulus type, font and contrast (Tripathy and Cavanagh, 2002; Pelli et al., 2004; Pelli and Tillman, 2008), crowding effects diminish with target and flanker stimuli of opposite polarity (Kooi et al., 1994; Chakravarthi and Cavanagh, 2007) and are exacerbated by increasing visual similarity between target and flanker stimuli (Kooi et al., 1994; Bernard and Chung, 2011). Beyond being the crucial limiting factor in normal peripheral vision, crowding poses particular problems for certain patient populations, such as individuals with amblyopia, macular degeneration or apperceptive agnosia (Song et al., 2014).

Although some theories of crowding suggest that it may arise from poor resolution of attention (Intriligator and Cavanagh, 2001) or unfocussed higher-order spatial attention (Strasburger, 2005), crowding tends to be considered a pre-attentive process related to the interaction between simple visual features. Three main classes of theories have been proposed: the first, a classic lateral masking perspective, associates crowding with low level masking—a consequence of competition between a finite number of feature detectors (Townsend et al., 1971; Wolford and Chambers, 1984), which may occur at the level of the retina, lateral geniculate nucleus, or the primary visual cortex (V1) (Chakravarthi and Cavanagh, 2007). Second, substitution accounts propose that flanker features (or whole flankers) are mistakenly intermixed with the target; this substitution is most commonly attributed to noisy processing of the position of flanker and targets and/or their features (Wolford, 1975; Krumhansl and Thomas, 1977; Nandy and Tjan, 2007) which may arise as a consequence of larger receptive fields (Dayan and Solomon, 2010). Third, averaging accounts suggest that crowding arises as a problem of excessive feature integration: when cells responsible for pooling/averaging information over a large area encounter flankers, information about the flanker stimuli is assimilated with information about the target stimulus (Levi et al., 2002; Pelli et al., 2004; Greenwood et al., 2009, 2010).

Of these three classes of theory, the first (lateral masking) has been distinguished from the second and third (substitution and averaging) with the suggestion that crowding limits identification of target stimuli, whereas lateral masking limits both identification and detection (Parkes et al., 2001). Levi’s (2008) review of crowding suggests a growing consensus around a two-stage model of crowding encompassing both detection of simple features, possibly in V1, and integration of features downstream from V1. Support for this multi-stage view comes from a recent functional MRI study of the neural correlates of crowding in healthy individuals. Using a change-detection paradigm involving oriented Gabor patches, crowding was found to influence neural responses throughout cortical visual areas V1–V4, an effect that increased in strength from early to late visual areas (Anderson et al., 2012). These findings differ from earlier studies that did not identify an effect of crowding upon V1 responses (Arman et al., 2006; Fang et al., 2008; Bi et al., 2009) and argue against depictions of crowding as a later-stage process only arising following feature detection.

The overwhelming majority of studies of crowding have been conducted in normal peripheral or amblyopic vision. Crowding purportedly occurs in normal foveal vision but only over tiny distances (a few minutes of arc; Bouma, 1970) so most conventionally printed text (like the 1.2° letters used in the current study) would have to overlap to induce crowding, thus making it difficult to distinguish effects of crowding from ordinary masking (Martelli et al., 2005). However, two recent case reports have suggested that individuals with the neurodegenerative syndrome posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) exhibit letter identification deficits in central vision that may be consistent with acquired excessive crowding (Crutch and Warrington, 2007, 2009). PCA is characterized by progressive visual impairment and tissue loss in posterior brain regions; patients tend to have underlying Alzheimer’s disease pathology, but pathological changes are more concentrated in occipital, parietal and temporo-occipital rather than medial temporal regions relative to patients with typical amnestic Alzheimer’s disease (Hof et al., 1997). The two case reports assessing flanked letter identification abilities in PCA identified beneficial effects of increased spacing on flanked letter identification, interactions between letter spacing and letter confusability and an ameliorating effect of reverse polarity flankers; all of these effects are characteristic of crowding. However, there have been no group investigations of excessive crowding in PCA or any other neurodegenerative condition. The incidence of crowding in PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease remains unknown, as does the extent to which crowding may contribute to more recognized deficits of object and space perception in these conditions.

This study aimed to examine crowding in the context of neurodegenerative disease, addressing the question of why individuals with PCA show excessive crowding in central vision. First, we evaluated whether the characteristics of flanked letter identification performance in PCA are consistent with the properties of crowding, and estimated the prevalence of crowding deficits in PCA. Second, given the attenuating effects of increased spacing and reverse polarity on crowding in normal peripheral vision, we tested whether manipulation of these variables would lead to facilitation of letter identification in PCA. Third, we evaluated whether the letter identification errors observed in PCA were more consistent with averaging or substitution accounts of crowding, by testing how well averaged or additive models of targets and/or flankers predicted error identity and frequency. Finally, we established the structural neural correlates of crowding in PCA, and considered their correspondence to previous functional imaging studies of crowding in healthy individuals.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study participants were 26 patients with PCA, 17 patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease and 14 healthy control subjects. The patients with PCA all fulfilled clinical criteria for a diagnosis of posterior cortical atrophy (Mendez et al., 2002; Tang-Wai et al., 2004; McMonagle et al., 2006). Patients with PCA and patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease fulfilled research criteria for probable Alzheimer’s disease (Dubois et al., 2007, 2010). Molecular pathology (18 F amyloid imaging performed as part of another investigation or CSF referred as part of their diagnostic work-up) was available for 7/26 patients with PCA and 11/17 patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease (Table 1); results for all typical Alzheimer’s disease patients and 6/7 patients with PCA were consistent or borderline consistent with Alzheimer’s disease pathology (positive amyloid scan on standard visual rating or CSF amyloid-β1-42 ≤ 450 pg/ml and/or tau/amyloid-β ratio >1). The healthy control subjects were matched to the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups on age and years of education, with the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease participants additionally matched for disease duration and Mini Mental State Examination score (MMSE; Table 2).

Table 1.

Molecular pathology data for patients with PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease

Diagnosis Amyloid 18 F imaging CSF total tau pg/ml CSF amyoid-β1-42 pg/ml CSF tau:amyloid-β ratio Interpretation
PCA Not available 310 488 0.64
PCA Not available 931 625 1.49 +
PCA positive 1072 126 8.51 ++
PCA Not available 151 147 1.03 +
PCA positive Not available Not available Not available ++
PCA positive 1082 365 2.96 ++
PCA positive Not available Not available Not available ++
tAD Not available 289 280 1.03 +
tAD Not available 757 285 2.66 ++
tAD Not available 940 348 2.70 ++
tAD Not available 952 195 4.88 ++
tAD Not available 977 322 3.03 ++
tAD Not available 625 277 2.26 ++
tAD Not available >1200 313 >3.83 ++
tAD Not available 913 191 4.78 ++
tAD Not available >1200 217 >5.52 ++
tAD Not available 1099 195 5.64 ++
tAD Not available 850 362 2.35 ++

tAD = typical Alzheimer’s disease.

Where results do not support Alzheimer’s disease pathology (−), are borderline consistent with Alzheimer’s disease pathology (+) and are >85% specific for Alzheimer’s disease pathology (++). Amyloid (florbetapir) PET scans were received as part of another investigation.

Table 2.

