Abstract
Individuals with schizophrenia often show substantial deficits in social cognitive abilities, which are strongly associated with social functioning. To advance our understanding of the genetic variation that is associated with social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, we genotyped 74 schizophrenia outpatients who completed social cognitive performance measures assessing mentalizing, social perception, and emotional intelligence, as well as clinical symptoms. We assessed seven single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) previously found to show replicable associations with socio-emotional processes. For one of the seven SNPs, rs2268493, the ‘T’ allele was significantly associated with poorer performance on a composite social cognition index, as well as specific tests of mentalizing and social perception. None of the SNPs were associated with clinical symptoms. Though the sample size is small, these findings provide initial support for the involvement of genetic variants of the OXTR in social cognitive impairments in schizophrenia.
Keywords: schizophrenia, social cognition, oxytocin, oxytocin receptor, single nucleotide polymorphism
1. Introduction
Individuals with schizophrenia often show marked deficits in social cognitive abilities, and these deficits are predictive of poor functioning (Fett et al., 2011; Horan et al., 2012). However, the mechanisms underlying impaired social cognition and its variability across patients are largely unknown. One possible contributor is the oxytocin system, which plays an important role in social cognition and behaviors in humans and other animals (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011). This system is of particular interest in schizophrenia, given several recent studies suggesting potential use of intranasal oxytocin as a therapeutic agent (MacDonald and Feifel, 2012).
Many genetic studies have investigated the role of the oxytocin system in social processes, primarily in healthy individuals, and most have focused on genetic variation of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) (Ebstein et al., 2012). The two most widely studied single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the OXTR are rs53576 and rs2254298. These SNPs have frequently shown associations with empathy and various pro-social behaviors, though not all studies have supported such links (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn, 2014). Other OXTR SNPs have also shown replicable associations with similar pro-social behaviors (Kumsta and Heinrichs, 2013).
In schizophrenia research, several OXTR SNPs have shown initial associations with the schizophrenia diagnosis in case-control studies (Montag et al., 2013b; Souza et al., 2010b; Watanabe et al., 2012), as well as with symptom severity and response to clozapine therapy (Montag et al., 2013b; Montag et al., 2012; Souza et al., 2010a). Very little is known about the relationship of OXTR in schizophrenia and social cognition. Only one previous report examined this connection and used a self-report measure of empathy. That study found schizophrenia patients with an ‘A’ allele of rs2254298 scored higher on one subscale (i.e., Empathic Concern) than those with the ‘GG’ genotype (Montag et al., 2012).
Given the potential importance of genetic variation in the OXTR and the limited knowledge of its role in schizophrenia, we genotyped seven OXTR SNPs in schizophrenia patients who had been characterized on social cognitive performance measures. The seven SNPs were chosen based on their replicated associations with socio-emotional processing variables in prior studies. Considering the relatively small sample size, we view these findings as hypothesis-generating for future larger studies.
Experimental Methods
1.1 Participants
Participants were a subset of a larger study on social cognitive determinants of outcome (Green et al., 2012) and consisted of 74 schizophrenia outpatients (Table 1) recruited from the VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System (VAGLAHS) and the surrounding community. Selection criteria included age 18–60, black or white race, no active substance use disorder within the prior 6 months, no neurological disorders, IQ > 70 based on medical record review, no prior loss of consciousness > 1 hour, fluency in English, and capacity to give informed consent. Current use of an antipsychotic medication was not an inclusion criterion; however, 80% of the participants in the same were taking an atypical antipsychotic; 7% were taking a typical antipsychotic; 7% were taking both atypical and typical antipsychotics; and 6% were not taking an antipsychotic. The study was approved by the VAGLAHS Institutional Review Board.
Table 1.
Sample Characteristics (N = 74)
Characteristic | Value | |
---|---|---|
Gender: | Male | 72% |
Female | 28% | |
Age: | mean years (SD) | 46.1 (10.5) |
Education: | mean years (SD) | 12.7 (1.7) |
Race: | White | 61% |
Black | 39% | |
Symptoms: | BPRS positive symptoms score (SD) | 2.2 (.9) |
SANS expression score (SD) | 1.0 (1.1) | |
SANS experience score (SD) | 2.6 (1.1) | |
Social Cognitive Performance: | TASIT III total (SD) | 45.7 (7.6) |
PONS total (SD) | 78.9 (7.7) | |
MSCEIT total (SD) | 81.4 (13.4) |
1.2 SNP Selection
We selected seven OXTR SNPs based on a literature review, genotyping SNPs with previous positive findings related to social cognitive or emotional processes in any type of sample, and at least one replication by an independent research group. All seven SNPs had a minor allele frequency > 0.10 (see Table 2).
Table 2.
List of Analyzed OXTR SNPs
obtained from 1000Genome dataset (Consortium, 2012)
In the third column, Position refers to the location of the SNP in the OXTR gene. UTR = untranslated region. IVS = intervening sequence (i.e., an intron). Ala → Thr = mutation in the coding sequence causing threonine to be incorporated in the protein instead of alanine.
1.3 Genotyping
Blood samples (30–40 mL) were obtained from participants to extract DNA. DNA extraction was performed with the QIAamp Blood Maxi Kit according to the manufacturer’s protocol (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). SNPs were genotyped using Life Technologies’ TaqMan SNP Genotyping Assays and TaqMan probes for 7 selected SNPs according to the manufacturer’s protocol (Life Technologies Corp., Carlsbad, CA). All SNPs were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium and repeat genotyping of a subset (~10%) of the samples produced complete concordance.
1.4 Social Cognition Assessments
The social cognitive battery included measures of social perception, mentalizing, and emotion intelligence and is described more fully in Green et al. (2012). Social perception was assessed using the Half-Profile of Nonverbal Sensitivity (PONS; (Rosenthal et al., 1979)) with administration modified to reduce demands on reading comprehension and attention (Green et al., 2012). The dependent measure was the number of correct items out of 110. Mentalizing was assessed using The Awareness of Social Inference Test - Part III (TASIT; (McDonald et al., 2003)), with the number of correct items out of 64 as the dependent measure. Emotional intelligence (the ability to identify, use, understand, and manage emotions) was assessed using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence 2.0 (MSCEIT; (Mayer et al., 2003)), with the Total Score (standardized score based on community norms) used as the dependent measure. A composite social cognition Summary Score was calculated based on the mean of the z-scores of the individual measures
1.5 Clinical Symptom Assessments
Psychiatric symptoms were assessed using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) 24-item version (Ventura et al., 1993) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS; (Andreasen, 1984)). We report the BPRS positive symptom score (mean of unusual thought content, hallucinations, and conceptual disorganization items) and the expression (mean of affective blunting and alogia) and experience (mean of avolition and anhedonia) subscores for the SANS.
1.6 Statistical Analysis
Statistical tests were performed with SPSS Statistics version 20 (IBM). Genotype associations with social cognitive and symptom measures were evaluated using a series of ANOVA’s with allele subgroups for each SNP entered as independent variables. Race and gender were initially included as covariates but were removed from all the reported models because no significant main or interaction effects involving race or gender were found. In situations in which there were ≤ 3 subjects in an allele group, we collapsed across categories by combining the low-frequency homozygotic individuals with the heterozygotic subjects. This occurred for 3 SNPs: rs2268493, rs2254298, and rs2686302. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s f, which is based on explained variance. It generalizes Cohen’s d to a multigroup ANOVA. Cohen’s f is equivalent to d/2 in SNPs that were collapsed to two levels (Cohen).
2. Results
2.1 Social Cognitive Performance
Of the 7 genotyped SNPs, only one showed statistically significant (p<.05) differences among allele subgroups on the social cognitive measures (Table 3). Rs2268493 genotypes showed significant differences on the social cognition summary score, as well as the TASIT and PONS. The differences involving this SNP (using z-scores of individual social cognitive measures) are shown in Figure 1. Participants with the ‘TT’ genotype (n=50), the identified risk allele (Campbell et al., 2011; Damiano et al., 2014; Kawamura et al., 2010), performed more poorly across these measures than those in the combined ‘CC + CT’ group (n=23). The only other SNP that approached significance was rs1042778, which showed trend level associations with the TASIT (p=.091).
Table 3.
Genotypic Differences in Social Cognitive Performance (expressed as effect size Cohen’s f, p value)
Measure | rs1042778 | rs7632287 | rs237887 | rs22684931 | rs22542982 | rs53576 | rs46863023 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Summary | .25, .108 | .07, .843 | .11, .647 | .25, .031 | .04, .710 | .11, .672 | .08, .519 |
TASIT III | .26, .091 | .01, .993 | .16, .388 | .25, .034 | .11, .371 | .12, .584 | .14, .225 |
PONS | .21, .208 | .08, .788 | .09, .758 | .23, .050 | .01, .953 | .05, .926 | .02, .883 |
MSCEIT | .21, .218 | .13, .543 | .05, .907 | .17, .148 | .05, .674 | .14, .518 | .09, .463 |
Compared TT vs. CC/CT combined
Compared GG vs. AA/AG combined
Compared CC vs. TT/TC combined
Figure 1.
Performance on Individual and Summary Social Cognitive Measures by rs2268493 Genotype: Summary represents the mean of z-scores from the three social cognitive measures. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean.
2.2 Psychiatric Symptoms
None of the SNPs we genotyped were significantly associated with BPRS positive symptoms or SANS scores (Table 4). There were trend level associations for the rs1042778 genotype with the SANS expression subscore (p=.087).
Table 4.
Genotypic Differences in Psychiatric Symptoms (expressed as effect size Cohen’s f, p value)
Measure | rs1042778 | rs7632287 | rs237887 | rs22684931 | rs22542982 | rs53576 | rs46863023 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BPRS positive symptoms | .18, .291 | .18, .302 | .21, .212 | .03, .833 | .15, .198 | .22, .161 | .13, .273 |
SANS expression | .26, .087 | .06, .885 | .17, .333 | .06, .604 | .19, .109 | .13, .529 | .07, .577 |
SANS experience | .09, .749 | .16, .395 | .12, .623 | .16, .181 | .02, .874 | .15, .438 | .03, .776 |
Compared TT vs. CC/CT combined
Compared GG vs. AA/AG combined
Compared CC vs. TT/TC combined
3. Discussion
These findings provide preliminary support for the involvement of genetic variants of the OXTR in social cognitive impairments in schizophrenia. Of the 7 SNPs examined, one of these, rs2268493, showed a significant association with social cognitive performance in this sample of schizophrenia individuals. Our finding that the “T” allele of this SNP was associated with poorer social cognition, particularly in the areas of mentalizing and social perception, is consistent with prior studies in other samples. The “T” allele of rs2268493 has been associated with risk for autism spectrum disorders (Campbell et al., 2011), depressive temperament (Kawamura et al., 2010), and decreased mesolimbic activation during reward anticipation (Damiano et al., 2014). This convergence of results bolsters support for further investigation of OXTR and related genetic polymorphisms in schizophrenia.
The genetic mechanism by which rs2268493 variants could affect social cognitive processes is not known, but it is possible that these variants alter OXTR expression. Rs2268493 is an intronic SNP and its locus appears to overlap 6 different regulatory motifs, as well as predict an enhancer chromatin state (HaploReg; (Ward and Kellis, 2012)). This indicates that variations in genotype may affect transcription factor binding and gene expression. Along these lines, differences in regional OXTR expression density have been found to affect the processing of social information and affiliative behaviors in animals (Ross and Young, 2009). Another explanation for these results could involve linkage disequilibrium. The rs2268493 locus is in disequilibrium r2 ≥ 0.7 with 6 other intronic OXTR SNPs (rs2268492, rs60902022, rs60345038, rs13316193, rs11131148, and rs11131149) with associated regulatory motifs and enhancing chromatin states (Ward and Kellis, 2012). This linkage among OXTR SNPs suggests that the effects we observe with rs2268493 could be caused by a related SNP.
The main limitation of this study is that the sample size is small. This limited our power to examine the effects of race or gender in our analyses. Futhermore, our statistical tests were not corrected for multiple comparisons so replication in larger samples will be essential. Our failure to replicate a prior association between rs2254298 and a social cognitive measure in schizophrenia (Montag et al., 2012) may be attributable to our use of performance-based rather than self-report outcome measures. Alternatively, this difference, as well as the absence of significant associations with clinical symptoms, may reflect limited statistical power associated with the modest sample size. Despite these limitations, this preliminary study supports further investigation of OXTR and related polymorphisms (e.g., oxytocin and vasopressin genes, vasopressin receptor genes) in schizophrenia and our results may be valuable for hypothesis generation and planning future studies.
Acknowledgements
We thank Mark McGee for study coordination and all of the individuals who participated in this study.
Role of Funding Source
This work was supported by NIH MH043292 to MFG; NIH MH094613-01 to ELN; and the VISN 22 MIRECC.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributors
All authors contributed significantly and collaboratively to this work. Dr. Davis, Dr. Horan, Dr. Nurmi, and Dr. Green designed this study. Dr. Horan and Dr. Green developed the social cognitive assessment battery and interpreted the social cognitive data. Dr. Nurmi and Ms. Li performed genetic analyses and helped interpret genetic data. Dr. Rizzo and Dr. Sugar performed statistical analyses and provided support for data analysis. Dr. Davis wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest
Potential Conflicts of Interest: None of the authors of this manuscript have conflicts of interest to disclose.
Author Disclosures: Dr. Green reports having been a consultant to AbbVie, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, Forum, and Roche; he is a member of the scientific board for Mnemosyne; and he has received research funds from Amgen.
References
- Andreasen NC. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) and serotonin transporter (5-HTT) genes associated with observed parenting. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2008;3(2):128–134. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatric genetics. 2014;24(2):45–51. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e3283643684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DB, Datta D, Jones ST, Lee EB, Sutcliffe JS, Hammock EA, Levitt P. Association of oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene variants with multiple phenotype domains of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of neurodevelopmental disorders. 2011;3(2):101–112. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9071-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang S-C, Glymour MM, Rewak M, Cornelis MC, Walter S, Koenen KC, Kawachi I, Liang L, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Kubzansky LD. Are genetic variations in< i> OXTR</i>,< i> AVPR1A</i>, and < i> CD38</i>genes important to social integration? Results from two large US cohorts. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;39:257–268. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen FS, Kumsta R, von Dawans B, Monakhov M, Ebstein RP, Heinrichs M. Common oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphism and social support interact to reduce stress in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(50):19937–19942. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113079108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Eribaum Associates; 1988. p. xxi 567. [Google Scholar]
- Consortium GP. An integrated map of genetic variation from 1,092 human genomes. Nature. 2012;491(7422):56–65. doi: 10.1038/nature11632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa B, Pini S, Gabelloni P, Abelli M, Lari L, Cardini A, Muti M, Gesi C, Landi S, Galderisi S. Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and adult attachment style in patients with depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(10):1506–1514. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damiano CR, Aloi J, Dunlap K, Burrus CJ, Mosner MG, Kozink RV, McLaurin RE, Carter RM, Huettel SA, McClernon FJ. Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and mesolimbic responses to rewards. Molecular Autism. 2014;5(1):7. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebstein RP, Knafo A, Mankuta D, Chew SH, Lai PS. The contributions of oxytocin and vasopressin pathway genes to human behavior. Hormones and behavior. 2012;61(3):359–379. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman R, Zagoory-Sharon O, Weisman O, Schneiderman I, Gordon I, Maoz R, Shalev I, Ebstein RP. Sensitive Parenting Is Associated with Plasma Oxytocin and Polymorphisms in the< i> OXTR</i> and< i> CD38</i> Genes. Biological psychiatry. 2012;72(3):175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):573–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Hellemann G, Horan WP, Lee J, Wynn JK. From perception to functional outcome in schizophrenia: modeling the role of ability and motivation. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69(12):1216–1224. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horan WP, Green MF, DeGroot M, Fiske A, Hellemann G, Kee K, Kern RS, Lee J, Sergi MJ, Subotnik KL, Sugar CA, Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH. Social cognition in schizophrenia, Part 2: 12-month stability and prediction of functional outcome in first-episode patients. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(4):865–872. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hostinar CE, Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism, perceived social support, and psychological symptoms in maltreated adolescents. Development and psychopathology. 2014;26(02):465–477. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Israel S, Lerer E, Shalev I, Uzefovsky F, Riebold M, Laiba E, Bachner-Melman R, Maril A, Bornstein G, Knafo A. The oxytocin receptor (OXTR) contributes to prosocial fund allocations in the dictator game and the social value orientations task. PLoS One. 2009;4(5):e5535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacob S, Brune CW, Carter C, Leventhal BL, Lord C, Cook EH., Jr Association of the oxytocin receptor gene (< i> OXTR</i>) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism. Neuroscience letters. 2007;417(1):6–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawamura Y, Liu X, Akiyama T, Shimada T, Otowa T, Sakai Y, Kakiuchi C, Umekage T, Sasaki T, Akiskal HS. The association between oxytocin receptor gene (< i>OXTR</i>) polymorphisms and affective temperaments, as measured by TEMPS-A. Journal of affective disorders. 2010;127(1):31–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim HS, Sherman DK, Sasaki JY, Xu J, Chu TQ, Ryu C, Suh EM, Graham K, Taylor SE. Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(36):15717–15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kogan A, Saslow LR, Impett EA, Oveis C, Keltner D, Saturn SR. Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and expression of the prosocial disposition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(48):19189–19192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112658108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumsta R, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin, stress and social behavior: neurogenetics of the human oxytocin system. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2013;23(1):11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerer E, Levi S, Salomon S, Darvasi A, Yirmiya N, Ebstein R. Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship to Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and cognition. Molecular psychiatry. 2007;13(10):980–988. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Kawamura Y, Shimada T, Otowa T, Koishi S, Sugiyama T, Nishida H, Hashimoto O, Nakagami R, Tochigi M. Association of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the Japanese population. Journal of human genetics. 2010;55(3):137–141. doi: 10.1038/jhg.2009.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucht MJ, Barnow S, Sonnenfeld C, Rosenberger A, Grabe HJ, Schroeder W, Völzke H, Freyberger HJ, Herrmann FH, Kroemer H. Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (< i> OXTR</i>) and affect, loneliness and intelligence in normal subjects. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2009;33(5):860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald K, Feifel D. Oxytocin in schizophrenia: a review of evidence for its therapeutic effects. Acta neuropsychiatrica. 2012;24(3):130–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2011.00634.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malik AI, Zai CC, Abu Z, Nowrouzi B, Beitchman JH. The role of oxytocin and oxytocin receptor gene variants in childhood-onset aggression. Genes, brain and behavior. 2012;11(5):545–551. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00776.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh AA, Henry HY, Pine DS, Gorodetsky EK, Goldman D, Blair R. The influence of oxytocin administration on responses to infant faces and potential moderation by OXTR genotype. Psychopharmacology. 2012;224(4):469–476. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2775-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR, Sitarenios G. Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion. 2003;3(1):97–105. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald S, Flanagan S, Rollins J, Kinch J. TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2003;18(3):219–238. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200305000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(9):524–538. doi: 10.1038/nrn3044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montag C, Brockmann E-M, Bayerl M, Rujescu D, Müller DJ, Gallinat J. Oxytocin and oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and risk for schizophrenia: a case-control study. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry. 2013a;14(7):500–508. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2012.677547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montag C, Brockmann EM, Bayerl M, Rujescu D, Muller DJ, Gallinat J. Oxytocin and oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and risk for schizophrenia: a case-control study. The world journal of biological psychiatry : the official journal of the World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry. 2013b;14(7):500–508. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2012.677547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montag C, Brockmann EM, Lehmann A, Muller DJ, Rujescu D, Gallinat J. Association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and self-rated 'empathic concern' in schizophrenia. PloS one. 2012;7(12):e51882. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moons WG, Way BM, Taylor SE. Oxytocin and Vasopressin Receptor Polymorphisms Interact With Circulating Neuropeptides to Predict Human Emotional Reactions to Stress. 2014 doi: 10.1037/a0035503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park J, Willmott M, Vetuz G, Toye C, Kirley A, Hawi Z, Brookes K, Gill M, Kent L. Evidence that genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor (< i> OXTR</i>) gene influences social cognition in ADHD. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2010;34(4):697–702. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106(50):21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R, Hall JA, DiMatteo MR, Rogers PL, Archer D. Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Ross HE, Young LJ. Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2009;30(4):534–547. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saphire-Bernstein S, Way BM, Kim HS, Sherman DK, Taylor SE. Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is related to psychological resources. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(37):15118–15122. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113137108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skuse DH, Lori A, Cubells JF, Lee I, Conneely KN, Puura K, Lehtimäki T, Binder EB, Young LJ. Common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with human social recognition skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302985111. 201302985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KE, Porges EC, Norman GJ, Connelly JJ, Decety J. Oxytocin receptor gene variation predicts empathic concern and autonomic arousal while perceiving harm to others. Social neuroscience. 2014;9(1):1–9. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.863223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Souza RP, de Luca V, Meltzer HY, Lieberman JA, Kennedy JL. Schizophrenia severity and clozapine treatment outcome association with oxytocinergic genes. The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology / official scientific journal of the Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum. 2010a;13(6):793–798. doi: 10.1017/S1461145710000167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Souza RP, Ismail P, Meltzer HY, Kennedy JL. Variants in the oxytocin gene and risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2010b;121(1–3):279–280. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sturge-Apple ML, Cicchetti D, Davies PT, Suor JH. Differential susceptibility in spillover between interparental conflict and maternal parenting practices: Evidence for OXTR and 5-HTT genes. Journal of Family Psychology. 2012;26(3):431. doi: 10.1037/a0028302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabak BA, McCullough ME, Carver CS, Pedersen EJ, Cuccaro ML. Variation in oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms is associated with emotional and behavioral reactions to betrayal. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2013 doi: 10.1093/scan/nst042. nst042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RJ, Parker KJ, Hallmayer JF, Waugh CE, Gotlib IH. Oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (rs2254298) interacts with familial risk for psychopathology to predict symptoms of depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36(1):144–147. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tops M, Van IJzendoorn MH, Riem MM, Boksem MA, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Oxytocin receptor gene associated with the efficiency of social auditory processing. Frontiers in psychiatry. 2011;2 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tost H, Kolachana B, Hakimi S, Lemaitre H, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR, Meyer–Lindenberg A. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(31):13936–13941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003296107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ventura J, Lukoff D, Nuechterlein KH, Liberman RL, Green MF, Shaner A. Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1993;3:227–243. [Google Scholar]
- Walum H, Lichtenstein P, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Pedersen NL, Anckarsäter H, Larsson H, Westberg L. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biological psychiatry. 2012;71(5):419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward LD, Kellis M. HaploReg: a resource for exploring chromatin states, conservation, and regulatory motif alterations within sets of genetically linked variants. Nucleic acids research. 2012;40:D930–D934. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkr917. Database issue. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe Y, Kaneko N, Nunokawa A, Shibuya M, Egawa J, Someya T. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and risk of schizophrenia: case-control and family-based analyses and meta-analysis in a Japanese population. Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences. 2012;66(7):622. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2012.02396.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu N, Li Z, Su Y. The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;138(3):468–472. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu S, Jia M, Ruan Y, Liu J, Guo Y, Shuang M, Gong X, Zhang Y, Yang X, Zhang D. Positive Association of the Oxytocin Receptor Gene (< i> OXTR</i>) with Autism in the Chinese Han Population. Biological psychiatry. 2005;58(1):74–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]