Abstract
We propose that executive dysfunction is an important component relating the socioeconomic status gradient of select health behaviors. We review and find evidence supporting an SES gradient associated with (1) negative health behaviors (e.g., obesity, excessive use of alcohol, tobacco and other substances), and (2) executive dysfunction. Moreover, the evidence supports that stress and insufficient cognitive resources contribute to executive dysfunction and that executive dysfunction is evident among individuals who smoke cigarettes, are obese, abuse alcohol, and use illicit drugs. Collectively these data supports the dual system model of cognitive control, referred to here as the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems hypothesis. The implications of these relationships for intervention and social justice considerations are discussed.
Keywords: competing neurobehavioral decision systems, executive function decision system, impulsive decision system, SES gradient, health disparities
Socioeconomic disparities in overall health, morbidity, and mortality are pervasive, prodigious, and remain largely unaffected with current approaches (Adler & Rehkopf, 2008). In western society a strong inverse relationships exists between employment grade and mortality among British civil service workers (Marmot, Shipley & Rose, 1984) in addition to educational level and mortality among American men (Kitagawa & Hauser, 1973). The relationship between SES and health status shows a relatively smooth monotonic gradient that extends from those at the lowest to those at the highest end of the socioeconomic spectrum and thus, is not entirely accounted for by poverty-induced deprivation or access to health care (Adler & Stewart, 2010b).
In health research, SES is a broad construct describing relative access to the basic resources required to achieve and/or maintain good health (Shavers, 2007, Galobardes et al., 2006a, Galobardes et al., 2006b). Given that the SES construct is complex consisting of numerous measures, indicators, or proxies, it demonstrates that no single measure is best for all studies and populations. Individual measures of SES have varying degrees of relevance depending on context. Measures often used include educational achievement, income, occupation, and wealth; each of which assess different, albeit related aspects of SES. Measures composed of multiple components often provide more multifaceted assessments (Galobardes et al., 2006b, Galobardes et al., 2006a). Educational achievement is a widely used indicator of SES in health research and is particularly useful in relatively homogeneous populations (Miech & Hauser, 2001) where it is likely to capture important aspects of lifestyle and behavior that have a significant effect on future occupational opportunities and earning potential (Galobardes et al., 2006b, Galobardes et al., 2006a). Educational achievement, however, comprises a variety of influences, which individually contribute to an educational gradient in health behaviors. For example, material resources account for about 30% of the education gradient in health behaviors, 10% of knowledge, and 20% of cognitive ability (Cutler & Lleras-Muney, 2010). Household income, often highly correlated with educational achievement, is considered the single best indicator of material living standards (Galobardes et al., 2006b, Galobardes et al., 2006a) and is a particularly useful SES indicator for a family’s primary wage earner (Adler & Newman, 2002). The components of SES are dynamic, multidimensional, and complex reinforcing the appreciation of its multiple pathways for achieving and maintaining good health.
Conceptual models propose that socioeconomic health disparities emerge because of higher levels of stress, less access to physical and environmental resources, greater environmental constraints, fewer affective and cognitive resources, and important health behaviors associated with preventable death, such as cigarette smoking, eating and physical activity, and alcohol and illicit drug use (Adler & Newman, 2002, Gallo & Matthews, 2003, Adler & Stewart, 2010a). Taken together, these health behaviors account for the vast majority of preventable death and disease in the U.S. (see Table 1, National Research Council, 2013, Rimm et al., 1999, Schroeder, 2007, Mokdad et al., 2004). While the relationship between SES and health behaviors is dynamic and multiply determined, an important commonality among all these health behaviors is that they entail repeatedly choosing to engage in behaviors despite long-term negative outcomes, and often times, despite serious intentions to change the behaviors and many attempts to do so. We propose that the experience of SES contributes to a particular type of decision-making that serves as a conceptual and functional link between SES and health behaviors. We propose that the tendency to choose immediate over long-term rewards is likely to contribute to the development and maintenance of unhealthy behaviors. Alternatively, the tendency to choose long-term over immediate rewards is likely to contribute to the development and maintenance of healthy behaviors. We propose that the environment associated with lower SES perpetuates the former decision-making and restricts tendencies toward the latter thus accounting for a portion of the socioeconomic gradient in health behaviors and perhaps offers new targets for prevention and treatment interventions.
Table 1.
Negative Health Behavior | Mortality | Cost |
---|---|---|
Alcohol Use | 88,000 | $223.5 Billion |
Illicit Drug Use | 20,000 | $600 Billion |
Cigarette Smoking | 440000 | $96 Billion |
Over Eating/Obesity | 300,000 | $147 Billion |
The model we use to describe and interpret this decision-making style is the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems model (Bickel et al., 2012). This model views decision-making processes as resulting from the competing influences from (1) the impulsive decision system, embodied by the limbic and paralimbic brain regions, which engenders individuals to choose immediate reinforcers; and (2) the executive decision system, embodied in parts of the prefrontal cortices, which engender individuals to favor long-term outcomes and inhibit impulses. The balance between these systems is affected by a variety of factors including perceived need, motivation, context, and perceived stress level. This model is grounded in the work of Jentsch and Taylor (1999) and Bechara (2005) and consistent with a variety of dual decision system models. For example, Jentsch proposes inhibitory and impulsive systems (Jentsch & Taylor, 1999) and Strack and Deutsch outline reflective and impulsive systems (Strack & Deutsch, 2004) as conceptualizations of normal behavior (see Evans & Stanovich, 2013, for a review of the different attributes of dual systems). The Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems hypothesis provides a conceptual framework within which we can conceptualize how neurological dysfunction of the dual system can lead to behaviors with negative health outcomes (see Figure 1 and Table 2).