Demographic information and neuropsychological scores of patients with PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease

Demographic information PCA (n = 26) tAD (n = 7) Control (n = 14)

Gender (male:female) 10/16 12/5 5/9
Age (mean years ± SD) 61.4 ± 7.7 65.0 ± 5.1 62.7 ± 5.0
Education level (mean years ± SD) 14.6 ± 2.3 14.9 ± 2.4 16.1 ± 2.4
Disease duration (mean years ± SD) 4.7 ± 3.1 5.0 ± 1.7
MMSEa (mean/30 ± SD) 17.7 ± 5.0 17.5 ± 4.9

Neuropsychology test Max score Raw score
Norms/comment
PCA tAD Difference P-value
Background Neuropsychology
Short Recognition Memory Testb for words* (joint auditory/visual presentation) 25 19.5 ± 3.7 14.7 ± 1.5 <0.0001 PCA: 5th–10th percentile, tAD: ∼<5th percentile (cut-off: 19)
Short Recognition Memory Test for faces* 25 17.8 ± 4.0 16.8 ± 3.0 >0.3 Both ∼<5th percentile (cut-off: 18)
Concrete Synonyms testc 25 20.0 ± 3.7 20.9 ± 2.5 >0.4 Both 10th–25th percentile
Naming (verbal description) 20 11.4 ± 6.6 13.7 ± 6.4 >0.2 Both ∼<5th percentile (cut-off: 15)
Cognitive estimatesd (error score) 30 14.6 ± 7.5 10.6 ± 5.0 0.074 Both ∼<1st percentile (cut-off: 9)
Calculation (GDAe)* 24 1.6 ± 2.9 4.9 ± 5.3 <0.05 PCA: ∼<5th percentile, tAD:5th-25th percentile
Spelling (GDSTf- Set B, first 20 items)* 20 8.9 ± 6.5 10.8 ± 5.6 >0.3 Both 10th–25th percentile
Gesture production testg 15 12.7 ± 3.4 14.1 ± 1.4 >0.1
Digit span (forwards) 12 6.0 ± 2.6 6.1 ± 1.4 >0.8 Both 25th–50th percentile
Max forwards 8 5.6 ± 1.8 5.5 ± 0.8 >0.9
Digit span (backwards) 12 2.6 ± 1.7 3.6 ± 1.9 0.078 Both 5th–10th percentile
Max backwards 7 2.3 ± 1.3 3.3 ± 1.1 <0.05
CORVISTh reading test 16 13.8 ± 3.0 15.7 ± 0.8 <0.05
Spatial attentioni:
    Small words 48 39.9 ± 10.7 47.2 ± 2.2 <0.0005
    Large words 48 37.8 ± 12.5 47.1 ± 1.6 <0.0005
Visual assessment
Early visual processing
Visual acuity (CORVIST): Snellen 6/9 (median 6/9) (median 6/9) Normal acuity
Figure-ground discrimination (VOSPj) 20 16.3 ± 3.0 18.6 ± 1.3 <0.01 PCA: ∼<5th percentile, tAD: 5th–10th percentile
Shape discriminationk 20 12.6 ± 3.9 17.2 ± 3.2 <0.0005 Healthy controls do not make any errors
Hue discrimination (CORVIST) 4 2.6 ± 1.1 3.0 ± 1.3 >0.3
Visuoperceptual processing
Object Decision (VOSP)* 20 10.0 ± 4.1 15.9 ± 2.4 <0.0001 PCA: ∼<5th percentile, tAD: 10th–25th percentile
Fragmented letters (VOSP) 20 2.9 ± 3.9 13.5 ± 6.6 <0.0001 Both ∼<5th percentile (cut-off: 16)
Unusual and usual views:l
    Unusual 20 6.6 ± 6.8 9.9 ± 5.1 >0.1 Both ∼<1st percentile (cut-off: 12)
    Usual 20 8.4 ± 5.5 16.5 ± 4.0 <0.0001 Both ∼<1st percentile (cut-off: 18)
Visuospatial processing
Number location (VOSP)* 10 1.8 ± 2.5 5.7 ± 3.8 <0.005 Both ∼<5th percentile (cut-off: 6)
Dot counting (VOSP) 10 3.4 ± 3.2 8.1 ± 3.1 <0.0001 Both ∼<5th percentile
A Cancellationm: Completion time 90 s 79.5 s ± 17.4 36.3 s ± 15.7 <0.0001 Both ∼<5th percentile (cut-off: 32 s)
A Cancellationm: Number of letters missed 19 6.6 ± 5.1 0.53 ± 1.1 <0.0005

*Behavioural screening tests supportive of PCA diagnosis; tAD = typical Alzheimer’s disease.

eGraded Difficulty Arithmetic test (GDA) (Jackson and Warrington, 1986).

fGraded Difficulty Spelling Test (GDST) (Baxter and Warrington, 1994).

gCrutch (unpublished).

hCortical Visual Screening Test (CORVIST) (James et al., 2001).

iPerceptual corpus (Yong et al., 2014).

jVisual Object and Space Perception Battery (VOSP) (Warrington and James, 1991).

kEfron (1968): oblong edge ratio 1:1.20.

Background neuropsychology and visual assessment

Patients with PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease were administered a battery of neuropsychological tests assessing memory, language, arithmetic, spelling, reading and early visual, visuoperceptual and visuospatial processing (Table 2).

Crowding assessment

All participants were requested to name target stimuli (uppercase letters excluding I, J, O, Q, W and X) under the following conditions (example stimuli are shown in Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Target/flanker arrays used in Tasks 2–6 under different spacing conditions.

Task 1: Unflanked letter identification

The target stimuli (n = 20) were alphabetic items presented in isolation. Letters were presented in random order for 6000 ms in a fixation box (3.2° in width, 2.9° in height) at the centre of the screen.

In each of the following tasks (Tasks 2–6), target letter identification was probed under two spatial conditions, condensed and spaced.

Task 2: Letter flankers

Target letters (n = 24) were flanked on each side by a letter, forming a 3-letter non-word combination.

Task 3: Shape flankers

Target letters (n = 24) were flanked on each side by a triangle presented at different orientations. Triangles were of equal height and line thickness to target letters.

Task 4: Number flankers

Target letters (n = 24) were flanked on each side by an Arabic numeral, chosen from a range between 2 and 9.

Task 5: Same-polarity flankers

Target letters (n = 24) were flanked on each side by black letters; presentation was as Task 2 except that items were presented on a grey background to match Task 6 (see below).

Task 6: Reverse-polarity flankers

Target black letters (n = 24) were flanked on each side by white letters, all presented on a grey background.

The edge-to-edge distance between the target letter and flankers was 0.1° of visual angle in the condensed condition and 1.0° in the spaced condition at a viewing distance of 50 cm; the height of stimuli (10.5 mm) corresponded to a visual angle of 1.20°. Participants were given one prompt for each trial where they named the flanker (‘Is that the letter in the middle?’); this prompt was intended to limit errors resulting from visual disorientation. The same combination of flankers was used for each target letter under both spatial conditions within each flanker condition. Alphabetic items occurred with equal frequency within each task. The stimuli were presented in blocks of six items, with blocks being administered in an ABBA design. All flanked stimuli were presented in the centre of the screen within a fixation box (6.4° in width, 2.9° in height). All 26 patients with PCA completed Tasks 1, 2 and 4; 24 completed Task 3; and 22 completed Tasks 5 and 6. Naming latencies were manually determined from the onset of each letter using Audacity (http://audacity.sourceforge.net).

Data analysis

Behavioural covariates

Tests of early visual, visuoperceptual and visuospatial processing (Table 2) were transformed and averaged to form composite scores for each visual domain. Raw scores were transformed into a standardized range (0–100) in which 0 and 100 corresponded to the minimum and maximum score achieved by any patient (PCA or typical Alzheimer’s disease), respectively. The following raw scores were also transformed into a standardized range for the PCA versus typical Alzheimer’s disease regression analysis: unflanked letter identification, digit span (backwards), A cancellation time, a measure of peripheral spatial attention (discrepancy in accuracy for a subset of words selected from the perceptual corpus (Yong et al., 2014) at two levels of size (large: n = 48; letter height 2°; small: n = 48; letter height 0.5°) words were matched for frequency, age of acquisition and concreteness and were administered in the same testing session as the crowding assessment. MMSE and disease duration were also used as behavioural covariates.

