Abstract
Background
Associations between early life maltreatment, social information processing (SIP) and aggression in childhood and adolescence have been widely documented. Few studies have examined the importance of childhood maltreatment independent of SIP in the etiology of adult aggression. Furthermore, moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the SIP–aggression links have not been explored.
Method
Hierarchical, multi-level models were fitted to data from n=2752 twins aged 20–55 years from the PennTwins Cohort. Adult aggression was assessed with the Life History of Aggression questionnaire. Childhood maltreatment was measured using the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Two aspects of SIP were examined : hostile attribution biases (HAB) ; negative emotional responses (NER).
Results
Childhood maltreatment was positively correlated with adult aggression, independently of HAB and NER. In addition, childhood maltreatment moderated the relationships between both aspects of SIP and adult aggression. Specifically, the relationship between NER and aggression was stronger among individuals with higher levels of childhood maltreatment and NER was not associated with aggression for adults who experienced low levels of childhood maltreatment. Moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the NER–aggression link were supported for total childhood maltreatment, emotional neglect and emotional abuse. In contrast, HAB was more strongly associated with adult aggression at lower levels of emotional abuse and physical neglect.
Conclusions
The current study provides insight into the mechanisms by which early life experiences influence adult aggression. Our findings suggest that childhood maltreatment may not only lead to increased levels of aggression in adulthood but may also modify the associations between SIP and adult aggression.
Keywords: Adult aggression, childhood maltreatment, hostile attribution biases, moderation, negative emotional responses
Introduction
Childhood maltreatment, namely, early life experiences of being neglected or abused by family members, is a serious social concern. Altogether, >1.25 million children (i.e. one in every 58 children) in the United States experienced maltreatment during 2005–2006 (Sedlak et al. 2010). Maltreatment not only impedes normal child development but also has detrimental effects on the development of later psychological disorders and behavioral problems. In particular, the long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment on adult aggression, defined as behavior directed toward another that is intended to cause harm (Berkowitz, 1993 ; Anderson & Bushman, 2002), have been widely documented (e.g. Widom, 1989 ; Nicholas & Bieber, 1996 ; Loos & Alexander, 1997 ; Frazzetto et al. 2007 ; Taft et al. 2008 ; Berzenski & Yates, 2010).
In his review, Briere (2002) concluded that childhood maltreatment affects multiple aspects of social-emotional development in adolescence and adulthood, including negative cognitive schema regarding self and others, conditioned associations between abuse stimuli and emotional distress, cognitions/memories of maltreatment events triggered by unpleasant environmental stimuli and in-sufficiently developed affect regulation. All of these traumagenic mechanisms are closely associated with cognitive processes involved in social information processing (SIP). Therefore, current models of the effects of childhood maltreatment have identified SIP as an important factor underlying individual differences in aggression.
The SIP model (Dodge, 1986 ; Crick & Dodge, 1994) delineates six steps of SIP : (1) encoding (i.e. attention to situational and internal cues) ; (2) interpretation and mental representation of these cues (e.g. attributions of intent) ; (3) clarification of goals (i.e. rationalization of the goals and desired outcome of a provocative situation) ; (4) response access or construction (i.e. generation of possible responses) ; (5) response evaluation and decision (i.e. determination of the quality of each alternative response and evaluation of the likelihood that each alternative will produce the desired outcomes) ; (6) behavioral enactment (Crick & Dodge, 1994). Biased processing during any of the six steps of the SIP model may lead to aggressive behavior (Dodge, 1986 ; Crick & Dodge, 1994 ; Orobio de Castro et al. 2002). To date, studies have provided ample evidence for the utility of SIP in the explanation of aggressive behavior in children and adolescents (e.g. Dodge & Crick, 1990 ; Quiggle et al. 1992 ; Pettit et al. 2001) and adults (e.g. Matthews & Norris, 2002 ; Basquill et al. 2004 ; Miller & Lynam, 2006 ; Bailey & Ostrov, 2008 ; Chen et al. in press), with many studies focusing on an individual’s attributions of another’s intent in social situations. Research consistently demonstrates a positive relationship between hostile attribution biases (HAB) and aggressive behavior (e.g. Dodge, 1980 ; Dodge & Somberg, 1987 ; Feldman & Dodge, 1987 ; Crick 1995 ; Hubbard et al. 2001 ; Crick et al. 2002 ; Bailey & Ostrov, 2008 ; Chen et al. in press).
SIP is also an integral part of the pathway from childhood maltreatment to later aggressive behavior. In 1990, Dodge and colleagues proposed a SIP model to explain ‘ the cycle of violence ’. They hypothesized that abused children are more likely to develop biased and deficient patterns of SIP, which may, in turn, result in increased levels of aggressive behavior. In support of this view, childhood maltreatment has been associated with inadequate decoding of social cues, increased levels of HAB and favorable evaluation of aggressive responses. These deficits in SIP mediate the effects of early life maltreatment on later aggressive behavior across the lifespan (e.g. Dodge et al. 1990, 1995 ; Weiss et al. 1992 ; Taft et al. 2008 ; Calvete & Orue, 2011). However, evidence generally supports a partial mediation model (Dodge et al. 1990, 1995 ; Weiss et al. 1992 ; Calvete & Orue, 2011), whereby childhood maltreatment predicts later aggressive behavior above and beyond the effects of SIP.
This prior research highlights the importance of controlling for the effects of SIP when examining relationships between childhood maltreatment and later aggressive behavior. The majority of research in this area has focused on childhood and adolescence (but see Taft et al. 2008). In addition, most prior research has neglected affective processes, although emotions have been proposed to be an integral part of the revised SIP model (Dodge, 1991 ; Crick & Dodge, 1994 ; Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). In particular, consistent with Berkowitz’s (1990) cognitive-neoassociationistic model of aggression, postulating that negative affect activates cognitive processes associated with aggressive behavior, the revised SIP model highlights the potential effects of negative emotions. In the context of ambiguous social situations, negative emotions can facilitate hostile attribution, distract from relationship-enhancing goals and can make it difficult for people to focus on generating alternative responses or evaluating these alternatives thoroughly (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). Empirically, negative emotions, such as anger and sadness, have been positively associated with aggressive behavior (e.g. Dodge & Coie, 1987 ; Dodge et al. 1997 ; Karatzias et al. 2002) and inversely related to cognitive ability to solve problems (e.g. Bodenhausen et al. 1994 ; Pakaslahti, 2000). Therefore, negative emotional responses (NER) should be incorporated and examined in SIP research exploring the cognitive mechanisms underlying aggressive behavior (Coccaro et al. 2009).
Finally, no studies to date have considered the potential moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on relationships between SIP and aggressive behavior, although theoretical work and emerging evidence from neurobiological studies suggest that the experience of childhood maltreatment might amplify relationships between deficits in SIP and increases in aggression. According to the self-trauma model developed by Briere (1992, 1996, 2002), a sustained sense of external security is essential for children to develop internal coping strategies for uncomfortable affect states and stressful experiences. Severely abused or neglected children are commonly exposed to enduring emotional pain, which prevents the establishment of a sense of security. Therefore, maltreated children are less likely to develop regulation skills to cope with negative cognitions or emotions (Briere, 2002). Adults who experienced childhood maltreatment may overact to distressing or threatening social cues and may respond to unpleasant cognitions or negative emotional arousal activated by these cues with aggression (Briere, 2002). Consistent with these hypotheses, findings from a growing number of neurobiological studies show effects of early life maltreatment on the development of brain structures that mediate self-regulation, including abnormalities in brain regions governing cognitive and emotional processing, such as the hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex (Teicher, 2000 ; Teicher et al. 2002 ; Bremner, 2003 ; Majer et al. 2010). Irregular activities in these brain structures are associated with impairments in emotion regulation and with poor impulse control (see Lee & Hoaken, 2007 for a review). These neuro-biological studies offer a mechanistic explanation for why hostile attributions and/or negative emotions may be more likely to result in aggressive behavior among people who experienced maltreatment in childhood. If individuals with history of maltreatment are less able to regulate their behavioral responses to unpleasant cognitive or emotional evocations, they would be more likely to externalize negative cognitions or emotions triggered during the social interaction. Thus, relationships between HAB and NER with aggression may be stronger in formally abused or neglected adults than among adults who did not experience childhood maltreatment. This hypothesis, however, has not been tested in prior studies.
Using a large, population-based sample of adults, the present study addresses limitations in prior research by examining associations between childhood maltreatment and aggressive behavior controlling for both cognitive (i.e. HAB) and emotional (i.e. NER) processes of SIP and by exploring the moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the SIP–aggression links. As prior research has documented both cumulative and unique impacts of multiple types of childhood maltreatment on later psychosocial problems (e.g. Briere & Runtz, 1990 ; Dubowitz et al. 2004 ; Higgins, 2004), the current study considers both overall maltreatment severity as well as individual types of childhood maltreatment (i.e. physical abuse, emotional abuse, physical neglect and emotional neglect).
Method
Sample and procedures
Participants were from the PennTwins Cohort, a population-based sample of twins born in Pennsylvania between 1959 and 1978 (Coccaro & Jacobson, 2006). In 1996, a list of 77 012 individuals who were likely to be part of a twin pair based on computerized birth records was cross-referenced with active driving license records, resulting in an address list of 30 801 individuals. Of these 30 801 individuals, 9341 returned consent-to-contact forms. Differences on various demographic characteristics (e.g. age, race/ ethnicity, household income) between individuals who consented to participate and those who were unresponsive or could not be located were negligible and non-systematic (Coccaro & Jacobson, 2006).
These 9341 twins were mailed a brief questionnaire containing basic demographic questions. Altogether, 7282 twins returned this questionnaire (response rate ~76 %). Differences between twins whose co-twins did and did not return the demographic questionnaire indicate that twin pairs were slightly more likely to be younger, female, Caucasian and unmarried (Coccaro & Jacobson, 2006). The Behavioral Health Questionnaire (BHQ), including the childhood maltreatment, SIP and aggression measures examined in the current study, was then sent to all twin pairs in which both twins had returned the demographic questionnaire (n=4204). The response rate to the BHQ was 70–75 %. BHQ data are currently available for 3070 participants. The multi-level regression models used in the current study require that missing data be processed with listwise deletion, resulting in a final study sample of n=2752 participants aged 20–55 years (58.5 % females). Analyses compared characteristics of the participants in the final sample with non-participants who provided BHQ data but were not included in the analyses. No significant differences (p>0.05) were found in key demographic characteristics [i.e. age, gender and social economic status (SES)] or levels of aggression.
Measures
Covariates
Age
Age was computed using participants’ date of birth and the date the BHQ questionnaire was received (mean=33.18, s.d.=6.01).
Gender
Gender was coded as 0=female and 1=male.
SES
Participants’ SES was measured by their current family income, based on a 6-point scale, where 1=<$15000 and 6=>$100000. On average, participants in the present study had a family income of $50000–$75000 (mean=4.03, s.d.=1.30).
Social desirability
Social desirability bias was assessed with the Minimization-Denial subscale of the 28-item Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ ; Bernstein et al. 1994), a self-report questionnaire for retrospective report of childhood maltreatment. The Minimization-Denial subscale consists of three items designed to identify individuals with a tendency to give socially desirable responses or produce false negative reports (e.g. ‘ When I was growing up, I had the perfect childhood ’ ; Bernstein & Fink, 1998). Responses were given in a 5-point Likert scale (i.e. 1=never true to 5=very often true). A social desirability score was computed using the sum of the responses to the three items (α=0.75, mean=9.92, s.d.=2.92).
Predictors
Childhood maltreatment
The remaining 25 items from the CTQ asked participants to rate statements about their childhood experiences of five types of maltreatment, including physical neglect (e.g. ‘ I didn’t have enough to eat ’ ; α=0.75), emotional neglect (e.g. ‘ I felt unloved ’ ; α=0.92), physical abuse (e.g. ‘ I was punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object ’ ; α=0.75), emotional abuse (e.g. ‘ People in my family said hurtful or insulting things to me ’ ; α=0.87) and sexual abuse (e.g. ‘ Someone tried to make me do sexual things or watch sexual things ’ ; α=0.95), on a 5-point Likert scale (i.e. 1=never true to 5=very often true). Prevalence rates for each type of childhood maltreatment are shown in Table 1. We excluded sexual abuse from the analyses as it has the lowest prevalence rates and weak correlations with other types of childhood maltreatment (r ranges from 0.29 to 0.31).1,† Cumulative prevalence rates of the other types of childhood maltreatment indicated that 44.8 % of participants did not experience any of the four types of maltreatment and 23.3 % reported at least one incident of moderate-to-severe abuse or neglect. For analyses, a total childhood maltreatment score was computed by summing responses to the 20 items measuring physical neglect, emotional neglect, physical abuse and emotional abuse (α=0.93). Scores for subtypes of maltreatment were created using the sum of the five items assessing each type of maltreatment respect ively. Mean CTQ scores and prevalence rates are similar to those observed in other community-based samples (Bernstein & Fink, 1998).
Table 1.
Prevalence of childhood maltreatment
Physical abuse |
Emotional abuse |
Physical neglect |
Emotional neglect |
Sexual abuse |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | Female | |
None | 73.9% | 77.6% | 69.1% | 59.9% | 80.8% | 82.2% | 56.8% | 63.6% | 91.3% | 86.4% |
Low | 17.1% | 11.4% | 21.6% | 22.7% | 12.0% | 9.3% | 31.2% | 22.4% | 4.2% | 3.6% |
Moderate | 6.2% | 6.3% | 4.7% | 8.6% | 5.4% | 5.2% | 7.9% | 7.2% | 2.7% | 4.7% |
Severe | 2.8% | 4.7% | 4.6% | 8.8% | 1.8% | 3.3% | 4.1% | 6.8% | 1.7% | 5.3% |
Clinical cut-offs for each type of childhood maltreatment are as follows – none: emotional abuse (5–8), physical abuse (5–7), sexual abuse (5), emotional neglect (5–9), physical neglect (5–7); low: emotional abuse (9–12), physical abuse (8–9), sexual abuse (6–7), emotional neglect (10–14), physical neglect (8–9); moderate to severe: emotional abuse (≥13), physical abuse (≥10), sexual abuse (≥8), emotional neglect (≥15), physical neglect (≥10).
SIP (HAB and NER)
HAB and NER were measured by the Social Information Processing-Attribution and Emotional Response Questionnaire (SIP-AEQ ; Coccaro et al. 2009). Based on similar protocols used in studies of children (Crick, 1995 ; Crick et al. 2002 ; Fite et al. 2008 ; Lansford et al. 2010), the SIP-AEQ consists of written descriptions of eight vignettes describing ambiguous social situations, designed specifically for adults. Each vignette was followed by two questions assessing direct (e.g. ‘ This person wanted to physically hurt me ’) and indirect (e.g. ‘ This person wanted to make me look bad ’) hostile intent. Responses were given in a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0=not at all likely to 3=very likely. The scale score for HAB was computed by summing responses of the two questions across all eight vignettes (α=0.87). Direct and indirect hostile intent correlated r=0.77 (p<0.001). The test–retest correlation of HAB over an average of 23.5 months (s.d.=14.7) in a subsample of n=336 individuals from the current study was 0.65 (p<0.001).
Each of the eight vignettes was also followed by two items reflecting anger (i.e. ‘ How likely is it that you would be angry if this happened to you ? ’) and embarrassment or upset (i.e. ‘ How likely is it that you would be embarrassed if this happened to you ? ’), measured using a 4-point Likert scale (i.e. 0=not at all likely to 3=very likely). A total NER score was created using the sum of the responses of the two items across all eight vignettes (α=0.85). Angry and embarrassed responses correlated r=0.50 (p<0.001). The test–retest correlation of NER was 0.71 (p<0.001).
Outcome
Adult aggression
Adult aggression was measured by the aggression subscale of the Lifetime History of Aggression Questionnaire (LHA ; Coccaro et al. 1997), a measure of trait tendency to engage in aggressive behavior that is generally stable over the lifetime. The LHA measure has been used in prior research with both clinical and normal subjects (e.g. Coccaro et al. 1997 ; Lee et al. 2009 ; Almeida et al. 2010 ; Coccaro & Kavoussi, 2010). The LHA captures both clinical and sub-clinical levels of aggression and was specifically designed to minimize the typically non-normal distribution of aggressive behavior (Coccaro et al. 1997). The aggression subscale of LHA (LHA-AGG) contains five items related to adult frequency of temper tantrums, general fighting, specific physical assault, specific property assault and verbal assault (e.g. ‘ Since you were 18 years old, how many times had you gotten into physical fights with other people ’). Each item was rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 0=no occurrences to 5=more occurrences than can be counted. The LHA-AGG has good concurrent validity (with both Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory and Overt Aggression Scale-Modified), internal consistency, inter-rater reliability and test–retest reliability (Coccaro et al. 1997). A total aggression score was computed by summing the responses of the five items (α=0.75). LHA aggression scores >12 indicate clinically significant levels of lifetime aggression (Coccaro et al. 1997) ; 18.8 % of current participants had scores >12, which is in between rates observed in samples of healthy volunteers and samples of patients with personality disorders (e.g. Coccaro et al. 1997, 2010 ; Lee et al. 2009).
Plan of analyses
Multi-level modeling using SPSS (Peugh & Enders, 2005) was implemented to account for non-independence between participants in the same family. Specifically, participants were identified by their own personal ID and by the family ID they shared with their co-twins. By incorporating the family ID as a random effect, the variance in aggression can be decomposed into variance attributable to differences between individuals and variance due to differences between families, the latter of which are accounted for by the random effect (Teachman & Crowder, 2002). The multi-level approach thus accounts for the clustered data and allows the model to produce accurate standard errors and significance tests (Papp, 2004). All study constructs were assessed at the individual level. Three hierarchical models were specified : an unconditional means model estimating the proportion of variability in aggression that exists between individuals (Model 1) ; a model examining main effects of total childhood maltreatment and measures of SIP (Model 2) ; a model exploring interactions between total childhood maltreatment and SIP measures (i.e. childhood maltreatmentrHAB, childhood maltreatmentrNER ; Model 3). Analyses in Model 3 were repeated for each individual subtype of maltreatment. Total childhood maltreatment, physical abuse, emotional abuse and physical neglect were positively skewed and were log transformed for analyses. To control for the effects of age, gender, SES and social desirability, measures of childhood maltreatment, SIP and aggression were first regressed onto these variables. Standardized residual scores were then used in multi-level modeling analyses. Hierarchical models were compared using change in log likelihood (−2LL) statistics, which follows a χ2 distribution. A significant decrease in −2LL indicates that the test model fit significantly better than the comparison model. Significant interactions were plotted and interpreted using methods outlined by Preacher et al. (2006) for calculation of simple slopes at different levels of the moderator variables (Aiken & West, 1991), specifically designed for interpretation of interactions with clustered data.
Results
Descriptive statistics and correlations
Table 2 shows descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations between measures of childhood maltreatment, SIP and adult aggression. Measures of childhood maltreatment and SIP were all positively associated with adult aggression. Measures of HAB and NER were strongly correlated, but were only modestly related to childhood maltreatment. Subtypes of childhood maltreatment were strongly correlated.
Table 2.
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among main study variables
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Aggression | ||||||||
2. HAB | 0.18*** | |||||||
3. NER | 0.17*** | 0.53*** | ||||||
4. Total maltreatment | 0.26*** | 0.18*** | 0.10*** | |||||
5. Physical abuse | 0.26*** | 0.10*** | 0.02 | 0.72*** | ||||
6. Emotional abuse | 0.26*** | 0.16*** | 0.13*** | 0.89*** | 0.60*** | |||
7. Physical neglect | 0.16*** | 0.17*** | 0.05*** | 0.74*** | 0.47*** | 0.55*** | ||
8. Emotional neglect | 0.19*** | 0.17*** | 0.09*** | 0.90*** | 0.50*** | 0.70*** | 0.64*** | |
Mean | 7.56 | 14.68 | 25.49 | 31.04 | 6.86 | 8.54 | 6.29 | 9.35 |
s.d. | 4.53 | 5.95 | 6.75 | 10.98 | 2.55 | 3.93 | 2.25 | 4.22 |
Min. | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 20.00 | 5.00 | 5.00 | 5.00 | 5.00 |
Max. | 25.00 | 39.00 | 48.00 | 98.00 | 24.00 | 25.00 | 24.00 | 25.00 |
HAB, Hostile attribution biases ; NER, negative emotional responses.
Correlation statistics between adjusted variables are reported.
** p< 0.01,
p< 0.001.
Total childhood maltreatment
Results for total childhood maltreatment are shown in Table 3. Findings from Model 1 revealed statistically significant variability in adult aggression between individuals (α2=0.69, p<0.001) and between families (τ00=0.31, p<0.001), supporting the use of multi-level modeling to correct for sample non-independence. Model 2, including main effects of total childhood maltreatment and SIP measures, fit significantly better than Model 1. Higher levels of HAB and NER were associated with higher levels of adult aggression. Total childhood maltreatment was significantly and positively associated with adult aggression controlling for HAB and NER. The moderating effects of total childhood maltreatment on relationships between SIP measures and adult aggression were tested in Model 3, which had a significantly better fit than Model 2. Total childhood maltreatment moderated the relationship between NER and adult aggression (β=0.06, p=0.002) but not the HAB–aggression link (β=−0.03, p=0.12).
Table 3.
Multi-level regression predicting adult aggression by total childhood maltreatment, HAB and NER
Model 1 |
Model 2 |
Model 3 |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | s.e. | β | s.e. | β | s.e. | |
Fixed effect | ||||||
Intercept | −0.00 | 0.02 | −0.00 | 0.02 | −0.00 | 0.02 |
HAB | 0.06** | 0.02 | 0.06** | 0.02 | ||
NER | 0.13*** | 0.02 | 0.13*** | 0.02 | ||
Childhood maltreatment | 0.14*** | 0.02 | 0.14*** | 0.02 | ||
HAB*Maltreatment | −0.03 | 0.02 | ||||
NER*Maltreatment | 0.06** | 0.02 | ||||
Random effect | ||||||
Residual | 0.69*** | 0.03 | 0.66*** | 0.03 | 0.65*** | 0.03 |
Intercept | 0.31*** | 0.03 | 0.28*** | 0.03 | 0.28*** | 0.03 |
ICC | 0.69 | |||||
Model fit | ||||||
−2LL | 7703.29 | 7549.34 | 7539.90 | |||
Comparison model | 1 | 2 | ||||
Δ–2LL | 153.95*** | 9.44** | ||||
Δdf | 3 | 2 | ||||
Variance explained | ||||||
Level 1 | 4.6% | 4.9% |
HAB, Hostile attribution biases ; NER, negative emotional responses ; ICC, intraclass correlation ; −2LL, log likelihood.
p < 0.01,
p < 0.001.
To explore the significant interaction effects, we plotted the relationship between NER and adult aggression as a function of different levels of total childhood maltreatment in Fig. 1a. The simple slopes of NER on adult aggression were estimated at a low level of total childhood maltreatment (1.5 s.d. below the mean), the mean level of total childhood maltreatment and a high level of total childhood maltreatment (1.5 s.d. above the mean). Significant positive associations between NER and adult aggression were found for individuals with the mean (β=0.13, s.e.=0.02, p=0.000) and high (β=0.22, s.e.=0.04, p=0.000) levels of childhood maltreatment but not for individuals with the low level of childhood maltreatment (β=0.04, s.e.=0.04, p=0.264). Therefore, the relationship between NER and adult aggression was stronger for individuals with higher levels of total childhood maltreatment.
Fig. 1.
Prototypical plots for moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on relationships between measures of social information processing and adult aggression. Relationships between standardized variables are plotted. Simple slopes of negative emotional responses (NER) were not significant at low levels of total childhood maltreatment, emotional abuse, and emotional neglect. Simple slopes of hostile attribution biases (HAB) were not significant at high levels of emotional abuse and physical neglect.
Subtypes of childhood maltreatment
Interaction effects between individual subtypes of childhood maltreatment and measures of SIP were also tested. Findings (Table 4) indicated significant interactions of emotional abuse (β=0.05, p=0.007) and emotional neglect (β=0.04, p=0.041) with NER and significant interactions of emotional abuse (β=−0.04, p=0.027) and physical neglect (β=−0.05, p=0.010) with HAB. Fig. 1b, c shows that NER had significantly positive associations with adult aggression at the mean and high levels of, but not at the low level of, emotional abuse (low : β=0.04, s.e.=0.04, p=0.226 ; mean : β=0.12, s.e.=0.02, p=0.000 ; high : β=0.20, s.e.=0.04, p=0.000) and emotional neglect (low : β=0.07, s.e.=0.04, p=058 ; mean : β=0.13, s.e.=0.02, p=0.000 ; high : β=0.19, s.e.=0.04, p=0.000). In contrast, Fig. 1d, e exhibits that HAB predicted increased levels of adult aggression at the low and mean levels of, but not at the high level of, emotional abuse (low : β=0.13, s.e.=0.04, p=0.000 ; mean : β=0.07, s.e.=0.02, p=0.003 ; high : β=−0.00, s.e.=0.04, p=0.964) and physical neglect (low : β=0.16, s.e.=0.04, p=0.000 ; mean : β=0.07, s.e.=0.02, p=0.000 ; high : β=−0.01, s.e.=0.04, p=0.839).
Table 4.
Multi-level regression predicting adult aggression by subtypes of childhood maltreatment, HAB and NER
Physical abuse |
Emotional abuse |
Physical neglect |
Emotional neglect |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | s.e. | β | s.e. | β | s.e. | β | s.e. | |
Fixed effect | ||||||||
Intercept | 0.00 | 0.02 | −0.00 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.02 | −0.00 | 0.02 |
HAB | 0.07** | 0.02 | 0.07** | 0.02 | 0.07*** | 0.02 | 0.07*** | 0.02 |
NER | 0.14*** | 0.02 | 0.12*** | 0.02 | 0.13*** | 0.02 | 0.13*** | 0.02 |
Childhood maltreatment | 0.17*** | 0.02 | 0.16*** | 0.02 | 0.04* | 0.02 | 0.04* | 0.02 |
HAB × maltreatment | 0.00 | 0.02 | −0.04* | 0.02 | −0.05* | 0.02 | −0.00 | 0.02 |
NER × maltreatment | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.05** | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 0.04* | 0.02 |
Random effect | ||||||||
Residual | 0.66*** | 0.03 | 0.65*** | 0.03 | 0.67*** | 0.03 | 0.66*** | 0.03 |
Intercept | 0.26*** | 0.03 | 0.28*** | 0.03 | 0.29*** | 0.03 | 0.30*** | 0.03 |
Variance explained | ||||||||
Level 1 | 3.5% | 5.0% | 3.2% | 3.6% |
HAB, Hostile attribution biases ; NER, negative emotional responses.
p< 0.05,
p< 0.01,
p< 0.001.
Discussion
The current study examined associations between childhood maltreatment, two aspects of SIP (i.e. HAB and NER) and aggressive behavior in a sample of adults. Effects of both overall levels and subtypes of childhood maltreatment (physical abuse, emotional abuse, physical neglect, emotional neglect) were considered. Our work extends previous research in three important ways. First, the study examined associations between childhood maltreatment, SIP and aggression using an adult sample. Second, the current study examined associations between childhood maltreatment and aggression controlling for both cognitive (i.e. HAB) and emotional (i.e. NER) processes of SIP. Third, to the authors’ knowledge, the current study is the first to explore the moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on associations between SIP and aggressive behavior.
We found that childhood maltreatment predicted adult aggression above and beyond the effects of HAB and NER. Consistent with findings from previous work, greater childhood maltreatment was associated with higher levels of aggressive behavior in adulthood (Widom, 1989 ; Nicholas & Bieber, 1996 ; Loos & Alexander, 1997 ; Frazzetto et al. 2007 ; Berzenski & Yates, 2010). Moreover, consistent with studies in childhood and adolescence (Dodge et al. 1990 ; Weiss et al. 1992 ; Dodge et al. 1995 ; Calvete & Orue, 2011), the relationship between childhood maltreatment and aggression persisted after controlling for SIP. Prior studies have generally not considered the relationship between childhood maltreatment and adult aggression independent of SIP, nor have they integrated emotional process into the SIP model. Our study addressed these limitations and provided evidence for a significant association between childhood maltreatment and adult aggression controlling for both the cognitive and emotional aspects of SIP. Taken together, findings from the current study and previous work suggest that childhood maltreatment may have long-term risks on the development of aggressive behavior.
The current study also explored another possible mechanism through which childhood maltreatment may be linked to later aggression. Specifically, we tested whether childhood maltreatment amplified the risk effects of HAB and NER on adult aggression. Our hypothesis was that formerly abused or neglected adults would show greater correlations between deficits in SIP and aggression because they are more likely to generate aggressive responses to negative cognitions and emotions activated in the social interaction. We found that childhood maltreatment moderated the associations between both aspects of SIP and aggression in adults. Consistent with our hypotheses, NER was more strongly correlated with increased levels of aggression for adults who experienced higher levels of childhood maltreatment. In contrast, NER did not predict aggression among individuals with low levels of childhood maltreatment. Moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the relationships between NER and aggression were found for both overall levels of childhood maltreatment and for emotional abuse and neglect, but not for physical abuse and neglect. The observed moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the NER–aggression link support assumptions of the self-trauma model (Briere, 2002), demonstrating that formerly abused or neglected adults may be more likely to externalize negative emotions because maltreatment impedes development of emotion regulation. These results are also consistent with evidence from neurobiological studies that indicated effects of childhood stressors on abnormalities in the brain structures that undergird emotional processing (Teicher, 2000 ; Teicher et al. 2002 ; Bremner, 2003 ; Majer et al. 2010). However, our finding that emotional maltreatment but not physical maltreatment moderated relationships between NER and aggression implies that emotional abuse and neglect may be more strongly associated with adjustment problems in emotional domains than are physical abuse and neglect.
Results of the current study also provide evidence for moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the relationship between HAB and aggression. Although the association between HAB and aggression did not vary as a function of the overall levels of childhood maltreatment, it was moderated by emotional abuse and physical neglect. However, unexpectedly, HAB had a stronger association with aggression among adults who experienced lower levels of emotional abuse or physical neglect during childhood. Moreover, HAB did not predict levels of aggression for adults with high levels of emotional abuse or physical neglect experiences. The observed interactions between childhood maltreatment and HAB could be related to links between childhood maltreatment and impulsivity. Findings from neurobiological studies suggest that childhood maltreatment may result in poor impulse control (e.g. Teicher et al. 2002 ; Majer et al. 2010). Aggressive behavior can be explained by both cognitively controlled factors (e.g. SIP) as well as automatic and thoughtless processes, such as impulsivity (Berkowitz, 2008). Furthermore, behavioral decisions may become automatic and involve minimal cognitive processing at high levels of impulsivity (Fontaine & Dodge, 2006). Therefore, it is possible that individuals who experienced higher levels of childhood maltreatment are more likely to react automatically to negative emotions, instead of generating responses based on cognitively processed information (e.g. cognitive attribution of intentions), because they have higher levels of impulsivity. On the other hand, given that interactions between childhood maltreatment and HAB were not supported for overall levels of childhood maltreatment, and moderating effects are in the opposite direction of our results on NER, we cannot rule out the possibility of a stochastic effect, so these results should be interpreted with caution.
Conclusions and limitations
Results from the current study support significant associations between childhood maltreatment and aggressive behavior in adulthood, even after controlling for deficits in social–emotional information processes. The findings also provide insight into another mechanism by which early life maltreatment may be linked to adult aggression, namely, via modification of relationships between SIP and aggression. Our study is the first to examine the moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the SIP–aggression links. Thus, the interactions between childhood maltreatment and the two aspects of SIP found in the current study should be replicated in other samples. While interactions were found for certain maltreatment subtypes, but not others, prior studies (including the present study) have documented a strong overlap between different types of childhood maltreatment (Higgins & McCabe, 2000) and it has been proposed that understanding the impact of the overall degree rather than the specific type of maltreatment may be of greater importance for research on childhood maltreatment (Higgins, 2004). Thus, variations in moderating effects across different types of childhood maltreatment observed in the current study should not be over-emphasized, especially given the lack of comprehensive theory and consistent empirical evidence for the specific consequences of the subtypes of maltreatment (Higgins, 2004). Nevertheless, findings from the current study suggest that, in order to thoroughly capture relationships between childhood maltreatment, SIP and aggression, future research should take into account moderating effects of different aspects of early life maltreatment on the SIP–aggression links.
The study has several limitations. First, the sample is modestly skewed towards younger, female, and unmarried adults. Likewise, given the racial/ethnic distribution of Pennsylvania during the subjects’ birth years, results may not generalize to minorities. Second, the present study only taps into two aspects of the full SIP model, and our measure of SIP was based on forced-choice items. Furthermore, both the self-trauma model and findings from the neurobiological studies highlight the importance of emotion regulation. Our aggression measure also did not provide a precise assessment of different forms of aggression (e.g. reactive vs. proactive aggression, direct vs. indirect aggression). Thus, the moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the SIP–aggression links should be further explored by including other aspects of SIP, by using measures that capture the spontaneous patterns of SIP in the real world, by considering the role of emotion regulation, and by examining variations in these effects across different forms of aggression.
Our study also used a cross-sectional design and relied on adults’ retrospective reports of childhood maltreatment. Because data on childhood maltreatment, SIP and aggression were collected at the same point in time, causal relationships between these study constructs cannot be addressed. Aggressive individuals could develop more negative patterns of SIP. Self-reports of childhood maltreatment may also be biased by patterns of HAB and NER, as individuals with greater deficits in SIP may be more likely to remember instances of abuse and neglect and/or to perceive childhood maltreatment that has not occurred, However, it is unclear how these issues would bias the interaction effects between childhood maltreatment and SIP observed in the present study. Retrospective reports of childhood maltreatment are also subject to recall bias. For example, there is evidence that effects of childhood maltreatment on drug abuse are strongest in studies using retrospective self-reports, compared with prospective studies using official records (Widom et al. 1999). However, cases reported to child protection agencies are more likely to be from the low SES families and to represent very severe maltreatment, which limit the generalizability of the findings based on official records. The CTQ is a well-established assessment of childhood maltreatment known for its validity and reliability and allows for assessment of a broad range of childhood maltreatment in both clinical and general population samples. Nevertheless, future longitudinal prospective studies that span childhood to adulthood are needed to more thoroughly examine causal relationships between childhood maltreatment, SIP and adult aggression.
Clinical implications.
The present study is the first analysis of moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on SIP–aggression links in a large population-based sample of adults. Our findings have important clinical implications. The associations between childhood maltreatment and adult aggression observed in the current study suggest that early prevention of aggressive behavior may be especially important for children who experienced maltreatment and that knowing patients’ history of childhood neglect or abuse may facilitate treatment of aggressive behavior in adults. Furthermore, the moderating effects of childhood maltreatment on the SIP–aggression links highlight the importance of developing effective prevention and intervention programmes based on SIP, with special focus on emotional processes, for individuals who experienced childhood maltreatment to help them develop more appropriate responses to social and emotional stimuli.
Acknowledgement
This study was funded by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH063262, E. Coccaro, PI). Drs Chen and Jacobson were partially supported by an NIH Director’s New Innovator Award (DP2 OD003021) to Dr Jacobson.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interest
None.
Analyses have been repeated for sexual abuse. Results provided no evidence for interaction effects between this type of childhood maltreatment and both aspects of SIP.
The notes appear after the main text.
References
- Aiken L, West S. Multiple Regression : Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA; Sage Publications: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida M, Lee R, Coccaro EF. Cortisol responses to ipsapirone challenge correlate with aggression, while basal cortisol levels correlate with impulsivity, in personality disorder and healthy volunteer subjects. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2010;44:874–880. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:27–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey C, Ostrov J. Differentiating forms and functions of aggression in emerging adults : associations with hostile attribution biases and normative beliefs. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:713–722. [Google Scholar]
- Basquill M, Nezu C, Nezu A, Klein T. Aggression-related hostility bias and social problem-solving deficits in adult males with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2004;109:255–263. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2004)109<255:AHBASP>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz L. On the formation and regulation of anger and aggression : a cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychologist. 1990;45:494–503. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.4.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz L. Pain and aggression : some findings and implications. Motivation & Emotion. 1993;17:277–293. [Google Scholar]
- Berkowitz L. On the consideration of automatic as well as controlled psychological processes in aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 2008;34:117–129. doi: 10.1002/ab.20244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein D, Fink L. Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) : A Retrospective Self-Report Questionnaire and Manual. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K, Sapareto E, Ruggiero J. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:1132–1136. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berzenski SR, Yates TM. A developmental process analysis of the contribution of childhood emotional abuse to relationship violence. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma. 2010;19:180–203. [Google Scholar]
- Bodenhausen G, Sheppard L, Kramer G. Negative affect and social judgment : the differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1994;24:45–62. [Google Scholar]
- Bremner J. Long-term effects of childhood abuse on brain and neurobiology. Child & Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2003;12:271–292. doi: 10.1016/s1056-4993(02)00098-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briere J. Child Abuse Maltreatment : Theory and Treatment of the Lasting Effects. Sage; Newbury Park, CA: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Briere J. Therapy for Adults Molested as Children : Beyond Survival. Springer Publishing Co; New York: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Briere J. Treating adult survivors of severe childhood abuse and neglect : Further development of an integrative model. In: Myers JEB, Berliner L, Briere J, Hendrix CT, Reid T, Jenny C, editors. The APSAC Handbook on Child Maltreatment. 2nd Sage Publications; Newbury Park, CA: 2002. pp. 175–204. [Google Scholar]
- Briere J, Runtz M. Differential adult symptomatology associated with three types of child abuse histories. Child Abuse & Neglect : The International Journal. 1990;14:357–364. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(90)90007-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvete E, Orue I. The impact of violence exposure on aggressive behavior through social information processing in adolescents. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2011;81:38–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen P, Coccaro EF, Jacobson KC. Hostile attributional bias, negative emotional responding, and aggression in adults : moderating effects of gender and impulsivity. Aggressive Behavior. doi: 10.1002/ab.21407. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro EF, Berman ME, Kavoussi RJ. Assessment of life history of aggression : development and psychometric characteristics. Psychiatry Research. 1997;73:147–157. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(97)00119-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro EF, Jacobson KJ. PennTwins : a population-based cohort for twin studies. Twin Research in Human Genetics. 2006;9:998–1005. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro EF, Kavoussi RJ. GH response to intravenous clonidine challenge : absence of relationship with behavioral irritability, aggression, or impulsivity in human subjects. Psychiatry Research. 2010;178:443–445. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro EF, Lee R, Kavoussi RJ. Inverse relationship between numbers of 5-HT transporter binding sites and life history of aggression and intermittent explosive disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2010;44:137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro EF, Noblett K, McCloskey M. Attributional and emotional responses to socially ambiguous cues : validation of a new assessment of social/emotional information processing in healthy adults and impulsive aggressive patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2009;43:915–925. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick N. Relational aggression : the role of intent attributions, feelings of distress, and provocation type. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:313–322. [Google Scholar]
- Crick N, Dodge K. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:74–101. [Google Scholar]
- Crick N, Grotpeter J, Bigbee M. Relationally and physically aggressive children’s intent attributions and feelings of distress for relational and instrumental peer provocations. Child Development. 2002;73:1134–1142. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K. Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child Development. 1980;51:162–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K. A social information processing model of social competence in children. In: Perlmutter M, editor. Cognitive Perspectives on Children’s Social and Behavioral Development. Hillsdale, NJ; Erlbaum: 1986. pp. 77–126. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology. [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K. Emotion and social information processing. In: Garber J, Dodge KA, editors. The Development of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation. Cambridge University Press; New York: 1991. pp. 159–181. [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K, Bates J, Pettit G. Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science. 1990;250:1678–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.2270481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K, Coie J. Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;53:1146–1158. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.53.6.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K, Crick N. Social information-processing bases of aggressive behavior in children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1990;16:8–22. [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K, Lochman J, Harnish J, Bates J, Pettit G. Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:37–51. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K, Pettit G, Bates J, Valente E. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:632–643. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.4.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge K, Somberg D. Hostile attributional biases among aggressive boys are exacerbated under conditions of threats to the self. Child Development. 1987;58:213–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1987.tb03501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubowitz H, Pitts S, Black M. Measurement of three major subtypes of child neglect. Child Maltreatment. 2004;9:344–356. doi: 10.1177/1077559504269191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman E, Dodge K. Social information processing and sociometric status : sex, age, and situational effects. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1987;15:211–227. doi: 10.1007/BF00916350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fite J, Goodnight J, Bates J, Dodge K, Pettit G. Adolescent aggression and social cognition in the context of personality : impulsivity as a moderator of predictions from social information processing. Aggressive Behavior. 2008;34:511–520. doi: 10.1002/ab.20263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontaine R, Dodge K. Real-time decision making and aggressive behavior in youth : a heuristic model of response evaluation and decision (RED) Aggressive Behavior. 2006;32:604–624. doi: 10.1002/ab.20150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frazzetto G, Di Lorenzo G, Carola V, Proietti L, Sokolowska E, Siracusano A, Gross C, Troisi A. Early maltreatment and increased risk for physical aggression during adulthood : the moderating role of MAOA genotype. PLoS One. 2007;2:e486. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000486. http:// www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0000486) Accessed 3 January 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins D. Differentiating between child maltreatment experiences. Family Matters. 2004;69:50–55. [Google Scholar]
- Higgins DJ, McCabe MP. Multi-type maltreatment and the long-term adjustment of adults. Child Abuse Review. 2000;9:6–18. [Google Scholar]
- Hubbard J, Dodge K, Cillessen A, Coie J, Schwartz D. The dyadic nature of social information processing in boys’ reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:268–280. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatzias A, Power K, Swanson V. Bullying and victimization in Scottish secondary schools : same or separate entities ? Aggressive Behavior. 2002;28:45–61. [Google Scholar]
- Lansford JE, Malone PS, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates J. Developmental cascades of peer rejection, social information processing biases, and aggression during middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:593–602. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee R, Ferris C, van de Kar LD, Coccaro EF. Cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin, life history of aggression, and personality disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1567–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee V, Hoaken P. Cognition, emotion, and neurobiological development : mediating the relation between maltreatment and aggression. Child Maltreatment. 2007;12:281–298. doi: 10.1177/1077559507303778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemerise E, Arsenio W. An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development. 2000;71:107–118. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loos ME, Alexander PC. Differential effects associated with self-reported histories of abuse and neglect in a college sample. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 1997;12:340–360. [Google Scholar]
- Majer M, Nater U, Lin J, Capuron L, Reeves W. Association of childhood maltreatment with cognitive function in healthy adults : a pilot study. BMC Neurology. 2010;10:61. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-10-61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews B, Norris F. When is believing ‘ seeing ’ ? Hostile attribution bias as a function of self-reported aggression. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2002;32:1–32. [Google Scholar]
- Miller J, Lynam D. Reactive and proactive aggression : similarities and differences. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;41:1469–1480. [Google Scholar]
- Nicholas KB, Bieber SL. Parental abusive versus supportive behaviors and their relation to hostility and aggression in young adults. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1996;20:1195–1211. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(96)00115-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orobio de Castro B, Veerman J, Koops W, Bosch J, Monshouwer H. Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior : a meta-analysis. Child Development. 2002;73:916–934. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pakaslahti L. Children’s and adolescents’ aggressive behavior in context : the development and application of aggressive problem-solving strategies. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2000;5:467–490. [Google Scholar]
- Papp LM. Capturing the interplay among within- and between-person processes using multilevel modeling techniques. Applied & Preventive Psychology. 2004;11:115–124. [Google Scholar]
- Pettit G, Polaha J, Mize J. Perceptual and attributional processes in aggression and conduct problems. In: Hill J, Maughan FB, editors. Conduct Disorders in Childhood and Adolescence. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2001. pp. 292–319. [Google Scholar]
- Peugh J, Enders C. Using the SPSS mixed procedure to fit cross-sectional and longitudinal multilevel models. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2005;65:717–741. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, Bauer DJ. Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 2006;31:437–448. [Google Scholar]
- Quiggle N, Garber J, Panak W, Dodge K. Social information processing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Development. 1992;63:1305–1320. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sedlak AJ, Mettenburg J, Basena M, Petta I, McPherson K, Greene A, Li S. Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS-4): Report to Congress, Executive Summary. US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families; Washington, DC: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Taft CT, Schumm JA, Marshall AD, Panuzio J. Family-of-origin maltreatment, posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms, social information processing deficits, and relationship abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:637–646. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.3.637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teachman J, Crowder K. Multilevel models in family research : some conceptual and methodological issues. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2002;64:280–294. [Google Scholar]
- Teicher M. Wounds that time wouldn’t heal : the neurobiology of childhood abuse. Cerebrum. 2000;2:50–67. [Google Scholar]
- Teicher M, Andersen S, Polcari A, Anderson C, Navalta C. Developmental neurobiology of childhood stress and maltreatment. Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2002;25:397–426. doi: 10.1016/s0193-953x(01)00003-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss B, Dodge K, Bates J, Pettit G. Some consequences of early harsh discipline : child aggression and a maladaptive social information processing style. Child Development. 1992;63:1321–1335. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01697.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widom CS. The cycle of violence. Science. 1989;224:160–166. doi: 10.1126/science.2704995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Widom CS, Weiler BL, Cottler LB. Childhood victimization and drug abuse : a comparison of prospective and retrospective findings. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67:867–880. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]