Table 2.
Impulsive System | Executive System | |
---|---|---|
Embodied | Limbic and Para-limbic brain regions | Prefrontal Cortex |
Function | Valuation of immediate outcomes | Valuation of delayed outcomes, planning, memory |
Health Characteristics | Regulatory Balance | Regulatory Balance |
Disease Characteristics | Relatively Hyperactive | Relatively Hypoactive |
We propose that the experience of SES affects the balance of these two competing systems. When the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems are in regulatory balance, individuals’ demonstrate the capacity to adaptively attend to temporally arrayed events, weigh input from the competing systems, and establish and follow-through with decisions that have healthy immediate and long-term outcomes. When the system is dysregulated or out of balance, perhaps due to stress or lack of cognitive resources, the capacity to establish and follow-through with decisions that have healthy immediate and delayed outcomes is constrained. When the executive decision system is less effective or compelling than is required for adaptive decision-making, we describe this as executive dysfunction. We will explore how the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems can explain the development and maintenance of negative health behaviors by the tendency to choose immediate over long-term rewards and how this tendency is rooted in neurological functioning. To achieve this end, we present a summary of the literature with reference to these important questions:
To what extent is the SES gradient a contributing factor in negative health behaviors (obesity, alcohol, tobacco, and substance abuse and dependence)?
What is the evidence that the SES gradient is associated with executive dysfunction?
What is the evidence that stress and insufficient cognitive resources contribute to executive dysfunction?
What is the evidence of executive dysfunction among individuals who smoke cigarettes, are obese, abuse alcohol, and use illicit drugs?
Finally we briefly discuss potential prevention opportunities and treatment interventions that may reduce the disparities seen across the SES gradient in decision-making and the implications for advancements in social justice.
What is the extent of the SES gradient among important health behaviors that negatively affect health?
Cigarette smoking, obesity, alcohol and illicit drug use, are the major causes of preventable death and disease in western society. They have different etiologies and long-term health consequences, but exhibit significant similarities in the socioeconomic pattern of prevalence rates.
Cigarette Smoking
The prevalence of smoking has steadily declined since the initial reports that it negatively affects health raised public awareness; however the decline in prevalence has had a remarkably different rate among higher and lower SES groups. At present, the prevalence of cigarette smoking demonstrates a significant socioeconomic gradient (BRFSS data - Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013). Lower SES groups smoke at 3 to 4 times the prevalence of higher SES groups making tobacco use one of the greatest contributors to health disparities (BRFSS - Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). While motivation to quit and attempts to quit show no socioeconomic gradient, lower SES groups are less likely to successfully quit once they begin smoking even when provided with a variety of treatments and treatment modalities (Agrawal et al., 2008, Barbeau, Krieger & Soobader, 2004, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2005, Fagan et al., 2007, Ferguson et al., 2005, Kotz & West, 2009, Reid et al., 2010, Trinidad et al., 2011, Wetter et al., 2005a, Sheffer et al., 2012b, Varghese et al., 2014, Kandel, Griesler & Schaffran, 2009). At present, tobacco use is the leading contributor to health disparities in the US and elsewhere, and unlike many other health disparities, tobacco-related cancer health disparities actually appear to be increasing (Jha et al., 2006, Kanjilal et al., 2006, Mokdad et al., 2004).
Obesity
In western culture obesity is generally recognized as an unhealthy condition and it demonstrates a consistent negative linear relationship with SES (BRFSS - Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). This relationship is the reverse of that observed in some developing countries where food scarcity contributes to a positive relationship between SES and obesity (Sobal & Stunkard, 1989). At present, the prevalence of obesity demonstrates a significant socioeconomic gradient (BRFSS data - Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013). About one-third of lower SES groups are obese while about one-quarter to one-fifth of higher SES groups are obese (BRFSS - Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). The negative relationship between SES and the prevalence of obesity is stronger and more consistent among western women than men, possibly indicating that SES is associated with different cultural norms among different SES groups (Wardle, Waller & Jarvis, 2002). Alternatively, SES might also be related to gender-specific roles and the socioeconomic positioning of women. Women continue to earn significantly less than men for the same jobs and continue to shoulder a greater burden of household and child-rearing responsibilities (Bianchi & Milkie, 2010). Women are more likely to be of lower socioeconomic status and live and raise children in poverty (US census). Consequently, increased responsibilities of child rearing combined with the stress of managing limited resources might have a more significant effect on obesity-related behaviors, such as physical activity and eating an obesogenic diet, among women than among men. Perceived stress is consistently related to obesogenic dietary behaviors among both men and women (Barrington et al., 2014). The negative relationship between SES and obesity is also greater among older individuals, suggesting that the effects of SES on obesity-related behaviors are, perhaps, cumulative (Baum II & Ruhm, 2009).
Alcohol and illicit drug use
The vast majority of individuals who drink alcohol and/or try illicit substances do so without negative health outcomes. However, significant negative linear relationships are observed between SES and alcohol and illicit drug use prevalence, health problems, and mortality (Makela, 1999, van Oers et al., 1999, Curran et al., 1999, Buka, 2002, Goodman & Huang, 2002, Harder & Chilcoat, 2007, Anthony, Warner & Kessler, 1994, BRFSS - Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). The negative relationship between drug use and SES is most robust in those countries with the largest socioeconomic differences in wealth. For example, Japan and Finland have some of the smallest socioeconomic differences in wealth and the lowest proportion of the population participating in illicit drug use; whereas, the United States and United Kingdom have the greatest differences in wealth and the highest proportion of the population participating in illicit drug use (Wilkinson & Pickett, 2011). The greater proportion of alcohol and illicit drug-related problems and mortality is accounted for by higher rates of alcohol and substance abuse or dependence in lower SES groups (Parker & Harford, 1992).
What is the evidence that SES is associated with executive dysfunction?
Conceptual models posit that executive function is embodied in the prefrontal cortices. Many of the models are very similar with significant overlap in the range and type of executive function abilities described and the neurological correlates of specific functions (Hackman & Farah, 2009, Chan et al., 2008). Bickel et al. (2012) provide a comprehensive model that is particularly useful for understanding decision-making comprised of eight systems: attention, behavioral flexibility, behavioral inhibition, planning, future valuation, working memory, control of affective states, and metacognitive processes.
Unlike most models of executive function, the Bickel et al. (2012) model includes future valuation, which is the ability to project value into the future. Future valuation is often assessed by delay discounting tasks, which assesses the decline in value of a reward as a function of time. Delay discounting, using psychophysical titration procedures (Bickel & Marsch, 2001, Hackman, Farah & Meaney, 2010), is a frequently used and highly validated research tool (Bickel et al., 2012). Extreme discounting signals an inability to modulate value by time, and is often an indicator of executive dysfunction and/or functional or neurological impairment. Surveys of large samples of individuals (n=42,863) show that lower SES groups tend to discount the value of future rewards more than higher SES groups (Reimers et al., 2009, see Green et al., 1996) suggesting a constrained capacity to modulate future valuation. Delay discounting, using psychophysical titration procedures show a consistent relationship between discounting and educational attainment (Jaroni et al., 2004, Lee et al., 2013), while use of truncated methods have failed to find that relationship (Cutler & Lleras-Muney, 2010).
The development of many executive function abilities including visuospatial skills, declarative memory, working memory, and cognitive control, appears to be affected by SES (Noble, McCandliss & Farah, 2007). The inverse relationship between executive dysfunction and SES is noticeable in the early academic career of children (Hackman et al., 2010), persists across the lifespan (Green et al., 1996, Richards & Wadsworth, 2004), and appears to be influenced and/or ameliorated by modifiable environmental factors (Mani et al., 2013) Working memory, cognitive control, and language skills demonstrate a positive relationship with SES in children as young as 6 months old (Farah et al., 2006, Hackman et al., 2010, Lipina, Martelli & Colombo, 2005). However, not all executive function abilities are dramatically affected by SES. For example, visual cognition appears to have only a moderate relationship with SES (Farah et al., 2006). Although not an executive function ability per se, it is worth noting that differences in reward processing as a function of SES are not observed (Farah et al., 2006) suggesting that the reward-seeking impulsive decision system is not deficient in lower SES individuals.
Language ability, a component of some models of executive function (Hackman & Farah, 2009), show significant positive relationships with SES, similar to most executive functions (Farah et al., 2006, Fluss et al., 2009, Hackman & Farah, 2009, Hackman et al., 2010, Perkins, Finegood & Swain, 2013). For example, 2½ year old children raised in professional families used on average, 1,116 unique words, while children from working class families used 749 unique words, and children living in homes receiving welfare benefits used 525 unique words (Hart & Risely, 1995).
Finally, a restriction of resources appears to have an immediate impact on certain executive function. A real-world extension of laboratory findings showed lower SES groups demonstrate significant declines in executive function abilities in high-cost, high stress conditions (Mani et al., 2013). Executive functioning was assessed in a sample of sugar cane farmers in India who naturally experience significant variability in financial resources throughout the year. The farmers demonstrated decreased attention and reasoning abilities during real-world conditions with decreased financial security.
What is the evidence that stress contributes to executive dysfunction?
Stress and stress regulation are significant components in biopsychosocial models of health, have a long history of being associated with the development of disease (Juster, McEwen & Lupien, 2010, Schulz et al., 2012), and are prominent in models of socioeconomic health disparities (Jackson, Knight & Rafferty, 2010). All individuals experience external events as stressful to a varying degrees and evidence indicates that perceived stress contributes to differences in executive function ability. Stress regulation includes critical decision-making such as the modulation of stress exposure and the performance of various restorative activities. Individuals act reciprocally within their environments to enhance or diminish the probability of experiencing stress. Restorative processes return individuals to baseline arousal levels and include retreating from daily stress, various relaxation behaviors, and sleep. Restorative activities play a significant role in the recovery from stress and presumably on the impact of stress on executive function abilities.
Being employed by multiple low-paying jobs with little flexibility and few available sick days and/or living arrangements that are not safe or not conducive to obtaining needed rest are likely to reduce individuals’ opportunities to retreat from daily stress, engage in relaxation behaviors, and obtain restorative sleep. Poor sleep is itself associated with impaired immune function and all-cause mortality (Dew et al., 2003, Lange et al., 2003, McEwen, 1998). Individuals in lower SES groups get less sleep and the sleep is of poorer quality than individuals in higher SES groups (Van Cauter & Spiegel, 1999, Moore et al., 2002). High pre-sleep arousal is associated with poor executive function abilities, a strong prognostic indicator of sleep disruption and chronic sleep difficulties (Bastien et al., 2008). Poor sleep quality can cause significant decreases in mental flexibility, the ability to accurately identify stressors, and the ability to inhibit rumination (Williams et al., 2009). Environmental factors and learned behaviors can also limit opportunities to engage in adequate restorative activities.