Crowding indices for voxel-based morphometry analysis

Scores on shape and number flankers (Tasks 3 and 4) were used as crowding indices, based on the rationale that errors with non-letter flankers were less likely to reflect attentional or executive deficits. Indices were based on raw score differences:

  1. Spacing (shape): difference in accuracy between spacing conditions (spaced-condensed) in Task 3 (shape flankers);

  2. Spacing (number): difference in accuracy between spacing conditions (spaced-condensed) in Task 4 (number flankers);

  3. Spacing (shapes/numbers): difference in accuracy between spacing conditions (spaced-condensed) in Tasks 3 and 4 combined;

  4. Polarity: difference in accuracy between Tasks 5 and 6 (reverse-same polarity);

  5. Polarity (condensed): difference in accuracy between Tasks 5 and 6 (condensed condition only).

Data trimming

Latency data for erroneous responses, responses where participants had become overtly distracted from the task, or responses following prompts were removed from the analysis [total n = 1397 (21.6%)]. Latency data greater than two standard deviations from the mean of each participant were considered outliers and removed [total n = 442 (8.73%)]. Prior to latency regression analysis, latency data were transformed using inverse transformation due to non-normal distribution of residuals. Naming latency data were only analysed for participants who made no errors or did not make enough errors to produce significant effects of spacing or flanker type on naming speed using logistic regression or chi-squared tests at the individual level (Crowding assessment 1: PCA: n = 12, MMSE = 18.8, disease duration = 3.9 years; Crowding assessment 2: PCA: n = 9, MMSE = 20.0, disease duration = 3.0 years; all typical Alzheimer’s disease and control participants).

Statistical analysis

Analysis of accuracy and latency data was conducted using logistic and linear models, respectively; both models used robust standard errors to account for clustering by participant. Spacing, flanker type, the interaction between spacing and flanker, and their interactions with participant group were the variables of interest, while the linear model also included accuracy as a covariate. Between patient group (PCA versus typical Alzheimer’s disease) regression analyses used the same logistic and linear models but also included diagnosis and one of the behavioural covariates listed above. Differences between PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups were calculated using a Wilcoxon rank-sum test and differences within groups were calculated using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Three models were used to determine whether similarity between target and/or flanker stimuli and error responses predicted error rates (Fig. 2):

  1. Model 1 (target/flanker averaging): similarity values generated from averaged target and flanker stimuli;

  2. Model 2 (individual flankers): similarity values independently generated from each flanker; both were included in the model; and

  3. Model 3 (individual flankers and target): identical to Model 2 except that similarity values independently generated from the target were also included.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Three models assessing whether the identity of error responses could be predicted on the basis of the similarity between that error response and the averaged (Model 1) or individual (Models 2 and 3) overlap with flanker and/or target stimuli. The current example involves the stimulus ‘TGX’ yielding an error response of ‘Y’. Values in red refer to the visual similarity of the error response (higher values represent greater similarity) generated from the overlap in pixels with each item (averaged target/flanker, or individual target/flankers). Values in black refer to responses which have similarity values closest to the error response.

Similarity values were derived from the overlap in pixels between target and/or flanker stimuli and error responses. All models were logistic mixed-effects models with random-effects of participant and error response and fixed effects of the similarity values for each of the three models detailed above.

Neuroimaging data

T1-weighted volumetric magnetic resonance images were acquired on a Siemens Trio TIM 3T scanner for 20 patients with PCA (see Supplementary material for details on image acquisition). Voxel-based morphometry was performed using SPM8 software (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Version 8; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) running on MATLAB R2012a (http://www.mathworks.com). Images were rigidly orientated to standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using the ‘New segment’ function in SPM8. Rigidly-orientated scans were segmented into grey matter, white matter and CSF. The Dartel toolbox (Ashburner, 2007) was used to perform spatial normalization, first aligning grey matter and white matter segmentations to their group-wise average (Ashburner and Friston, 2009), then combining this transformation with an affine mapping to MNI space. Normalized segmentations were modulated to preserve native-space tissue volumes and smoothed with a 6 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. A group-wise custom template in MNI space was created by arithmetically averaging the Dartel-normalized bias-corrected MP-RAGE images of all 20 PCA participants. Associations between regional grey matter volume and indices of crowding were assessed using voxel-wise linear regression models in SPM8. Total intracranial volume, age, gender and MMSE score were included as covariates. A whole-brain grey matter mask was defined to include voxels for which the intensity was >0.1 in at least 80% of the images; this has been shown to be appropriate for participants with greater atrophy (Ridgway et al., 2009). A region of interest mask was defined for the occipital lobe; this was created using the Hammers atlas, which was warped to the custom template. Associations between grey matter volume and behavioural performance were assessed over the whole brain and within the occipital region of interest specified by previous anatomical hypotheses. A voxel-wise statistical threshold of P < 0.05, family-wise error corrected for multiple comparisons was applied in all analyses. Statistical parametric maps were overlaid on the custom template.

Results

Background neuropsychology and visual assessment

Mean scores for the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups and an estimate of their performance relative to normative data sets appropriate for the mean age of each group are shown in Table 2. On tasks without a visual component, the performance of the PCA group was mostly equivalent to (Concrete Synonyms, Naming, Digit Span forwards) or better than (Short Recognition Memory Test: words) that of the typical Alzheimer’s disease group; however, performance on measures of reading, visual processing and some non-visual tasks associated with parietal function (Calculation, Digit Span backwards) was worse than in the typical Alzheimer’s disease group.

Visual status

Of the 26 patients with PCA, a measure of visual acuity (Cortical Vision Screening Test) administered to both eyes identified normal visual acuity (Snellen) in 18 patients (69.2%) had a visual acuity of 6/9–6/12 (Snellen chart); four patients (15.4%) demonstrated a visual acuity of 6/18, one patient demonstrated a visual acuity of 6/24 and one patient demonstrated a visual acuity of 6/36. This acuity measure could not be effectively administered to the remaining two patients with PCA due to profound visuospatial impairment. All patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease demonstrated normal corrected visual acuity on this measure.

Eleven patients with PCA were assessed by ophthalmologist or neuro-ophthalmologist; thorough examination found no evidence of an ophthalmologic condition and/or attributed any loss of vision to a disorder of the visual cortex in eight patients. One patient had cataracts but demonstrated normal visual acuity (6/6 in each eye) and results were not available for the remaining two patients.

Crowding assessment 1: flanker and spacing effects

The mean and standard deviation percentage accuracy and naming latency results for each group on Tasks 1–6, plus group comparisons, are shown in Table 3.

Table 3.

Comparisons between PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease group accuracy and latency data

Task N Naming accuracy (%)
Group comparisons (p values)
Groups
PCA tAD Controls PCA vs tAD PCA vs controls tAD vs controls
1. Unflanked letter identification 20 99.8 ± 0.2 100 ± 0 100 ± 0 p > .4 p > .4
2. Letter flankers 24 75.8 ± 25.1 99.3 ± 1.6 100 ± 0 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .1
3. Shape flankers 24 83.5 ± 18.6 99.7 ± 1.0 100 ± 0 p < .0005 p < .0005 p > .3
4. Number flankers 24 83.6 ± 23.5 99.7 ± 1.0 100 ± 0 p < .0005 p < .0005 p > .3
5. Same polarity letter flankers 24 78.8 ± 22.5 98.5 ± 2.9 100 ± 0 p < .001 p < .0005 p = .065
6. Reverse polarity letter flankers 24 86.5 ± 15.6 99.3 ± 2.2 100 ± 0 p < .001 p < .0005 p > .2
Summary data
Total (Tasks 2–4) 72 81.3 ± 19.7 99.6 ± 1.1 100 ± 0 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .1
Total condensed (Tasks 2–4) 36 72.0 ± 26.7 99.7 ± 0.9 100 ± 0 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .2
Total spaced (Tasks 2–4) 36 90.0 ± 16.0 99.5 ± 1.5 100 ± 0 p < .001 p < .001 p > .2
Total (Tasks 5–6) 48 82.7 ± 18.3 98.9 ± 2.2 100 ± 0 p < .005 p < .0005 p < .05
Task N Naming latency (ms)
Group comparisons (p values)
Groups
PCA tAD Controls PCA vs tAD PCA vs controls tAD vs controls
1. Unflanked letter identification 20 785 ± 315 583 ± 95 497 ± 53 p = .056 p < .005 p < .05
2. Letter flankers 24 2726 ± 3196 634 ± 128 570 ± 113 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .2
3. Shape flankers 24 1631 ± 867 546 ± 86 538 ± 95 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .9
4. Number flankers 24 2072 ± 1842 548 ± 92 469 ± 119 p < .0005 p < .0005 p < .05
5. Same polarity letter flankers 24 1836 ± 1167 609 ± 128 518 ± 124 p < .001 p < .0005 p = .063
6. Reverse polarity letter flankers 24 2133 ± 1635 591 ± 91 506 ± 131 p < .005 p < .001 p < .05
Summary data
Total (Tasks 2–4) 72 2054 ± 1694 573 ± 94 524 ± 100 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .2
Total condensed (Tasks 2–4) 36 2510 ± 2481 591 ± 98 537 ± 100 p < .0001 p < .0001 p > .1
Total spaced (Tasks 2–4) 36 1659 ± 1073 555 ± 95 515 ± 101 p < .0005 p < .0005 p > .3
Total (Tasks 5–6) 48 2004 ± 140 597 ± 104 512 ± 126 p < .001 p < .0005 p = .090

Task 1: Unflanked letter identification

There was no significant difference in unflanked letter identification accuracy between the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease or control groups. One PCA patient made one error, while the typical Alzheimer’s disease and control groups did not make any errors. There was a trend towards the PCA group having longer naming latencies than the typical Alzheimer’s disease group. Both PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups were slower than the control group.

