Abstract
Alzheimer's disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disease that gradually induces cognitive deficits. Impairments of working memory have been typically observed in AD. It is well known that spikes and local field potentials (LFPs) as well as the coordination between them encode information in normal brain function. However, the abnormal coordination between spikes and LFPs in the cognitive deficits of AD has remained largely unexplored. As amyloid-β peptide (Aβ) is a causative factor for the cognitive impairments of AD, developing a mechanistic understanding of the contribution of Aβ to cognitive impairments may yield important insights into the pathophysiology of AD. In the present study, we simultaneously recorded spikes and LFPs from multiple electrodes implanted in the prefrontal cortex of rats (control and intra-hippocampal Aβ injection group) that performed a Y-maze working memory task. The information changes in spikes and LFPs during the task were assessed by calculation of entropy. Then the coordination between spikes and LFPs was estimated by the correlation of LFP entropy and spike entropy. Compared with the control group, the Aβ group showed significantly weaker coordination between spikes and LFPs. Our results indicate that the incoordination between spikes and LFPs may provide a potential mechanism for the cognitive deficits in working memory of AD.
Keywords: Alzheimer's disease (AD), working memory, spike-LFP coordination, entropy correlation, rat
Introduction
Alzheimer's disease (AD), the most common cause of dementia in the elderly, is a neurodegenerative disorder that gradually induces cognitive deficit (Haffen et al., 2011). Impairment of working memory, in particular, is typically observed in AD (Baddeley et al., 1991; Kensinger et al., 2003). Working memory—a system for the temporary holding and manipulation of information—is important for a range of cognitive tasks such as learning, comprehension and reasoning (Baddeley, 1986, 2010). The research on working memory deficit is beneficial for lifting the veil of the mechanisms underlying memory deficits in AD.
Accumulating evidence has identified prefrontal cortex (PFC) as playing a critical role in working memory (Baddeley et al., 2000; Fuster, 2001; Dalley et al., 2004; Vertes, 2006; Horst and Laubach, 2009). Hippocampus is also an essential structure for working memory. Inactivation or lesion to the hippocampus produces a severe deficit in working memory (Steele and Morris, 1999; Bannerman et al., 2008; O'Neill et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2013). Furthermore, neural activity in the PFC becomes synchronized with activity in the hippocampus during working memory tasks, and the strength of hippocampal-PFC synchrony is correlated with animals' behavioral performance (Jones and Wilson, 2005; Hyman et al., 2010; Sigurdsson et al., 2010). Lesion studies have suggested such hippocampal-PFC interaction is critical for successful task performance (Izaki et al., 2008). Since Aβ is a causative factor for the cognitive impairments in AD, we expect to develop a mechanistic understanding of the effects of intra-hippocampal Aβ on PFC, which may yield important insights into the pathophysiology of AD.
Much of our mechanistic understanding of brain function comes from extracellular recordings. The neural signals recorded with extracellular microelectrodes—commonly decomposed into spikes and local field potentials (LFPs)—are measurements for studying the spatiotemporal organization of information processing circuits underlying learning and memory. Accumulated evidence has suggested that spikes and LFPs in cortical circuits encode information in cognitive processes (Pesaran et al., 2002; Baeg et al., 2003; Lawhern et al., 2011; Li et al., 2012) as well as cooperatively encode cognitive information (Lee et al., 2005; Berens et al., 2008; Zanos et al., 2012). Elucidating the relationship between spikes and LFPs therefore has important implications for understanding the properties of in vivo cortical neurons, the links between single-cell and network activity, and the organization of cortical circuits (Zanos et al., 2012).
Popular methods for studying spike-LFP coordination include spike-triggered averaging, spike-field coherence and phase synchronization. These analyses have been proven useful for inferring the role of feedforward and feedback circuitry in functions as diverse as memory, perception, attention, and motor control (Jacobs et al., 2007; Chalk et al., 2010; Saleh et al., 2010; Ray and Maunsell, 2011; Zanos et al., 2011). Since neural responses are generally non-linear, information entropy was proposed as useful tool for extracting the non-linear characteristics of neural responses in cognitive functions. The entropy of a random variable is defined in terms of its probability distribution and can be shown to be a good measure of the randomness or the uncertainty (Strong et al., 1998; Quiroga and Panzeri, 2009). Entropy has been widely applied to analyze electrophysiological data and explore neural information characteristics (Rosso, 2007; Belitski et al., 2008; Kayser et al., 2009; Dorval, 2011). In particular, entropy has been used for the analysis of EEGs and MEGs in AD (Dauwels et al., 2010; Gómez and Hornero, 2010; Mizuno et al., 2010; Bruña et al., 2012). A growing body of research indicates that entropy-based analytic methods may be effective approaches for characterizing and understanding abnormal cortical dynamics in AD (Tsai et al., 2012; Chen and Pham, 2013; Yang et al., 2013; McBride et al., 2014). Our previous study has revealed strengthened spike-LFP coordination during working memory tasks in healthy subjects with normal brain function (Li et al., 2014). It is reasonable to study the spike-LFP coordination in working memory deficit, because the abnormal coordination between spikes and LFPs may be a candidate mechanism for the pathological progression of AD.
Therefore, in the present study, we simultaneously recorded the spikes and LFPs in the PFC of rats (normal and intra-hippocampal Aβ injection groups) while the rats performed a Y-maze working memory task. We then examined the coordination between the spikes and LFPs based on Shannon entropy, to study how the spikes and LFPs cooperatively encode information during the working memory task and obtain insights into how abnormal neural coordination could contribute to the memory deficits in AD.
Materials and methods
All surgical and experimental procedures conformed to the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Tianjin Medical University Animal Care and Use Committee. The drug usage in the experiments complied with the Chinese Pharmacopeia (2010 edition), approved by Chinese Pharmacopeia Commission.
Subjects
Sprague-Dawley rats (male, 300–350 g, 12–14 weeks) were obtained from the Experimental Animal Center of Tianjin Medical University. Rats were housed in plastic cages (3–4 per cage) in a climate-controlled room (24°C) with a 12 h light/12 h dark cycle. Food and water were available ad libitum. The rats were divided randomly into two groups: Group-I (Aβ group) was comprised of Aβ1−42-induced toxicity rats, bilaterally injected with Aβ1−42 in the dentate gyrus (DG) area of dorsal hippocampus; Group-II (control group) was comprised of healthy rats, bilaterally injected with saline (pH 7.4).
Aβ1−42-induced toxicity rat model
Diverse lines of evidence suggested that Aβ deposition has a causal role in inducing neuronal dysfunction and cognitive decline in AD (Palop and Mucke, 2010; Karran et al., 2011). In previous studies, intra-hippocampal injection with Aβ in rodents has been widely used as a model for AD since measurable Aβ deposition is associated with persistent memory decline (Nomura et al., 2012). The details of model establishment have been described previously (Tan et al., 2013).
Aβ1−42 peptide (Sigma, USA) was prepared from 1 μg/μl soluble Aβ1−42 solution, which was dissolved in filtered phosphate buffered saline (PBS: 10 mM NaH2PO4 \ Na2HPO4, 100mM NaCl, dissolved in glass-distilled deionized water, pH = 7.5). Aβ1−42 solution was then incubated under vigorous agitation using a Tefion-coated stirbar at 23°C for 36 h. Then, the incubated Aβ1−42 solution was ready for injection. For Aβ1−42 injection, the rats were anesthetized with chloral hydrate (300 mg/kg, i.p.) and placed in a stereotaxic apparatus. Incubated Aβ1−42 (5 μl, 1 μg/μl) was injected into DG of dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) bilaterally (anterior posterior, 3.2 mm; lateral, 2.5 mm; horizontal, 3.5 mm from bregma) (Christensen et al., 2008). Rats with subsequent memory deficits as identified using a Y-maze test were considered to have Aβ1−42-induced toxicity model.
Immunohistochemistry
We investigated whether the incubated Aβ1−42 had been successfully injected into the hippocampus in the Aβ1−42injection group using immunohistochemistry, as described previously (Tan et al., 2013). Rats were deeply anesthetized and transcardially perfused with PBS (0.01 mol/l) and fixative (4% paraformaldehyde, 0.2% picric acid, diluted in 0.1 mol/l phosphate buffer, pH = 7.4). Rat brains were dissected and post-fixed in 4% PFA (diluted in 0.1 mol/l PB buffer) for 24 h at 4°C. Brains were embedded in paraffin and cut into 5-μm coronal sections. The sections were then dewaxed in xylene and rehydrated in a series of graded alcohols according to histopathological standards. To remove residual peroxidase activity, the sections were treated with H2O2 (3%, for 30 min) and rinsed with PBS. Microwave antigen retrieval was applied with slides immersed in 10 mM citrate buffer (pH = 6.0). Slides were blocked with 10% normal goat serum, and incubated with rabbit polyclonal anti-Aβ1−42 antibody (1:250, Abcam 10148) at 4°C overnight and then incubated with corresponding biotinylated secondary antibodies (1:200). The immunoreactivity was developed using DAB for 3–10 mins.
Delayed-alternation task in Y-maze
Fourteen days after Aβ1−42/salineinjection, all the rats were trained on a delayed-alternation Y-maze task. First, the rats were acclimatized to the handling procedures for 2 days. Next, food access was limited to 2 h per day. The quantity of food was adjusted to maintain body weights at 85% of individual baseline free-feeding weights, adjusted for growth. Water was freely available in cages. Following habituation, the groups of rats were trained on a delayed-alternation task in Y-maze. The rats were given two training sessions (10 trials per session) per day. Each trial consisted of a sample run and a choice run. On the sample run, the rats were allowed to go either left or right to get a small piece of food reward in the food well at the end of the arm. After consuming the reward, the rat voluntarily went back to the start of Y-maze. After 5 s delay, the rats would have a “choice run.” The rats were rewarded for choosing the previously unvisited arm. After completing a trial, the rats were allowed to return and start next trial. To perform the task correctly, rats had to remember which arm had been visited in the previous trial and select the opposite one. For the control group, electrophysiological recording was initiated once rats' performance was stable at ~80% correct on two consecutive days. For the Aβ group, regardless of whether the rats could reach the criterion, the rats received chronic implants after same training sessions as control. The experimental procedures are shown in Figure 1.
Electrophysiological recordings and data processing
After training, all the rats underwent a chronic implant surgery. The coordinates for PFC were determined according to the rat brain atlas in stereotaxic coordinates (mPFC, anterior, 2.5–4.5 mm; lateral, 0.2–1.0 mm; horizontal, 2.5–3.0 mm from bregma). 16-channel microelectrode arrays (2*0.3 mm, with 0.25 mm interelectrode spacing, nickel-chromium, <1 MΩ) were implanted into rat mPFC under aseptic conditions and chloral hydrate (350 mg/kg) anesthesia.
After recovery, neural activity was recorded while rats again performed the delayed-alternation Y-maze task. Wideband neural signals were recorded with a Cerebus Data Acquisition System (Cyberkinetics, USA). The timing of behavioral events was marked online by an infrared sensor in the Y-maze. Time 0 indicates the moment when the rat was at the choice point on Y-maze, which was measured by an infrared sensor. Local field potentials (low-pass filter: 0.3–300 Hz) were extracted via digital filters in the Neural Signal Processor. Spikes (high-pass filter: 250–7500 Hz) exceeding a preset voltage threshold were sampled at 30 kHz per channel and were stored with time stamps. Units with low signal-to-noise (<3.0) or a very low baseline firing rate (<30 spikes/min) were discarded. The data analysis workflow is shown in Figure 2.
Data analysis and statistics
We recorded spikes and LFPs from 8 rats (4 control group; 4 Aβ group) for 2 sessions using 16-channel implanted electrode arrays while they performed the Y-maze working memory task. Time 0 indicates the moment when the rat was at the choice point on Y-maze, which was measured by an infrared sensor. In total, we describe 122 trials (66 trials with control and 56 trials with Aβ rats) in the present paper. Data in the text and figures are expressed as means ± s.e.m. Statistical differences were evaluated by using ANOVA and t-test (Student's t-test/Welch-Satterthwaite t-test). Specifically, behavioral accuracy was analyzed using ANOVA. Comparisons of entropy and spike-LFP coordination between the two groups were done by using t-test. Comparisons of entropy and spike-LFP coordination between correct and incorrect trials were evaluated by using t-test. P-values are marked statistically significant as follows: *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.
Time-frequency spectral analysis
Recorded LFPs were first filtered by a 50 Hz notching filter and baseline drifts were removed. Spectral analysis was used to assess the dominant frequencies in the LFPs during the task. All spectra are calculated using multi-tapers. To illustrate the temporal modulation of power in different frequency bands, the LFP spectrum was estimated on a 500-ms window (125-ms moving step, 1 Hz resolution). Then the sub-bands were obtained by filtering the raw LFPs.
Entropy estimation of spikes and LFPs
The variability of LFP (in particular theta and gamma bands) and spikes were quantified by estimating Shannon entropy. The mathematical computation of entropy has been described in a previous paper (Shannon, 1948). When estimating the LFP entropy on each trial, we used a sliding window (500 ms) with a moving step (125 ms). And in each window, we got a distribution of LFP amplitudes in each channel. The entropy of LFP in each channel was estimated and then averaged over channels.
In each window, the amplitudes of LFPs were binned and the distribution of amplitude values was estimated with a histogram. The number of amplitudes in the i-th bin was counted (Ai) and the probability of amplitude values in the i-th bin (i = 1, 2, …, n) calculated as following:
(1) |
Entropy of the LFP amplitude values distribution was defined using Shannon's formula:
(2) |
where H(X) denotes the entropy of LFP amplitude distribution, and Pi denotes the relative frequency of the i-th bin. The entropy of LFP in each channel was estimated and then averaged over different channels. The same method was used to compute the entropy of theta- and gamma- band LFPs.
To estimate the entropy of spikes (representing the randomness of the distribution of population spiking), the inter-spike intervals (ISIs) were measured and the distribution of ISIs was estimated in a histogram with a 0.5 ms bin width. The entropy of the spike train in each channel was estimated and averaged over channels. Then the number of spikes in the i-th bin was counted (Si) and the firing probability of the i-th bin (i = 1, 2, …, n) was calculated:
(3) |
The entropy of the spiking was computed using Shannon's formula:
(4) |
where H(X) denotes the entropy of spiking distribution, and Pi denotes the firing probability of the i-th bin. The entropy values of the spikes and LFPs were estimated in a selected window (500 ms) with a moving step (125 ms) across the entire task.
Estimation of spike-LFP correlation based on entropy
The coordination between spikes and LFPs were assessed via the Pearson correlation between spike entropy and LFP entropy. This was done separately for each trial, and for the theta and gamma bands.
We estimated the correlation between the spike entropy and LFP entropy across the entire task using the formula as follows:
(5) |
where i denotes the i-th window and Xi, Yi denotes the spike entropy and LFP entropy in the i-th window. The entropy correlation of spikes and LFPs were calculated in 500-ms windows across the entire task.
Results
Immunohistochemistry on Aβ1−42-induced toxicity model
First, we investigated whether Aβ1−42 could be detected in the hippocampus with Aβ1−42 intra-hippocampal injection. Using immunohistochemistry, we found a number of Aβ1−42 deposits in the hippocampus (Figure 3). The results indicated that the incubated Aβ1−42 had been successfully injected into rat hippocampus and amyloid plaques depositions were observed.
Notably, we noticed the Aβ diffusion after Aβ injection in hippocampus. To clarify the extent of Aβ diffusion, we further quantified Aβ deposition. The quantification was performed using the image processing system to analyze the area occupied by positive immunostaining. We found the most (85.07 ± 1.11% of total positive areas) Aβ deposits in the dorsal and ventral hippocampus as well as a small amount (14.93 ± 1.11%) deposits outside of the hippocampus proper, in the lateral habenula (LHb) and thalamus.
Behavioral performance
The performance of each rat in each training session was measured by the accuracy of responses (percent correct, Figure 4). The response accuracy in the Aβ group was significantly worse than the control group [ANOVA, F(1, 6) = 82.620, P < 0.05]. The percentage of correctly completed trials increased as training progressed in both control (black) and Aβ group (red). For the control group, in eight training sessions, the behavioral performance gradually reached a criterion level of performance (80% correct for two consecutive days). However, none of the rats from the Aβ group reached the criterion. During the recording sessions, the response accuracy in the Aβ group (64.47 ± 3.82%) was also worse than the control group (86.11± 5.77%) (t-test, t = 3.608, P < 0.05).
Power changes of LFPs in rat mPFC during the working memory task
Spectral analysis was used to assess the dominant frequencies in the LFPs during the task. To illustrate the temporal modulation of power in different frequency bands, the LFP spectrum was estimated on a 500-ms window with 1 Hz resolution. Figure 5A shows exemplar LFP of a single recording site during the working memory task. Time-frequency power spectrums are illustrated for two trials when the rat succeeded (Figure 5B) and when the rat failed (Figure 5C). As can be seen in Figure 5B, theta power was much larger than gamma power all through the task. Interestingly, both theta and gamma power increased during correct trials. However, on incorrect trials, the power was much lower than on correct trials and the theta and gamma power did not increase during the trials. The sub-bands were obtained by band-pass filtering (theta: 4–12 Hz; gamma: 30–60 Hz, shown in Figure 5D).
Dynamic entropy of neural activity in rat PFC during the working memory task
The entropy value strongly depends on the bin size, especially for the estimation of spike entropy. For the purpose of entropy estimation, a bin width of 0.5 ms was employed in the present paper, determined in accordance with the literature (Reeke and Coop, 2004). To assess the influence of bin width on the entropy value, we plotted the entropy value with different bin sizes and then linearly extrapolate the curve to the point that the bin size is zero, following the methods of Brenner et al. (2000). As can be seen from Figure 6, entropy values decreased slightly with the increasing bin size and entropy values did not change when the bin sizes <0.5 ms. Therefore, a bin width of 0.5 ms was employed.
We then analyzed the dynamic entropy changes of neural activity in rat PFC during the working memory task in Aβ and control groups. In the control group, across the whole task time, the entropy values of spikes and LFPs (theta and gamma bands) were observed to increase up until the choice point, and then decline. The peaks of the entropy appeared before the choice point in the trials. However, there was no statistically significant change in the Aβ group (Figure 7). Further analyses were performed to compare the peaks and averages of entropy between the Aβ and control groups (Table 1). The peaks and averages of spike entropy in Aβ group were significantly lower than those in control (peak: t-test, t = 9.155, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 11.664, P < 0.01; Figures 7A–D). The peaks and averages of LFPtheta entropy in Aβ group were significantly higher than those in control (peak: t-test, t = 3.200, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 2.655, P < 0.01; Figures 7E–H). Moreover, the peaks and averages of LFPgamma entropy in control were significantly higher than those in Aβ group (peak: t-test, t = 4.224, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 4.437, P < 0.01; Figures 7I–L).
Table 1.
Aβ | Control | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peak | Averaged | Peak | Averaged | |
Spike entropy | 0.197 ± 0.012** | 0.118 ± 0.003** | 0.271 ± 0.008 | 0.206 ± 0.006 |
LFPtheta entropy | 0.897 ± 0.014** | 0.754 ± 0.017** | 0.949 ± 0.011 | 0.792 ± 0.008 |
LFPgamma entropy | 0.893 ± 0.013** | 0.836 ± 0.010** | 0.943 ± 0.008 | 0.855 ± 0.005 |
All entropy values in bit.
Data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.
P < 0.01 for tests of significant difference.
Estimation of spike-LFP coordination based on entropy
Spike-LFP coordination was assessed by estimating the correlation between spike entropy and LFP entropy, across the entire task time. Results in all panels (Figure 8) report data collected during the trials and show the average over the entire dataset. In the control group, across the whole task time, correlations (in both theta and gamma band) were observed to increase up until the choice point, and then to decrease. Moreover, the correlations were highest during the period from 2 to 0 s prior to the choice-point crossing. The spike-LFPtheta correlation in Aβ group was significantly lower than that in control group (peak: t-test, t = 8.648, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 9.954, P < 0.01; Figures 8A–C). Moreover, the spike-LFPgamma correlation in control was significantly higher than that in Aβ group (peak: t-test, t = 9.823, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 9.731, P < 0.01; Figures 8D–F, Table 2).
Table 2.
Aβ | Control | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peak | Averaged | Peak | Averaged | |
Spike-LFPtheta | 0.329 ± 0.015** | 0.353 ± 0.010** | 0.475 ± 0.012 | 0.217 ± 0.013 |
Spike-LFPgamma | 0.320 ± 0.016** | 0.216 ± 0.012** | 0.484 ± 0.013 | 0.358 ± 0.010 |
Data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.
P < 0.01 for tests of significant difference.
Spike-LFP coordination on correct and incorrect trials in the control group
The overall success rates of the rats in the control group were generally high. We observed a stereotypical spike-LFP coordination: a pattern of increase, peak, and decline in correctly performed trials. To obtain insights into the nature of information conveyed in correct trials, we further analyzed the neuronal activity and compared the spike-LFP coordination between correct and incorrect trials.
As shown in Figure 9, in correct trials (n = 56), the entropy of spikes/LFPs were observed to increase up until the choice point, and then decline. The peaks appeared before the choice point. However, there was no significant difference in the entropy of spikes/LFPs in the incorrect trials (n = 10). Further statistic analyses were performed to compare the peaks and averages of entropy between the correct and incorrect trials (Table 3). The spike entropy values in correct trials were significantly higher than those in incorrect trials (peak: t-test, t = 3.124, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 2.903, P < 0.01; Figures 9A–C). The LFPtheta entropy and LFPgamma entropy in correct trials were also higher than those in incorrect trials (theta: peak: t-test, t = 2.287, P < 0.05; average: t-test, t = 2.242, P < 0.05; Figures 9D–F; gamma: peak: t-test, t = 2.142, P < 0.05; average: t-test, t = 2.128, P < 0.05; Figures 9G–I).
Table 3.
Correct | Incorrect | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peak | Averaged | Peak | Averaged | |
Spike entropy | 0.271 ± 0.008 | 0.206 ± 0.006 | 0.208 ± 0.006** | 0.161 ± 0.005** |
LFPtheta entropy | 0.949 ± 0.011 | 0.792 ± 0.008 | 0.883 ± 0.025* | 0.741 ± 0.017* |
LFPgamma entropy | 0.943 ± 0.008 | 0.855 ± 0.005 | 0.912 ± 0.009* | 0.843 ± 0.005* |
All entropy values in bit.
Data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.
P < 0.05,
P < 0.01 for tests of significant difference.
In the control group, the spike-LFP coordination in correct trials was observed to increase up until the choice point, and then to decline. However, no significant change was found in the correlation for the incorrect trials (Table 4). The spike-LFPtheta correlations in correct trials were significantly higher than those in incorrect trials (peak: t-test, t = 5.183, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 4.489, P < 0.01; Figures 9J–L). The spike-LFPgamma correlations in control were also higher than those in incorrect trials (peak: t-test, t = 5.923, P < 0.01; average: t-test, t = 5.408, P < 0.01; Figures 9M–O). Since increasing tendencies of spike-LFP coordination were found in correct trials while there was no significant change in incorrect trials, we propose that the strengthened spike-LFP coordination could be necessary for information manipulation in working memory.
Table 4.
Correct | Incorrect | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peak | Averaged | Peak | Averaged | |
Spike-LFPtheta | 0.475 ± 0.012 | 0.353 ± 0.010 | 0.328 ± 0.017** | 0.242 ± 0.018** |
Spike-LFPgamma | 0.484 ± 0.013 | 0.358 ± 0.010 | 0.298 ± 0.019** | 0.225 ± 0.015** |
Data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.
P < 0.01 for tests of significant difference.
Spike-LFP coordination on correct and incorrect trials in the Aβ group
The success rates of the rats in the Aβ group were relatively low and as the spike-LFP coordination showed no statistically significant change. To determine whether the spike-LFP coordination is associated with correct/incorrect performances, we further compared the spike-LFP coordination on correct and incorrect trials. In the Aβ group, as shown in Figure 10, there was no statistically significant difference in spikes/LFPs entropy between the correct and incorrect trials.
The spike entropy in correct (n = 35) and incorrect (n = 21) trials showed no significant difference (peak: t-test, t = 0.453, P > 0.05; average: t-test, t = 1.039, P > 0.05). The LFPtheta entropy (peak: t-test, t = 1.271, P > 0.05; average: t-test, t = 0.846, P > 0.05) and LFPgamma entropy (peak: t-test, t = 1.588, P > 0.05; average: t-test, t = 0.366, P > 0.05) in correct and incorrect trials showed no significant difference (Table 5). Moreover, the spike-LFP correlations in correct and incorrect trials showed no significant difference (theta: peak: t-test, t = 0.453, P > 0.05; average: t-test, t = 1.039, P > 0.05; gamma: t-test, t = 0.050, P > 0.05; average: t-test, t = 0.250, P > 0.05; Table 6). The results indicated that the spike-LFP coordination showed no significant difference, whether the rats in Aβ group performed the task correctly or incorrectly.
Table 5.
Correct | Incorrect | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peak | Averaged | Peak | Averaged | |
Spike entropy | 0.174 ± 0.008 | 0.116 ± 0.006 | 0.167 ± 0.011 | 0.105 ± 0.008 |
LFPtheta entropy | 0.890 ± 0.012 | 0.754 ± 0.010 | 0.913 ± 0.017 | 0.770 ± 0.014 |
LFPgamma entropy | 0.889 ± 0.009 | 0.826 ± 0.005 | 0.910 ± 0.012 | 0.827 ± 0.007 |
All entropy values in bit.
Data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.
Table 6.
Correct | Incorrect | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Peak | Averaged | Peak | Averaged | |
Spike-LFPtheta | 0.329 ± 0.015 | 0.216 ± 0.007 | 0.306 ± 0.026 | 0.202 ± 0.010 |
Spike-LFPgamma | 0.295 ± 0.018 | 0.204 ± 0.008 | 0.294 ± 0.024 | 0.210 ± 0.010 |
Data are shown as mean ± s.e.m.
Discussion
Our results show that the spike-LFP coordination in the control group strengthened during working memory and the spike-LFP coordination in the Aβ group was weaker than that in the control group. We propose that the strengthened spike-LFP coordination is necessary for information manipulation in working memory and the Aβ-induced incoordination between spikes and LFPs causes the working memory impairment. Multi-disciplinary research has implicated Aβ with the cognitive impairments in AD and the mechanistic understanding of the ability of Aβ to interfere with synaptic plasticity and memory yields important insights into the pathophysiology of AD. Therefore, the findings on incoordination between spikes and LFPs may open a new perspective for mechanistic investigation of working memory deficits in AD.
Notably, Aβ was injected into dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) and were observed in dorsal and ventral hippocampus (dHPC and vHPC) and in LHb and thalamus. Since Aβ deposit was not shown in PFC, how this could affect spike-LFP coordinations in PFC?
As the most Aβ deposits were found in hippocampus after Aβ injection, the neuronal activity in mPFC was primarily affected by the alteration of hippocampus function. Accumulating evidence has suggested the hippocampal-PFC interaction is critical for successful task performance (Jones and Wilson, 2005; Izaki et al., 2008; Hyman et al., 2010; Sigurdsson et al., 2010; O'Neill et al., 2013). It is well recognized that the vHPC directly projects to the PFC (Hoover and Vertes, 2007). Inactivating the vHPC led to a reduction in hippocampal- PFC coherence and impaired working memory function (O'Neill et al., 2013). However, the dHPC does not project directly to the PFC (Gordon, 2011). The information from the dHPC must arrive at the mPFC through an indirect route and the vHPC may facilitate spatial working memory by relaying spatial information from the dHPC to the mPFC. A recent study has identified that the dHPC can affect the mPFC through indirect pathways (Zingg et al., 2014), which can explain how Aβ injection in the dHPC alters the neuronal functioning in the PFC.
Meanwhile, we also noticed a small amount deposits outside of the hippocampus proper, in the LHb and thalamus. These two structures are known to play roles in memory and are connected to the PFC, so the Aβ deposits in the two structures may induce the mPFC alteration. Since the information processing in cortico-hippocampal areas in cognitive behaviors is influenced by various neuromodulators (including dopamine, serotonin etc.) and the release of these neuromodulators is under the control of the LHb (Lecourtier and Kelly, 2007; Hikosaka, 2010), the dysfunction in LHb may induce the cognitive impairments (Lecourtier et al., 2004, 2006). On the other hand, thalamus links the multiple pathways in multiple cognitive processes and some thalamic neurons project to the PFC, so the thalamus is also in a good position to influence the PFC activity (Otake and Nakamura, 1998; Sesack and Grace, 2010). Therefore, the dysfunction in LHb and thalamus might partly have contributed to the PFC alteration.
Another interesting result in this study is the lack of differential modulation between correct and incorrect choices in Aβ injected animals, compared to the healthy rats. For the control group, the spike-LFP coordination strengthened on the correct trials. The results were not completely surprising since working memory has long been linked with an increase of power in the theta- and gamma-band in human hippocampus and cortex (Tesche and Karhu, 2000; Bastiaansen et al., 2002; Düzel et al., 2010) and monkey extrastriate visual cortex (Lee et al., 2005). Successful memory encoding is associated with increased synchronization of theta- and gamma-band oscillations in human (Friese et al., 2012). In particular, the activation of working memory is characterized by both increases in theta- and gamma-band synchronization in EEGs (Klimesch, 1996) and MEGs in human (Stam et al., 2002). Moreover, a previous study has reported strengthened spike-LFP coordination in healthy rats during working memory (Li et al., 2014). The results in the present paper are consistent with these previous findings. The results also revealed weaker spike-LFP coordination on incorrect trials, compared with correctly performed trials. Moreover, the spike-LFP coordination experienced the pattern of increase, peak, and decline on correct trials. By contrast, there was no significant difference on incorrect trials. However, for the Aβ group, the spike-LFP coordination showed no significant difference on either correct or incorrect trials. A possible explanation is that, for healthy subjects, the stronger spike-LFP coordination successfully encoded working memory information on correct trials while the weaker spike-LFP coordination failed to encode on the incorrect trials. However, for the Aβ-injected rats, the Aβ deposits induced synaptic plasticity failure (Small et al., 2001; Ma and Klann, 2012) in hippocampus and caused the dysfunction of hippocampal- PFC circuit, which further contributed to the lack of modulation in spike-LFP coordination.
In summary, our results demonstrate that the spike-LFP coordination in healthy subjects increases during the working memory task. In contrast, the spike-LFP coordination in Aβ group was weaker and did not significantly change across the entire task. The incoordination between spikes and LFPs in working memory deficits may thus provide a potential mechanism for cognitive deficits in AD.
Author contributions
Xin Tian designed the experiment; Wenwen Bai, Tiaotiao Liu, Hu Yi, and Jing Wei carried out the experiments; Wenwen Bai analyzed the data; Wenwen Bai and Xin Tian wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Joseph E. O'Doherty for proofreading the manuscript and providing insightful comments and Tao Tan for assistance in immunohistochemistry. This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (91132722, 61375113, and 61401311).
References
- Baddeley A. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. (2010). Working memory. Curr. Biol. 20, 136–140 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A., Bueno O., Cahill L., Fuster J. M., Izquierdo I., McGaugh J. L., et al. (2000). The brain decade in debate: I. Neurobiology of learning and memory. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 33, 993–1002. 10.1590/S0100-879X2000000900002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A. D., Bressi S., Della Sala S., Logie R., Spinnler H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer's disease. A longitudinal study. Brain 114(Pt 6), 2521–2542. 10.1093/brain/114.6.2521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baeg E. H., Kim Y. B., Huh K., Mook-Jung I., Kim H. T., Jung M. W. (2003). Dynamics of population code for working memory in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 40, 177–188. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00597-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bannerman D. M., Niewoehner B., Lyon L., Romberg C., Schmitt W. B., Taylor A., et al. (2008). NMDA receptor subunit 2A is required for rapidly acquired spatial working memory but not incremental spatial reference memory. J. Neurosci. 28, 3623–3630. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3639-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastiaansen M. C., van Berkum J. J., Hagoort P. (2002). Syntactic processing modulates the theta rhythm of the human EEG. Neuroimage 17, 1479–1492. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belitski A., Gretton A., Magri C., Murayama Y., Montemurro M. A., Logothetis N. K., et al. (2008). Low-frequency local field potentials and spikes in primary visual cortex convey independent visual information. J. Neurosci. 28, 5696–5709. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0009-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berens P., Keliris G. A., Ecker A. S., Logothetis N. K., Tolias A. S. (2008). Comparing the feature selectivity of the gamma-band of the local field potential and the underlying spiking activity in primate visual cortex. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2:2. 10.3389/neuro.06.002.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brenner N., Strong S. P., Koberle R., Bialek W., de Ruyter van Steveninck R. R. (2000). Synergy in a neural code. Neural Comput. 12, 1531–1552. 10.1162/089976600300015259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruña R., Poza J., Gómez C., García M., Fernández A., Hornero R. (2012). Analysis of spontaneous MEG activity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease using spectral entropies and statistical complexity measures. J. Neural Eng. 9:036007. 10.1088/1741-2560/9/3/036007t [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chalk M., Herrero J. L., Gieselmann M. A., Delicato L. S., Gotthardt S., Thiele A. (2010). Attention reduces stimulus-driven gamma frequency oscillations and spike field coherence in V1. Neuron 66, 114–125. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Y., Pham T. D. (2013). Sample entropy and regularity dimension in complexity analysis of cortical surface structure in early Alzheimer's disease and aging. J. Neurosci. Methods. 215, 210–217. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen R., Marcussen A. B., Wortwein G., Knudsen G. M., Aznar S. (2008). Abeta (1-42) injection causes memory impairment, lowered cortical and serum BDNF levels, and decreased hippocampal 5-HT (2A) levels. Exp. Neurol. 210, 164–171. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley J. W., Cardinal R. N., Robbins T. W. (2004). Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 771–784. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauwels J., Vialatte F., Musha T., Cichocki A. (2010). A comparative study of synchrony measures for the early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease based on EEG. Neuroimage 49, 668–693. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dorval A. D. (2011). Estimating neuronal information: logarithmic binning of neuronal inter-spike intervals. Entropy 13, 485–501. 10.3390/e13020485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Düzel E., Penny W. D., Burgess N. (2010). Brain oscillations and memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 143–149. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friese U., Köster M., Hassler U., Martens U., Trujillo-Barreto N., Gruber T. (2012). Successful memory encoding is associated with increased cross-frequency coupling between frontal theta and posterior gamma oscillations in human scalp-recorded EEG. Neuroimage 66C, 642–647. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex—an update: time is of the essence. Neuron 30, 319–333. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00285-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gómez C., Hornero R. (2010). Entropy and complexity analyses in Alzheimer's disease: an MEG study. Open Biomed. Eng. J. 4, 223–235. 10.2174/1874120701004010223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon J. A. (2011). Oscillations and hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 486–491. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.02.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haffen E., Chopard G., Pretalli J. B., Magnin E., Nicolier M., Monnin J., et al. (2011). A case report of daily left prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) as an adjunctive treatment for Alzheimer disease. Brain Stimul. 5, 264–266. 10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hikosaka O. (2010). The habenula: from stress evasion to value-based decision- making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 503–513. 10.1038/nrn2866 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoover W. B., Vertes R. P. (2007). Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. Brain Struct. Funct. 212:149–179. 10.1007/s00429-007-0150-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horst N. K., Laubach M. (2009). The role of rat dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in spatial working memory. Neuroscience 164, 444–456. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyman J. M., Zilli E. A., Paley A. M., Hasselmo M. E. (2010). Working memory performance correlates with prefrontal- hippocampal theta interactions but not with prefrontal neuron firing rates. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 4:2. 10.3389/neuro.07.002.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izaki Y., Takita M., Akema T. (2008). Specific role of the posterior dorsal hippocampus-prefrontal cortex in short-term working memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 3029–3034. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06284.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs J., Kahana M. J., Ekstrom A. D., Fried I. (2007). Brain oscillations control timing of single-neuron activity in humans. J. Neurosci. 27, 3839–3844. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4636-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones M. W., Wilson M. A. (2005). Theta rhythms coordinate hippocampal -prefrontal interactions in a spatial memory task. PLoS Biol. 3:e402. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karran E., Mercken M., De Strooper B. (2011). The amyloid cascade hypothesis for Alzheimer's disease: an appraisal for the development of therapeutics. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 10, 698–712. 10.1038/nrd3505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser C., Montemurro M. A., Logothetis N. K., Panzeri S. (2009). Spike-phase coding boosts and stabilizes information carried by spatial and temporal spike patterns. Neuron 61, 597–608. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger E. A., Shearer D. K., Locascio J. J., Growdon J. H., Corkin S. (2003). Working memory in mild Alzheimer's disease and early Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology 17, 230–239. 10.1037/0894-4105.17.2.230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 61–100. 10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00057-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawhern V., Nikonov A. A., Wu W., Contreras R. J. (2011). Spike rate and spike timing contributions to coding taste quality information in rat periphery. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 5:18. 10.3389/fnint.2011.00018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lecourtier L., Deschaux O., Arnaud C., Chessel A., Kelly P. H., Garcia R. (2006). Habenula lesions alter synaptic plasticity within the fimbria-accumbens pathway in the rat. Neuroscience. 141, 1025–1032. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lecourtier L., Kelly P. H. (2007). A conductor hidden in the orchestra? Role of the habenular complex in monoamine transmission and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 658–672. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lecourtier L., Neijt H. C., Kelly P. H. (2004). Habenula lesions cause impaired cognitive performance in rats:implications for schizophrenia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 2551–2560. 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03356.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H., Simpson G. V., Logothetis N. K., Rainer G. (2005). Phase locking of single neuron activity to theta oscillations during working memory in monkey extrastriate visual cortex. Neuron 45, 147–156. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S., Bai W., Liu T., Yi H., Tian X. (2012). Increases of theta-low gamma coupling in rat medial prefrontal cortex during working memory task. Brain Res. Bull. 89, 115–123. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S., Ouyang M., Liu T., Bai W., Yi H., Tian X. (2014). Increase of spike-LFP coordination in rat prefrontal cortex during working memory. Behav. Brain Res. 261, 297–304. 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma T., Klann E. (2012). Amyloid beta: linking synaptic plasticity failure to memory disruption in Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurochem. 120(Suppl. 1), 140–148. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07506.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McBride J. C., Zhao X., Munro N. B., Smith C. D., Jicha G. A., Hively L., et al. (2014). Spectral and complexity analysis of scalp EEG characteristics for mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 114, 153–163. 10.1016/j.cmpb.2014.01.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuno T., Takahashi T., Cho R. Y., Kikuchi M., Murata T., Takahashi K., et al. (2010). Assessment of EEG dynamical complexity in Alzheimer's disease using multiscale entropy. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1438–1446. 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nomura I., Takechi H., Kato N. (2012). Intraneuronally injected amyloid beta inhibits longterm potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 2526–2531. 10.1152/jn.00589.2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Neill P. K., Gordon J. A., Sigurdsson T. (2013). Theta oscillations in the medial prefrontal cortex are modulated by spatial working memory and synchronize with the hippocampus through its ventral subregion. J. Neurosci. 33, 14211–14224. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2378-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otake K., Nakamura Y. (1998). Single midline thalamic neurons projecting to both the ventral striatum and the prefrontal cortex in the rat. Neuroscience 86: 635–649. 10.1016/S0306-4522(98)00062-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palop J. J., Mucke L. (2010). Amyloid-β induced neuronal dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: from synapses toward neural networks. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 812–818. 10.1038/nn.2583 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G., Watson C. (2005). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. 5th Edn London: Elsevier Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pesaran B., Pezaris J. S., Sahani M., Mitra P. P., Andersen R. A. (2002). Temporal structure in neuronal activity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 805–811. 10.1038/nn890 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga R. Q., Panzeri S. (2009). Extracting information from neuronal populations: information theory and decoding approaches. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 173–185. 10.1038/nrn2578 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray S., Maunsell J. H. (2011). Network rhythms influence the relationship between spike-triggered local field potential and functional connectivity. J. Neurosci. 31, 12674–12682. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1856-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reeke G., Coop A. (2004). Estimating the temporal interval entropy of neuronal discharge. Neural Comput.16, 941–970. 10.1162/089976604773135050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosso O. A. (2007). Entropy changes in brain function. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 64, 75–80. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleh M., Reimer J., Penn R., Ojakangas C. L., Hatsopoulos N. G. (2010). Fast and slow oscillations in human primary motor cortex predict oncoming behaviorally relevant cues. Neuron 65, 461–471. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sesack S. R., Grace A. A. (2010). Cortico-basal ganglia reward network: microcircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 27–47. 10.1038/npp.2009.93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shannon C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. AT&T Bell Labs Tech. J. 27, 379–423 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1948.tb01338.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sigurdsson T., Stark K. L., Karayiorgou M., Gogos J. A., Gordon J. A. (2010). Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature 464, 763–767. 10.1038/nature08855 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small D. H., Mok S. S., Bornstein J. C. (2001). Alzheimer's disease and abeta toxicity: from top to bottom. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 595–598. 10.1038/35086072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stam C. J., van Cappellen van Walsum A. M., Pijnenburg Y. A., Berendse H. W., de Munck J. C., Scheltens P., et al. (2002). Generalized synchronization of MEG recordings in Alzheimer's disease: evidence for involvement of the gamma band. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 19, 562–574. 10.1097/00004691-200212000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele R. J., Morris R. G. (1999). Delay-dependent impairment of a matching-to-place task with chronic and intrahippocampal infusion of the NMDA-antagonist D-AP5. Hippocampus 9, 118–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strong S. P., Koberle R., de Ruyter van Steveninck R., Bialek W. (1998). Entropy and information in neural spike trains. Phys. Rev. Lett. 80, 197–200 10.1103/PhysRevLett.80.197 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tan T., Xie J., Liu T., Chen X., Zheng X., Tong Z., et al. (2013). Low- frequency (1Hz) repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) reverses Aβ1-42-mediated memory deficits in rats. Exp. Gerontol. 48, 786–794. 10.1016/j.exger.2013.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tesche C. D., Karhu J. (2000). Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 919–924. 10.1073/pnas.97.2.919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsai P. H., Lin C., Tsao J., Lin P. F., Wang P. C., Huang N. E., et al. (2012). Empirical mode decomposition based detrended sample entropy in electroencephalography for Alzheimer's disease. J. Neurosci. Methods. 210, 230–237. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes R. P. (2006). Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience 142, 1–20. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang A. C., Wang S. J., Lai K. L., Tsai C. F., Yang C. H., Hwang J. P., et al. (2013). Cognitive and neuropsychiatric correlates of EEG dynamic complexity in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 52–61. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanos S., Zanos T. P., Marmarelis V. Z., Ojemann G. A., Fetz E. E. (2012). Relationships between spike-free local field potentials and spike timing in human temporal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1808–1821. 10.1152/jn.00663.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanos T. P., Mineault P. J., Pack C. C. (2011). Removal of spurious correlations between spikes and local field potentials. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 474–486. 10.1152/jn.00642.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang X. H., Liu S. S., Yi F., Zhuo M., Li B. M. (2013). Delay-dependent impairment of spatial working memory with inhibition of NR2B-containing NMDA receptors in hippocampal CA1 region of rats. Mol. Brain. 6:13. 10.1186/1756-6606-6-13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zingg B., Hintiryan H., Gou L., Song M. Y., Bay M., Bienkowski M. S., et al. (2014). Neural networks of the mouse neocortex. Cell 156, 1096–1111. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]