What is the evidence of executive dysfunction among individuals who smoke cigarettes, are obese, abuse alcohol, and use illicit drugs?
A plethora of evidence has linked executive dysfunction to cigarette smoking, obesity, and alcohol and illicit drug use as well as the recovery from these conditions. Much of this evidence has been reviewed elsewhere. See Bickel, et al (2012) for a review of executive dysfunction in substance abuse by type of executive dysfunction and Blume and Marlatt (2009) for a review of the role of executive function in substance abuse recovery. Smith et al. (2011) provide a systematic review of the association between executive function and obesity across the lifespan. Raman et al (2013) integrates executive function in a multifaceted model of clinical obesity. Thus, we provide a limited summary here.
Patterns of executive dysfunction are widely documented among adolescents, college students, adults, and older adults who smoke cigarettes (Cervilla, Prince & Mann, 2000, Richards & Sacker, 2003), abuse alcohol (Alterman et al., 1984, Beatty et al., 1995, Bechara et al., 2001, Bickel, Odum & Madden, 1999, Chamberlain et al., 2012), cannabis, opiates, and stimulants (Beatty et al., 1995, Berry et al., 1993, Bickel et al., 2012). Excessive discounting of delayed rewards is associated with a poorer treatment response for some cigarette smokers (MacKillop & Kahler, 2009, Sheffer et al., 2012a, Washio et al., 2011) and alcoholics (Blume, Schmaling & Marlatt, 2005, Houben, Wiers & Jansen, 2011). Nonetheless, executive function appears to remain impaired well into recovery from alcohol abuse (Bates et al., 2005). Whether substance use causes executive dysfunction and/or is a pre-existing vulnerability for developing substance abuse is unclear.
Executive function abilities are associated with the development and maintenance of obesity in children, adolescents, and adults (Davis, 2004, Galioto et al., 2012, Guxens et al., 2009). Obese individuals show deficits in a wide variety of executive functions including attention (Cserjesi et al., 2009), behavioral flexibility (Cserjesi et al., 2009, Verdejo-García et al., 2009), behavioral inhibition (Maayan et al., 2011, Verdejo-García et al., 2009), planning / reflection impulsivity (Lokken et al., 2010), discounting of delayed rewards (Bickel et al., 2010, Epstein et al., 2010), working memory (Gunstad et al., 2007, Maayan et al., 2011), emotional information processing and initiating and maintaining goal-directed responding (Bonato & Boland, 1983). Greater discounting of delayed rewards is predictive of treatment outcome for obese children (Best et al., 2012). Although causal relations between obesity and executive dysfunction abilities may be reciprocal, the mechanisms by which obesity might result in executive dysfunction are, as yet, unclear.
Discussion
Based on our review of the literature, we found evidence that the SES gradient is associated with negative health behaviors and executive function abilities. We found evidence that SES is associated with stress regulation and that increased stress levels contribute to executive dysfunction. We also found evidence of significant executive dysfunction among individuals who smoke cigarettes, are obese, and abuse alcohol or illicit drugs. This associative evidence supports our proposal that the experience of SES affects executive function and the regulatory balance of the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems; that is, SES diminishes the capacity for individuals to establish and follow-through with immediate and delayed healthy decisions.
Most would agree that multiple factors play a role in the development and maintenance of these important health behaviors. Once a behavior becomes entrenched, the biological, behavioral, and psychological aspects feed back into the bio-behavioral system through multiple iterative and multidirectional processes that make altering the behavior difficult. Nonetheless, the executive decision system provides a potential target for intervention. Executive function abilities are becoming more prominent in empirical and conceptual investigations of these complex processes (Bickel et al., 2012, Blume & Marlatt, 2009). As noted above, executive dysfunction can result in dysregulation of the dual system such that the impulsive decision system exerts greater influence than the executive decision system. This dysregulation could result in preference for immediate reinforcers consistent with choosing to smoke, over consume food and alcohol, and use illicit drugs, even when serious long-term adverse consequences may result. If executive function skills are not suitably developed or are dysfunctional, the result is greater difficulty making optimal decisions in a wide variety of health-related situations as well as greater difficulty in achieving and maintaining good health.
The implications of this line of investigation are wide reaching. The prefrontal cortices, the primary seat of executive function abilities, are highly plastic and highly adaptive to extant conditions. This plasticity is consistent with the view that executive dysfunction is likely an adaption to environmental conditions and is a potential strength in treatments designed to improve executive function and its influence on health behavior (see Bickel et al., in press). The recognition of significant executive dysfunction among individuals who abuse alcohol, illicit substances, and use tobacco has generated interest in the use of executive function remediation strategies as part of treatment. For example, in a random assignment study, Bickel and colleagues (2011) examined the effects of working memory versus control training among individuals with stimulant abuse and dependence and found that working memory training improved the ability to value delayed outcomes. Similarly, Sheffer et al. (2013) used neuromodulatory brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to improve the value of delayed outcomes among smokers. Other studies have also documented the positive effects of working memory training on alcohol consumption and obesity treatment outcomes (Houben et al., 2011, Verbeken et al., 2013). At present, whether executive function interventions will result in long-term abstinence from substances and improve other health behaviors is unclear (Bickel et al., 2011, Blume & Marlatt, 2009). Most likely these techniques will need to be paired with cognitive-behavioral approaches to achieve optimum results. Additionally, interventions could be offered to children at an early age to improve executive function alone or as part of prevention efforts aimed at improving long-term health behaviors (c.f., Ramey, Sparling & Ramey, 2012). Considerable research remains to be conducted.