Tasks 2–4: Letter, shape and number flankers

A summary of the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease accuracy and naming latency data is shown in Fig. 3. The patients with PCA were consistently worse than both typical Alzheimer’s disease patients and controls in terms of both naming accuracy and latency on Tasks 2–4. The accuracy of patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease was not significantly different from that of control subjects on any task, but they were slower on Task 4.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Accuracy and naming latency data for the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease (tAD) group for letter, shape and number flankers in both spatial conditions. Rts = reaction time; Con = condensed; Spa = spaced. Error bars show standard deviation.

Tasks 2–4: Effects of spacing

Patients with PCA (n = 26) showed significantly poorer accuracy for target letter identification in the condensed than spaced condition (z = 7.81, P < 0.001). Analysis of latency data (n = 10) also identified longer naming latencies in the condensed condition (t = 3.33, P < 0.01). At the individual patient level, 18/26 (69.2%) showed a spacing effect in accuracy and/or latency: all but three of those (15/26: 57.7%) showed this spacing effect even when analysis was restricted to non-letter flankers. Similar proportions of patients with PCA with no clinically identifiable ophthalmological impairment showed a spacing effect in accuracy and/or latency (6/8: 75.0%) and a spacing effect when analysis was restricted to non-letter flankers (4/8: 50.0%). Of the patients with PCA with impaired visual acuities (visual acuity <6/12), 3/6 (50.0%) showed a spacing effect on their accuracy and/or latency, with 1/6 (16.7%) showing a spacing effect when analysis was restricted to non-letter flankers. Patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease did not demonstrate an effect of spacing (P > 0.5) on accuracy but latencies were significantly longer in the condensed condition (t = 4.73, P < 0.001). Controls made no errors, but did show longer latencies in the condensed condition (t = 2.89, P < 0.05). Analysis of combined accuracy data from the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups revealed a significant interaction between diagnosis and spacing (z = −2.77, P < 0.01), with patients with PCA showing a greater spacing effect; no such interaction was found for naming latencies (P > 0.1). Analysis of combined latency data for the typical Alzheimer’s disease and control groups found no evidence of an interaction between diagnosis and spacing (P > 0.4).

Tasks 2–4: Effects of flanker category

Patients with PCA showed significantly poorer accuracy for letters relative to other flanker categories (versus shapes: z = 2.68, P < 0.01; versus numbers: z = 2.61, P < 0.01). However, this between-category difference only held in the spaced condition (versus shapes: z = 3.65, P < 0.001; versus numbers: z = 3.55, P < 0.001); in the condensed condition, there was no significant difference between letters and other flankers (versus shapes/numbers: P > 0.1). In contrast, both typical Alzheimer’s disease and control groups showed longer latencies for letter flankers in both spaced and condensed conditions. Patients with PCA showed significantly slower naming latencies for letters relative to other flanker categories (versus shapes: t = 3.74, P < 0.005; versus numbers: t = 2.60, P < 0.05); this between-category difference was consistent across spacing conditions. Patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease did not show poorer accuracy for letters (versus shapes/numbers: P > 0.1) but did show longer latencies (versus shapes: t = 6.13, P < 0.001; versus numbers: t = 5.13, P < 0.001). Controls made no errors, but showed longer latencies for letter flankers (versus shapes: t = 2.59, P < 0.05; versus numbers: t = 3.55, P < 0.005). Analysis of combined accuracy or latency data from the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups did not find significant interactions between diagnosis and flanker type. Analysis of combined latency data for the typical Alzheimer’s disease and control groups revealed a significant interaction between diagnosis and flanker type, with patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease slower in the letter relative to the shape (t = 2.38, P < 0.05) but not the number conditions (t = −1.90, P =0 .067).

Post hoc analysis of covariates

None of the behavioural covariates [early visual, visuoperceptual and visuospatial processing, unflanked letter identification, Digit Span (backwards), A Cancellation time, a measure of peripheral spatial attention, MMSE or disease duration] could account for the spacing effect within the PCA or typical Alzheimer’s disease groups. None of the covariates could account for the overall group difference in naming accuracy between the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups. Similarly, none of the covariates could account for group differences when considering the condensed condition alone, however, visuoperceptual (z = 2.13, P < 0.05; PCA versus typical Alzheimer’s disease: P > 0.2) and visuospatial (z = 3.60, P < 0.001; PCA versus typical Alzheimer’s disease: P > 0.1) function did account for the group difference in the spaced condition. Linear regression analysis found that none of the covariates could account for the group difference between the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease groups for naming latency overall or in either spacing condition.

Post hoc eyetracking analysis

Additional post hoc eyetracking data were gathered on three individuals with PCA (one of whom took part in the original crowding assessments) to confirm that patients were fixating centrally when making errors naming flanked target stimuli. These three patients demonstrated spacing effects on their naming accuracy and/or latency across flanker conditions. A head-mounted Eyelink II system was used to record gaze location at 250 Hz, with fixations and saccades parsed at standard velocity and acceleration thresholds (30°/s and 8000°/s2). Fixation data suggest that erroneous responses were made when participants were using central vision (Fig. 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

(A) Eyetracking data for trials where three patients with PCA made errors naming flanked letter stimuli across different conditions of spacing/flanker (Tasks 2–4). Heat maps for Participants 1 and 2 show total maximum fixation duration within an area averaged across trials; individual fixation duration is shown for Participant 3. (B) Accuracy and naming latency data for letter, shape and number flankers in both spatial conditions. Rts = reaction time; Con = condensed; Spa = spaced.

Crowding assessment 2: Polarity effects

Tasks 5 and 6: Same and reverse-polarity flankers

A summary of the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease accuracy and naming latency data is shown in Fig. 5. The patients with PCA were consistently worse than both patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease and control subjects in terms of both naming accuracy and latency on Tasks 5–6. Differences in accuracy and latency between the typical Alzheimer’s disease and control groups did not reach formal levels of significance except for patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease being slower on Task 6.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Accuracy and naming latency data for the PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease (tAD) group for same and reverse polarity flankers in both spatial conditions. Rts = reaction time; Con = condensed; Spa = spaced; RP =reverse polarity ; SP = same polarity. Error bars show standard deviation.

Tasks 5 and 6: Effects of polarity

Patients with PCA (n = 22) showed significantly poorer accuracy for target letter identification with same rather than reverse-polarity flankers (z = −3.07, P < 0.005). This polarity effect only occurred for condensed flankers (condensed: z = 4.82, P < 0.001; spaced: P > 0.8). Analysis of latency data (n = 10) found a significant effect of spacing (t = 2.66, P < 0.05) but not polarity (P > 0.9) on naming speed. In the typical Alzheimer’s disease group, there was no significant effect of polarity on naming speed (P > 0.8) or accuracy (P > 0.1); however, there was an interaction between spacing and polarity (t = −2.63, P < 0.05), with condensed flankers of same polarity having longer naming latencies. Although none of the control subjects made any errors, there was a trend towards longer naming latencies with same polarity flankers (t = −2.18, P = 0.050), although there was no interaction between spacing and polarity (P > 0.1).

