Abstract
International comparisons were conducted of preschool children’s behavioral and emotional problems as reported on the Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1½–5 by parents in 24 societies (N =19,850). Item ratings were aggregated into scores on syndromes; Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–oriented scales; a Stress Problems scale; and Internalizing, Externalizing, and Total Problems scales. Effect sizes for scale score differences among the 24 societies ranged from small to medium (3–12%). Although societies differed greatly in language, culture, and other characteristics, Total Problems scores for 18 of the 24 societies were within 7.1 points of the omnicultural mean of 33.3 (on a scale of 0–198). Gender and age differences, as well as gender and age interactions with society, were all very small (effect sizes <1%). Across all pairs of societies, correlations between mean item ratings averaged .78, and correlations between internal consistency alphas for the scales averaged .92, indicating that the rank orders of mean item ratings and internal consistencies of scales were very similar across diverse societies.
Preschool children’s behavioral and emotional problems have received much less research attention than older children’s behavioral and emotional problems (Campbell, 2002; Egger & Angold, 2006). Egger and Angold’s (2006) review of epidemiological research on diagnoses in preschoolers identified only four studies, all done in the United States. Two studies (Earls, 1982; Keenan, Shaw, Walsh, Delliquadri, & Giovannelli, 1997) included fewer than 150 children, whereas two had larger samples (Egger et al., 2006; Lavigne et al., 1993). Across these four studies, prevalence for “any disorder” ranged from 14% to 26%. Lavigne, Le Bailly, Hopkins, Gouze, and Binns (2009) subsequently published a fifth study from the United States. In their sample of 796 4-year-olds in metropolitan Chicago, prevalence rates for various disorders ranged from less than 0.1% to 13%, depending on the disorder as well as on the impairment criterion used.
One reason for widely varying prevalence rates is that troubling behaviors displayed by most preschoolers with diagnosable disorders (except perhaps autism) differ mainly in degree from behaviors manifested by typical preschoolers. That is, preschoolers generally come to clinical attention because they are overly aggressive, hyperactive, defiant, anxious, volatile, disruptive, stubborn, or distractible, but these behaviors are quite common in typically developing preschoolers (Campbell, 2002, Wakschlag et al., 2007).
To distinguish levels of problems that are typical for preschoolers from levels that are extreme enough to warrant clinical attention, data from general population samples are needed to establish the prevalence of problems. Furthermore, multicultural data are required to identify possible differences in the prevalence of particular kinds of problems across different cultural groups. Instruments such as the Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1½–5 (CBCL/1½–5; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) are well suited to large-scale investigations of children’s problems because they are inexpensive, do not require training to administer, can be self-administered, and yield quantitative scores.
Findings for the CBCL/2–3, the predecessor of the CBCL/1½–5, were presented by Achenbach (1992), who reported a mean Total Problems score of 34.4 for 368 preschoolers from a U.S. general population sample. Mean Total Problems scores of 27.5 were reported for 109 Icelandic children (Hannesdóttir & Einarsdóttir, 1995) and of 30.4 for 374 Finnish children (Sourander, 2001). Erol, Simsek, Oner, and Munir (2005) reported a mean CBCL/2–3 Total Problems score of 39.5 for a nationally representative sample of 638 Turkish children. For a sample of 684 3-year-olds in the United Arab Emirates (UAE), Eapen, Yunis, Zoubeidi, and Sabri (2004) reported a mean CBCL/2–3 Total Problems score of 34.6 for boys and 30.8 for girls. In the Netherlands, Van den Oord, Koot, Boomsma, Verhulst, and Orlebeke (1995) obtained mean CBCL/2–3 Total Problems scores of 34.4 for boys and 32.3 for girls (N =420). Osa, Ezpeleta, and Navarro (1996) obtained a mean Total Problems score of 27.26 in a sample of 188 Spanish preschoolers assessed with the CBCL/2-3.
When the CBCL/2–3 was revised to span ages 1½ to 5 (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000), two items were changed, new normative and clinical samples were obtained, and new factor analyses yielded a seven-syndrome model. To norm the CBCL/1½–5, a national probability sample of 744 18- to 71-month-olds, which closely matched the demographics of the U.S. population according to census data, was obtained by sampling households from 40 states. Data were obtained via home interviews (completion rate =94%). Consistent with the procedures used to norm other versions of the CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000), the children who had received mental health or special education services in the preceding 12 months (5% of the sample) were excluded when deriving norms, to yield what epidemiologists term a “healthy sample.” Mean Total Problems score was 33.3, with minimal age and gender differences (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). Four international studies have also reported Total Problems scores for the CBCL/1½–5. Kristensen, Henriksen, and Bilenberg (2010) reported a mean Total Problems score of only 17.3 for 850 Danish children, whereas Total Problems scores of 30.5 have been reported for 672 Dutch children (Tick, van der Ende, Koot, & Verhulst, 2007), of 33.4 for 466 Italian children (Frigerio et al., 2006), and of 33.6 for 1,385 Chinese children (Liu, McCauley, Zhao, Pinto-Martin, & Jintan Cohort Study Group, 2010).
To our knowledge, multicultural comparisons of pre-schoolers’ problem scores have not been conducted to date. However, multicultural comparisons of problems among 6- to 16-year-olds (N =55,508) have indicated considerable similarity in findings across 31 societies (Rescorla et al., 2007). Although societal groups had an 8% effect size (ES) on Total Problems scores, 19 of 31 societies had a mean Total Problems score within 5.7 points of the omnicultural mean of 22.5 on a scale that could range from 0 to 224. Correlations between societies for mean item ratings averaged .74, and internal consistency alpha coefficients were very similar across societies. Age and gender effects, all small, were also quite consistent across societies.
The Rescorla et al. (2007) study was etic in orientation, meaning that the same instrument was used to measure behavioral and emotional problems in many different societies. This contrasts with emic research, whereby the meanings of items are explored in different societies. When etic research reveals important differences between societies, emic research may illuminate possible reasons for those differences. Because, to our knowledge, no rigorous comparisons of preschoolers’ behavioral and emotional problems across many societies have been published, etic studies are needed.
PURPOSE OF THE PRESENT STUDY
The present study conducted multicultural comparisons of parent-reported CBCL scores for 19,850 1½- to 5-year-olds from 24 societies. In a related study, Ivanova et al. (2010) conducted confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) of data from the 23 non-U.S. societies. The CFA procedure applied was the “weighted least squares with standard errors and mean- and variance-adjusted chi-square estimator” on tetrachoric correlations of 0 versus 1 and 2 item ratings. This was the same procedure Achenbach and Rescorla (2000) applied to the seven-syndrome model in the United States. Although the Ivanova et al. (2010) findings supported the U.S. CBCL seven-syndrome model in all 23 samples, this does not mean that scores on the syndromes or on other scales would be similar in all the societies. An important purpose of our study was therefore to determine the magnitude of differences between societies on each scale and to identify societies that had particularly low or high scale scores. Whether societies differed much in scale scores, societal differences in gender roles and in customs related to children’s age argued for testing interactions between gender, age, and society in our analyses. Because societies might also differ in the kinds of problems that parents rated high versus low, we tested these differences by computing correlations between the mean ratings of the 99 items in each society versus each other society. Finally, to measure societal variations in internal consistencies of scale scores, we computed correlations between Cronbach’s alpha coefficients on all scales for each society versus every other society.
METHOD
Samples
As presented in Table 1, samples were obtained from 24 societies, with total sample sizes ranging from 301 for Singapore to 2,327 for Korea. Following the recommendation of Nunnally and Bernstein (1994), we required a minimum total sample size of 300 per society. The total sample comprised 19,850 children ages 1½ to 5, but the full age range was not represented in 8 societies. Boys comprised from 47% to 55% of the samples. The sampling frame was national in 8 societies and regional in 16 societies. CBCLs were mailed to parents, sent home from schools, completed at school, or completed at home in the presence of a research assistant or during an interview. Response rates varied widely, ranging from 30% in Germany to 99% in Peru. In most of the data sets sent to us for analysis, children referred for mental health or special education services had not been excluded or counted. In a few data sets, they were included and coded as such, and in four samples they had been explicitly excluded. In each society, conventions for obtaining informed consent required by the investigator’s research institution were followed. Cases were identified only by numerical codes. Based on procedures used in norming the CBCL (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000), children were excluded if ratings were missing for more than eight problem items, with 1% or fewer of cases excluded for 22 societies and 2% to 3% excluded for 2 societies.
TABLE 1.
Characteristics of Samples From 24 Societies
Society | Reference | n | Ages | % Male | Sample | Method | Response Ratea | Referred Excludedb |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Australia | Zubrick et al., 2007 | 1,793 | 2–3 | 51% | Regional birth cohort | mailed | 85% | no |
Belgium | De Pauw et al., 2009; Van Leeuwen et al., 2009 | 1,117 | 1.5–5 | 52% | Regional school-based | at home; from schools | 73% | no |
Chile | Lecannelier et al., 2009 | 483 | 1.5–5 | 50% | Regional school-based | at home; from schools | 80% | yes |
China | Liu et al., 2011 | 908 | 4–5 | 53% | Regional school-based | at home; from schools | 91% | no |
Denmark | Kristensen et al., 2010 | 851 | 1.5–5 | 50% | Regional household | mailed | 52% | no |
Finland | Sourander, 2001 | 370 | 3 | 50% | Regional well-baby clinic | at clinic | 71% | no |
France | Capron & Duyme, 2009 | 1,204 | 1.5–5 | 53% | Regional school-based and pediatric | from schools; medical offices | 91% | no |
Germany | Plück et al., 2010 | 850 | 2–5 | 55% | Regional school-based prevention project | from schools | 30% | no |
Iceland | Gudmundsson & Bjarnadóttir, 2009 | 305 | 1½–5 | 51% | National school-based | from schools | 60% | no |
Iran | Esmaeili, 2009 | 1,346 | 1½–5 | 49% | National school-based | interviews at schools | 97% | no |
Italy | Frigerio et al., 2006 | 466 | 1½–5 | 50% | Regional school-based | from schools | 70% | yes |
Korea | Kim et al., 2009; Oh & Kim, 2009; | 2,327 | 1½–5 | 51% | National school-based | from schools | 92% | yes |
Kosovo | Shahini et al., 2009 | 481 | 1½–5 | 52% | Regional school-based | from schools | 77% | no |
Lithuania | Jusiené et al., 2007 | 931 | 1½–5 | 53% | National school-based (some households) | at home | 80% | no |
Netherlands | Tick et al., 2007 | 608 | 1½–5 | 53% | Regional household | at home | 84% | no |
Peru | Pomalina et al., 2009 | 1,027 | 1½–5 | 52% | Regional household-based | at home | 99% | no |
Portugal | Dias et al., 2009 | 407 | 1½–5 | 50% | Regional school based | from schools | 44% | no |
Romania | Dobrean et al., 2008 | 938 | 2–5 | 47% | National school-based | from schools | 75% | no |
Singapore | Ooi et al., 2009 | 301 | 1½–5 | 51% | Regional school-based | from schools | 36% | no |
Spain | Ezpeleta et al., 2010 | 570 | 3–5 | 51% | Regional school-based | from schools | 58% | no |
Taiwan | Wu et al., 2009 | 306 | 1½–5 | 54% | Birth cohort; regional school-based | by mail; from schools | 53%c (56%, 50%) | no |
Turkey | Erol et al., 2005; Yurdusen, 2004 | 825 | 2–3; 1½–5 | 50% | National household; regional school-based | at home/from schools | 86%d (95%, 60%) | no; yes |
UAE | Eapen et al., 2004 | 692 | 3 | 50% | National household | at home | 96% | no |
USA | Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000 | 744 | 1½–5 | 51% | National household | at home | 94% | no |
Note: Complete references for each sample are available from the first author. UAE =United Arab Emirates; USA =United States of America.
Response rates, which were calculated by the investigators who provided the data for each society, represent the ratio of completed forms to the target sample; in some societies, settings in which data were collected (e.g., different schools) may have varied widely in response rate.
Samples in which the investigator indicated that referred children had been excluded are coded “yes”; for all other samples, it is assumed that some referred children may have been included and these samples are coded “no”.
The overall response rate for Taiwan was the weighted average of the response rates of the two subsamples.
The overall response rate for Turkey was the weighted average of the response rates of the two subsamples.
Measure
The CBCL/1½–5 was used for all children in 21 societies. The CBCL/2–3 was used in the UAE, Finland, and for 625 children in the Turkish sample, as these data were collected prior to 2000. The Turkish sample included 200 additional children assessed with the CBCL/1½–5 at a later date. When the CBCL/2–3 was revised, Items 51 and 79 were replaced by new items: 51. Shows panic for no good reason and 79. Rapid shifts between sadness and excitement. For samples in which the CBCL/2–3 was used, scores for Items 51 and 79 were replaced with imputed scores, as described later.
Foreign language versions were created by translators fluent in both English and the foreign language in question. To verify that translations captured the original meanings, independent back-translations into English were done, which then guided additional fine-tuning of the translation in an iterative process.
Each of the 99 CBCL/1½–5 problem items was rated 0 (not true [as far as you know]), 1 (somewhat or sometimes true), and 2 (very true or often true), based on the preceding 2 months. Item 100, an open-ended item, was excluded from all analyses. With the exception of Items 51 and 79 (for which values were imputed in the samples assessed with the CBCL/2–3), missing ratings were recoded as 0, after excluding all children with more than eight missing ratings.
Investigators in each society provided raw data for our analyses, namely, 0–1–2 ratings on the 99 problem items for each participant. These item ratings were used to compute scores for the seven syndromes derived by factor analysis for the CBCL/1½–5 (Emotionally Reactive, Anxious/Depressed, Somatic Complaints, Withdrawn, Sleep Problems, Attention Problems, and Aggressive Behavior), for the two second-order factors (Internalizing and Externalizing), and for Total Problems (the sum of all 99 items). Scores were also computed for five scales identified by an international group of child psychologists and psychiatrists as being very consistent with diagnostic categories of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed. [DSM–IV]; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). These DSM-oriented scales included Affective Problems, Anxiety Problems, Pervasive Developmental Problems, Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Problems, and Oppositional Defiant Problems (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). Scores were also computed for a seven-item Stress Problems scale (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2010), derived from research with preschoolers who varied in their exposure to traumatic events. The Stress Problems scale includes items such as 5. Can’t concentrate, can’t pay attention for long; 47. Nervous, highstrung, or tense; and 82. Sudden changes in mood or feelings.
Items 51 and 79 both load on the Emotionally Reactive syndrome and hence are also included in calculating Internalizing and Total Problems. Item 51 also loads on the DSM-Anxiety Problems scale. Missing values for Items 51 and 79 were imputed for Turkey, Finland, and the UAE using the Missing Values Multiple Imputation Module of SPSS, Version 17. Ratings obtained on all nine Emotionally Reactive items in 21 societies, plus ratings on the seven nonimputed items in the three societies subjected to imputation, were used as predictors in the linear regression imputation procedure, which was constrained to yield values of 0, 1, or 2. Five imputed data sets were generated, in which mean scores for the four scales containing the imputed items were calculated. Mean scores on these scales for the five imputed data sets usually differed only in the third decimal place.
Data Analysis
Scale scores were positively skewed, as is typical for problem scores in general population samples where most children have relatively few problems. However, because the analysis of variance (ANOVA) and multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) models used were very robust with respect to deviations from normality, especially with large samples manifesting the same skew pattern (Kirk, 1995), we analyzed untransformed raw scores.
MANOVAs tested the differences between scale scores by society, gender, and age group (18 to 47 months vs. 48 to 71 months) when multiple scales with nonoverlapping items could be tested in a single analysis (i.e., the seven syndromes, the five DSM-oriented scales, and Internalizing/Externalizing). Total Problems and the Stress Problems scale were tested in separate ANOVAs. Because the large samples used in this study provided such high statistical power that even very small effects could be statistically significant, we used a stringent p value of .001. ESs were measured by partial eta squared, which represents the percentage of total variance uniquely accounted for by a given factor with the other factors partialed out. These ESs were interpreted using Cohen’s (1988) criteria (small =1–5.9%, medium =6–13.9%, and large ≥14%). Correlations between mean item ratings for every pair of societies were computed, with mean imputed ratings for Items 51 and 79 for Finland, Turkey, and UAE. Cronbach’s alphas were calculated for each scale within each sample, and correlations were computed between alphas for every pair of societies.
RESULTS
Mean Scale Score Comparisons
For each scale, Table 2 displays the range of mean scores, the omnicultural mean (derived by averaging the 24 society means) and its standard deviation, as well as the omnicultural standard deviation (derived by averaging the 24 SDs). The omnicultural mean for Total Problems was 33.3. Mean Total Problems scores for the 24 societies arrayed in ascending order are displayed in Figure 1. On a scale that could range from 0 to 198, three societies (Denmark, Iceland, and Spain) had scores greater than 7.1 points (1 SD) below the omnicultural mean of 33.3, three other societies (Taiwan, Lithuania, and Chile) had scores greater than 7.1 points above the omnicultural mean, and 18 of the 24 societies had scores within 7.1 points of the omnicultural mean. Student-Newman-Keuls (S-N-K) post hoc tests indicated that scores for Denmark, Iceland, and Spain were significantly lower (p <.001) than those of all other societies, with Denmark’s score significantly lower than Iceland’s and Spain’s. Korea’s score (the fourth lowest) was not significantly different from the next seven scores in the ascending array (i.e., Germany through France). S-N-K post hoc tests also indicated that Chile’s mean Total Problems score was significantly higher (p <.001) than those of all other societies. The four next highest mean scores (Iran, Turkey, Taiwan, and Lithuania) were not significantly different from each other.