All of the health behaviors and conditions mentioned here are modifiable with standard cognitive-behavioral interventions, biofeedback, meditation, and other stress reduction techniques, but are particularly effortful for individuals with limited executive function abilities. Compliance and treatment outcomes could be improved by using contingency management to increase adherence with executive function interventions among alcohol and drug dependent individuals in treatment (Silverman, DeFulio & Sigurdsson, 2012), the obese (Jeffery, 2012) and individuals with other problematic health behaviors (Higgins et al., 2012).
Given that the effects of SES on executive function are a result of adaption to the local environment and circumstances and are amenable to change supports the consideration of social justice. Indeed, social justice is an important component of conceptualizing the various policy responses to SES disparities (Greenberg & Renne, 2005, see U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA), 1998, pp 7–8 for a discussion of environmental justice). For example, Adler and Stewart (2010a) have advocated for behavioral justice, which we think may be more aptly referred to as resource justice; that is, they state, “that no group should bear a disproportionate share of health problems resulting from inadequate resources for engaging in healthy behaviors.” (p. 12) (see also Adler & Stewart, 2009). We agree and wish to support another sense of what we call ‘behavioral justice.’ Specifically, we consider it unjust for an individual not to learn the skills or be provided the means and motivation to prevent, diminish, or change health risky behaviors. The extant evidence suggests that this necessarily includes the opportunity to achieve relative balance between the executive and impulsive decision systems that enable individuals to follow through with healthy choices as predicted by the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems hypothesis. Such efforts would not only address behaviors that result in poor health and costly later stage interventions, but would also improve the quality of the lives of individuals at lower and middle segments of the SES gradient.
Highlights.
A dual decision system model is proposed as the mechanism linking SES and negative health behaviors.
Negative health behaviors are associated with the SES gradient.
Executive dysfunction is associated with the SES gradient.
Some negative health behaviors are associated with executive dysfunction.
SES related effects on executive function lead to more negative health behaviors.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Adler NE, Newman K. Socioeconomic disparities in health: Pathways and policies. Health Affairs. 2002;21:60–76. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.21.2.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler NE, Rehkopf DH. U.S. Disparities in health: Descriptions, causes, and mechanisms. Annual Review of Public Health. 2008;29:235–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler NE, Stewart J. Reducing obesity: Motivating action while not blaming the victim. The Milbank quarterly. 2009;87:49–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0009.2009.00547.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler NE, Stewart J. Health disparities across the lifespan: Meaning, methods, and mechanisms. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010a;1186:5–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler NE, Stewart J. Using team science to address health disparities: Macarthur network as case example. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010b doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05335.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A, Sartor C, Pergadia ML, Huizink AC, Lynskey MT. Correlates of smoking cessation in a nationally representative sample of u.S. Adults. Addictive behaviors. 2008;33:1223–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alterman AI, Tarter RE, Petrarulo EW, Baughman TG. Evidence for impersistence in young male alcoholics. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1984;8:448–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1984.tb05699.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anthony JC, Warner LA, Kessler RC. Comparative epidemiology of dependence on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances, and inhalants: Basic findings from the national comorbidity survey. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology. 1994;2:244–268. [Google Scholar]
- Barbeau EM, Krieger N, Soobader MJ. Working class matters: Socioeconomic disadvantage, race/ethnicity, gender, and smoking in nhis 2000. American Journal of Public Health. 2004;94:269–78. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.2.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrington WE, Beresford SA, McGregor BA, White E. Perceived stress and eating behaviors by sex, obesity status, and stress vulnerability: Findings from the vitamins and lifestyle (vital) study. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2014.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastien CH, St-Jean G, Morin CM, Turcotte I, Carrier J. Chronic psychophysiological insomnia: Hyperarousal and/or inhibition deficits? An erps investigation. Sleep Research Online. 2008;31:887–898. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.6.887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates ME, Voelbel GT, Buckman JF, Labouvie EW, Barry D. Short-term neuropsychological recovery in clients with substance use disorders. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2005;29:367–77. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000156131.88125.2a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baum CL, II, Ruhm CJ. Age, socioeconomic status and obesity growth. Journal of Health Economics. 2009;28:635–648. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beatty WW, Katzung VM, Moreland VJ, Nixon SJ. Neuropsychological performance of recently abstinent alcoholics and cocaine abusers. Drug and alcohol dependence. 1995;37:247–53. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(94)01072-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1458–63. doi: 10.1038/nn1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Dolan S, Denburg N, Hindes A, Anderson SW, Nathan PE. Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:376–89. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00136-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry J, van Gorp WG, Herzberg DS, Hinkin C, Boone K, Steinman L, Wilkins JN. Neuropsychological deficits in abstinent cocaine abusers: Preliminary findings after two weeks of abstinence. Drug and alcohol dependence. 1993;32:231–7. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(93)90087-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Best JR, Theim KR, Gredysa DM, Stein RI, Welch RR, Saelens BE, Perri MG, Schechtman KB, Epstein LH, Wilfley DE. Behavioral economic predictors of overweight children’s weight loss. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80:1086–96. doi: 10.1037/a0029827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bianchi SM, Milkie MA. Work and family research in the rst decade of the 21st century. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2010;72:705–725. [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Jarmolowicz DP, Mueller ET, Gatchalian KM, McClure SM. Are executive function and impulsivity antipodes? A conceptual reconstruction with special reference to addiction. Psychopharmacology. 2012;221:361–387. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2689-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Jones BA, Landes RD, Christensen DR, Jackson L, Mancino M. Hypothetical intertemporal choice and real economic behavior: Delay discounting predicts voucher redemptions during contingency-management procedures. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology. 2010;18:546–52. doi: 10.1037/a0021739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Marsch LA. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Addiction. 2001;96:73–86. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:447–454. doi: 10.1007/pl00005490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Quisenberry AJ, Moody L, Wilson AG. Therapeutic opportunities for self-control repair in addiction and related disorders: Change and the limits of change in trans-disease processes. Clinical Psychological Science. doi: 10.1177/2167702614541260. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, Hill PF, Baxter C. Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological psychiatry. 2011;69:260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blume AW, Marlatt GA. The role of executive cognitive functions in changing substance use: What we know and what we need to know. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;37:117–25. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9093-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blume AW, Schmaling KB, Marlatt GA. Memory, executive cognitive function, and readiness to change drinking behavior. Addictive behaviors. 2005;30:301–14. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonato DP, Boland FJ. Delay of gratification in obese children. Addictive behaviors. 1983;8:71–74. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(83)90059-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buka SL. Disparities in health status and substance use: Ethnicity and socioeconomic factors. Public Health Reports. 2002 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Annual smoking-attributable mortality, years of potential life lost, and economic costs--united states, 1997–2001. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 2005;54:3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Behavioral risk factors surveillance system: Comparability of data. Atlanta: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Behavioral risk factors surveillance system: Comparability of data. Atlanta: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Cervilla JA, Prince M, Mann A. Smoking, drinking, and incident cognitive impairment: A cohort community based study included in the gospel oak project. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 2000;68:622–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.68.5.622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain SR, Odlaug BL, Schreiber LR, Grant JE. Association between tobacco smoking and cognitive functioning in young adults. The American journal on addictions / American Academy of Psychiatrists in Alcoholism and Addictions. 2012;21(Suppl 1):S14–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2012.00290.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan RC, Shum D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EY. Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists. 2008;23:201–16. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cserjesi R, Luminet O, Poncelet AS, Lenard L. Altered executive function in obesity. Exploration of the role of affective states on cognitive abilities. Appetite. 2009;52:535–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran GM, Stoltenberg SF, Hill EM, Mudd SA, Blow FC, Zucker RA. Gender differences in the relationships among ses, family history of alcohol disorders and alcohol dependence. Journal of studies on alcohol. 1999;60:825–32. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler DM, Lleras-Muney A. Understanding differences in health behaviors by education. Journal of Health Economics. 2010;29:1–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis CL. Highlights of niddk’s translational diabetes and obesity research conference. Diabetes technology & therapeutics. 2004;6:750–4. doi: 10.1089/dia.2004.6.750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dew MA, Hoch CC, Buysse DJ, Monk TH, Begley AE, Houck PR, Hall M, Kupfer DJ, Reynolds CF., 3rd Healthy older adults’ sleep predicts all-cause mortality at 4 to 19 years of follow-up. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:63–73. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000039756.23250.7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein LH, Salvy SJ, Carr KA, Dearing KK, Bickel WK. Food reinforcement, delay discounting and obesity. Physiology and Behavior. 2010;100:438–45. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.04.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans JSBT, Stanovich KE. Dual-process theories of higher cognition-advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2013;8:223–241. doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fagan P, Shavers V, Lawrence D, Gibson JT, Ponder P. Cigarette smoking and quitting behaviors among unemployed adults in the united states. Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco. 2007;9:241–8. doi: 10.1080/14622200601080331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farah MJ, Shera DM, Savage JH, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Malmud EK, Hurt H. Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain research. 2006;1110:166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson J, Bauld L, Chesterman J, Judge K. The english smoking treatment services: One-year outcomes. Addiction. 2005;100(Suppl 2):59–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01028.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fluss J, Ziegler JC, Warszawski J, Ducot B, Richard G, Billard C. Poor reading in french elementary school: The interplay of cognitive, behavioral, and socioeconomic factors. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP. 2009;30:206–16. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181a7ed6c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galioto R, Spitznagel MB, Strain G, Devlin M, Cohen R, Paul R, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, Gunstad J. Cognitive function in morbidly obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2012;53:490–5. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallo LC, Matthews KA. Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:10–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galobardes B, Shaw M, Lawlor DA, Lynch JW, Davey Smith G. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1) Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2006a;60:7–12. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.023531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galobardes B, Shaw M, Lawlor DA, Lynch JW, Davey Smith G. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 2) Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2006b;60:95–101. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.028092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman E, Huang B. Socioeconomic status, depressive symptoms, and adolescent substance use. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2002;156:448–53. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.5.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green L, Myerson J, Lichtman D, Rosen S, Fry AF. Temporal discounting in choice between delayed rewards: The role of age and income. Psychology and Aging. 1996;11:79–84. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.11.1.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg MR, Renne J. Where does walkability matter the most? An environmental justice interpretation of new jersey data. Journal of urban health : bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine. 2005;82:90–100. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jti011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Spitznagel MB, Gordon E. Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Comprehensive psychiatry. 2007;48:57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guxens M, Mendez MA, Julvez J, Plana E, Forns J, Basagana X, Torrent M, Sunyer J. Cognitive function and overweight in preschool children. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;170:438–46. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2009;13:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:651–9. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harder VS, Chilcoat HD. Cocaine use and educational achievement: Understanding a changing association over the past 2 decades. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97:1790–3. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.091108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart B, Risely TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young american children. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing Co; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ST, Silverman K, Sigmon SC, Naito NA. Incentives and health: An introduction. Preventive Medicine. 2012;55(Suppl):S2–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben K, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Getting a grip on drinking behavior: Training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science. 2011;22:968–75. doi: 10.1177/0956797611412392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson JS, Knight KM, Rafferty JA. Race and unhealthy behaviors: Chronic stress, the hpa axis, and physical and mental health disparities over the life course. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100:933–9. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.143446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaroni JL, Wright SM, Lerman C, Epstein LH. Relationship between education and delay discounting in smokers. Addictive behaviors. 2004;29:1171–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffery RW. Financial incentives and weight control. Preventive Medicine. 2012;55(Suppl):S61–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.12.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch JD, Taylor JR. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:373–390. doi: 10.1007/pl00005483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jha P, Peto R, Zatonski W, Boreham J, Jarvis MJ, Lopez AD. Social inequalities in male mortality, and in male mortality from smoking: Indirect estimation from national death rates in england and wales, poland, and north america. Lancet. 2006;368:367–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68975-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juster RP, McEwen BS, Lupien SJ. Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2010;35:2–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, Griesler PC, Schaffran C. Educational attainment and smoking among women: Risk factors and consequences for offspring. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2009;104:S24–S33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanjilal S, Gregg EW, Cheng YJ, Zhang P, Nelson DE, Mensah G, Beckles GL. Socioeconomic status and trends in disparities in 4 major risk factors for cardiovascular disease among us adults, 1971–2002. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2006;166:2348–55. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.21.2348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitagawa E, Hauser P. Differential mortality in the united states. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Kotz D, West R. Explaining the social gradient in smoking cessation: It’s not in the trying, but in the succeeding. Tobacco Control. 2009;18:43–6. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.025981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange T, Perras B, Fehm HL, Born J. Sleep enhances the human antibody response to hepatitis a vaccination. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:831–5. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000091382.61178.f1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee NC, de Groot RH, Boschloo A, Dekker S, Krabbendam L, Jolles J. Age and educational track influence adolescent discounting of delayed rewards. Frontiers in psychology. 2013;4:993. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipina SJ, Martelli MI, Colombo JA. Performance on the a-not-b task of argentinean infants from unsatisfied and satisfied basic needs homes. Revista interamericana de psicología= Interamerican journal of psychology. 2005;39:49–60. [Google Scholar]
- Lokken KL, Boeka AG, Yellumahanthi K, Wesley M, Clements RH. Cognitive performance of morbidly obese patients seeking bariatric surgery. American Surgeon. 2010;76:55–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maayan L, Hoogendoorn C, Sweat V, Convit A. Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity. 2011 Jul;:1382–7. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKillop J, Kahler CW. Delayed reward discounting predicts treatment response for heavy drinkers receiving smoking cessation treatment. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2009;104:197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makela P. Alcohol-related mortality as a function of socio-economic status. Addiction. 1999;94:867–86. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94686710.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mani A, Mullainathan S, Shafir E, Zhao J. Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science. 2013;341:976–80. doi: 10.1126/science.1238041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marmot MG, Shipley MJ, Rose G. Inequalities in death--specific explanations of a general pattern? Lancet. 1984;1:1003–6. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(84)92337-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1998;840:33–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinnis JM, Foege WH. Actual causes of death in the united states. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Association. 1993;270:2207–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miech RA, Hauser RM. Socioeconomic status and health at midlife. A comparison of educational attainment with occupation-based indicators. Annals of Epidemiology. 2001;11:75–84. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(00)00079-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mokdad AH, Marks JS, Stroup DF, Gerberding JL. Actual causes of death in the united states, 2000. JAMA. 2004;291:1238–1245. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.10.1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore PJ, Adler NE, Williams DR, Jackson JS. Socioeconomic status and health: The role of sleep. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2002;64:337–44. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200203000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute on Drug Abuse. Drugfacts: Understanding drug abuse and addiction. Washington, D.C: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council. U S Health in international perspective: Shorter lives, poorer health. Washington, D. C: The National Academies Press; 2013. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Sciences. 2007;10:464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker DA, Harford TC. The epidemiology of alcohol consumption and dependence across occupations in the united states. Alcohol Health and Research World. 1992;16:97–105. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins SC, Finegood ED, Swain JE. Poverty and language development: Roles of parenting and stress. Innovations in clinical neuroscience. 2013;10:10–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raman J, Smith E, Hay P. The clinical obesity maintenance model: An integration of psychological constructs including mood, emotional regulation, disordered overeating, habitual cluster behaviours, health literacy and cognitive function. Journal of obesity. 2013;2013:240128. doi: 10.1155/2013/240128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramey CT, Sparling JL, Ramey SL. Abecedarian: The ideas, the approach, and the findings. Los Altos, CA: Sociometrics Corporation; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Reid RD, Mullen KA, Slovinec D’Angelo ME, Aitken DA, Papadakis S, Haley PM, McLaughlin CA, Pipe AL. Smoking cessation for hospitalized smokers: An evaluation of the “ottawa model”. Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco. 2010;12:11–8. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reimers S, Maylor EA, Stewart N, Chater N. Associations between a oneshot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;47:973–978. [Google Scholar]
- Richards M, Sacker A. Lifetime antecedents of cognitive reserve. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 2003;25:614–24. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.614.14581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards M, Wadsworth ME. Long term effects of early adversity on cognitive function. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 2004;89:922–7. doi: 10.1136/adc.2003.032490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimm EB, Williams P, Fosher K, Criqui M, Stampfer MJ. Moderate alcohol intake and lower risk of coronary heart disease: Meta-analysis of effects on lipids and haemostatic factors. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed) 1999;319:1523–8. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7224.1523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder SA. We can do better improving the health of the american people. New England Journal of Medicine. 2007;357:1221–1228. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa073350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz AJ, Mentz G, Lachance L, Johnson J, Gaines C, Israel BA. Associations between socioeconomic status and allostatic load: Effects of neighborhood poverty and tests of mediating pathways. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102:1706–14. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shavers VL. Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2007;99:1013–23. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheffer CE, MacKillop J, McGeary J, Landes RD, Carter L, Yi R, Jones B, Christensen D, Stitzer M, Jackson L, Bickel WK. Delay discounting, locus of control, and cognitive impulsiveness independently predict tobacco dependence treatment outcomes in a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic group of smokers. American Journal on Addictions. 2012a;21:221–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1521-0391.2012.00224.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheffer CE, Mennemeier M, Landes RD, Bickel WK, Brackman S, Dornhoffer J, Kimbrell T, Brown G. Neuromodulation of delay discounting, the reflection effect, and cigarette consumption. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2013.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheffer CE, Stitzer M, Landes R, Brackman SL, Munn T, Moore P. Socioeconomic disparities in community-based treatment of tobacco dependence. American Journal of Public Health. 2012b;102:e8–16. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman K, DeFulio A, Sigurdsson SO. Maintenance of reinforcement to address the chronic nature of drug addiction. Preventive Medicine. 2012;55(Suppl):S46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Hinderliter A, Sherwood A. Association of vascular health and neurocognitive performance in overweight adults with high blood pressure. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. 2011;33:559–66. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.537648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sobal J, Stunkard AJ. Socioeconomic status and obesity: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin. 1989;105:260–75. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strack F, Deutsch R. Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc. 2004;8:220–47. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trinidad DR, Perez-Stable EJ, White MM, Emery SL, Messer K. A nationwide analysis of us racial/ethnic disparities in smoking behaviors, smoking cessation, and cessation-related factors. American Journal of Public Health. 2011;101:699–706. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2010.191668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Overweight and obesity: A major public health issue. Prevention Report 2001 [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) Guidance for incorporating environmental justice concerns in epa’s nepa compliance analyses. Washington, D. C: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Van Cauter E, Spiegel K. Sleep as a mediator of the relationship between socioeconomic status and health: A hypothesis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999;896:254–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08120.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Oers JA, Bongers IM, van de Goor LA, Garretsen HF. Alcohol consumption, alcohol-related problems, problem drinking, and socioeconomic status. Alcohol and alcoholism. 1999;34:78–88. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/34.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varghese M, Sheffer CE, Stitzer M, Landes R, Brackman SL, Munn T. Socioeconomic disparities in community-based treatment of tobacco dependence. American Journal of Public Health. 2014 doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbeken S, Braet C, Goossens L, van der Oord S. Executive function training with game elements for obese children: A novel treatment to enhance self-regulatory abilities for weight-control. Behaviour research and therapy. 2013;51:290–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo-García A, Pérez-Expósito M, Schmidt-Río-Valle J, Fernández-Serrano M, Cruz F, Pérez-García M, López-Belmonte G, Martín-Matillas M, Martín-Lagos J, Marcos A, Campoy C. Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Behavior and Psychology. 2009 doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle J, Waller J, Jarvis MJ. Sex differences in the association of socioeconomic status with obesity. American Journal of Public Health. 2002;92:1299–304. doi: 10.2105/ajph.92.8.1299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Washio Y, Higgins ST, Heil SH, McKerchar TL, Badger GJ, Skelly JM, Dantona RL. Delay discounting is associated with treatment response among cocaine-dependent outpatients. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology. 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0023617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetter DW, Cofta-Gunn L, Fouladi RT, Irvin JE, Daza P, Mazas C, Wright K, Cinciripini PM, Gritz ER. Understanding the associations among education, employment characteristics, and smoking. Addictive behaviors. 2005a;30:905–914. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson R, Pickett K. Greater equality: The hidden key to better health and higher scores. American Educator. 2011 Spring;:5–9. [Google Scholar]
- Williams LM, Gatt JM, Schofield PR, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E. ‘Negativity bias’ in risk for depression and anxiety: Brain-body fear circuitry correlates, 5-htt-lpr and early life stress. NeuroImage. 2009;47:804–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]