Error analysis

The error responses in the PCA group (overall error rate: 17.7%) fell into three categories: (i) Type A (no response) in 29.9% of errors, the target remaining unidentified, which could result from participants being unable to either detect or identify the target; (ii) Type B (flanker identification), 36.3% error responses resulted from the participant providing the name of a flanker rather than the target (e.g. ZNH → Z); and (iii) Type C (letter not present in target/flanker array), in 33.8% error responses, the participant named a letter which was neither the target nor a flanker. The majority of these errors were suggestive of perceptual integration of flanker and target stimuli (YMT → V, 3T6 → C). This was despite accurate unflanked letter identification (overall error rate: 0.2%).

Averaging versus feature substitution

An error analysis was carried out in order to assess the extent to which averaging of target and flanker features (Pelli et al., 2004; Greenwood et al., 2009) or substitution of flanker features (Wolford, 1975) predicted the errors made by participants. Three models were constructed (Fig. 2), and applied to Type B and C errors (see above; total n = 358): (i) Model 1 (target/flanker averaging): similarity between the error response and the averaged target and flanker stimuli was a significant predictor of error rate (z = 13.40, P < 0.001); (ii) Model 2 (individual flankers): similarity between the error response and the individual flanker stimuli did not significantly predict error rate (right flanker: z = 1.68, P = 0.093; left flanker: P > 0.4; both: P > 0.5); and (iii) Model 3 (individual flankers and target): similarity between the error response and the target or individual flanker stimuli did not predict error rate (target: P > 0.4; right flanker: P > 0.1; left flanker: P > 0.4; both P > 0.7).

Neuroimaging findings

T-contrast whole brain effect maps showing neuroanatomical associations between performance on Tasks 3–4 and grey matter volume in the PCA group are displayed in Fig. 6. No significant associations between indices of crowding [crowding (shapes), crowding (numbers), crowding (shapes/numbers)] and grey matter volume were found when correcting for multiple comparisons over whole brain volume. When restricting analysis to the pre-specified occipital region, a significant negative correlation was found between crowding (shapes/numbers) and grey matter volume in the right collateral sulcus, between the fusiform and lingual gyri after correcting for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05): a more pronounced crowding effect for letters surrounded by shapes and numbers was associated with reduced grey matter volume in this region (Fig. 7). The location of the maximal association within this region lies at the boundary of the pre-specified occipital region; this, along with associations shown in the whole brain effect maps, suggest the critical region is not confined to the occipital lobe but extends into the temporal cortices. In Tasks 5–6, there were no significant associations between the discrepancy in accuracy between flankers of opposite polarity [polarity, polarity (condensed)] and grey matter over whole brain volume or within the pre-specified occipital region.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

T-contrast effect maps showing associations between a measure of crowding [spacing (shapes/numbers)] and grey matter volume displayed on axial sections. Warmer colours indicate stronger positive associations between a greater degree of crowding and lower grey matter volume, with cooler colours representing the reverse contrast. The colour-map indicates t-values for this association.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Statistical parametric map of grey matter volume associated with a measure of crowding [spacing (shapes/numbers)]. The SPM is displayed on axial (A), coronal (B) and sagittal (C) sections of the custom template in MNI space: the right hemisphere is shown on the right in coronal and axial sections. When restricting analysis to a prespecified region of interest (outlined in blue), there was an association between a greater degree of crowding and lower grey matter volume in the collateral sulcus (FWE corrected: P < 0.05; peak t = 6.61, location: x = 30 y = −58 z = −8): the colour-map indicates t-values for this association.

Discussion

The study presents the first systematic investigation of crowding in a neurodegenerative disease population. In a series of centrally presented flanked letter identification tasks, the quality of patients with PCA performance was consistent with crowding in healthy or amblyopic individuals. Spacing, not flanker type, was the primary determinant of accuracy, and this spacing effect was significantly ameliorated when target letters were surrounded by reverse-polarity flankers. The PCA group were considerably slower and less accurate than the typical Alzheimer’s disease and the healthy control group in flanked letter identification tasks (Tasks 2–4) despite similar accuracy (and speed, relative to patients with typical Alzheimer’s disease) in naming letters presented in isolation (Task 1). This detrimental effect of closely-positioned flankers upon letter identification ability was associated with significant reductions in grey matter volume in the right collateral sulcus.

Does the observed behaviour constitute (excessive) crowding?

Before considering the implications of this neurodegenerative perspective on crowding, it is first necessary to rule out various alternative explanations for the observed pattern of performance. First, the crowding effect in PCA could not be attributable to a generic ‘dementia effect’ as individuals with typical Alzheimer’s disease showed no difference compared to healthy controls in their accuracy on any of the flanked letter identification tasks, and estimates of disease severity could not account for the magnitude of the spacing effect. Second, attentional dyslexia is an unlikely candidate explanation as it involves deficits in the recognition of multiple, concurrently presented stimuli of the same category, for example letters presented with other letters (Warrington et al., 1993; Humphreys and Mayall, 2001). In Tasks 2–4, spacing, not flanker type, consistently determined naming accuracy. While there was an interaction between spacing and flanker type, this may be a consequence of perceptual similarity, and hence crowding, as opposed to the category-specific deficit previously linked with attentional dyslexia. Third, it is doubtful that patients with PCA’s pattern of performance can be attributed to a deficit in peripheral spatial attention, one that might underlie a restriction in the effective visual field (Russell et al., 2004, 2013). This deficit becomes apparent through the diminished recognition of large rather than small stimuli (Crutch, 2013; Yong et al., 2014); however, the discrepancy in reading accuracy for large and small words did not account for the current findings. Fourth, similarly it is unlikely that simultanagnosia (a concept which relates closely to the notion of ‘restriction in the effective visual field’) can account for the observed phenomena, as simultanagnosia would be predicted to yield equally poor performance with reverse as same polarity flankers (given in both conditions the target and flankers are clearly perceptible and well within contrast acuity thresholds). Fifth, despite the grave visuospatial deficits often observed in PCA, there is little evidence for these findings being wholly accounted for by visuospatial impairment. Performance on tasks involving unflanked letter identification, early visual, visuoperceptual or visuospatial processing, digit span backwards or the A Cancellation task did not account for the spacing effect within the PCA group or the overall difference in accuracy between the PCA and typical Alzheimer's disease groups in Tasks 2–4. However, measures of visuospatial and visuoperceptual ability did account for the group difference in the spaced, but not condensed, condition. This suggests that poor accuracy in the spaced condition may largely reflect visuospatial and visuoperceptual impairment, with poor accuracy in the condensed condition primarily arising from crowding. Sixth, the normal ophthalmological assessments suggest that the pattern of performance on flanked letter tasks cannot be accounted for by poor acuity, macular or retinal abnormalities. Finally, seventh, the post hoc evaluation of fixation patterns during trials on which crowding errors were made indicate that at least these participants were using central vision, hence demonstrating that performance does not merely reflect normal peripheral crowding.

Theoretical implications

The types of errors made by the patients with PCA have implications for accounts of crowding. Error responses were classified into three categories: no response (Type A), naming of a flanker (Type B) and responses not present in the target/flanker array (Type C). Different accounts of crowding would predict a differential weighting of errors towards these three types. Classic lateral masking (Polat and Sagi, 1993) would primarily predict no response (Type A) errors due to flankers preventing detection of the target stimuli. By contrast, substitution (Wolford, 1975; Krumhansl and Thomas, 1977) and integration/averaging accounts (Parkes et al., 2001; Pelli et al., 2004; Greenwood et al., 2009) would primarily predict naming of a flanker (Type B) or a letter that combines features of the flanker(s) and target (Type C). Patients with PCA generated approximately equal numbers of Type A, B and C errors. This variation in error type is consistent with both a multi-stage view of crowding (crowding encompasses both detection and integration of features; Levi, 2008), and the relative heterogeneity in patterns of atrophy across individuals with PCA (variation in the pathological burden upon different cortical visual areas). All three types of errors are also observed in flanked item identification tasks in healthy peripheral vision (Strasburger, 2005).

Furthermore, in the current study we attempted not only to establish the frequency of error types on our central flanked letter identification task, but also to distinguish between averaging and substitution accounts. This was achieved by testing the visual similarity between individual error responses (Type B and C) and (i) the averaged image of the flankers and target (to test the averaging account); or (ii) the images of the individual flankers and target items (to test the substitution account). Analysis revealed that error identity was only significantly predicted by the averaged flanker/target images. These findings are most compatible with compulsory averaging accounts of crowding (Greenwood et al., 2009) and suggest that PCA patient responses in our flanked letter identification task are at least partially accounted for by excessive pooling of target and flanker information. The finding of an association between error response and the averaged target/flanker image does not rule out a role for the substitution of flanker features or whole flankers, but does suggest such substitution accounts alone cannot explain the pattern of errors made within the PCA group.

In healthy vision, crowding has been interpreted as a pre-attentive process that uses averaging to regularize the noisy representation of (feature) position in the periphery (Greenwood et al., 2009). Our data suggest that in patients with neurodegeneration of the occipital cortices, at least one component of the acquired excessive crowding observed can also be regarded as a pre-attentive process that uses averaging to regularize the pathologically noisy representation of feature information in central vision.

Neural correlates of crowding

Relatively little is known about the neural correlates of crowding. Although the visual cortex has been implicated in most theories of crowding, a divergence of opinion occurs with more specific localization, ranging from V1 (Blake et al., 2006), V2 (Chung et al., 2007) and V4 (Motter, 2006; Liu et al., 2009). A two-stage model of crowding including both feature detection and integration might involve V1 (Levi, 2008) and the extrastriate cortex (Robertson, 2003), respectively. Anderson et al. (2012) recently identified how crowded stimuli provoked increased functional MRI activation from areas V1–V4, also suggesting that crowding may be a multistage process.

Our imaging data suggest that, within the occipital region, scores indicative of prominent crowding effects were associated with lower grey matter volume within the right collateral sulcus, between the fusiform and lingual gyri. The maximal association within this region lay at the boundary of the occipital region, extending into temporal cortices. Without retinotopic mapping, it is difficult to be confident of the exact correspondence between anatomical location and visual area. However, in previous studies, similar regions have been classed as area V4 (Sereno et al., 1995; DeYoe et al., 1996; Hadjikhani et al., 1998; Gallant et al., 2000), V3 (Yeatman et al., 2013) and V3a (Grill-Spector and Malach, 2004). Interestingly, V4 fulfils a variety of criteria that make it a promising locus for crowding. V4 is an area in which information from different stimulus types, orientations and spatial frequencies converge (Logothetis and Charles, 1990; Ferrera et al., 1992, 1994; David et al., 2006), receptive field size and anisotropy in V4 are similar in orientation and size to the radial/tangential anisotropy of crowding (Toet and Levi, 1992; Pinon et al., 1998) and estimates of V4 receptive field size overlap with the extent of crowding in peripheral vision (Smith et al., 2001; Chung et al., 2007). Bias competition, in which patterns within receptive fields compete to determine the firing rate of individual neurons, has been localized in areas V4 and higher (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Reynolds et al., 1999; Chelazzi et al., 2001); this may underlie crowding as a possible consequence of competitive feature integration processing (Nandy and Tjan, 2007). Anderson et al. (2012) cite how there is a significant increase in population-based receptive field size from V1 to V4 (Smith et al. 2001; Amano et al., 2009), and suggest that crowding effects might accumulate from pooling of target and flanker stimuli over receptive fields of increasing size. Thus there appears to be a convergence between the current localization of the spacing effect in individuals with acquired excessive crowding and an array of normal human and animal data concerning neural correlates of feature averaging and integration.

The clear effect of polarity observed in these results (Tasks 5 and 6) might suggest which regions are implicated in at least one stage of crowding. Reverse polarity has been shown to segregate information via on and off pathways at the level of bipolar cells in the outer retina, which continue to stay relatively distinct until reaching the early visual cortex (Schiller, 1992). Although there is evidence of interaction between the two pathways (Wassle and Boycott, 1991; Harris and Parker, 1995), this segregation of information between target stimuli and flankers of reverse polarity may account for the observed alleviation of the crowding effect in our patients. Regarding the neural correlates of on and off pathway integration, Zhou et al. (2000) found that 48% of V1 and 20% V2 and V4 neurons in macaques encoded local contrast polarity, while the majority of neurons in V2 and V4 encoded direction-specific contrast polarity edges. Motoyoshi and Kingdom (2007) proposed a two stream model of second order processing, with the first stream composed of complex V1 cells sensitive to orientation and the second composed of lateral geniculate nucleus or V1 blob cells sensitive to polarity. However, similar temporal limits of the polarity advantage and attention (Chakravarthi and Cavanagh, 2007) contest the notion of this effect being an exclusively low-level process. Measures of this reverse polarity effect were not significantly associated with grey matter volume in our current data set, but this may reflect a simple power issue and replication with a larger patient cohort may enable us to discriminate between low- and high-level theories of the polarity effect.

Establishing a model of acquired excessive crowding in individuals with neurodegenerative disease may offer an opportunity to test mechanistic accounts of crowding through future pathological studies. For example, integration and averaging models of crowding (Parkes et al., 2001; Greenwood et al., 2009) propose that, while isolated contours are processed by simple cells, a high concentration of flanking contours in a small region or ‘integration field’ (Pelli et al., 2004) leads to a greater response of complex cells which then suppress simple cell activity within their receptive field area. Applying this hypothesis to individuals with PCA, it is possible that simple cell activity in areas such as the primary visual cortex may be less disrupted by Alzheimer’s disease pathology than complex cells in more downstream visual areas, such as V4. Cells in V4 which suppress simple cell activity through connections with areas earlier in the visual system may be particularly susceptible to Alzheimer’s disease pathology: this would result in a diminished ability to suppress signals of high contour concentrations within an integration field. This proposal is at least consistent with previous neuroimaging (greater atrophy of lateral than medial occipital lobes; Lehmann et al., 2011; Ridgway et al., 2012) and histopathological reports in PCA (increasing density of neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques from areas V1 to visual association areas: Hof et al., 1997). Pathological studies have also pointed to the selective vulnerability of certain neurons to Alzheimer’s disease, particularly cells with long axonal projections (Lewis et al., 1987; Morrison et al., 1987) such as V1 Meynert cells in PCA (Hof et al., 1990), and extensive disruption of feedforward and feedback projections by neurofibrillary tangles and senile plaques in Brodmann areas 17 to 19 (von Gunten et al., 2006).

Clinical implications, challenges and future directions

Both quantitative (spacing and polarity effects) and qualitative (error types) aspects of flanked letter identification performance, and the cortical localization of the spacing effect, indicate that individuals with PCA commonly develop excessive crowding in central vision. Just as crowding in the normal periphery operates irrespective of object type (e.g. faces: Martelli et al., 2005; scenes: To et al., 2011; see Pelli, 2008), so might acquired excessive crowding in the central vision of individuals with PCA and other neurodegenerative conditions have an impact upon a host of important everyday visually-guided behaviours and functions such as reading, face recognition or navigation. Studying crowding in neurodegenerative conditions such as PCA is not without its challenges; for example, the patients’ broader spatial cognition impairment has implications for stimulus selection and response modality (e.g. multi-feature letter stimuli were used in the current task as they can be verbally labelled, whereas Gabor patches or cross stimuli require manually or verbally-mediated spatial judgements about angle or spatial position). Nonetheless, complementing studies of the occurrence of crowding in structurally stable human models with studies of the emergence of crowding in degenerative models like PCA offers particular opportunities. In a neurodegenerative disease model of crowding, developments in high-resolution quantitative MRI which provide an in vivo window on cortical microstructure at the level of cortical layers, ocular dominance columns and stripes (Yacoub et al., 2008; Sereno et al., 2013), and amyloid plaques in Alzheimer’s disease (Meadowcroft et al., 2009) can be exploited to improve understanding of the role of different cell populations in crowding. Examining the evolution of crowding in individuals with degenerating visual cortices (Yong et al., submitted) will permit a novel, dynamic perspective upon the phenomenon and its interaction with basic and higher-order visual processes.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank those who participated in this study for their patience and goodwill. We would like to thank Alexander Leff and Sam Schwarzkopf for their perspectives on the localization of occipital lobe anatomy. We would also like to thank Jonathan Bartlett for assisting with queries relating to statistical methods and analysis.

Glossary

Abbreviations

MMSE

Mini Mental State Examination

PCA

posterior cortical atrophy

Funding

This work was undertaken at UCLH/UCL which received a proportion of funding from the Department of Health’s NIHR Biomedical Research Centres funding scheme. The Dementia Research Centre is an Alzheimer’s Research UK Co-ordinating Centre. This work was supported by an Alzheimer’s Research UK Senior Research Fellowship and ESRC/NIHR grant (ES/K006711/1) to S.C. J.D.W. is supported by a Wellcome Trust Senior Clinical Fellowship (Grant No 091673/Z/10/Z). G.R.R. is supported by the Medical Research Council (grant number MR/J014257/1). The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (grant number 091593/Z/10/Z). This work was supported by the NIHR Queen Square Dementia Biomedical Research Unit. The amyloid PET study mentioned was supported by funding from AVID Radiopharamaceuticals (a wholly owned subsidiary of Eli Lilly) and the The National Brain Appeal – Frontotemporal Dementia Research Fund’.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain online.

References

  1. Arman AC, Chung ST, Tjan BS. Neural correlates of letter crowding in the periphery. J Vis. 2006;6:804. [Google Scholar]
  2. Amano K, Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO. Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. J Neurophysiol. 2009;102:2704–18. doi: 10.1152/jn.00102.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson EJ, Dakin SC, Schwarzkopf DS, Rees G, Greenwood JA. The neural correlates of crowding-induced changes in appearance. Curr Biol. 2012;22:1199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Computing average shaped tissue probability templates. Neuroimage. 2009;45:333–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baxter DM, Warrington EK. Measuring dysgraphia: a graded-difficulty spelling test. Behav Neurol. 1994;7:107–16. doi: 10.3233/BEN-1994-73-401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bernard JB, Chung STL. The dependence of crowding on flanker complexity and target-flanker similarity. J Vis. 2011;11:pii 1. doi: 10.1167/11.8.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bi TY, Cai P, Zhou TG, Fang F. The effect of crowding on orientation-selective adaptation in human early visual cortex. J Vis. 2009;9:13.1–10. doi: 10.1167/9.11.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blake R, Tadin D, Sobel KV, Raissian TA, Chong SC. Strength of early visual adaptation depends on visual awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:4783–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509634103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bouma H. Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature. 1970;226:177–8. doi: 10.1038/226177a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chakravarthi R, Cavanagh P. Temporal properties of the polarity advantage effect in crowding. J Vis. 2007;7:11.1–3. doi: 10.1167/7.2.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R. Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:761–72. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.8.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chung STL, Li RW, Levi DM. Crowding between first- and second-order letter stimuli in normal foveal and peripheral vision. J Vis. 2007;7:10.1–13. doi: 10.1167/7.2.10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crutch SJ. Seeing why they cannot see: understanding the syndrome and causes of posterior cortical atrophy. Journal of Neuropsychology. 2013 doi: 10.1111/jnp.12011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crutch SJ, Warrington EK. Foveal crowding in posterior cortical atrophy: a specific early-visual-processing deficit affecting word reading. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2007;24:843–66. doi: 10.1080/02643290701754240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Crutch SJ, Warrington EK. The relationship between visual crowding and letter confusability: towards an understanding of dyslexia in posterior cortical atrophy. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26:471–98. doi: 10.1080/02643290903465819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. David SV, Hayden BY, Gallant JL. Spectral receptive field properties explain shape selectivity in area V4. J Neurophysiol. 2006;96:3492–505. doi: 10.1152/jn.00575.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dayan P, Solomon JA. Selective Bayes Attentional load and crowding. Vis Res. 2010;50:2248–60. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeYoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, et al. Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:2382–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Cummings JL, DeKosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, et al. Revising the definition of Alzheimer's disease: a new lexicon. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:1118–27. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70223-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Dekosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, Cummings J, et al. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer"s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:734–46. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Efron R. In: Proceedings of the Boston Colloquium for the Philosophy of Science 1966/1968. Netherlands: Springer; 1968. What is perception? pp. 137–73. [Google Scholar]
  24. Fang F, He S. Crowding alters the spatial distribution of attention modulation in human primary visual cortex. J Vis. 2008;8:6.1–9. doi: 10.1167/8.9.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ferrera VP, Nealey TA, Maunsell JH. Mixed parvocellular and magnocellular geniculate signals in visual area V4. Nature. 1992;358:756–61. doi: 10.1038/358756a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ferrera VP, Nealey TA, Maunsell JH. Responses in macaque visual area V4 following inactivation of the parvocellular and magnocellular LGN pathways. J Neurosci. 1994;14:2080–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-04-02080.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fiset D, Arguin M, Bub D, Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ. How to make the word-length effect disappear in letter-by-letter dyslexia —implications for an account of the disorder. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:535–41. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01571.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Flom MC, Heath GG, Takahashi E. Contour interaction and visual resolution: contralateral effects. Science. 1963;142:979–80. doi: 10.1126/science.142.3594.979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatry Res. 1975;12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gallant JL, Shoup RE, Mazer JA. A human extrastriate area functionally homologous to macaque V4. Neuron. 2000;27:227–35. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00032-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Greenwood JA, Bex PJ, Dakin SC. Positional averaging explains crowding with letter-like stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:13130–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901352106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Greenwood JA, Bex PJ, Dakin SC. Crowding changes appearance. Curr Biol. 2010;20:496–501. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Grill-Spector K, Malach R. The human visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:649–77. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hadjikhani N, Liu AK, Dale AM, Cavanagh P, Tootell RBH. Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:235–41. doi: 10.1038/681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Harris JM, Parker AJ. Independent neural mechanisms for bright and dark information in binocular stereopsis. Nature. 1995;374:808–11. doi: 10.1038/374808a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hof PR, Bouras C, Constantinidis J, Morrison JH. Selective disconnection of specific visual association pathways in cases of alzheimers-disease presenting with balints syndrome. J Neuropathol Exp Neur. 1990;49:168–84. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199003000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hof PR, Vogt BA, Bouras C, Morrison JH. Atypical form of Alzheimer's disease with prominent posterior cortical atrophy: a review of lesion distribution and circuit disconnection in cortical visual pathways. Vis Res. 1997;37:3609–25. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00240-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Humphreys GW, Mayall K. A peripheral reading deficit under conditions of diffuse visual attention. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2001;18:551–76. doi: 10.1080/02643290042000242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Intriligator J, Cavanagh P. The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cogn Psychol. 2001;43:171–216. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jackson M, Warrington EK. Arithmetic skills in patients with unilateral cerebral-lesions. Cortex. 1986;22:611–20. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(86)80020-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. James M, Plant GT, Warrington EK. Thames Valley Test Company, Bury St Edmonds, UK; 2001. CORVIST: Cortical vision screening test: manual & test materials. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kooi FL, Toet A, Tripathy SP, Levi DM. The effect of similarity and duration on spatial interaction in peripheral-vision. Spatial Vis. 1994;8:255–79. doi: 10.1163/156856894x00350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Krumhansl CL, Thomas EAC. Effect of level of confusability on reporting letters from briefly presented visual-displays. Percept Psychophys. 1977;21:269–79. [Google Scholar]
  44. Lehmann M, Crutch SJ, Ridgway GR, Ridha BH, Barnes J, Warrington EK, et al. Cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in posterior cortical atrophy and typical Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32:1466–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Levi DM. Crowding—an essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vis Res. 2008;48:635–54. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Levi DM, Hariharan S, Klein SA. Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in amblyopic vision. Vis Res. 2002;42:1379–94. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00061-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Levi DM, Song SA, Pelli DG. Amblyopic reading is crowded. J Vis. 2007;7:21.7–17. doi: 10.1167/7.2.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lewis DA, Campbell MJ, Terry RD, Morrison JH. Laminar and regional distributions of neurofibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in Alzheimer's disease: a quantitative study of visual and auditory cortices. J Neurosci. 1987;7:1799–808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-06-01799.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Liu TT, Jiang Y, Sun XH, He S. Reduction of the crowding effect in spatially adjacent but cortically remote visual stimuli. Curr Biol. 2009;19:127–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Logothetis NK, Charles ER. The minimum motion technique applied to determine isoluminance in psychophysical experiments with monkeys. Vis Res. 1990;30:829–38. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90052-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Martelli M, Majaj NJ, Pelli DG. Are faces processed like words? A diagnostic test for recognition by parts. J Vis. 2005;5:58–70. doi: 10.1167/5.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. McMonagle P, Deering F, Berliner Y, Kertesz A. The cognitive profile of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology. 2006;66:331–8. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000196477.78548.db. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Meadowcroft MD, Connor JR, Smith MB, Yang QX. MRI and histological analysis of beta-amyloid plaques in both human Alzheimer's disease and APP/PS1 transgenic mice. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2009;29:997–1007. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mendez MF, Ghajarania M, Perryman KM. Posterior cortical atrophy: Clinical characteristics and differences compared to Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn. 2002;14:33–40. doi: 10.1159/000058331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Morrison JH, Lewis DA, Campbell MJ, Huntley GW, Benson DL, Bouras C. A monoclonal antibody to non-phosphorylated neurofilament protein marks the vulnerable cortical neurons in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res. 1987;416:331–6. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90914-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Motoyoshi I, Kingdom FA. Differential roles of contrast polarity reveal two streams of second-order visual processing. Vis Res. 2007;47:2047–54. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Motter BC. Modulation of transient and sustained response components of V4 neurons by temporal crowding in flashed stimulus sequences. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9683–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5495-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nandy AS, Tjan BS. The nature of letter crowding as revealed by first- and second-order classification images. J Vis. 2007;7:5.1–26. doi: 10.1167/7.2.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Parkes L, Lund J, Angelucci A, Solomon JA, Morgan M. Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:739–44. doi: 10.1038/89532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pelli DG. Crowding: a cortical constraint on object recognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:445–51. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pelli DG, Palomares M, Majaj NJ. Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: distinguishing feature integration from detection. J Vis. 2004;4:1136–69. doi: 10.1167/4.12.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pelli DG, Tillman KA. The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:1129–35. doi: 10.1038/nn.2187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pelli DG, Tillman KA, Freeman J, Su M, Berger TD, Majaj NJ. Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. J Vis. 2007;7:20.1–36. doi: 10.1167/7.2.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pinon MC, Gattass R, Sousa AP. Area V4 in Cebus monkey: extent and visuotopic organization. Cereb Cortex. 1998;8:685–701. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.8.685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Polat U, Sagi D. Lateral Interactions between spatial channels—suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments. Vis Res. 1993;33:993–9. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L, Desimone R. Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J Neurosci. 1999;19:1736–53. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01736.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ridgway GR, Lehmann M, Barnes J, Rohrer JD, Warren JD, Crutch SJ, Fox NC. Early-onset Alzheimer disease clinical variants multivariate analyses of cortical thickness. Neurology. 2012;79:80–4. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31825dce28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ridgway GR, Omar R, Ourselin S, Hill DLG, Warren JD, Fox NC. Issues with threshold masking in voxel-based morphometry of atrophied brains. Neuroimage. 2009;44:99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Robertson LC. Binding, spatial attention and perceptual awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:93–102. doi: 10.1038/nrn1030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Russell C, Malhotra P, Deidda C, Husain M. Dynamic attentional modulation of vision across space and time after right hemisphere stroke and in ageing. Cortex. 2013;499:1874–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Russell C, Malhotra P, Husain M. Attention modulates the visual field in healthy observers and parietal patients. Neuroreport. 2004;15:2189–93. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200410050-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Schiller PH. The ON and OFF channels of the visual system. Trends Neurosci. 1992;15:86–92. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90017-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, et al. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic-resonance-imaging. Science. 1995;268:889–93. doi: 10.1126/science.7754376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sereno MI, Lutti A, Weiskopf N, Dick F. Mapping the human cortical surface by combining quantitative T-1 with retinotopy. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:2261–8. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Shallice T, Evans ME. The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex. 1978;14:294–303. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(78)80055-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Smith AT, Singh KD, Williams AL, Greenlee MW. Estimating receptive field size from fMRI data in human striate and extrastriate visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:1182–90. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.12.1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Song S, Levi DM, Pelli DG. A double dissociation of the acuity and crowding limits to letter identification, and the promise of improved visual screening. J Vis. 2014;14:3. doi: 10.1167/14.5.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Strasburger H. Unfocussed spatial attention underlies the crowding effect in indirect form vision. J Vis. 2005;5:1024–37. doi: 10.1167/5.11.8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Tang-Wai DF, Graff-Radford NR, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, Parisi JE, Crook R, et al. Clinical, genetic, and neuropathologic characteristics of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology. 2004;63:1168–74. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000140289.18472.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. To MPS, Gilchrist ID, Troscianko T, Tolhurst DJ. Discrimination of natural scenes in central and peripheral vision. Vis Res. 2011;51:1686–98. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Toet A, Levi DM. The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vis Res. 1992;32:1349–57. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90227-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Townsend JT, Taylor SG, Brown DR. Lateral masking for letters with unlimited viewing time. Percept Psychophys. 1971;10:375–378. [Google Scholar]
  83. Tripathy SP, Cavanagh P. The extent of crowding in peripheral vision does not scale with target size. Vis Res. 2002;42:2357–69. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(02)00197-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Tripathy SP, Levi DM. Long-range dichoptic interactions in the human visual cortex in the region corresponding to the blind spot. Vis Res. 1994;34:1127–38. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90295-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. von Gunten A, Bouras C, Kovari E, Giannakopoulos P, Hof PR. Neural substrates of cognitive and behavioral deficits in atypical Alzheimer's disease. Brain Res Rev. 2006;51:176–211. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Warrington EK. Vol. 1. Hove: Psychology Press; 1996. The Camden memory tests: manual. [Google Scholar]
  87. Warrington EK, Cipolotti L, McNeil J. Attentional dyslexia: a single case study. Neuropsychologia. 1993;31:871–85. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90145-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Warrington EK, James M. Visual apperceptive agnosia—a clinico-anatomical study of 3 cases. Cortex. 1988;24:13–32. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(88)80014-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Warrington EK, James M. Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Company, Bury St. Edmunds; 1991. VOSP: the visual object and space perception battery. [Google Scholar]
  90. Warrington EK, McKenna P, Orpwood L. Single word comprehension: a concrete and abstract word synonym test. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1998;8:143–54. [Google Scholar]
  91. Wassle H, Boycott BB. Functional architecture of the mammalian retina. Physiol Rev. 1991;71:447–80. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1991.71.2.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Willison JR, Warrington EK. Cognitive retardation in a patient with preservation of psychomotor speed. Behav Neurol. 1992;5:113–6. doi: 10.3233/BEN-1992-5207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Wolford G. Perturbation model for letter identification. Psychol Rev. 1975;82:184–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Wolford G, Chambers L. Contour interaction as a function of retinal eccentricity. Percept Psychophys. 1984;36:457–60. doi: 10.3758/bf03207498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Yacoub E, Harel N, Ugurbil K. High-field fMRI unveils orientation columns in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:10607–12. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804110105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Yeatman JD, Rauschecker AM, Wandell BA. Anatomy of the visual word form area: Adjacent cortical circuits and long-range white matter connections. Brain Lang. 2013;125:146–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Yong KX, Shakespeare TJ, Cash D, Henley S, Warren JD, Crutch SJ. (Con) text-specific effects of visual dysfunction on reading in posterior cortical atrophy. Cortex. 2014;57:92–106. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Zhou H, Friedman HS, von der Heydt R. Coding of border ownership in monkey visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6594–611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06594.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

Articles from Brain are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES