Abstract
Objective
To determine: (1) the effect of exposure to multiple social risks on cognitive delay at nine-months of age; and (2) whether obstetric factors mediate the relationship between cumulative social risk and cognitive delay.
Methods
Data were from 8,950 mother-child dyads participating in the first wave of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort. Cognitive delay was defined as falling in the lowest 10% of mental scale scores from the Bayley Short Form-Research Edition. Five social risk factors were combined and categorized into a “social risk index.” Staged multivariable logistic regressions were used to investigate whether obstetric factors mediated the impact of social risk on the odds of cognitive delay.
Results
Infants with cognitive delay were more likely to live with social risks than infants without cognitive delay. The percentage of infants with cognitive delay increased with the number of social risks. In adjusted analyses, exposure to multiple social risk factors was associated with higher odds of cognitive delay at nine-months of age (adjusted odds ratio 2.11; 95% confidence interval: 1.18-3.78 for four or more risks versus no risks). Accounting for birthweight attenuated this relationship (p<0.001).
Conclusions
This is the first population-based study to investigate the independent and cumulative effects of social risk factors on cognitive delay in infancy. Findings show a significant cumulative relationship between exposure to social risk and cognitive delay, which is partly mediated by birthweight. Programs that address the social context of US infants are needed to improve their developmental trajectories.
What's New
National data show a significant cumulative relationship between exposure to social risks and cognitive delay in infancy, which is mediated by infant birthweight. Programs that address the social context of US infants are needed to improve their developmental trajectories.
Keywords: Child development, infancy, social environment, birthweight, lifecourse
INTRODUCTION
Social risk factors including poverty,1 single-parent families,2 and low parental education3 are consistently among the strongest correlates of poor cognitive and developmental outcomes among children. However, despite the well-established relationship between social risk factors and children's cognitive outcomes and academic achievement,4-8 comparatively little work has investigated the relationship between social risk factors and cognitive delay in infancy. This represents a vital gap in the literature as infancy marks a critical period with significant implications for children's cognitive development. In fact, the prevalence of cognitive delay has been shown to be highly dynamic in early childhood, whereby a substantial proportion of infants with cognitive delays at nine months of age display normal cognitive development by 24 months (Cheng ER et al, under review). Nevertheless, for some children, early cognitive delays persist over time4 threatening their school-readiness9 and long-term health and functioning.10 Improving our understanding of the relationship between social risk factors and cognitive delay during this critical stage in the lifespan may therefore have substantial impacts on prevention, treatment, and rehabilitation strategies.
The relationship between social risk factors and early cognitive outcomes may be best explored from the lifecourse perspective, which conceptualizes child health as arising from cumulative effects of events and exposures at different life periods, as well as from intergenerational effects.11-15 For example, social risk factors like race, poverty, and education are known to influence the risk of adverse obstetric outcomes,16-20 and obstetric outcomes have been associated with cognitive delay at school-age.21-23 A linked pathway may therefore exist among maternal social risk factors, adverse obstetric outcomes, and early child cognition; as such exposure to social risk factors increases women’s risk of having poor obstetric outcomes, and obstetric outcomes in turn contribute to early cognitive delay. A cumulative risk model would suggest that social risk factors influence early childhood cognition via an “additive effect,” in which there is an accumulation of risk caused by individual negative exposures.15 However while several studies have demonstrated that poor child health and developmental outcomes are accelerated by exposure to multiple risks relative to singular risk exposure,24-28 it is unknown whether social risk factors influence cognition in infancy via the same cumulative mechanism.
In this study, we investigated the relationship among social risks, including family demographic and socioeconomic factors, and cognitive delay among infants participating in the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B). We examined the independent and cumulative effects of social risk factors on cognitive delay, and explored whether these relationships were mediated by obstetric factors. We hypothesized that social risks would be associated with early cognitive delay in a cumulative manner, but that this risk would be partially attributable to social disparities in obstetric outcomes. To our knowledge, this is the first nationally representative lifecourse study in the US to examine the cumulative influence of social risk factors on cognitive delay in infancy.
METHODS
Data Source
Data were drawn from the first wave of the ECLS-B, a nationally representative, population-based cohort study of nearly 11,000 US children from birth through kindergarten. A detailed discussion of the ECLS-B's data collection procedures and methodology is available elsewhere.22 Briefly, the ECLS-B selected a nationally representative probability sample of the approximately four million children born in 2001, with oversampling of children from minority groups, twins, and children born at low and very low birthweights, based on registered births from the National Center for Health Statistics vital statistics system.29 Children born to mothers under the age of 15 and those who were adopted or died before nine months of age were excluded from the sampling frame.30 The first data collection wave occurred in 2001 when the children were approximately nine months old
Restricted data for this study were obtained by approval from the Institute for Education Sciences Data Security Office of the US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics (NCES). In accordance with NCES guidelines, all reported unweighted sample sizes are rounded to the nearest 50.29 The University of Wisconsin-Madison Health Sciences Institutional Review Board considered this study exempt from review.
Participants were eligible for the current study if the biological mother was the main survey respondent and if the infant participated in the cognitive assessment (n=10,000); we excluded 250 infants with missing birth certificate information. The ECLS-B dataset included individual records for each child sampled as a twin; for these families, a random number generator was used to randomly select one child from each pair to remain in the sample, leaving 8,950 participants.
Measures
Cognitive Delay
Cognitive delay was determined using the mental scale of the Bayley Short Form Research Edition (BSF-R).29 The BSF-R comprised a subset of items from the revised Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II), a standardized screening instrument of developmental status designed for children ages birth to 42 months.31 The NCES recorded age-normed mental scale scores for the BSF-R (mean=50, standard deviation=10); the age at administration for children who were born preterm was recorded as their chronological age minus the number of weeks preterm. We considered children falling within lowest 10th percentile of the age-normed BSF-R mental scale (i.e., those in the lowest 10% of ability relative to other children of the same age in our sample) to have a cognitive delay, similar to previous research.32-34 We also tested alternative specifications of cognitive delay using the 5th and 15th percentile of the BSF-R as cutoffs and found consistent results with those using the 10th percentile delineation.
Social Risk Index
We examined five social risks assessed during the nine-month interview: family income below 100% of the Federal poverty level (FPL); single-parent household status; maternal black (non-Hispanic) race; maternal education less than high school; and three or more children other than the index child living in the household. These factors were selected on the basis of their individual relationships with child health in existing research.35,36 Bivariate analyses revealed that each factor was significantly associated with cognitive delay (Table 1).
Table 1.
Cognitive Delay at Nine Months of Age | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
TOTAL | Yes | No | p-value | |
TOTAL (unweighted) | 8,950 | 900 | 8,050 | |
Social Risk Factors | % | % | % | |
Family Income ≤ 100% of the FPL | 0.02 | |||
Yes | 24.2 | 30.8 | 23.8 | |
No | 75.8 | 69.2 | 76.2 | |
Maternal Race/ethnicity | 0.16 | |||
White (non-Hispanic) | 60.1 | 56.1 | 60.4 | *** |
Black (non-Hispanic) | 14.7 | 17.4 | 14.5 | * |
Other (non-Hispanic) | 4.2 | 4.7 | 4.1 | |
Hispanic | 21.0 | 21.7 | 21.0 | |
Maternal Education | 0.19 | |||
No or Some High School | 19.7 | 23.3 | 19.5 | * |
High School Graduate | 28.6 | 30.5 | 28.5 | |
Some College | 27.6 | 26.0 | 27.7 | |
Bachelor's Degree or Higher | 24.1 | 20.2 | 24.4 | |
Family Structure | 0.003 | |||
Single-Parent Household | 19.8 | 26.0 | 19.3 | |
Two-Parent Household | 80.2 | 74.0 | 80.7 | |
Number of Other Children in the Household | ≤.0001 | |||
0 | 41.1 | 30.3 | 41.8 | * |
1 | 33.7 | 38.9 | 33.4 | ** |
2 | 16.6 | 18.4 | 16.5 | ** |
3+ | 8.6 | 12.4 | 8.3 | ** |
Number of Social Risk Factors | 0.02 | |||
0 | 52.0 | 44.5 | 52.6 | ** |
1 | 23.7 | 23.0 | 23.7 | |
2 | 13.4 | 16.8 | 13.1 | ** |
3 | 7.8 | 10.2 | 7.7 | *** |
4+ | 3.1 | 5.5 | 2.9 | *** |
Obstetric Factors | ||||
Pregnancy Complications | 0.01 | |||
Yes | 27.7 | 35.5 | 27.3 | |
No | 73.3 | 66.5 | 72.7 | |
Labor and Delivery Complications | 0.002 | |||
Yes | 35.8 | 44.1 | 35.2 | |
No | 64.2 | 55.9 | 64.8 | |
Congenital Anomaly | 0.05 | |||
Yes | 1.2 | 2.9 | 1.1 | |
No | 98.8 | 97.1 | 98.9 | |
Birthweight Status | ≤.0001 | |||
Very Low | 1.2 | 5.7 | 0.9 | *** |
Low | 5.6 | 15.4 | 4.9 | *** |
Normal | 83.8 | 74.7 | 84.4 | *** |
High | 9.5 | 4.2 | 9.8 | *** |
Infant Characteristics | ||||
Gender | 0.02 | |||
Male | 51.0 | 55.7 | 50.7 | |
Female | 49.0 | 44.3 | 49.3 | |
Plurality | ≤.0001 | |||
Singleton Birth | 98.3 | 95.3 | 98.5 | |
Twin or Triplet | 1.7 | 4.7 | 1.5 |
Notes. Weighted estimates. Percentages may not sum to 100 due to rounding. Unweighted sample sizes were rounded to the nearest 50 in accordance with NCES guidelines. Data were extracted from the ECLS-B 9-month data collection wave.
Abbreviations: OR, Odds Ratio; CI, Confidence Interval; FPL, Federal Poverty Level.
p<0.05
p<0.01
p<0.0001
To address our hypothesis that these risks would be associated with cognitive delay in a cumulative manner, we generated a social risk index by giving infants one point for the presence of each social risk. This risk-index approach, developed by Sameroff et al,37 has been used in numerous studies of children's social, behavioral, and health development.24-28 Scores ranged from 0 to 5, with higher scores reflecting more social risk (Cronbach's α=0.53).
Covariates
Birth certificates provided obstetric factors: (1) birthweight in grams (<1,500 [very low]; 1,500-2,499 [low]; 2,500-3,999 [normal]; and ≤4,000 [high]); (2) pregnancy complications (flag for: anemia, diabetes, (oligo) hydramnios, lung disease, hypertension during pregnancy, cardiac disease, eclampsia, hemoglobinopathy, incompetent cervix, Rh sensitization, uterine bleeding, renal disease, genital herpes, or other medical risk factors); (3) labor and/or delivery complications (flag for: febrile, moderate/heavy meconium, premature rupture of membranes, abruptio placenta, placenta previa, excessive bleeding, seizures during labor, precipitous or prolonged labor, dysfunctional labor, breech/malpresentation, cephalopelvic disproportion, cord prolapse, anesthetic complications, fetal distress, the use of forceps or vacuum, or any other complication); and (4) congenital anomalies (e.g., Down's syndrome, spina bifida).
Additional covariates included the infant's gender and plurality status (e.g., singleton versus twin or triplet).
Analytic Approach
Analyses were conducted using SAS v9.2. Means and percentages were obtained to describe the sample characteristics using appropriate weights to account for ECLS-B's complex sampling design. Chi-square tests were used to determine the statistical significance of differences in social risk and obstetric factors by cognitive delay status.
Multivariable logistic regression was used to examine associations between each social risk factor and levels of cumulative social risk with cognitive delay. Staged multivariable logistic regression analyses were used to determine whether the association between categorical social risk (with 0 risks as the reference category) and cognitive delay was mediated by obstetric factors: first pregnancy complications, labor/delivery complications and congenital anomalies; then infant birthweight. The relationship between social risk and cognitive delay was determined to be mediated by obstetric factors if the regression coefficient for cognitive delay was attenuated. The statistical significance of any mediating effects was formally tested using Imai et al.'s mediation package in R.38 All models controlled for infant gender and plurality.
Complementary analyses evaluated the relationship between social risk and continuous BSF-R scores using weighted linear regression analysis, adjusted first for infant gender and plurality and then obstetric factors. These analyses sought to further evaluate the cumulative effect of social risk and to quantify the magnitude of the change in BSR-F scores associated with increasing levels of social risk.
RESULTS
Sample Description
Sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. In this national sample, a substantial proportion of infants lived with social risks: approximately 24% lived in poverty, nearly 20% lived with mothers who had less than a high school education, 14.7% had black (non-Hispanic) mothers, and 19.8% lived in single-parent households. 24.3% of infants had two or more social risk factors and 3.1% had four or more.
Independent Associations of Social Risk Factors with Cognitive Delay
In logistic regression analyses adjusting for infant gender and plurality, every social risk factor independently increased the odds of cognitive delay at nine months of age (Table 2, Model 1). However, only single-parent household status (adjusted odds ratio [AOR] 1.34; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.05-1.71) and having three or more siblings (AOR 1.43; 95% CI: 1.05-1.96) had significant independent associations with cognitive delay controlling for all of these risks simultaneously (Table 2, Model 2).
Table 2.
Cognitive Delay at Nine-Months of Age | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | |||
AOR | 95% CI | AOR | 95% CI | |
Social Risk Factor | ||||
Single-Parent Household | 1.47 | 1.18 – 1.82 | 1.34 | 1.05 – 1.71 |
Family Income ≤ 100% of the FPL | 1.45 | 1.16 – 1.82 | 1.22 | 0.94 – 1.59 |
Maternal Black (non-Hispanic) Race | 1.30 | 1.02 – 1.66 | 1.00 | 0.76 – 1.31 |
Maternal Education Less than High School | 1.49 | 1.06 – 2.10 | 1.09 | 0.81 – 1.47 |
Three or More Other Children in the Household | 1.52 | 1.09 – 2.10 | 1.43 | 1.05 – 1.96 |
Notes. Weighted analyses. Data were extracted from the ECLS-B 9-month data wave. All models control for infant gender and plurality. Model 1 examines each social risk factor separately; Model 2 mutually adjusts for the social risk factors.
Abbreviations: AOR, Adjusted Odds Ratio; CI, Confidence Interval; FPL, Federal Poverty Level
Social Risk Index
There was a significant increasing association of social risk on cognitive delay (Figure 1), such that the percentage of infants with indications of cognitive delay increased with the number of social risks (p-value for trend ≤0.001). 5.4% of infants with no social risks had indications of cognitive delay compared to nearly 12.0% of infants with four or more risks.
In adjusted analyses, the association between social risk and the odds of cognitive delay increased with the number of social risk factors (Table 3, Model 1 and Figure 2), such that the relative odds of cognitive delay was over two-fold higher for infants living with four or more social risk than infants with no risks (AOR 2.11; 95% CI 1.18-3.78). Infants with one risk factor were not statistically different from those with zero risk factors. These associations were largely unaffected by adjustment for pregnancy complications, labor/delivery complications, and congenital anomalies (Table 3, Model 2). However, when infant birthweight was added to the model (Model 3), the social risk coefficients attenuated for infants living with three (AOR from 1.62 to 1.46; 95% CI: 1.04-2.04) and four or more risks (AOR from 2.13 to 1.85; 95% CI: 1.04-3.32. The inclusion of infant birthweight completely mediated the association between social risk and cognitive delay for children living with two risks (AOR from 1.52 to 1.41; 95% CI: 0.99-1.98). The overall mediation effect of social risk on cognitive delay by birthweight was statistically significant, as verified by Imai's approach (pmediation ≤0.001).
Table 3.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AOR | 95% CI | AOR | 95% CI | AOR | 95% CI | |
Number of Social Risk Factorsa | ||||||
0 | 1.00 | Reference | 1.00 | Reference | 1.00 | Reference |
1 | 1.15 | 0.87 – 1.52 | 1.16 | 0.88 – 1.53 | 1.11¥ | 0.83 – 1.47 |
2 | 1.52 | 1.08 – 2.13 | 1.52 | 1.09 – 2.13 | 1.41¥ | 0.99 – 1.98 |
3 | 1.60 | 1.16 – 2.20 | 1.62 | 1.17 – 2.24 | 1.46¥ | 1.04 – 2.04 |
4+ | 2.11 | 1.18 – 3.78 | 2.13 | 1.20 – 3.80 | 1.85¥ | 1.04 – 3.32 |
Pregnancy Complications | ||||||
Any | 1.20 | 0.98 – 1.45 | 1.08 | 0.88 – 1.33 | ||
None | 1.00 | Reference | 1.00 | Reference | ||
Labor or Delivery Complications | ||||||
Any | 1.43 | 1.14 – 1.78 | 1.33 | 1.06 – 1.67 | ||
None | 1.00 | Reference | 1.00 | Reference | ||
Congenital Anomaly | ||||||
Any | 1.98 | 0.95 – 4.15 | 1.98 | 0.95 – 4.15 | ||
None | 1.00 | Reference | 1.00 | Reference | ||
Birthweight Status | ||||||
Very Low | 5.69 | 4.57 – 7.09 | ||||
Low | 3.10 | 2.55 – 3.77 | ||||
Normal | 1.00 | Reference | ||||
High | 0.47 | 0.30 – 0.74 |
Notes. Weighted analyses. Data were extracted from the ECLS-B 9-month data wave. All models control for infant gender and plurality.
Abbreviations: AOR, Adjusted odds ratio; CI, Confidence interval.
Social risk factors include: family income below 100% of the Federal poverty level, single-parent household status, maternal black (non-Hispanic) race, maternal education less than high school, and three or more children other than the index child living in the household.
Denotes statistical significance of the overall mediation effect at p<.0001. Mediational analyses were conducted using Imai et al.'s mediation package in R. Significance levels are based upon two-tailed hypothesis tests.
Complementary analyses confirmed an increasing effect of social risk on mean infant cognition scores, such that each social risk exerted an additional negative effect on continuous (standardized) BSF-R scores (Beta coefficient= -0.90, p≤0.001; adjusted for gender and plurality). When birthweight was added to the model, the effect of social risk attenuated (B=-0.62; p≤0.001). This mediation effect was highly significant, with 42% of the association between social risk and BSF-R scores acting via mediation by birthweight (pmediation ≤0.001; data not shown).
DISCUSSION
This nationally representative, population-based study investigated the relationship between social risk factors and cognitive delay in infancy. Our results contribute three important findings to the literature. First, we found significant associations between exposure to social risk factors and cognitive delay at nine months of age, extending to infancy previous studies illustrating the strong relationship between social disadvantage and children's cognitive and intellectual achievement.1,7,39-41 Second, in addition to demonstrating independent associations of each social risk factor, we observed a significant cumulative relationship between exposure to these social risks and the likelihood of cognitive delay, informing the mechanism of the effects of social risk on early cognitive delay. Third, the relationship between exposure to multiple social risks and cognitive delay was mediated by birthweight, elucidating an important target for preventive interventions. These findings are discussed with respect to their implications for pediatric practice, public health policy, and future research.
Our findings suggest that disparities in the prevalence of early cognitive delay may reflect a cumulative effect of multiple social risks. In our national sample, social risks, including living in poverty, in a single-parent household, with three or more siblings, or having a mother of black (non-Hispanic) race or with less than a high school education, was associated with cognitive delay at nine months of age. However, when risks were combined into a single social risk index, large cumulative associations were observed, with over two-fold higher odds of cognitive delay among infants with four or more risk factors compared to those with no risks. This overall pattern is consistent with previous research on the importance of the total number of risk factors for child developmental outcomes.24-28 For example, Sameroff et al found that the cumulative effect of ten risk factors had a stronger impact on cognitive functioning and social- emotional development of four-year-old children than the effect of any individual risk factor.24,37,42 Larson et al recently reported that eight social risk factors cumulatively impacted children's global, dental, socio-emotional, and physical health.25
Our mediation analysis suggests that the association between exposure to multiple social risks and early cognitive delay may be partly attributable the increased likelihood of low birthweight among those living with social risk. In the empirical literature, African American race, social disadvantage, and poverty are consistently associated with low birthweight and prematurity;16-20 by school age, these social risk factors are also associated with lower levels of IQ, academic achievement, and school performance.5,7,8 The exact timing of these effects is not well understood, although it has been proposed that social disparities in child cognition develop during early childhood through pathways like inadequate nutrition, poor access to healthcare, or low quality child care.33 Using a lifecourse approach,11,13,15 we suggest that the relationship between social risk factors and cognitive delay in infancy may additionally operate through a linked health trajectory of children and their mothers, whereby social risk contributes to disparities in birthweight, which, in turn, contribute to differences in the prevalence of cognitive delay in infancy. However, this finding should be viewed as only descriptive and exploratory in nature. Our cross-sectional data could not establish temporality; moreover, the relationship between social risk and cognitive delay was not mediated by pregnancy or labor/delivery complications, which are indicative of maternal health status. Further, our data did not account for the dynamics of the constituents of the social environment, such as the length of exposure to or number of episodes of poverty both for infants and mothers over their lifetimes. For example, the number of years of family poverty has been shown to be more strongly associated with adverse childhood outcomes than the family's current economic status.1 Additional research that links maternal lifetime exposure to social risk with her children's health outcomes over time is needed to test directionality and clarify the intergenerational relationships among the constructs we considered in this study.
To our knowledge, only one other nationally representative study has investigated risk factors for cognitive delay in infancy. Consistent with our findings, Hillemeier et al33 demonstrated that obstetric factors, including low birthweight, were significantly associated with cognitive delay at nine months of age; however, unlike our study, socioeconomic status was not. Hillemeier's study, however, examined a composite socioeconomic status measure as an independent variable, which did not account for the cumulative effect of multiple clusters of social risk factors to which children are often exposed. Social risks likely impact early cognition through dynamic, multifaceted interactions with environmental, psychological, and biologic systems over time. Additional research is needed to elucidate the influence of these multiple dimensions to understand the pathways and patterns by which social disadvantage impacts cognition in infancy. Understanding these mechanisms will facilitate the design of effective interventions and improve our understanding of periods when such interventions would have the greatest impact on improving outcomes.
Several limitations to this study should be noted. First, even though our social risk indicator encompassed several factors, it was not comprehensive of all social risks associated with child health. Further, our use of an index to assess social risk has some limitations, including the assignment of equal weight to each factor.43 Future research could consider alternate approaches to social risk assessment (e.g., factor analysis)43 that may more accurately capture the relative importance of individual risks. Such work could also identify specific combinations of risk factors most pertinent to outcomes. Second, the present study is cross- sectional, limiting the interpretation of our mediation analyses and the causal inference of our results. Similarly, we did not examine whether cumulative social risk relates to health outcomes over time. We are following children over multiple ECLS-B waves to examine such relationships. Third, there may be overlap between our definition of cognitive delay, based on the cognitive component of the BSF-R, and other developmental delays to executive or sensorimotor functions. Fourth, due to small numbers, we operationalized obstetric outcomes as dichotomous variables and therefore did not determine independent or cumulative impacts of such events. Findings may also be subject to survival bias, as the ECLS-B did not include infants who died within nine months of their birth. Finally, birth certificate data may under or incorrectly report some information.44
This study also has several notable strengths, including the use of a large, national database which allowed us to examine multiple social and health factors in one model. Further, the high quality measurements provided in the ECLS-B allowed us to objectively assess cognitive delay, avoiding misclassification error and biases associated with parental report of childhood conditions.45,46 Finally, evaluating these associations from a theoretical perspective enhanced our understanding of the mechanisms and pathways by which social risk factors contribute to early cognitive delay.
There are several implications of our findings. Clinically, our results suggest that approaches to identify young children with or at risk for cognitive delay may be most effective if they incorporate multiple biologic and social risk criteria. While these factors are not measures of cognitive delay themselves, they are associated with higher rates of cognitive delay and thus can be regarded as important risk factors. Pediatricians should be aware social risk factors may have additive effects on cognitive delay in infancy and focus identification and referral efforts to those living with two or more risk factors. In the absence of standard well-child practices to assess social risks in children, our findings support recommendations25 to develop a social risk assessment screening tool that pediatricians can administer early in children's lives.
Our findings also suggest that efforts need to be made to improve birth outcomes as they are clearly tied to early cognitive delay. The linkages between women's obstetric health and infant cognition observed in this study acknowledge the importance of intergenerational factors for health development47 and advocate for increased attention to more upstream factors (e.g., women's health) in order to combat downstream outcomes such as early childhood cognition. Future work should test whether preconceptional and prenatal interventions improve cognitive outcomes for children, bearing in mind that multifaceted interventions that address social and biologic risks over prolonged periods may have the greatest chance at success.
The high proportion of infants in our sample living with least one social risk (nearly 50%) and the cumulative association between these social risk factors and cognitive delay underscores the need for programs and policies to address the social context of US infants in an effort to improve their developmental trajectories. The independent associations between living in a single-parent household or with three or more siblings and cognitive delay provide some guidance regarding potential starting points for intervention. While we did not investigate the mechanisms of these associations, these variables may reflect constrained economic and other family resources.45 However, our findings also suggest that the accumulated burden of risk, rather than any particular risk factor, is most important. Therefore, interventions aimed at high-risk groups (i.e., infants with multiple risks and/or with these particular risk factors), may ultimately be necessary to improve outcomes.
Finally, this work supports state efforts to extend eligibility under Part C of IDEA beyond diagnoses of developmental delay to include biologic, social, and environmental risk criteria. Early intervention and other support systems not based on qualification due to developmental delay, including the newly-funded Maternal, Infant Early Childhood Home Visiting (MIECHV) program, likely offer excellent opportunities and resources for infants and families living with multiple social risks associated with cognitive delays and other poor developmental outcomes. These new investments in maternal and child health may be critical to ensure specialized services are serving a broader community of children who need them.
CONCLUSION
In this nationally representative, population-based study, we noted associations between the accumulation of social risk factors and cognitive delay among infants, which was partly mediated by birthweight. Our findings support a lifecourse approach and indicate that programs to address the social context of US infants are needed to improve their developmental trajectories.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by a Science and Medicine Graduate Research Scholars Fellowship from the University of Wisconsin, College of Agriculture and Life Sciences and School of Medicine and Public Health, and a dissertation grant from The New York Community Trust, Fahs-Beck Fund for Research and Experimentation, awarded to ER Cheng. ER Cheng was additionally supported by a grant from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Research Training in Prevention and Care of Chronic Illness in Childhood (T32HD075727-01; PI: JA Finkelstein). The authors wish to thank Dr. Kristin Litzelman, Dr. Mari Palta, Dr. Stephanie Robert, Dr. Bridget B. Catlin, and Hyojun Park for their invaluable assistance during the preparation of this manuscript.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflicts of interest: None to disclose.
REFERENCES
- 1.Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. The future of children. 1997:55–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gennetian LA. One or two parents? Half or step siblings? The effect of family structure on young children's achievement. Journal of Population Economics. 2005;18(3):415–436. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Altarac M, Saroha E. Lifetime prevalence of learning disability among US children. Pediatrics. 2007;119(Suppl 1):S77–S83. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2089L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hillemeier MM, Morgan PL, Farkas G, Maczuga SA. Perinatal and socioeconomic risk factors for variable and persistent cognitive delay at 24 and 48 months of age in a national sample. Maternal and child health journal. 2011;15(7):1001–1010. doi: 10.1007/s10995-010-0656-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual review of psychology. 2002;53(1):371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2009;13(2):65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American psychologist. 1998;53(2):185–204. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of educational research. 2005;75(3):417–453. [Google Scholar]
- 9.High PC. School readiness. Pediatrics. 2008;121(4):e1008–e1015. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Martin LT, Fitzmaurice GM, Kindlon DJ, Buka SL. Cognitive performance in childhood and early adult illness: a prospective cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2004;58(8):674–679. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.016444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lu MC, Halfon N. Racial and ethnic disparities in birth outcomes: a life-course perspective. Maternal and child health journal. 2003;7(1):13–30. doi: 10.1023/a:1022537516969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Halfon N, Hochstein M. Life course health development: an integrated framework for developing health, policy, and research. Milbank Quarterly. 2002;80(3):433–479. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Elder GH., Jr The life course as developmental theory. Child development. 1998;69(1):1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Pickles A, De Stavola B. An overview of models and methods for life course analysis. In: Pickles A, Maughan B, Wadsworth M, editors. Epidemiological Methods in Life Course Research. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2007. pp. 181–220. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y, Lynch J, Hallqvist J, Power C. Life course epidemiology. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2003;57(10):778–783. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.10.778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Goldenberg R, Culhane J, Iams J, Romero R. Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. Lancet. 2008;371(9606):75–84. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60074-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Alexander GR, Slay M. Prematurity at birth: Trends, racial disparities, and epidemiology. Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews. 2002;8(4):215–220. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kramer MS. Determinants of low birth weight: Methodological assessment and meta-analysis. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 1987;65(5):663–737. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Parker JD, Schoendorf KC, Kiely JL. Associations between measures of socioeconomic status and low birth weight, small for gestational age, and premature delivery in the United States. Annals of epidemiology. 1994;4(4):271–278. doi: 10.1016/1047-2797(94)90082-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kogan M. Social causes of low birth weight. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 1995;88(11):611. doi: 10.1177/014107689508801103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hack M, Klein N, Taylor H. School-age outcomes of children of extremely low birth weight and gestational age. Semin Neonatol. 1996;1:277–288. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hack M, Klein NK, Taylor HG. Long-term developmental outcomes of low birth weight infants. The future of children. 1995:176–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Shenkin SD, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Birth weight and cognitive ability in childhood: a systematic review. Psychological Bulletin; Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130(6):989. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sameroff AJ. Environmental risk factors in infancy. Pediatrics. 1998;102(Suppl E1):1287–1292. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Larson K, Russ SA, Crall JJ, Halfon N. Influence of multiple social risks on children's health. Pediatrics. 2008;121(2):337–344. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sameroff AJ, Bartko WT, Baldwin A, Baldwin C, Seifer R. Family and social influences on the development of child competence. In: Lewis M, Feiring C, editors. Families, Risk, and Competence. Lawrence Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1998. pp. 161–185. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bauman LJ, Silver EJ, Stein RE. Cumulative social disadvantage and child health. Pediatrics. 2006;117(4):1321–1328. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stevens GD. Gradients in the health status and developmental risks of young children: the combined influences of multiple social risk factors. Maternal and child health journal. 2006 Mar;10(2):187–199. doi: 10.1007/s10995-005-0062-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort, Nine-Month Data Collection. U.S. Department of Education, National Center For Education Statistics; Washington, D.C.: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Snow K, Derecho A, Wheeless S, et al. Kindergarten 2006 and 2007 Data File User's Manual (2010-010) National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: 2009. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dale PS, Price TS, Bishop DVM, Plomin R. Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46(3):544–560. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/044). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hillemeier MM, Farkas G, Morgan PL, Martin MA, Maczuga SA. Disparities in the prevalence of cognitive delay: How early do they appear? Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 2009;23(3):186–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2008.01006.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Webster RI, Majnemer A, Platt RW, Shevell MI. The predictive value of a preschool diagnosis of developmental language impairment. Neurology. 2004;63(12):2327. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000147472.33670.b6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bramlett MD, Blumberg SJ. Family structure and children's physical and mental health. Health Affairs. 2007;26(2):549–558. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.26.2.549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Montgomery LE, Kiely JL, Pappas G. The effects of poverty, race, and family structure on US children's health: Data from the NHIS, 1978 through 1980 and 1989 through 1991. American Journal of Public Health. 1996;86(10):1401–1405. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.10.1401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sameroff AJ, Seifer R, Barocas R, Zax M, Greenspan S. Intelligence quotient scores of 4- year-old children: social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics. 1987;79(3):343–350. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Imai K, Keele L, Tingley D, Yamamoto T. Causal mediation analysis using R. Lecture Notes in Statistics. In: Vinod HD, editor. Advances in Social Science Research Using R. Springer; New York: 2010. pp. 129–154. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand K. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm. JAMA: the journal of the American Medical Association. 2002;288(6):728–737. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Resnick MB, Gueorguieva RV, Carter RL, et al. The impact of low birth weight, perinatal conditions, and sociodemographic factors on educational outcome in kindergarten. Pediatrics. 1999;104(6):e74–e74. doi: 10.1542/peds.104.6.e74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental science. 2007;10(4):464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sameroff AJ, Seifer R, Zax M, Garmezy N. Early development of children at risk for emotional disorder. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1982;47(7):1–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Burchinal MR, Roberts JE, Hooper S, Zeisel SA. Cumulative risk and early cognitive development: a comparison of statistical risk models. Developmental psychology. 2000;36(6):793. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.6.793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Martin J, Hamilton B, Ventura S, Osterman M, Wilson E, Mathews T. Institute of Medicine. National Academies Press; Washington, DC: 2004. Children's Health, the Nation's Wealth: Assessing and Improving Child Health. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK92206/ [Google Scholar]
- 45.Simpson GA, Colpe L, Greenspan S. Measuring functional developmental delay in infants and young children: Prevalence rates from the NHIS-D. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. 2003;17(1):68–80. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2003.00459.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Reichman NE, Corman H, Noonan K. Impact of child disability on the family. Maternal and child health journal. 2008;12(6):679–683. doi: 10.1007/s10995-007-0307-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Russ SA, Larson K, Tullis E, Halfon N. A Lifecourse Approach to Health Development: Implications for the Maternal and Child Health Research Agenda. Maternal and child health journal. 2013:1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10995-013-1284-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]