TABLE 2.
Ranges, Omnicultural Means, Standard Deviations, and Alphas Across 24 Societies
CBCL Scale | Range of Mean Scale Scoresa | Omnicultural M (SD)b | Omnicultural SDc | M α |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total Problems | 17.2–47.5 | 33.3 (7.1) | 19.0 | .94 (99)d |
Internalizing | 3.9–13.9 | 9.6 (2.6) | 6.7 | .84 (36) |
Externalizing | 6.7–16.9 | 12.0 (2.3) | 7.3 | .88 (25) |
Emotionally Reactive | 1.1–4.0 | 2.7 (0.7) | 2.4 | .69 (9) |
Anxious/Depressed | 1.1–4.6 | 3.1 (1.0) | 2.3 | .65 (8) |
Somatic Complaints | 1.0–3.3 | 2.3 (0.6) | 2.1 | .56 (11) |
Withdrawn | 0.7–2.9 | 1.7 (0.6) | 1.8 | .62 (8) |
Sleep Problems | 1.8–4.0 | 2.7 (0.3) | 2.4 | .67 (7) |
Attention Problems | 0.9–3.2 | 2.3 (0.6) | 1.8 | .60 (5) |
Aggressive Behavior | 5.8–13.8 | 9.7 (1.8) | 6.1 | .87 (19) |
DSM-Affective Problems | 1.0–3.5 | 2.3 (0.7) | 2.1 | .57 (10) |
DSM-Anxiety Problems | 1.6–6.0 | 3.8 (1.2) | 2.6 | .63 (10) |
DSM-Pervasive Developmental Problems | 1.6–4.8 | 3.2 (0.9) | 2.8 | .67 (13) |
DSM-Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Problems | 2.3–6.4 | 4.7 (1.0) | 2.7 | .74 (6) |
DSM-Oppositional Defiant Problems | 2.3–4.9 | 3.3 (0.6) | 2.4 | .75 (6) |
Stress Problems | 0.9–3.0 | 2.0 (0.5) | 1.8 | .59 (7) |
Note: CBCL =Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000); DSM =Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994).
All analyses utilized raw data for all participants in each society.
Omnicultural mean (standard deviation) obtained by averaging 24 society means.
Omnicultural standard deviation obtained by averaging 24 society standard deviations.
Number of items per scale.
FIGURE 1.
Mean Total Problems scores from 24 societies (N =19,850). Note: USA =United States of America; UAE =United Arab Emirates.
Table 2 also indicates that the omnicultural standard deviation (the mean of the 24 SDs) was 19.0, more than double the standard deviation of 7.1 for the omnicultural mean. This finding indicates that there was much greater variation within than between societies in Total Problems scores. As shown in Table 3, the ANOVA for Total Problems yielded a medium ES for society of 9%. Gender, age, and the Society × Age interactions yielded ESs less than 1%, indicating very small effects. No other effects were significant. Boys and younger children obtained slightly higher Total Problems scores than girls and older children.
TABLE 3.
Significant Effect Sizes (η2) at p <.001 for CBCL Scale Scores
CBCL Scale | Society | Gender | Age | S × G | S × A |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total Problems | 9% | <1% | <1% | ns | <1% |
Internalizing | 10% | ns | ns | ns | <1% |
Externalizing | 7% | <1% | <1% | <1% | <1% |
Emotionally Reactive | 7% | ns | ns | ns | <1% |
Anxious/Depressed | 12% | ns | ns | ns | ns |
Somatic Complaints | 7% | ns | ns | <1% | <1% |
Withdrawn | 5% | <1% | ns | ns | <1% |
Sleep Problems | 3% | ns | <1% | ns | <1% |
Attention Problems | 9% | <1% | <1% | ns | ns |
Aggressive Behavior | 6% | <1% | <1% | ns | <1% |
DSM-Affective Problems | 6% | ns | ns | ns | <1% |
DSM-Anxiety Problems | 11% | ns | ns | ns | <1% |
DSM-Pervasive Developmental Problems | 6% | <1% | <1% | ns | <1% |
DSM-Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Problems | 11% | <1% | <1% | ns | <1% |
DSM-Oppositional Defiant Problems | 6% | <1% | <1% | ns | <1% |
Stress Problems | 6% | 1% | ns | 1% | 1% |
Note: CBCL =Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000); S =Society; G =gender; A =Age; DSM =Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). No Gender × Age or Society × Gender × Age interactions were significant at p <.001.
We used an ANOVA to test the effects of response rate on Total Problems scores by grouping the 24 societies into three response rate categories: low <70% (8 societies, N =3,220, M =30.6), medium 70–89% (9 societies, N =7,531, M =35.3), and high ≥90% (7 societies, N =8,248, M =32.8). The ES for response rate was significant (p <.001) but very small (<1%), with S-N-K tests indicating significant differences between all three groups (lowest mean score in the “low” group and highest mean score in the “medium” group). When the societies were dichotomized into low versus medium/high, the significant ES for response rate was also very small (<1%), with mean Total Problems scores of 30.6 versus. 34.0. The r of .19 between response rate and mean Total Problems scores, which falls in the “small” range according to Cohen (1988), was not significant, perhaps due in part to the small sample size (24 societies). Taken together, these findings suggest that low response rates (<70%) were associated with slightly lower problem scores but that variations in response rates from 70% to 99% were not monotonically related to Total Problems scores.
Internalizing, Externalizing, and the 13 narrow-band scales (seven syndrome scales, five DSM-oriented scales, and Stress Problems) all showed the same pattern of larger within-society than between-society variation (i.e., their omnicultural standard deviations were much larger than the standard deviations of their omnicultural means). As can be seen in Table 3, the ESs for society were 10% for Internalizing and 7% for Externalizing. For Externalizing, boys and younger children scored higher than girls and older children (both ES <1%). S-N-K post hoc tests for Internalizing indicated that only Denmark’s mean was significantly lower than all other means, with the next three lowest societies (Iceland, Spain, and Australia) not significantly different from one another. The six societies with the highest mean Internalizing scores (Singapore, Iran, Romania, Lithuania, Turkey, and Chile) did not differ significantly from each other. For Externalizing, Denmark’s mean was significantly lower than those of the next four societies (Spain, Korea, Iceland, and Kosovo), which did not differ significantly from each other. Chile had a significantly higher Externalizing mean than the next two societies (Lithuania and Taiwan), which did not differ significantly from each other.
The MANOVAs for the two sets of narrow-band scales (syndrome scales and DSM-oriented scales) yielded ESs for society ranging from 3% (Sleep Problems) to 12% (Anxious/Depressed). For scales with significant gender or age differences, boys and younger children scored significantly higher than girls and older children (all ESs < 1%). The few significant interactions all had ES less than 1%. The ANOVA for Stress Problems yielded an ES for society of 6%, with all interaction ESs less than 1%. Boys scored slightly higher than girls, whereas older children scored slightly higher than younger children, with very small but significant variations across societies.
Denmark obtained the lowest mean on 11 of the 13 narrow-band scales (with Iceland lowest on DSM-Affective and Korea lowest on DSM-Oppositional Defiant). Denmark’s mean was significantly lower than those of all other societies on five scales: Anxious/Depressed, Aggressive Behavior, DSM-Pervasive Developmental Problems, DSM-Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity, and Stress Problems. Chile’s mean was the highest of all societies on seven scales, whereas Lithuania scored highest on Emotionally Reactive, Attention Problems, and DSM-Attention Deficit Hyperactivity. Singapore scored highest on Withdrawn, Taiwan scored highest on Sleep Problems, and Romania scored highest on Stress Problems. The highest mean was significantly different from all other means on only four scales: Withdrawn, Sleep Problems, Aggressive Behavior, and DSM-Oppositional Defiant Problems.
Mean Item Ratings
For each society, within-society mean ratings for each item were calculated by averaging the 0–1–2 ratings for the entire sample from that society. These 24 sets of 99 mean item ratings were then correlated with one another, yielding a matrix of bi-society Q correlations (so designated because they are calculated over items rather than cases). All bi-society Q correlations between mean item ratings for all pairs of societies were large according to Cohen (1988), ranging from .63 (UAE with Denmark) to .94 (Chile with Peru). When the 23 bi-society Q correlations for each society were averaged, the mean bi-society Q for each society ranged from .70 (UAE) to .84 (United States), with a mean of .78 across all 24 societies. These findings indicate strong similarity with regard to which items received relatively high versus relatively low mean ratings.
To further explore multicultural findings at the item level, we identified the 10 items that had the highest mean ratings for the full sample of 19,850 children (Table 4). Table 4 also lists the number of societies for which these items made each society’s own “top 10” list. The first 2 items (8. Can’t stand waiting; wants everything now and 16. Demands must be met immediately) were among the top 10 items for all 24 societies. The 3rd item (96. Wants a lot of attention) was in the top 10 list for 21 societies (not China, Taiwan, or the UAE). The 4th item (59. Quickly shifts from one activity to another) was in the top 10 list for 19 societies (not Turkey, Iran, Korea, Iceland, or Germany). The remaining 6 items were in the top 10 lists for 15, 9, 12, 14, 12, and 9 societies, respectively. Four additional items made the top 10 lists for at least 7 societies when societies were analyzed separately: 15. Defiant (10 societies); 81. Stubborn, sullen, or irritable (8 societies); 10. Clings to adults or too dependent (7 societies), and 85. Temper tantrums or hot temper (7 societies). These four items fell at positions 11, 13, 12, and 17 for the full sample.
TABLE 4.
The 10 Highest Scoring Items Across All 24 Societies
Item | M Rating | N of Societiesa | % Rated 1b | % Rated 2c |
---|---|---|---|---|
8. Can’t stand waiting; wants everything now | 1.00 | 24 | 47 | 27 |
16. Demands must be met immediately | .88 | 24 | 50 | 19 |
96. Wants a lot of attention | .84 | 21 | 45 | 20 |
59. Quickly shifts from one activity to another | .78 | 19 | 44 | 17 |
22. Doesn’t want to sleep alone | .76 | 15 | 29 | 24 |
30. Easily jealous | .73 | 9 | 44 | 14 |
33. Feelings are easily hurt | .71 | 12 | 44 | 14 |
20. Disobedient | .71 | 14 | 57 | 7 |
6. Can’t sit still, restless, or hyperactive | .71 | 12 | 36 | 17 |
36. Gets into everything | .67 | 9 | 37 | 15 |
Note: For all items, 0 =not true, 1 =somewhat or sometimes true, 2 =very true or often true.
Number of societies for which item was in its “top 10” list.
Percentage of children rated 1 across the full sample of 19,850.
Percentage of children rated 2 across the full sample of 19,850.
Also listed in Table 4 is the percentage of children in the full sample (N =19,850) whose parents gave ratings of 1 (somewhat or sometimes true) or 2 (very true or often true) to the 10 highest scoring items. All 10 items received ratings of 1 or 2 for greater than 50% of children in the full sample, with the most common being 8. Can’t stand waiting; wants everything now (74%). On average, items were about twice as likely to be rated 1 as 2, with the exception of item 22. Doesn’t want to sleep alone (29% 1 s and 24% 2 s) and item 20. Disobedient (57% 1 s and 7% 2 s). Although an in-depth analysis of item ratings by age group within each society is beyond the scope of this report, for the most commonly endorsed item (8. Can’t stand waiting; wants everything now), we calculated the percentage of children rated 0 in the two age groups by society. For the 20 societies with children in both age groups, the difference in younger versus older percentage was 0–2% for five societies, 3–6% for five societies, 7–9% for four societies, 10–12% for five societies, and 19% for one society (Denmark). In Turkey and Kosovo, younger children were more likely to be rated 0, whereas in all other societies older children were more likely to be rated 0, consistent with the age trends for scales, whereby younger children tended to have higher scores.
Internal Consistency
For each scale, we averaged the alphas across societies to yield a mean alpha (see Table 2). Mean alphas for Total Problems, Internalizing, and Externalizing were .94, .84, and .88, respectively, with the minimum alpha being .91, .80, and .85. As shown in Table 2, three narrow-band scales had mean alphas less than .60, seven had mean alphas from .60 to .69, two had mean alphas from .70 to .79, and one had a mean alpha greater than .80. When alphas for each society were correlated with those for every other society, bi-society correlations ranged from .78 (Italy with China) to .99 (Denmark with Peru, Germany with France, and Germany with the United States), with a mean bi-society r of .92. Mean bi-society rs for each society ranged from .89 for China to .95 for the United States, France, and Germany. These high correlations indicate that the internal consistencies of the CBCL/1½–5 scales were very consistent across societies, with Total Problems, Internalizing, Externalizing, and Aggressive Behavior having the largest alphas and most narrow-band scales having alphas less than .70.
DISCUSSION
The current study provided systematic multicultural comparisons of quantitative data on behavioral and emotional problems for 19,850 preschool children from general population samples in 24 societies (all Ns >300). Our findings revealed small-to-medium differences between societies in mean scale scores but miniscule differences between societies in gender and age effects. We also found large correlations between mean item ratings and between internal consistency alphas across the 24 societies. These results complement Ivanova et al.’s (2010) CFAs, which supported the seven-syndrome U.S. model in the 23 non-U.S. samples analyzed for the present study.
Our results revealed substantial consistency in CBCL mean scores across many societies, despite great variation among them in geography, political/economic system, size, population, ethnicity/race, and religion. Whereas the mean of the 24 standard deviations for the 24 Total Problems scores was 19.0, the standard deviation of the Total Problems score omnicultural mean was only 7.1. This indicates much more variance within than between societies in problems scores. The 18 societies scoring within 7.1 points of the omnicultural mean on Total Problems score (on a scale ranging 0–198) included, among others, the United States, Iran, China, Peru, the UAE, France, the Netherlands, Australia, Korea, and Portugal. These societies differ in many ways. It is hard to find features in common for the 18 middle-scoring societies, the three lowest scoring societies, or the three highest scoring societies, or features that differentiate among the low-, middle-, high-scoring societies. For example, although Denmark and Iceland had low mean Total Problems scores, Finland, another Nordic society, did not. Similarly, Lithuania had a high mean Total Problems score, but Romania and Kosovo, the two other former Eastern Bloc societies in our sample, did not. Chile had a much higher mean Total Problems score than Peru, and Taiwan had a higher mean Total Problems score than China.
The eight societies with the lowest response rates (30–67%) had significantly lower Total Problems scores than the rest of the societies, but the ES was very small (<1%). Societies with response rates ranging from 70% to 89% had significantly higher mean Total Problems scores than societies with response rates at or greater than 90%, indicating a nonmonotonic association between response rate and problem scores. This may have contributed to the small r of .19 between response rate and Total Problems scores, which was not significant. The societies with the three lowest response rates (Singapore, Germany, and Portugal) had mean Total Problems scores within 1 SD of the omnicultural mean.
Effect sizes for differences between societies ranged from 3% (Sleep Problems) to 12% (Anxious/Depressed), with 12 of the 16 ESs less than 10%. Nevertheless, ESs for society were consistently higher than ESs for gender and age, which never exceeded 1%. No ESs for society reached Cohen’s (1988) threshold for large effects. S-N-K post hoc tests indicated that the highest and lowest scoring societies often differed significantly from all other societies, but most other societies did not differ significantly from each other.
In interpreting our findings, we focus on major trends in the data supported by statistically significant differences. Our results suggested that variations among societies were due more to a tendency to score high or low overall than to a tendency to score high or low on only a few specific scales. Denmark and Chile were the most extreme cases of this tendency. To a somewhat lesser extent, Iceland and Spain tended to have low scores and Lithuania and Taiwan tended to have high scores, regardless of the scale. However, a few within-society differences are worth mentioning. Because several previous studies have reported elevated scores for Turkish school-age children on the Anxious/Depressed syndrome (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2007), it is noteworthy that Turkey was the second highest-scoring society on Anxious/Depressed in this study and was fourth or fifth highest on the other three Internalizing syndromes. However, it was only sixth to ninth highest on the three Externalizing syndromes. This suggests that Turkish parents may have a tendency to report more Internalizing than Externalizing problems in their preschool children. An even more striking example is Korea, which ranked 3rd from the bottom on Externalizing but 11th from the bottom on Internalizing. Although it is tempting to attribute this pattern to persons in Asian cultures tending to internalize rather than externalize problems, it should be noted that Taiwan and Singapore, also Asian societies, ranked 3rd and 4th from the top on Externalizing.
Despite the fact that an iterative translation and back-translation process was used to adapt the CBCL, we cannot be certain that CBCL items hold identical meanings for all parents in every society. Even parents speaking the same language might interpret some items in slightly different ways. However, the mean bi-society r of .78 suggests that parents’ ratings in 24 societies were quite consistent in terms of which CBCL items tended to receive high, medium, or low ratings. This finding suggests that the items operated similarly in very different societies. Furthermore, although variations in translations may have resulted in subtle differences in meaning across languages, CBCL scale scores from societies with extremely different cultures and languages were very comparable. For example, as can be seen in Figure 1, the United States, Italy, the UAE, China, and Peru had mean Total Problems scores that were virtually identical.
Six of the seven syndrome scales and three of the five DSM-oriented scales had mean alphas less than .70. However, in Achenbach and Rescorla’s (2000) U.S. sample, all nine of these scales had test–retest reliabilities at or greater than .80 and significantly differentiated referred from nonreferred children. Thus, low alphas did not preclude scales from having strong test–retest reliability and criterion-related validity. Furthermore, the mean bi-society r of .92 for scale alphas in the current study indicated that the 24 societies were very similar with regard to which scales had the highest alphas (Total Problems, Internalizing, Externalizing, and Aggressive Behavior) and the lowest alphas (Withdrawn and DSM-oriented Affective Problems). These internal consistency findings, in conjunction with Ivanova et al.’s (2010) CFA findings, support the multi-cultural consistency of the preschool CBCL’s scales across 24 very different societies.
Limitations
A possible limitation of our study is its etic approach, whereby the same standardized assessment instrument was used in all 24 societies. However, several of our etic findings provide a basis for emic studies exploring differences in scale scores. For example, it would be important to test why Danish scores are so low and Chilean scores are so high, compared to those from other societies in the same region (e.g., Peru vs. Chile). It would also be important to test why Turkey had a higher rank order on Internalizing than on Externalizing (second vs. eighth highest) but Iran, a neighboring Muslim country, did not (fourth vs. fifth highest).
Although the current study only used parents’ ratings, a parallel study (Rescorla et al., 2011) used ratings on the Caregiver-Teacher Report Form (C-TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) for 10,521 children in 15 societies of the 24 societies studied here. C-TRF results were very similar to those for the CBCL: ESs for society ranged from 4% to 13%, the overall r when mean item ratings were correlated between societies was .73; scale alphas were very comparable across societies; and six of the top 10 C-TRF items matched those for the CBCL.
The wide range in response rates constitutes a limitation of our study. However, response rates were not significantly correlated with scale scores, and the association between response rates and Total Problems scores was not monotonic. Differences in sample sizes could also have affected our findings. It is possible that societies in which response rate was low and the sample size was small relative to the overall population (e.g., Singapore, Portugal, and Taiwan) might have yielded somewhat different scores had the response rate been higher and the sample larger.
Finally, information about children’s referral status was unavailable for most societies. However, the paucity of services for preschoolers in most societies suggests that few would have been referred. For example, in the Lavigne et al. (2009) study, only 12 of 796 U.S. 4-year-olds (2%) had received any mental health services. To further address the effect of including versus excluding referred children, we analyzed data for the U.S. sample both with and without the 5% of the children who had been referred in the preceding 12 months for mental health or special education services. Mean Total Problems score differed by only 0.5 points when referred children were excluded. Inspection of mean scores in the present study suggests that inclusion versus exclusion of referred children had little effect. For example, the Danish sample included referred children and had the lowest mean problem scores, whereas the Chilean sample excluded referred children and had the highest mean scores.
Implications for Research, Policy, and Practice
To our knowledge, ours is the largest and most diverse international database for comparing parents’ reports of preschoolers’ behavioral and emotional problems across many societies. Although 18 of 24 societies had very similar scores, 3 societies had substantially lower mean scores and 3 had substantially higher mean scores according to a commonly used—albeit arbitrary—1 SD threshold. To take account of these differences in Total Problems scores, separate multicultural norms for the CBCL/1½–5 have been constructed for societies with mean Total Problems scores lower than 1 SD below the omnicultural mean, between −1 and +1 SD from the omnicultural mean, and higher than 1 SD above the omnicultural mean (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2010).
Our findings should prove useful to both researchers and practitioners. Researchers from societies not represented in this study can collect CBCL/1½–5 data using a general population sample of at least 300 and then compare the mean scores, mean item ratings, and scale alphas obtained from these data with those reported here. The mean Total Problems score they obtain can indicate which CBCL/1½–5 multicultural norm group is appropriate for their society (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2010). Researchers can also use our etic findings as a basis for emic studies of why mean scale scores in some societies were significantly lower or higher than in all other societies or why scores on one scale were lower than those on another scale in certain societies. For practitioners, parents’ ratings can be scored using norms appropriate for relevant societies. For example, ratings by a Chinese father can be evaluated in relation to the middle-scoring norms that include mainland China, whereas ratings by a Taiwanese mother are evaluated in relation to the high-scoring norms that include Taiwan. Moreover, combined with the Ivanova et al. (2010) findings, our findings support use of the CBCL/1½–5 to assess preschool children from many societies in terms of ratings by their parents. Finally, our findings are very consistent with findings reported by Rescorla et al. (2007) for 55,508 children ages 6 to 16 from 31 societies, indicating that the patterns of multicultural similarities and differences reported here for preschoolers resemble those found for older children.
References
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2–3 and 1992 Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Manual for the ASEBA pre-school forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Multicultural understanding of child and adolescent psychopathology: Implications for mental health assessment. New York: Guilford; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. Multicultural supplement to the Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell SB. Behavior problems in preschool children. New York: Guilford; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Capron C, Duyme M. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for French preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. New York: Academic Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- De Pauw SSW, Mervielde I, Van Leeuwen K. How are traits related to problem behavior in preschool children? Similarities and contrasts in temperament and personality. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;39:309–325. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9290-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dias P, Machado BC, Silva J, Goncalves M. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for Portuguese preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Dobrean A. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for Romanian preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Döpfner M, Plück J. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for German preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Eapen V, Yunis F, Zoubeidi T, Sabri S. Problem behaviors in 3-year-old children in the United Arab Emirates. Journal of Pediatric Health Care. 2004;18:186–191. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Earls F. Cultural and national differences in the epidemiology of behavior problems of preschool children. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry. 1982;6:45–56. doi: 10.1007/BF49470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:313–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger HL, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Potts E, Walter B, Angold A. The test–retest reliability of the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:538–549. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000205705.71194.b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erol N, Simsek Z, Oner O, Munir K. Behavioral and emotional problems among Turkish children at ages 2 to 3 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44:80–87. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000145234.18056.82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esmaeli EM. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for Iranian preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Ezpeleta L, Osa N, de la, Granero R, Domenech JM. The Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents for parents of preschool children. 2010. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frigerio A, Cozzi P, Pastore V, Molteni M, Borgatti R, Montirosso R. The evaluation of behavioral and emotional problems in a sample of Italian preschool children using the Child Behavior Checklist and the Caregiver-Teacher Report Form. Infanzia e Adolescenza. 2006;5:24–32. [Google Scholar]
- Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.ep9709033852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guđmundsson H, Bjarnadóttir G. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for Icelandic preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Hannesdóttir H, Einarsdóttir S. The Icelandic Child Mental Health Study, an epidemiological study of Icelandic children 2–18 years of age using the Child Behavior Checklist as a screening instrument. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;4:237–248. doi: 10.1007/BF01980488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanova MY, Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA, Harder V, Ang RP, Bilenberg N, et al. Preschool psychopathology reported by parents in 23 societies: Testing the seven-syndrome model of the Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 1.5–5. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49:1215–1224. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jusiené R, Raižiené S, Barkauskiené R, Bielauskaité R, Dervinyté-Bongarzoni A. The risk factors of emotional and behavioral problems in preschool age. Visuomenes Ssveikata [Public Health] 2007;4:46–54. [Google Scholar]
- Keenan K, Shaw DS, Walsh B, Delliquadri E, Giovannelli J. DSM–III–R disorders in preschool children from low-income families. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:620–627. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199705000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirk RE. Experimental design: Procedures for behavioral sciences. 3. New York: Wadsworth; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Kristensen S, Henriksen TB, Bilenberg N. The child behavior checklist for ages 1.5–5 (CBCL/1½–5): Assessment and analysis of parent- and caregiver-reported problems in a population-based sample of Danish preschool children. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;64:203–209. doi: 10.3109/08039480903456595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavigne JV, Binns HJ, Christoffel KK, Rosenbaum D, Arend R, Smith K, et al. Behavioral and emotional problems among preschool children in pediatric primary care: Prevalence and pediatricians’ recognition. Pediatrics. 1993;91:649–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavigne JV, Le Bailly SA, Hopkins J, Gouze KR, Binns HJ. The prevalence of ADHD, ODD, depression and anxiety in a community sample of 4-year-olds. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38:315–328. doi: 10.1080/15374410902851382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lecanneler F. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for Chilean preschool children. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, McCauley LA, Zhao Y, Pinto-Martin J Jintan Cohort Study Group. Cohort profile: The China Jintan Child Cohort Study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;39:668–674. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Cheng H, Leung PWL. The application of the preschool Child Behavior Checklist and the Caregiver-Teacher Report Form to Mainland Chinese children: Syndrome structure, gender differences, country effects, and inter-informant agreement. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:251–264. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9452-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. 3. New York: McGraw- Hill; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Oh KJ, Kim YA. Manual of the CBCL/-5. Seoul: Huno Consulting; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Ooi YP, Fung DSS. Child Behavior Checklist/-5 scores for preschool children in Singapore. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- De la Osa N, Ezpeleta L, Navarro JB. Adaptation and norms of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/2–3) for preschoolers: Preliminary results. Ciencia Psicológica. 1996;4:19–31. [Google Scholar]
- Plück J, Beiling M, Kuschel A, Heinrichs N, Schmeck K, Döpfner M. Assessment of behavior problems in preschool children using the German version of the CBCL/-5. 2010. Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
- Pomalima R, Valverde J, Cutipe Y, Vargas H, Rojas E, Alvarez M. Epidemiological study of children and adolescents in Lima and Callao. 2009. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Achenbach TM, Ivanova MY, Dumenci L, Almqvist F, Bilenberg N, et al. Behavioral and emotional problems reported by parents of children ages 6 to 16 in 31 societies. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2007;15:130–142. doi: 10.1177/10634266070150030101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla L, Achenbach TM, Ivanova MY, Bilenberg N, Bjarnadóttir G, Dias P, et al. Multicultural comparisons of behavioral and emotional problems reported by caregivers/teachers of preschoolers in 15 societies. 2011. Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
- Shahini M, Pranvera J, Ahmeti A. Emotional and behavioral problems in young children 2–5 years in Kosovo. 2009. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Sourander A. Emotional and behavioral problems in a sample of Finnish three-year-olds. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2001;10:98–104. doi: 10.1007/s007870170032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tick NT, van der Ende J, Koot HM, Verhulst FC. 14-year changes in emotional and behavioral problems of very young Dutch children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2007;46:1333–1340. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3181337532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Oord EJCG, Koot HM, Boomsma DI, Verhulst FC, Orlebeke JF. A twin-singleton comparison of problem behavior in 2–3 year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1995;36:449–458. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.ep11520774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Leeuwen K, Bourgonjon L, Huijsman L, Van Meenen M, De Pauw S. Temper tantrums in young children: Relations with child temperament, child problem behavior and parenting. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; Denver, CO. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Hill C, Danis B, Keenan K, et al. A developmental framework for distinguishing disruptive behavior from normative misbehavior in pre-school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:976–987. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01786.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu YT, Hsieh WS, Chen PC, Chen WJ, Liao HF, Su YN, Jeng SF. Behavioral and emotional problems in 2- to 3-year-old Taiwanese children. Poster presented at International ASEBA Conference on Empirically Based Mental Health Knowledge; Burlington, VT. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Yurdusen S. The effects of mothers’ parental attitudes on their preschool children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. 2004. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Zubrick SR, Taylor CL, Rice ML, Sleggers DW. Late language emergence at 24 months: An epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50:1562–1592. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/106). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]