Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 May 15.
Published in final edited form as: Lang Cogn Neurosci. 2014 May 15;29(10):1325–1341. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.917193

Effect of sound similarity and word position on lexical selection

Megan Reilly 1, Sheila E Blumstein 1
PMCID: PMC4243184  NIHMSID: NIHMS591450  PMID: 25436217

Abstract

Spoken word production research has shown that phonological information influences lexical selection. It remains unclear, however, whether this phonological information is specified for its phonological environment (e.g., word position) or its phonetic (allophonic) realization. To examine this, two definition naming experiments were performed during which subjects produced lexical targets (e.g., “balcony”) in response to the targets’ definitions (“deck higher than a building’s first floor”) after naming a series of phonologically related or unrelated primes. Subjects produced target responses significantly more often when the primes were phonologically related to the target, regardless of whether the phonologically related primes matched the target’s word position or did not. For example, subjects were equally primed to produce the target “balcony” after the prime “ballast” or “unbalanced” relative to unrelated primes. Moreover, equal priming occurred irrespective of phonological environment or phonetic realization. The results support models of spoken word production which include context-independent phonological representations.

Keywords: spoken word production, phonological encoding, lexical access, phonological representations

Introduction

Models of spoken word production agree on the existence of at least two stages of processing (Levelt, 1989): a high-level lexical-semantic stage at which lexical items are retrieved from semantic memory and mapped onto an abstract lexical representation and a low-level phonological stage at which the sounds of a word are prepared for articulation. Psycholinguistics research has debated about the representations at each substage of spoken word production, as well as the extent to which information flows between adjacent stages (Dell, 1986; Rapp & Goldrick, 2000). The current research seeks to examine the representation of abstract phonological units and their ability to independently impact lexical-semantic processing. In particular, this study will investigate the extent to which information at the phonological stage impacts processing at the lexical stage, and whether these phonological representations are position-sensitive and specified for acoustic realization in a given environment.

Current models of spoken word production implement two substages during which phonological information about a word is available. First, abstract information about the phonemes becomes available during access to the word form; second, during preparation for articulation, this information is specified for each phoneme’s relative position and phonetic realization. Evidence for this division between abstract and phonetically-specified representations is reported in both patient and covert speech research.

Buchwald and Miozzo (2011) report the repetition shadowing patterns of a patient with aphasia who showed a frequent word-initial s-deletion deficit. In English, stop consonants which follow /s/ word-initially are unaspirated, whereas word-initial voiceless stops are aspirated (Klatt, 1975). Buchwald & Miozzo’s patient produced stops which followed these rules even after an s-deletion as if the preceding /s/ were never present; that is, he produced utterances with aspirated initial stop consonants (e.g., “phil” following s-deletion of intended “spill”). The authors argue that since the word position of the stop consonant was specified after the deletion, the original target could not have been specified for phonetic position. Other studies have identified similar cases with specific deficits for abstract phoneme representations (Béland, Caplan & Nespoulous, 1990; Goldrick & Rapp, 2007; Ash et al., 2011).

Studies which use covert speech (or “inner speech”), during which subjects retrieve a lexical form but must suppress articulation, also suggest a division between the lexical-phonological word form and the fully specified articulatory output. During a covert speech task, subjects necessarily activate the abstract phonological forms but do not need to complete acoustic specification. Oppenheim and Dell (2008) presented subjects with four-word lists and analyzed self-reported errors when subjects were asked to rehearse the lists either overtly or covertly. Both covert and overt speech showed a lexical effect, such that subjects reported errors that were much more likely to be words than nonwords (“reef leach”→“leaf reach” was more common than “heath leech” → “leath heach”). This lexical effect, which is robust among speech errors (Dell & Reich, 1980), reflects activation of a lexical-phonological representation during both overt and covert speech. However, only the overt speech showed a phonemic similarity effect: there was a tendency for more phonetically similar phonemes to be exchanged (“reef” → “leaf”, with only one altered phonetic feature, was more likely that “reef” → “beef”). Covert speech showed no phonemic similarity effect, suggesting that the activated representations for the covert speech were not sensitive to the phonetic properties of each phoneme. An alternative explanation is that overt speech limits the opportunity for self-repair of errors through an internal monitoring mechanism (Postma, 2000), but Oppenheim & Dell concluded that covert speech is “impoverished” in that it is unspecified for phonetic detail.

Although the existence of separate stages of phonological processing is widely accepted, it is less clear how these two substages of phonological processing differentially influence access to the lexical word form. If phonological information influences lexical selection, heightened activation of a word’s phonemes will increase the likelihood that the word will be produced. For example, a speaker who is trying to think of a word for the deck outside his second-story hotel room might activate the words “balcony” and “veranda”, and the semantic representations of each word will compete for selection based on their relevance, frequency, and the recency with which the speaker has used them, among other factors. If the speaker has recently used the word “ballast”, the first three phonemes of “balcony” may still be residually activated. If these phonemes share residual activation with the lexical entry for “balcony”, the activation of “balcony” during lexical selection will be boosted relative to that of “veranda”.

There is evidence from both cloze sentence completion and tip-of-the-tongue states that phonological information has some influence on lexical-semantic processes, but these studies do not distinguish between the abstract and phonetically specific levels of phonological processing. Rapp and Samuel (2002) asked subjects to fill in the final words of open-ended sentences. The rate with which subjects used a given word was modulated by whether a rhyme for that word appeared earlier in the sentence. Subjects were more likely to choose a word if a phonologically similar word had already been activated (e.g., “The man walked into the bank and slipped on some ice. He’d gone to deposit his payment/check and nearly broke his ________.”; target, “neck”). Ferreira and Griffin (2003) used a similar paradigm and found that subjects were more likely to produce a word if either a semantic competitor or a homophone of a semantic competitor was present in the preceding sentence (e.g., “nun” and “none” both increased the likelihood of a “priest” response). These findings indicate that lexical selection of a target (“neck”, “nun”) is influenced by the activation of words with shared phonology (“check”, “none”).

Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) research has found similar effects. This line of investigation attempts to characterize the phenomenon in which speakers have accessed some aspects of a word’s form, e.g., first phoneme, metrical structure, or number of syllables, but fail to fully encode and produce the intended word. The TOT effect was first induced experimentally by Brown and McNeill (1966), who presented subjects with the definitions of infrequent words and asked them to report whether they knew the words being defined or if they were “stuck” in a TOT state. Later studies have shown that exposure to phonologically related primes before presentation of the target definitions increased the number of target responses and decreased the number of TOT states reported (James & Burke, 2000; Meyer & Bock, 1992). It also decreased the number of non-target responses (Perfect & Hanley, 1992), suggesting that the locus of the phonological priming effect is lexical access rather than phonological encoding. If the effect of priming was simply to facilitate encoding of the sounds of the target word, the identity of the response would not be affected.

The studies described above provide ample evidence that phonological activation influences lexical access. However, these studies used phonological primes which matched the target in word position and phonetic realization. It has not been shown whether the phonological representations which show this influence are specified for the position and phonetic information which is not available until the latest stage of production. If the sound units which influence lexical selection are specified for word position and phonetic realization, then activated phonemes should fail to show any influence on word selection unless they match the target word’s position and/or phonetic context. For example, the /b/ in “unbalanced” is in a different context (/n_a/ or C_V) than the /b/ in “balcony” (/#_a/or #_V). Likewise, voiceless stops are produced differently in English when they begin a stressed syllable (aspirated) then when they do not (unaspirated). If the /p/ representation in “apron” is specified as “unaspirated” or “word-medial”, then it should fail to prime the aspirated initial /p/ in “principal”. On the other hand, if these representations are abstract, then any /p/ realization should prime all other /p/’s.

Although the spoken word production literature has not addressed this question, the auditory word recognition literature suggests that at least some aspects of word retrieval are influenced by phonological priming independent of a phoneme’s word position or realization. Subjects are able to identify words whose onsets are embedded in the middle of other words or nonwords (Norris, McQueen, & Cutler, 2002). Luce and Cluff (1998) found that subjects access the semantics of embedded words; they made facilitated lexical decisions for target words which were semantically related to an embedded word in the prime (e.g., “key” primed by “hemlock”). Van Alphen and van Berkum (2010) also found that the N400, an ERP measure of violated semantic expectations, was modulated by whether a stimulus sentence contained a semantically relevant word embedded in an otherwise semantically incoherent word. That is, subjects showed semantic processing of the word “pain” when they heard the word “champagne” in a sentence about an injury.

It is unknown whether abstract phonological representations influence lexical access in production. To this end, the current research investigates whether phonological information can influence the selection of a word when preceding phonological primes are presented in a different position than the target (Experiment 1) or when this information is presented in a different allophonic context than the target (Experiment 2). If lexical access is only primed by the production of phonological information which matches the target word in word position and phonetic realization (e.g., ‘balcony’, ‘ballast’), then the sound forms which influence lexical selection are specified for both their phonetic position and articulatory properties. If, on the other hand, positionally and acoustically different forms also prime lexical access (e.g., ‘balcony’, ‘cannibal’), then the units of phonological information which influence lexical access are abstract.

Experiment 1

The goal of experiment 1 is to investigate the effect of phonological information on lexical selection, and in particular to determine whether phonological priming is possible for sounds which share abstract phonemic information, but not their word position. Experiment 1 adopts a modified TOT definition naming paradigm, modeled after James & Burke (2000). James & Burke presented subjects with the definitions of low-frequency target words and asked them if they could identify the word being defined, and if not, whether they were in a TOT state. Each definition was preceded by ten visually presented prime words, which subjects overtly produced. Trials were in one of two conditions: Unrelated, in which primes were not semantically or phonologically related to the target; and Phonologically Related, in which five of the primes shared some phonemes with the target (e.g., for the target “sextant”: “secondary”, “semblance”, “restrain”, “pheasant”, “feint”). Five filler words were used in each trial in the Related condition to prevent subjects from using the primes as a strategy to identify the target (a concern raised by Hamburger & Slowiaczek, 1996; Sullivan & Riffel, 1999; Monsell & Hirsh, 1998; and Norris, McQueen, & Cutler, 2002). James & Burke were interested in the effect of phonological information on TOT states in different age groups. The phonological information always appeared in the same word position in the prime and target and they did not control for repetition of primed information or for the amount of phonological overlap (e.g., “ant” in “pheasant” vs. “nt” in “feint”) nor did they ensure complete overlap between the target and its corresponding set of primes (e.g., the /k/ in “sextant” is never primed).

The current experiment uses a similar task but more highly controlled phonologically related stimuli, and manipulates the word position of the overlapping phonemes between the prime and target. Because this is a priming study and not a TOT investigation, we did not collect data on TOT experiences, nor did we attempt to elicit TOT states; rather, we investigated the proportion of trials on which subjects selected and produced the target word.

Stimuli

Seventy-two three-to-five syllable words were selected as potential stimuli from previous studies (Meyer & Bock, 1992; Burke, McKay, Worthley & Wade, 1991; Harley & Brown, 1998; Morsella & Kraus, 1999) and from the MRC Linguistic Database (Coltheart, 1981). Definitions were adapted from the Merriam-Webster dictionary to between six and ten words each, to provide consistency across stimuli and to minimize variation in reading times.

Targets were selected from a pool of 106 words during a norming pilot experiment in which 15 subjects named the candidate target words in response to their definitions. In order to minimize variation in difficulty between items, targets were eliminated if fewer than 60% of pilot subjects named the target word. Targets were also eliminated if the mean naming latency across conditions and subjects was longer than 4000 ms. The mean number of words per definition in the 72 selected target words for Experiment 1 was 8.18 (SD 1.23) and the average Kučera-Francis frequency (Kučera & Francis, 1967) of each target word was 18.34 (SD = 47.5).

In the experiment proper, a single trial consisted of the presentation of six prime words presented one at a time, followed by the definition of a target word. Subjects were told to produce each prime word aloud and then indicate by button press whether they thought they had pronounced it correctly. Subjects were told that when presented with a definition, they were to produce the word being defined as quickly as possible, and then indicate by button press whether they had pronounced it correctly.

In every trial, three of the six primes were fillers which were phonologically and semantically unrelated to the target. The other three primes (or, in rare instances, compound words, e.g., “dry cleaner”) were in one of three conditions. In the Control condition, all primes were phonologically unrelated to the target. In the Match condition, three of the six primes contained the same initial, middle, or final syllable as the target word (e.g., for the target “balcony”, “ballast”, “reconcile”, “villainy”). The shared syllables were in the same word position as the target. In the Mismatch condition, three primes contained the initial, middle, and final syllable of the target word in a different word position (e.g. for the target “balcony”, “unbalanced”, “contagion”, “cranium”). No primes across conditions were semantically related to their targets.

For targets with four or more syllables, the medial syllable with primary or secondary stress was selected for overlap with a prime. To account for ambisyllabicity in English, any consonant which could be both the coda of the preceding syllable and the onset of the following syllable (e.g., the /l/ in “calorie”) was treated as both part of the onset and part of the coda.

Three unrelated filler words were also used for each trial to reduce the likelihood that subjects would use a strategy. Match and Mismatch primes were used as fillers for phonologically unrelated targets; for example, the Match primes for the target “balcony” for one subject might be used as unrelated fillers for the target “studio” for another subject. This controlled for frequency differences between primes, since every prime was presented to one-third of all subjects in each condition. No subject saw a prime more than once and no subject reported employing a strategy to use phonologically related primes to identify targets. The six primes, including both critical and filler words, were presented in random order before their corresponding target.

Each of the 72 targets was presented to every subject once, such that any given subject was presented with 24 targets in each of three conditions. The stimuli presented in each condition were counterbalanced for difficulty; difficulty was estimated using the mean target reaction time of participants in the pilot study. Order of presentation was not counterbalanced except that each block of trials (three blocks in Experiment 1, two blocks in Experiment 2) contained the same number of trials in each condition. Additionally, the pattern of response results is the same in each presentation block for both experiments. See Appendix A for a listing of the stimuli.

Procedure

Stimulus presentation was controlled using the BLISS software suite (Mertus, 2002) on a Dell PC. Verbal responses were recorded using a Sony microphone and an Edirol R-09 24-bit Digital Recorder. Responses were recorded as 24-bit uncompressed WAV files sampled at 44.1 kHz, and then down-sampled, using BLISS, to 16-bit WAV before analysis.

Thirty-three subjects (8 male) participated in the experiment and all received payment for their involvement. All participants were native English speakers, reported normal hearing and had no known history of neurological disorders. Each subject gave written informed consent in accordance with the guidelines established and approved by the Human Subjects Committee of Brown University. Data from three subjects were discarded due to a failure to meet an a priori response criterion of 67% correctly produced targets. The experiment took approximately 40 minutes to complete.

Participants were seated in front of a screen and button box and were visually presented with all primes and target definitions. Each trial consisted of the 6 prime words followed by a definition. Figure 1 shows the experimental design and an example trial in each condition. Subjects were asked to read each word aloud as quickly as possible without sacrificing accuracy, then to indicate on the button box whether or not they had pronounced it correctly. After each set of six primes, the target definition was presented. Subjects were asked to produce the word best described by the definition, and then to indicate by button press whether they had pronounced it correctly. Experiment 1 was self-timed unless a subject did not respond within 12000 ms, in which case the response was scored as a timeout and the next trial was initiated.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experimental Design.

Responses were scored as correct if the subject named the target word. If a subject changed his or her mind and produced the correct target after a nontarget response or a meaningless utterance such as “uh”, the response was scored as correct, irrespective of reaction time latencies.

Reaction times (RTs) were measured from the onset of sentence definition to the fully correct response; for example, the RT of a response, “b… balcony” would be measured from the presentation of the definition to the onset of the second “b”. This metric was used to ensure that the phonological form of the word had been accessed before the utterance onset. RTs were measured for all correct responses using the waveforms on BLISS. For the RT analysis, RTs were discarded if the subject’s response was more than two standard deviations above his or her mean RT for that condition or if the target had fewer than 20 measured reaction times across participants (i.e., fewer than 67% of participants correctly named the target).

Results

Target responses

Figure 2 shows the mean number of correct responses to the target across conditions (Match, M = 0.851; Mismatch, M = 0.854; Control, M = 0.811). As can be seen, both the Match and Mismatch conditions increased the proportion of correct target responses relative to the Control condition.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Figure 2

Proportion of target responses from Experiment 1. a) Within-subject analysis; b) Item analysis.

A one-way ANOVA confirmed these observations. There was a main effect of condition in the within-subject analysis (F[2,28] = 3.45, MSE = 0.017, p = 0.038, Figure 2a) and marginal effect in the item analysis (F[2,70] = 2.76, MSE = 0.042, p = 0.067, Figure 2b). We attribute the weaker effects in the within-subject analysis to the high variability of difficulty across items.

Post-hoc pairwise two-tailed t-tests showed a difference for the subject analysis between the Match and Control conditions (subject analysis: t[29] = 2.53, p = 0.017) and a marginal difference in the item analysis (t[71] = 1.78, p = 0.080), as well as significant differences between the Mismatch and Control conditions (subject analysis: t[29] = 2.21, p = 0.035; item analysis, t[71] = 2.17, 0.034), but no difference between Match and Mismatch conditions. These results indicate that the phonologically related conditions improved the likelihood that subjects produced the target, e.g., “balcony”, and that this effect emerged irrespective of syllable position. This pattern of results holds even if we eliminate trials during which subjects gave multiple responses (“veranda… balcony”, accounting for 1.4% of total responses) or if we eliminate the ‘hesitation’ trials (“uh… balcony”, accounting for 2.4%).

Because ANOVA-type tests are nonideal for binomial dependent variables (Jaeger, 2008), we also ran treatment-coded comparisons between pairs of conditions using a mixed logit model, using lme4 (Bates, Maechler, & Bolker, 2012) in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2011). The dependent variable was target response, and the model used maximal random effect structure (Condition was included as a fixed effect as well as Subject and Item as random effects). The pattern of significance was the same as the analyses above: there was a significant difference between both the Match and Control (z = 2.15, p = 0.031) and Mismatch and Control (z = 2.28, p = 0.026) conditions. A Helmert-coded model was also tested to further examine differences between conditions. One contrast used coefficients that grouped together the Match and Mismatch conditions and compared them to the Control condition (z = 2.57, p = 0.010), and a second contrast compared the Match and Mismatch conditions (p > 0.1). These contrasts confirm the pattern of results above.

Nontarget response analysis

When subjects failed to give a correct target response, these responses were coded based on what type of error was made. These types included: the production of a semantic competitor, a word which did not fit the definition, a nonword, a phonologically related word to the target, an expressed TOT state, a mispronunciation of the target, a partial but incomplete production of the target, and a failure to respond within 12 seconds. The two most common types of errors were production of semantic competitors, e.g., “veranda” in response to the definition of “balcony”; and a failure to respond within the response window, which was coded as a timeout. These responses were still relatively rare, accounting for 9.8% of error types. Nonetheless, the pattern of differences between conditions is of interest to the question of what drives the target response effect. Qualitative analysis revealed that the proportion of semantic competitor responses was highest in the Control condition, but the proportion of timeouts did not differ between conditions. See Table 1 for a summary of the response types in Experiment 1.

Table 1.

Data from Experiment 1: Item analysis. Mean (SE)

Match Mismatch Control
Proportion of correct target responses 0.851 (0.131) 0.854 (0.126) 0.811 (0.166)
Proportion of semantic competitor responses 0.082 (0.012) 0.093 (0.012) 0.118 (0.015)
Proportion of timeouts 0.044 (0.008) 0.034 (0.008) 0.039 (0.009)
Other responses 0.022 (0.006) 0.021 (0.006) 0.033 (0.007)
Reaction time 2897 (67) 2845 (61) 2754 (54)

Reaction time analysis

Figure 3 shows the RT results. A one-way ANOVA indicated there was no effect of reaction time across conditions in the subject analysis (F[2,28] = 1.35, MSE = 107837, p > 0.1), but there was a marginal effect of condition in the item analysis (F[2,70] = 2.64, MSE = 347679, p = 0.078, Figure 3b). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons showed marginally higher reaction times in the Match condition than in the Control condition in the subject analysis (t[29] = 1.73, p = 0.094) and significantly higher reaction times in the item analysis (t[65] = 2.25, p = 0.028). No other comparisons were significant.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Figure 3

Reaction times from Experiment 1. a) Within-subject analysis; b) Item analysis.

Discussion

It was the goal of Experiment 1 to investigate the extent to which phonological priming of lexical access is sensitive to word position of the primed sound units. The data showed a significant effect of phonological priming even when the primes did not match the target’s word position. There was also no difference in the proportion of target responses between the Match and Mismatch conditions, suggesting that there is no priming advantage to the position-matched primes over the mismatched primes.

Most trials during which subjects did not name the target were either semantic competitor responses (e.g., balcony-veranda) or timeouts (no response). In Experiment 1, the proportion of semantic competitor responses was not equally distributed across conditions. There were fewer semantic competitor responses in the phonologically matched conditions than in the Control condition, suggesting that the increased activation of the phonological properties of a word result in its selection over semantic competitors. On the other hand, timeouts did not differ across conditions.

Experiment 1 also showed slower reaction times in the Match condition than in the Control condition. These slowed response latencies in the Match condition may reflect competition between the phonologically unshared portions of the prime and target. For example, the phonological segments of “last” in “ballast” compete with the unshared segments “cony” in the target word “balcony”. Consistent with this interpretation is evidence that slowed response times during word naming are proportional to the number of unshared phonemes between target words and competing lexical items (Wheeldon, 2003; Dufour & Peereman, 2003). Indeed, Sevald & Dell (1994) and Sullivan & Riffel (1999) conclude that articulatory plans are constructed sequentially from initial to final sounds, even though early phonological encoding occurs in parallel. As a consequence, the slowed RT latencies in the Match condition could be due to the greater competition during articulatory preparation from primes which matched the target word than from the unrelated Control primes. Consistent with this interpretation is the failure to show a difference in RT latencies between the Mismatch and Control conditions. Here, phonological planning and implementation stages are not influenced by the phonological prime because they occur in different “slots” within the word frame.

Experiment 2

The response data from Experiment 1 support the notion that context-independent phonological representations influence lexical access (Stemberger, 1990; Buchwald & Miozzo, 2011) since there was no difference in the magnitude of the effect for either type of phonological prime (whether or not they matched the syllable context of the target). However, in the Mismatch condition, some of the primes shared phonetic properties with the target and in other cases they did not. For example, on some Mismatch trials, the syllables in the prime and target shared syllable stress, e.g., “balcony” and “unbalanced”. On other trials, the prime and target stimuli differed in syllable stress, e.g., “intersection” and “secretary”. The same was true for other aspects of phonetic realization (e.g., the /k/ in “balcony” is unaspirated, but the /k/ in its prime “contagion” is aspirated). Thus, it is unclear whether the phonetic realization of a mismatched phonological prime influences the activation of the lexical representation of a target word.

To address this possibility, Experiment 2 manipulated the phonetic similarity between the prime and target stimuli by examining the effects of two Mismatch conditions: one in which the position of the primed syllable varies but the phonetic realization of the target is similar to that of the target stimulus, and one in which both the phonetic position and the phonetic realization of the prime stimulus vary in relation to the target stimulus. If phonological priming of lexical selection is influenced by the similarity between the phonetic realization of the prime and target, then the two Mismatch conditions should differ in the proportion of target responses. If, on the other hand, lexical selection processes are driven by abstract phonological representations, then the Mismatch Phonetics condition will show the same proportion of target responses as the Match and Mismatch Position conditions.

Stimuli

Sixty-eight three- to five-syllable targets were used in Experiment 2. Fifty-six of these targets were used in Experiment 1; the other twelve were selected from an earlier norming pilot. Some targets from Experiment 1 were not included in Experiment 2 because suitable prime words were not available. An independent t-test showed that there were no significant differences in frequency between the stimuli used in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 (t[126] = 0.08; p > 0.1).

Following the methods of Experiment 1, subjects were presented with target definitions following the presentation of six consecutive prime words. As in Experiment 1, definitions were adapted from the Merriam-Webster dictionary to between six and ten words each (average number of words per definition: 8.29 (SD = 1.23); average Kûcera-Francis frequency: 22.3 (SD = 54.5). The primes were in one of four conditions. In the Control condition, all six primes were phonologically unrelated to the target. In the Match condition, three of the six primes contained the same initial, middle, or final syllable as the target word. These shared syllables were in the same word position as the target and they matched the target in phonetic realization (e.g., for the target “balcony”, “ballast”, “reconcile”, “villainy”). In the Mismatch Position condition, three primes shared a syllable with the target word; the shared syllable occurred in a different word position from the target syllable but shared the acoustic realization of the target syllable (e.g. for “balcony”, the primes were “unbalanced”, “outspoken”, “cranium”). In the Mismatch Phonetics condition, both the word position and the acoustic realization of the shared phonemes differed from the target. For example, for “balcony”, the primes were “counterbalance” (syllable stress), “contagion” (aspiration of /k/), “needle” (syllable stress). Parameters along which acoustic realization was manipulated were: stress (conversion-shiver); vowel quality (abacus-diabolic); aspiration of stop consonants (calendar-localize); /l/-darkening (adolescent-lessening); tapping of /t/ and /d/ or stop consonant release (battery-wombat); /n/-velarization (insomnia-disinclined); and vowel nasalization (graduate-pomegranate). Table 3 shows the distribution of these parameters across the stimuli in the Mismatch Phonetics condition. See Appendix B for a listing of the stimuli.

Table 3.

Summary of Phonetic Changes to Mismatched Phonetics stimuli in Experiment 2.

Phonetic Change Number of instances
stress 140
vowel quality 40
aspiration 52
/l/-darkening 10
tap/t/release 41
/n/ velarization 4
vowel nasalization 4

Procedure

Like in Experiment 1, subjects were seated in front of a computer and were visually presented with primes and target definitions. A trial consisted of the presentation of six primes and one target definition. After each prime, subjects were asked to produce the word aloud and then indicate on a button box whether or not they had pronounced it correctly. During a definition presentation, they were asked to produce the word which best fit the definition and then indicate by button box whether they had pronounced it correctly. Because the mean reaction time for subjects in Experiment 1 was less than 3000 ms and the distribution showed very few responses over 4000 ms, the time window for a response was 6000 ms, shortened from the 12000 ms in Experiment 1. Three practice trials were conducted before the experiment to acclimate participants to the procedure and timing of the experiment.

Targets were presented in the same manner as Experiment 1, such that each subject received 17 targets in each condition. Thirty-three native English speakers participated (10 male) and reported no known neurological disorders. None of the Experiment 1 participants participated in Experiment 2. Participants were compensated for their time and provided written informed consent in accordance with the guidelines established and approved by the Human Subjects Committee of Brown University. Data from 3 participants were discarded due to extremely low rate of correct target responses (<67%, the same criterion as Experiment 1). Scoring and analysis of responses were identical to Experiment 1.

Results

Target responses

Figure 4 shows the proportion of correct target responses across conditions (Match, M = 0.839; Mismatch Position, M = 0.820; Mismatch Phonetics, M = 0.827; Control, M = 0.775). A one-way ANOVA showed a marginal main effect of condition in the subject analysis (F[3,28] = 2.67, MSE = 0.024, p = 0.052, Figure 4a) and a significant main effect in the item analysis (F[3,66] = 2.93, MSE = 0.057, p = 0.035, Figure 4b). Post-hoc t-tests showed a significant difference between Match and Control conditions in the subject (t[67] = 2.19, p = 0.036) and item (t[67] = 2.86, p = 0.006) analyses; a marginal difference between Mismatch Position and Control conditions in both subject (t[29] = 1.93, p = 0.064) and item (t[67] = 1.79, p = 0.077) analyses; and a marginal difference between Mismatch Phonetic and Control conditions in the subject analysis (t[29] = 1.83, p =0.078) and a significant difference in the item analysis (t[67] = 2.18, p = 0.033). The Match, Mismatch Position and Mismatch Phonetics conditions did not differ from each other (all p-values > 0.10).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Figure 4

Proportion of target responses from Experiment 2. a) Within-subject analysis; b) Item analysis.

In a mixed logit model as described in Experiment 1, the pattern of results was the same: the Match vs. Control (z = 2.82, p = 0.005), Mismatched Position vs. Control (z = 2.00, p = 0.045) and Mismatched Phonetics vs. Control (z = 2.32, p = 0.021) were the only comparisons that yielded significant differences. As in Experiment 1, a Helmert-coded model contrasted the Control with the mean of the other conditions and showed a significant effect (z = 2.99, p < 0.01), but no effect for any contrasts between phonologically related conditions (p > 0.1).

Nontarget responses

The proportion of semantic competitor (e.g., balcony-veranda) and timeout (no response) trials were also analyzed (Table 2). Although there were very few datapoints to evaluate (these responses occurred in only 8.5% of trials), a qualitative analysis of the pattern of results revealed that the mean proportion of semantic competitor responses was higher in the Control condition than the Match and Mismatch conditions, similar to the findings in Experiment 1. The proportion of timeouts and other response subtypes (false starts, phonological competitors, etc.) were also too infrequent to analyze. As in Experiment 1, removing the trials on which subjects produced multiple responses (0.2% of the total) or those on which subjects hesitated (5.5% of responses) did not change the pattern of results.

Table 2.

Data from Experiment 2: Item analysis. Mean (SE)

Match Mismatched Position Mismatched Phonetics Control
Proportion of target responses 0.839 (0.020) 0.820 (0.022) 0.827 (0.021) 0.775 (0.026)
Proportion of semantic competitor responses 0.065 (0.013) 0.081 (0.014) 0.074 (0.014) 0.099 (0.014)
Proportion of timeouts 0.073 (0.014) 0.080 (0.015) 0.091 (0.016) 0.098 (0.018)
Other responses 0.020 (0.006) 0.010 (0.005) 0.011 (0.005) 0.025 (0.007)
Reaction times 2537 (66) 2485 (60) 2520 (58) 2605 (76)

Reaction times

The RT data were subject to the same analysis cutoffs as the Experiment 1 data. The overall distribution of reaction times was extremely similar to Experiment 1, suggesting that the shortened response window did not impact subjects’ responses. However, no significant main effect or simple effect emerged in either the subject (Figure 5a) or the item (Figure 5b) analyses.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Figure 5

Reaction times from Experiment 2. a) Within-subject analysis; b) Item analysis.

Discussion for Experiment 2

The goal of Experiment 2 was to investigate whether the lexical selection of a target word requires a phonetic match between the prime and the target. The results of Experiment 2 replicated the target response data from Experiment 1. Phonologically related primes increased the rate of target responses in both the Match and Mismatch Position conditions relative to the Control condition. Furthermore, the effect emerged in the Mismatch Phonetics condition, where neither the syllable position nor the acoustic realizations of shared phonemes matched the target. This finding provides strong support for models of spoken word production in which abstract phonological representations influence lexical access.

It is possible that the failure to show a difference between the two Mismatch conditions was because the phonetically mismatched stimuli were not perceptually distinct. To explore this possibility. we conducted an additional experiment. Four subjects from Experiment 2, two male and two female, were randomly selected and their recorded utterances were used as stimuli in a speaker identification task. Each target response in either of the Mismatch conditions was paired with a randomly selected prime utterance, e.g., “cannibal” and “balcony”. Match primes were also randomly paired with each other as filler trials. Five subjects who did not participate in Experiment 2 were auditorily presented with word pairs and asked to identify whether the speaker was the same for both words, or different. There were a total of 86 critical trials and 93 filler trials, presented in random order. In critical trials, the speaker was always the same, and in filler trials, the speaker was different. All five subjects gave slower “same” responses to the Mismatch Position pairs than to the Mismatch Phonetics pairs (Figure 6; paired t(4)=8.35, p=0.001). The Mismatch Position condition had more phonetic overlap with the target word than the Mismatch Phonetics condition suggesting that subjects experienced interference in the speaker identification task when there was greater phonetic overlap. In any case, these findings indicate that there is a significant perceptual difference between the stimuli in the two Mismatch conditions, and the lack of difference between them in Experiment 2 was not due to a failure to perceive phonetic distinctions across the 2 Mismatch conditions.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Speaker identification results for each subject, perceptually comparing the two Mismatch conditions in Experiment 2.

In contrast to Experiment 1, the RT analysis showed no difference between conditions. The implications of these findings are discussed below.

General Discussion

The results of Experiments 1 and 2 show that phonological information has an influence on lexical selection, and that this influence is independent of both position and phonetic context. Experiment 1 showed that phonological priming increases the proportion of target responses regardless of whether the position matched between prime and target. Experiment 2 replicated this finding and extended it to trials in which neither the syllable position nor phonetic realization are shared between prime and target. These findings together support models of spoken word production which posit abstract phonological representations. Furthermore, the failure of the Match condition to show a stronger effect than any of the Mismatch conditions suggests that phonetically specified representations have little effect on the priming of lexical-level processes, compatible with the view that there are restrictions on the extent to which interaction is possible between the underlying phoneme representations and the phonetically specified articulatory output (Goldrick, 2006; Rapp & Goldrick, 2000). It is noteworthy that in the current paradigm, subjects articulated the primes in addition to reading them, requiring the activation of the fully specified phonetic form of each prime. In spite of this activation, the phonetic detail failed to influence lexical access.

An alternative explanation for these results is that perhaps the target response effects in the Match and Mismatch conditions are being driven by the overall acoustic similarity between prime and target words, which is not present in the Control condition. This would be possible if, for example, the lexicon is structured such that any given word form reflects a multidimensional phonetic space that includes a range of acoustic properties which could be mapped onto an exemplar of that word. In the Match and Mismatch conditions, the critical syllables from the primes are within that phonetic space for the target words and therefore prime access to the target word; in the Control condition, the primes are not in that phonetic space and thus no priming occurs. This explanation would not necessitate explicit representations of abstract phonological units. However, of interest, the results are consistent with the hypothesis that the phonetic space belonging to a target word is accessible even when the position of overlapping sound information does not match the sound’s position in that word. The results of both experiments also suggested that phonological priming impacts production of semantic competitors. The pattern of results supports the hypothesis that the phonologically related trials impact lexical selection processes, since phonological priming both increases the proportion of target responses and decreases the proportion of semantic competitor responses (Ferreira & Griffin, 2003; Rapp & Samuel, 2002; but see Jaeger et al. 2012, who found an inhibitory effect of phonological overlap).

These priming effects are somewhat surprising in light of the speech error literature, which shows a strong bias towards preserving the word position of phonemes during exchange errors between words (MacKay, 1969; Shattuck-Hufnagel, 1987). This phenomenon in speech errors suggests that such errors occur in the motor plan of already-specified phonological units. However, the current results indicate that the motor plan and implementation of phonetic detail do not influence access to the lexical form of a word. They also suggest that although parallel structure is common during speech errors, priming effects transcend syllable and word boundaries.

Experiment 1 showed slower RT latencies for the production of target responses in the Match condition compared to the Control condition. This finding suggests that a partial phonological match between prime and target words slows phonological planning and articulatory implementation by increasing competition between mismatched phonemes. The failure of Experiment 2 to replicate the reaction time effect in Experiment 1 may be due to the reduced response window from 12 seconds to 6 seconds in Experiment 2, forcing mean latencies in each condition to converge.

Because the stimuli were presented visually, it is possible that there was some effect of orthographic similarity when the phonologically overlapping syllable had the same spelling in both the prime and the target. For example, Damian and Bowers (2002) found that during a form-preparation task, overlapping initial sounds only showed priming if the onsets matched orthographically as well; that is, subjects could more quickly produce “kidney” if they had learned to pair it with “kennel” than if they had learned to pair it with “camel”. However, form-preparation paradigms involve extensive visual exposure to the prime-target pairs during the memorization phase, and the current experiments required only a single exposure to each prime and no visual exposure to the target. Additionally, subjects were asked to focus on the pronunciation of each prime word, not its spelling.

To ensure that orthographic overlap was not driving the current results, we analyzed a subset of trials on which at least two of the three critical syllables did not share their orthography with the target (for example, target “audible”, primes “odyssey”, “wildebeest”, “cannibal”). There were not enough trials to conduct a statistical analysis on this subset of the data, but the pattern of results resembled that of the full dataset for both experiments. In particular, the mean proportions of all Match and Mismatch conditions were higher than those for their corresponding Control conditions. This pattern is compatible with findings that phonological overlap is sufficient for priming in production without orthographic overlap (Lukatela & Turvey, 1994), and that orthographic overlap does not show priming effects during production tasks which do not require explicit consideration of a word’s spelling (Bi, Wei, Janssen, & Han, 2009).

It should be noted that our description of spoken word production has assumed, for convenience, that the phoneme is the fundamental sound representation in spoken word production. However, because our primes and targets shared full syllables, the same results would be predicted if the basic units of phonological processing were syllables (Levelt & Wheeldon, 1994), completely unspecified phonemes (Roelofs, 1999), or phonemes which are specified for syllable position but not for contextual environment (Dell, 1986, Collins & Ellis, 1992). Our broad interpretation of the results is compatible with any of these proposed phonological representations. This experiment expressly aimed to investigate the specification of word position and acoustic realization during lexical-semantic access. The data indicate that neither of these parameters needs to be specified to elicit priming during the definition naming task.

Because three target syllables were primed in each trial of the current experiments, we cannot distinguish between the effects of priming onsets and rhymes. There is evidence that word onsets have a special status in word form representations; they are often available during TOT states (Brown & McNeil, 1966), priming of naming latency is greater when primes and targets share onsets (Schiller, 2004), and onsets are most often the locus of speech errors (Shattuck-Hufnagel, 1987). Indeed, Jaeger, Furth, and Hilliard (2012) found that subjects avoid producing two consecutive words which overlap in onset (“Hannah passed a book to the child” is produced more often than “Hannah handed a book to the child”). Since this effect is in the opposite direction of Rapp & Samuel (2002), who used rhyme-related primes, Jaeger et al. conclude that onsets and rhymes produce different priming effects on production. Further research will be needed to determine the nature of onset vs. rhyme effects on lexical access in spoken word production.

Finally, the Mismatch primes were not controlled for whether the phonologically matched syllables shared morpheme boundaries with the target. For example, the syllable “bal” is morpheme-initial in both “balcony” and “unbalanced”, but “unbalanced” was used as a Mismatch Position prime for “balcony”. It is possible that shared morpheme boundaries could be treated as a “position match” despite the fact that the word position of the shared syllable between the prime and the target mismatched. A review of the stimuli used in both Experiments 1 and 2 indicated that there were too few stimuli to examine whether morpheme position played any role in the results of the current experiments. It would be of interest in future research to examine the extent to which morpheme boundaries do or do not influence lexical selection processes.

In conclusion, Experiments 1 and 2 show that activation of phonological information influences lexical access, and that this influence is not sensitive to the word position and phonetic realization of the prime-target overlap. We interpret these results as supporting the existence of abstract phonological representations in the spoken word production network.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by NIH Grant RO1 DC006220 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Deafness.

Appendix A.

Experimental stimuli for Experiment 1.

Target Match Mismatch
abacus abdicate sabotage hibiscus diabolize battalion cassava
abduction obliterate conductive conversation pliable ductility instructional
accelerate acoustic outselling recuperate elegiac cellular exhilarated
adolescent celestial subjacent admonition lessening adjudicate absentee
albino allopathic unbinding piano hypoallergenic binary noticing
alchemy algebra sycamore salami geniality comprise meteor
ambulance amateur phobially opulence iambic adverbial balancing
amputate ambient reputable acetate Preamble putative vegetative
architect Arkansas nicotine protect anarchical catalysis tectonic
audible odyssey wildebeest cannibal periodic debase Belinda
audience oddity odious brilliance idiotic deify leniency
autopsy ottoman ectopic queasy patriotic topical seepage
balcony balustrade reconcile villainy unbalanced contagion cranium
battery battlement guttural actuary sabbatical tureen realigning
biology biennial vitriolic apogee alibi Oliver geocentric
calendar calcium Valentine plunder incalculable stolen derive
calorie calcified Polaroid ornery recalcitrant Lorraine reapply
Canada cantaloupe unadorned Linda bicameral arachnid delicious
cartographer carcinogen plastogamy camphor incarnation toggling forensic
chandelier shantytown debris cavalier scrimshander delineate leering
cigarette signaling invigorate minaret insignia gorilla reticence
circumference sorority encumbent conference exacerbate compass inferencing
collision colloquial elicit disillusion accolade listener occasionally
composer compunction symposium accuser cucumber predispose misery
congregation confidential sagacious discretion Yukon geisha devotional
continent malignant consecrate detonate reconnaissance tenacious pregnantly
conversion conception reversal circumcision drunkenly verbiage divisional
Democrat demagogue stomachache plutocrat pandemic hammock hippocratic
eavesdropper evenly name-dropping caliper naively raindrop parental
epidermis episodic hypodermic litmus narcolepsy dervishes mosquito
evolution evidential polluted narration whoever lucrative cautionary
excavate extradite Muscovite inactivate inexpert cavort innovated
graduate gradually extrajudicial asphyxiate Leningrad judicious creativity
gravity gratifying covetous obscenity ingratitude vituperate tiara
hibernate hyacinth tuberose dominate Ohio barometer natal
hurricane Hercules oracle cocaine rehearsal raccoon canine
hypochondriac hybridization reconstitute aphrodisiac dehydration confiscated academic
innocent instigate menacing adjacent reinsulate necessity accented
insomnia insipidly ensemble gardenia reinstate somnolence perennially
interrogate inversion Ontario litigate ruination terrify obligating
intersection instrument dissecting malefaction uninjured secretary actionless
intestine indefinite contestable simpleton reincarnate tessellate tenacity
journalist Germany monologue nihilist adjourn provisional illustrate
Jupiter julep serendipity adulter rejuvenate carpet loitering
kimono chameleon ammonia Milano predicament moaning notational
literate litany flatterer conglomerate paralytic commenter retaliate
magnetic magnolia Connecticut traumatic demagnify networking vertical
marinade marathon quarantine lemonade intermarry barren tornado
mutiny mutilate tetanus calumny permuted tonight Neolithic
nostalgia nestorian italic hydrangea monastery talisman Japan
nucleus nutrient dry-cleaner euphonious connubial Cleopatra assertive
papyrus papoose inspiring vigorous hippopotamus pirate responsive
passenger pasteurize dissonant conjure capacity waxen marjoram
pentagon pendulum autograph foregone appendix together goner
pharmacy farmstead enormously delicacy safari misogynist siesta
photosynthesis focused exscinded colossus xenophobic syndrome sustaining
president presbyter supposedly Occident oppressive stanza dentition
principal princess disappear Episcopal misprinted suppressed impolite
prohibition procreate ambitious vacation opprobrium bishop rational
reception receding acceptance adoption terraces scepter optional
recipe respirate municipal teepee arrested suppressible peony
referee reformation aphorism jamboree irreverent ferocious Eureka
repellent rapacious compelling fraudulent parapet pelican voluntary
reunion reelect biuniqueness Pennsylvanian creamery opinionated unicycle
rhetorical retention notorious mechanical conspirator torturous calligraphy
singular single-handed ambiguous tutelar asynchronous argue tolerant
studio stooping ideology pistachio festooning de-icer oasis
submarine substituted glamorize wolverine resubjugate miraculous ballerina
synonyms synapse pregnancy platinum vicinity vernon Namibia
veteran veterinary trajectory heron inveterate seismometer occurrences
volunteer voluble islander racketeer convolving watermelon teary-eyed
vulnerable vulcanizing generate symbol divulging milliner ballistic

Appendix B.

Experimental stimuli for Experiment 2.

Target Match Mismatch Position
abacus abdicate sabotage hibiscus diabolize battalion focusing
abduction abhor conductive conversation reabsorb duckling commissioner
accelerate accede outselling recuperate elegiac cellular irately
adolescent additive celestial subjacent triad convalesce complacently
albino allopathic unbinding piano hypoallergenic binary Nova Scotia
allergy albatross cutlery psychology joviality controller geometry
ambulance amateur phobially opulence iambic adverbial balancing
amputate ambient reputation rotate Preamble disrepute unstately
architect Arkansas nicotine protect anarchical certificate tectonic
audience oddity odious brilliance theodicy malady expediency
autopsy autumnal ectopic bossy megawatt housetop seeker
balcony ballast buccaneer villainy unbalanced outspoken cranium
battery battlement guttural actuary sabbatical diameter realigning
biology biennial alcoholic apogee alibi Oliver geocentric
calendar calcium silences plunder incalculable stolen derive
calorie calcified Polaroid ornery recalcitrant Lorraine reapply
cameo camouflage bohemian leo bicameral mediate geodesic
cartographer carcinogen plastogamy transfer incarcerate toggling forensic
cigarette signaling kangaroo minaret insignia gorilla retina
circumference serenity incumbent conference exacerbate cumbersome inferencing
collision colloquial elicit illusion broccoli endless occasionally
composer compunction opposable accuser miscommunicate poses misery
congregation concrete sagacious discretion incongruous gaiety devotional
continent conjugate latency malignant anaconda bitten pregnantly
conversion confetti reversal circumcision disconnect verbiage divisional
counterfeit countenance interstate discomfit accountable enter fitness
decagon decadent recognize paragon ambidextrous kaput gondolier
ellipsis electric apocalyptic Pegasus apparel lipreader Narcissism
finale finesse analogy actually definite Nalgene kiloliter
graduate graphite nonjudicial asphyxiate telegraphic judicious sedately
gravity gratifying covetous obscenity ingratitude velvet committeemen
hibernate hyacinth tuberose dominate Ohio barometer innately
hurricane Hercules oracle cocaine rehearsal euphoric canine
hypochondriac hybridization reconstitute zodiac dehydration confiscated accident
innocent insect dinosaur adjacent reinsulated necessity absently
insomnia insipidly ensemble gardenia reinstate somber neoplasm
insurance interment assuredly severance reinvite sherbet tolerances
interrogate inversion Ontario litigate ruination terrify gateway
intersection instrument dissecting malefaction uninjured secretarial actionless
interview intellect tottering purview reintegrate encounter televiewer
intestine indefinite contestable captain reinstall tessellate obstinate
journalist Germany analyze nihilist adjourn provisional philistine
Jupiter julep serendipity pentameter rejuvenate carpet loitering
literate litigate flatterer conglomerate paralytic auditor separately
magnetic magnolia clarinetist traumatic demagnify nettling vertical
marinade marathon quarantine lemonade intermarry barren unaided
metronome metric extrovert garden gnome symmetrical electrolyte misnomer
military millionaire posterity cannery familiar Terrence reanimate
mutiny mutant tetanus detainee commute curtain Neolithic
nucleus nutrient dry-cleaner euphonious connubial Cleopatra assertive
passenger pasteurize dissonant conjure capacity waxen conjuring
pharmacy farmstead thermostat delicacy safari infamous siesta
photosynthesis focused exscinded colossus xenophobic Cynthia systematic
potpourri poetize chaperon debris apropos juniper regal
poverty populist feverish literati hypocrisy clever protean
president predicate supposedly rodent supremacy stanza imprudently
principal princess participate Episcopal misprinted gossip impolite
prohibition procreate ambitious vacation opprobrium bishop rational
recipe respirate municipal teepee arrested supplanted timepieces
referee reformation aphorism jamboree hereditary ferocious Eureka
reunion reelect immunity Pennsylvanian armory unicycle opinionated
rhetorical retention notorious mechanical heretic torturous alkali
submarine substituted tamarind wolverine resubjugate marooning ballerina
summary sumptuous numeral owlery consumption meridian rearrange
synonym synapse pregnancy denim vicinity Vernon animal
veteran vetted catering heron Helvetica seismometer occurrences
volunteer voluble colonize racketeer convolving watermelon teary-eyed
vulnerable vulcanizing generate symbol divulging milliner ballistic
Target Mismatch Phonetics
abacus diabolic banana cassava
abduction diabetes ductility shindig
accelerate reactive celebration serrated
adolescent inadmissible lessening concentration
albino dial binoculars noticing
allergy causally lurking genus
ambulance disambiguate proverbial Lansing
amputate overambitious putative agitated
architect matriarch catastrophe detected
audience periodicity dehumanize truancy
autopsy patriotic topography seeded
balcony counterbalance contagion needle
battery wombat turban rehab
biology unbiased alcohol genius
calendar localize lending moderate
calorie local loris regal
cameo encamping mediocrity deodorant
cartographer incarnation cryptogram furry
cigarette insignificant vinegar reticence
circumference disservice income conferencing
collision semicolon listener revisionist
composer uncommon predispose reserving
congregation Yukon gaily sensational
continent misconstrue tenacious tenantry
conversion inconsequent shiver delusional
counterfeit recount altar befitting
decagon indexes capacity agony
ellipsis citadel tulip abscissa
finale infinity functional leadership
graduate pomegranate jubilant creativity
gravity programmer vituperate tiara
hibernate knee-high Berkeley natal
hurricane cowherder rickety archangel
hypochondriac Shanghai reconnect accredit
innocent reinvest bonus accented
insomnia disinclined sombrero perennially
insurance evincing pressure preferences
interrogate reincrease pterodactyl obligating
intersection coincide secretary relationship
interview uninvited terrain viewfinder
intestine reincarnate protest continue
journalist sojourner terminal enlisting
Jupiter injudicious patella Torino
literate debilitation commenter laboratory
magnetic Armageddon networking antiquity
marinade remarry rinsing tornado
metronome mimetic trochaic binomial
military similar terrific rebate
mutiny permutation tonight kneecap
nucleus maneuver Kleenex acidic
passenger impassivity sensitive juror
pharmacy dairy farming masseuse sequel
photosynthesis bifocals syncopation persistent
potpourri tempo peripheral aerial
poverty pickpocket virtuous tedious
president impressionistic visa dentition
principal apron suppressed politicize
prohibition approbate rubbish stationery
recipe heiress sipping pediatric
referee irreversible furnish rebirth
reunion arena uniqueness leniency
rhetorical regretful elector calligraphy
submarine insubstantial marital careening
summary opossum nightmare retail
synonym assassin cannon nimble
veteran Corvette tureen irony
volunteer revolution befallen materialistic
vulnerable convolution nurture baloney

References

  1. Ash S, McMillan C, Gunawardena D, Avants B, Morgan B, Khan A, Moore P, Gee J, Grossman M. Speech errors in progressive non-fluent aphasia. Brain & Language. 2011;113(1):13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.12.001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B. Lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999-0. 2012 http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
  3. Béland R, Caplan D, Nespoulous J. The role of abstract phonological representations in word production: Evidence from phonemic paraphasias. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 1990;5(2/3):125–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0911-6044(90)90009-N. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bi Y, Wei T, Janssen N, Han Z. The contribution of orthography to spoken word production: Evidence from Mandarin Chinese. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009;16(3):555–560. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.3.555. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.3.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown R, McNeill D. The “Tip of the Tongue” Phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1966;5:325–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80040-3. [Google Scholar]
  6. Buchwald A, Miozzo M. Finding levels of abstraction in speech production: Evidence from sound-production impairment. Psychological Science. 2011;22(9):1113–19. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417723. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Burke DM, MacKay DG, Worthley JS, Wade E. On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults? Journal of Memory and Language. 1991;30:542–579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90026-G. [Google Scholar]
  8. Collins AF, Ellis AW. Phonological priming of lexical retrieval in speech production. British Journal of Psychology. 1992;83(3):375–88. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02448.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02448.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Coltheart M. The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology. 1981;33(4):497–505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640748108400805. [Google Scholar]
  10. Damian MF, Bowers JS. Effects of orthography on speech production in a form-preparation paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language. 2002;49:119–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00008-1. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dell GS. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological review. 1986;93(3):283–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dell GS, Reich PA. Stages in sentence production; An analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1980;20(6):611–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90202-4. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dufour S, Peereman R. Lexical competition in phonological priming: assessing the role of phonological match and mismatch lengths between primes and targets. Memory & Cognition. 2003;31(8):1271–83. doi: 10.3758/bf03195810. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ferreira VS, Griffin ZM. Phonological influences on lexical (mis)selection. Psychological Science. 2003;14(1):86–90. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01424. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Goldrick M. Limited interaction in speech production: Chronometric, speech error, and neuropsychological evidence. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2006;21(7-8):817–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960600824112. [Google Scholar]
  16. Goldrick M, Rapp B. Lexical and post-lexical phonological representations in spoken production. Cognition. 2007;102:219–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hamburger M, Slowiaczek LM. Phonological priming reflects lexical competition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1996;3(4):520–525. doi: 10.3758/BF03214558. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03214558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Harley TA, Brown HE. What causes a tip-of-the-tongue state? Evidence for lexical neighborhood effects in speech production. British Journal of Psychology. 1998;89(1):151–174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02677.x. [Google Scholar]
  19. James LE, Burke DM. Phonological priming effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue experiences in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 2000;26(6):1378–1391. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.6.1378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.6.1378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jaeger TF. Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language. 2008;59(4):434–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jaeger TF, Furth K, Hilliard C. Phonological overlap affects lexical selection during sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 2012;38(5):1439–49. doi: 10.1037/a0027862. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Klatt DH. Voice onset time, frication, and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1975;18:686–706. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1804.686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kučera H, Francis WH. Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  24. Levelt WJM. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  25. Levelt WJM, Wheeldon L. Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? Cognition. 1994;50:239–269. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90030-2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90030-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Luce PA, Cluff MS. Delayed commitment in spoken word recognition: Evidence from cross-modal priming. Perception & Psychophysics. 1998;60(3):484–490. doi: 10.3758/bf03206868. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lukatela G, Turvey MT. Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 2. Evidence from phonological priming by homophones and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1994;4:331–53. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.123.4.331. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. MacKay DG. Spoonerisms: The structure of errors in the serial order of speech. Neuropsychologia. 1969;8:323–50. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(70)90078-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(70)90078-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mertus JA. BLISS: The Brown Lab interactive speech system. Providence, RI: Brown University; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  30. Meyer AS, Bock K. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation? Memory & Cognition. 1992;20(6):715–726. doi: 10.3758/bf03202721. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03202721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Monsell S, Hirsh KW. Competitor priming in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1998;24(6):1495–1520. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.24.6.1495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.6.1495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Morsella E, Krauss RM. Electromyography of arm during the lexical retrieval of abstract and concrete words. Psychophysiology; Poster session; 1999. p. S82. [Google Scholar]
  33. Norris D, McQueen JM, Cutler A. Bias effects in facilitatory phonological priming. Memory & Cognition. 2002;30(3):399–411. doi: 10.3758/bf03194940. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03194940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Oppenheim GM, Dell GS. Inner speech slips exhibit lexical bias, but not the phonemic similarity effect. Cognition. 2008;106(1):528–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.02.006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Perfect J, Hanley JR. The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon : Do interlopers have any effect? Cognition. 1992;45(1):55–75. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90023-b. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90023-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Postma A. Detection of errors during speech production: A review of speech monitoring models. Cognition. 2000;77(2):97–132. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00090-1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00090-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rapp B, Goldrick M. Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review. 2000;107(3):460–499. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.460. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rapp DN, Samuel AG. A reason to rhyme: Phonological and semantic influences on lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2002;28(3):564–571. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2011. http://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  40. Roelofs A. Phonological Segments and Features as Planning Units in Speech Production. Language and Cognitive Processes. 1999;14(2):173–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/016909699386338. [Google Scholar]
  41. Schiller NO. The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory & Language. 2004;50:477–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.004. [Google Scholar]
  42. Sevald CA, Dell GS. The sequential cuing effect in speech production. Cognition. 1994;53(2):91–127. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90067-1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90067-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Shattuck-Hufnagel S. The role of word-onset consonants in speech production planning: New evidence from speech error patterns. In: Keller E, Gopnik M, editors. Motor and sensory processes of language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1987. pp. 17–51. [Google Scholar]
  44. Stemberger JP. Wordshape errors in language production. Cognition. 1990;35(2):123–37. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90012-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90012-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sullivan M, Riffel B. The Nature of Phonological Encoding During Spoken Word Retrieval. Language and Cognitive Processes. 1999;14(1):15–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/016909699386365. [Google Scholar]
  46. Van Alphen PM, van Berkum JJA. Is there pain in champagne? Semantic involvement of words within words during sense-making. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:2618–26. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wheeldon L. Inhibitory form priming of spoken word production. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2003;18(1):81–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960143000470. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES