Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Res Methods. 2015 Jun;47(2):494–505. doi: 10.3758/s13428-014-0486-7

Rotation to a Partially Specified Target Matrix in Exploratory Factor Analysis in Practice

Nicholas D Myers 1, Ying Jin 2, Soyeon Ahn 3, Seniz Celimli 4, Cengiz Zopluoglu 5
PMCID: PMC4258186  NIHMSID: NIHMS602853  PMID: 24903693

Abstract

The purpose of the current study was to explore the influence of the number of targets specified on the quality of exploratory factor analysis solutions with a complex underlying structure and incomplete substantive measurement theory. Previous research in this area was extended by (a) exploring this phenomenon in situations in which both the common factor model and the targeted pattern matrix contained specification errors and (b) comparing the performance of target rotation to an easier to use default rotation criterion (i.e., geomin) under conditions commonly observed in practice. A Monte Carlo study manipulated target error, number of targets, model error, overdetermination, communality, and sample size. Outcomes included bias (i.e., accuracy) and variability (i.e., stability) with regard to the rotated pattern matrix. The effects of target error were negligible for both accuracy and stability, while small effects were observed for the number of targets for both outcomes. Further, target rotation outperformed geomin rotation with regard to accuracy but generally performed worse than geomin rotation with regard to stability. These findings underscore the potential importance (or caution in the case of stability) of using extant, even if incomplete and somewhat inaccurate, substantive measurement theory to inform the rotation criterion in a non-mechanical way.

Keywords: Monte Carlo, simulation, exploratory structural equation modeling, target error, model error


MacCallum (2003) argued that the mathematical models used in practice (i.e., theoretical model) often only approximate a more complex reality (i.e., population model). Monte Carlo studies that assume that the theoretical model is identical to the population model (i.e., an absence of model error), “might be of interest to theorists, [but] they are of limited value to users of methods. A more realistic and relevant question is: How do our methods behave and perform when the model in question is not correct in the population?” (MacCallum, p. 135). The current study extended the literature by (a) investigating the performance of exploratory factor analysis with target rotation under conditions (e.g., model and target error) commonly observed in practice, and then (b) comparing the performance of target rotation to the performance of an easier to use default rotation criterion (i.e., geomin).

Factor analysis has been closely linked with investigations of construct validity in the social sciences for several decades (Nunnally, 1978). Investigations of construct validity have frequently occurred in studies where only factor analytic measurement models, exploratory (EFA) and/or confirmatory (CFA), were specified – typically guided by incomplete substantive measurement theory (DiStefano & Hess, 2005; Henson & Roberts, 2006). Incomplete measurement theory often manifests as model error and offers an explanation as to why nearly all construct validity studies fail the test of exact fit under a CFA approach (Jackson, Gillaspy, & Purc-Stephenson, 2009). Incomplete measurement theory may be better handled by EFA with rotation of the pattern matrix rather than a more restrictive CFA approach that relies on post hoc modifications (MacCallum, Roznowski, & Necowitz, 1992). EFA with target rotation can be conceptualized as “situated between CFA and EFA” (Asparouhov & Muthén, p. 399, 2009).

Direct analytic rotation of the pattern matrix is based on several decades of research within the EFA framework (e.g., Jennrich, 2007; Jennrich & Sampson 1966) as detailed in Browne (2001). Rotation of the pattern matrix is accomplished via post-multiplication of the pattern matrix by the inverse of an optimal transformation matrix:

Λp×r=Λp×r(Hr×r)-1. (1)

An optimal transformation matrix, H*, is determined by minimizing a continuous complexity function of the elements in the pattern matrix, f (Λ). A mechanical rotation criterion can be thought of as being relatively easy to implement but providing little to no opportunity to incorporate a priori measurement theory into the f (Λ).Various rotation techniques define f (Λ) differently but each was typically designed to provide the simplest solution.

For decades a simple solution has often been interpreted as having one non-zero pattern coefficient per row (variable complexity, vc, = 1, for each “pure” variable); though this “perfect simple structure” is a more restrictive approach than advocated by Thurstone (1947). For example, Thurstone’s conceptualization of simple structure allowed for r1 non-zero elements per row (i.e., vc ≤ = r1). Complex structures where r ≥ 2 and vc ≥ 2 for one or more variables are not uncommon in practice, though commonly used rotation criterion (e.g., direct quartimin) typically do not perform well in such situations (Yates, 1987). The failure of most rotation criteria to perform well with complex structures is not surprising given that most f (Λ) were designed in such a way that a (perfect) simple structure is sought (Browne, 2001).

Geomin rotation (Yates, 1987) minimizes row (e.g., variable) complexity in a way that is more consistent with Thurstone’s (1947) conceptualization of simple structure as compared to the more restrictive perfect simple structure. Accordingly, geomin has performed relatively well when vc > 1 both in empirical examples (e.g., Marsh et al., 2009; McDonald, 2005) and in a simulation study when compared to other mechanical rotation criteria (Sass & Schmitt, 2010). Currently geomin is the default rotation criterion in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012) and, therefore, may be used frequently in practice. The f(Λ) for geomin implemented in Mplus is:

i=1p(j=1r(λij2+ε))1/r, (2)

where

ε is a small positive constant added by Browne (2001) to reduce the problem of indeterminacy.

In Asparouhov and Muthén (2009), geomin performed well when vc was moderate (vc ≤ 2), m was small (m = 2), and the factors were moderately correlated. Geomin, however, “…fails for more complicated loading matrix structures involving three or more factors and variables with complexity 3 and more;… For more complicated examples the Target rotation criterion will lead to better results” (Asparouhov & Muthén, p. 407). The performance of target rotation as compared to geomin rotation, however, has yet to be systematically studied. For example, the previous quote from the seminal work of Asparouhov and Muthén seemed to be based on results where the outcome was either bias (p. 427) or coverage (p. 428) and not directly on variability for complex loading matrix structures. Additional studies are important, in part, because target rotation requires more from the user than a mechanical rotation criterion does. This additional demand on the user is especially noteworthy because a solution will be mathematically equivalent under competing rotation criterion.

Target rotation has been developed over several decades and can be thought of as “a non- mechanical exploratory process, guided by human judgment” (Browne, 2001, p.125). Early versions of target rotation were indirect and based on a fully specified target matrix (e.g., Horst, 1941; Tucker, 1944). Later versions of target rotation in EFA were direct and could be based on only a partially specified target matrix, Bpxr (Browne, 1972a, 1972b; Gruvaeus, 1970). The f(Λ) for target rotation can be written as:

i=1pj=1raij(λij-bij)2, (3)

where

  • aij = 1 if λij is a target and 0 if λij is not a target, and

  • bij = the targeted value.

Note that the user must provide aij and bij, and therefore, helps to define f (Λ) for target rotation. Thus, target rotation does not seek a (perfect) simple structure unless specified to do so.

Target rotation was designed to rotate Λ to a least squares fit to a targeted pattern matrix, B, which can utilize incomplete a priori measurement theory without fixing coefficients to particular values as in CFA. For example, Figure 1 provides a matrix A that designated whether each pattern coefficient was (1) or was not (0) a target, and a B that provided correct values (i.e., bij = λij) that targeted elements would be rotated towards. Under the assumption that the targeted values are correct the observed rotated pattern matrix, Λ̂*, generally should more accurately and reliably recover Λ as the number of targets increase. Target error (i.e., bij ≠ λij) would likely be present in practice because the true value of any particular λij would likely be unknown. It has long been believed that target rotation may be relatively robust to such misspecification due to the least squares fit to B (Browne, 2001). However, the potential role for target error to influence the quality of the rotated solution has yet to be systematically studied. For example, all of the relevant previous studies of which we are aware have either assumed the absence of target error and/or have set all targets to a single value (e.g., 0) often consistent with a pure variable approach.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Matrix A designated whether each pattern coefficient was (1) or was not (0) a target. Matrix B provided values that targeted elements would be rotated towards and denoted non-targeted elements with a ? symbol. Matrix Λ provided population values.

The role for systematically increasing the number of error-free targets specified per factor to influence the quality of the rotated solution with a complex underlying structure and incomplete measurement theory has been studied by Myers, Ahn, and Jin (2013). A positive effect of the number of targets specified on the accuracy and stability of Λ̂* was observed but the magnitude of this effect varied by level of communality (h; where hi is the proportion of variance in the ith variable accounted for by the common factors) and overdetermination of Λ (i.e., the ratio of the number of observed variables to the number of underlying factors, p:r). The effect of the number of targets specified generally was strongest when h = .20 to .40 (i.e., low) and p = 10 (i.e., small) or 20 (i.e., average) and r = 3 (i.e., average). For all levels of h and p:r, however, a positive effect of the number of targets specified on the accuracy and stability of Λ̂* generally was observed. Further, only a small number of targets (e.g., r) per factor needed to be specified in order to achieve good accuracy when p:r = 10:3 or 20:3, h > low, and N ≥ 100. Limitations of the Myers et al. study included the absence of: (a) model and target error and (b) a comparison of the performance of target rotation to an easier to use mechanical rotation criterion.

A strength of Myers et al. (2013) was conceptualizing the performance of target rotation within MacCallum and Tucker’s (1991) broader mathematical framework for identifying sources of error in factor analysis. This framework informed hypotheses in the current study and is summarized below with an emphasis on model error. The purpose of the emphasis on model error was to inform expectations (which will be stated in the hypotheses section) regarding the degree to which the findings of Myers et al. may hold under the presence of model error.

Model error is the discrepancy between the true covariance matrix for the observed variables, Σyy, and the implied covariance matrix that would result from fitting the theoretical model to Σyy, Σ̃yy (θ), where θ is a vector of parameter values If the theoretical model is fit to sample data, Syy, the covariance matrix implied from the fitted model is Σ̂yy (θ̂), where θ̂ is a vector of parameter estimates. The discrepancy between Σ̃ yy (θ), and Σ̂yy (θ̂), is due to sampling error. Overall discrepancy is the difference between Σ̂yy (θ̂), and Σyy, which under model misspecification and sufficiently large N is given by the sum of model error and sampling error (Cudeck & Henly, 1991).

Under standard assumptions of the common factor model the structure for both Σyy and Syy in the presence of model error can be made explicit. The structure for Σyy is given by:

yy=ΛΦΛ+Θ2+ΔME(P), (4)

where

  • Φ = population correlation matrix for the common (and standardized) major factors,

  • Θ = diagonal matrix of unique pattern coefficients, and

  • ΔME (P) = model error in the population covariance structure.

The structure of Syy is given by:

Syy=ΛΦΛ+Θ2+ΔME(S)+ΔSE, (5)

where

  • ΔME (S) = model error in the sample covariance structure, and

  • ΔSE = sampling error.

Holding estimation method constant, differences between θ and θ̂ are due to the differences between the error terms in Equation 4, ΔME (P), and Equation 5, ΔME (S) +ΔSE. MacCallum, Widaman, Preacher, and Hong (2001) demonstrated that ΔME (S)ΔME (P) over repeated sampling, and thus, ΔSE is the primary unique factor accounting for differences between θ and θ̂.

Sampling error introduces differences between the sample, Cxx, and population, Σxx, covariance matrix for the common and unique factor scores (MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, & Hong, 1999). Submatrices of Cxx can be viewed as weights for the Syy implied by the common factor model by expanding Equation 5 (while ignoring ΔME (S)):

Syy=ΛCccΛ+ΛCcuΘ+ΘCucΛ+ΘCuuΘ, (6)

where subscripts c and u denote common and unique factors, respectively. Equation 6 is useful for identifying some key consequences of ΔSE (e.g., ΣuuCuuI; ΣcuCcu0) that provide a general framework to explain why fitting Syy to a reduced form of Equation 5, ΛΦΛ′ + Θ2, generally results in a θ̂ that differs from θ.

MacCallum et al. (1999) hypothesized and then demonstrated in a Monte Carlo study that some key specific factors affecting the quality of an EFA solution include N, h, and p:r. Outcomes included a measure of accuracy and a measure of stability with regard to Λ̂*. The problematic effects of ΔSE decreased under conditions when the right-hand side of Equation 6 moved toward a reduced, yet familiar, form: ΛΦΛ′ + Θ2. Movement toward this reduced form of the right-hand side of Equation 6 occurred when Θ approached a null matrix (e.g., level of h = high) and/or submatrices of Cxx approached population values, ΣuuCuuI, ΣcuCcu0,(e.g., N was large), and/or the common factors became more overdetermined (e.g., moving from p:r = 20:7 to p:r = 20:3) decreasing the size, and hence the influence, of Ccu and Cuc.

The major findings of MacCallum et al. (1999) summarized in the previous paragraph were also observed in MacCallum et al. (2001) who extended the earlier study by demonstrating the same general patterns in the presence of model error in addition to sampling error. A feature of the MacCallum et al. (2001) study was that population pattern matrices were used as target matrices following direct quartimin rotation. The role for systematically increasing the number and/or the accuracy of targets specified to influence the quality of the rotated solution after controlling for the influence of other well-known factors (e.g., model error) was not studied.

Hypotheses for the Current Study

Myers et al. (2013) provided evidence for the unique role of the number of targets specified and replicated the findings of MacCallum et al. (1999) with regard to N, h, and p:r. Based on MacCallum et al. (2001), the findings of Myers et al. with regard to the number of targets should be observed in the presence of model error in addition to sampling error. Hypotheses unique to this study in regard to the accuracy and stability of Λ̂* are provided below.

Hypothesis 1

Target error would not exert a meaningfully large impact due to the flexibility afforded by the least squares fit to B.

Hypothesis 2

There would be an effect for the number of targets specified. As the number of targets increased Λ̂* would become more accurate and more stable because each Λ̂ would be rotated toward a common B that was increasingly overdetermined (i.e., more targets) and thereby reduce the effect of sampling error.

Hypothesis 3

There would be an interactive effect between h and the number of targets specified. Specifically, when h was high the effect of the number of targets would be smaller because Θ would approach a null matrix and thereby reduce the effect of sampling error.

Hypothesis 4

Target rotation would perform at least as well as geomin rotation with regard to accuracy, particularly as the number of targets specified increased, due to the ability of target rotation to be specified in such a way as to search for a complex underlying structure. An a priori hypothesis regarding the performance of target rotation versus geomin rotation with regard to stability was not put forth due to a lack of directly related research.

Method

The design consisted of two studies. Study 1 tested Hypothesis 1 through Hypothesis 3. Study 2 tested Hypothesis 4 under some of the conditions imposed in Study 1. Both studies used the same 18 population models.

Eighteen population models were specified for the purpose of achieving the desired characteristics with regard to overdetermination of Λ, h, and model error (detailed in the Manipulated Factors section). Nine of the 18 Λ were nearly identical to nine of the Λ used in MacCallum et al. (2001) where p = 20, r = 3.1 The remaining nine Λ in the current study were specified in order to achieve the desired characteristics for population models where p = 10, r = 3 because MacCallum et al. did not study population models where p = 10, r = 3.

The set of population models in the current study was selected for three reasons. First, the population models logically followed previous research regarding target rotation by Myers et al. (2013). Second, the population models had a link to two seminal studies in the factor analysis area as the population model generation approach taken in MacCallum et al. (2001) closely followed the approach taken by Tucker, Koopman, and Linn (1969). Third, the population models had some key characteristics that were similar to EFA in practice (Henson & Roberts, 2006). For example, each Λ was realistic in the sense that there was “variation in importance of major factors as well as numerous small to moderate secondary loadings” (MacCallum, et al., p. 620). As an example, values within a Λ are provided in Figure 1. Table 1 summarizes some key characteristics of the common factors for each of the 18 Λ. Population correlation values between the common factors equaled zero which was consistent with MacCallum et al.

Table 1.

Some Key Characteristics of the Common Factors by p:r

key characteristics p:r = 20:3 ≈ 7:1
ε = .025
ε = .065
ε = .090
h = high h = wide h = low h = high h = wide h = low h = high h = wide h = low



average number of high pattern coefficients per r 6.67 3.33 0.33 6.00 4.33 0.00 6.33 3.33 0.00
number of pure variables 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

p:r = 10:3 ≈ 3:1
average number of high pattern coefficients per r 3.33 1.67 0.00 3.00 2.33 0.33 3.33 2.33 0.00
number of pure variables 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

Note. ε = population RMSEA value; h = communality; p = number of observed variables; r = number of factors; high pattern coefficient = λ ≥ .63 (Comrey & Lee, 1992); pure variable = observed variable has only one non-zero pattern coefficient.

Study 1

Manipulated Factors

Six factors were manipulated: overdetermination of Λ (p:r; two conditions), model error (three conditions), h (three conditions), N (three conditions), number of targets (nine or four conditions depending on the level of p:r), and target error (four conditions). Manipulation of the first three factors was accomplished via specification of 18 population models. Table 2 provides an overview of the experimental design, which had a total of 1404 cells. The p:r = 20:3 (i.e., Model 1 – Model 9) and the p:r = 10:3 (i.e., Model 10–18) conditions contributed 972 (3*3*3*9*4) and 432 cells (3*3*3*4*4), respectively.

Table 2.

Overview of Experimental Design for Study 1

Population model Overdetermination of Λ (p:r) Model error (me) Communality (h) N Number of targets (t) Target error (te) cells
Model 1 20:3 fair high 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 1–108
Model 2 20:3 fair low 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 109–216
Model 3 20:3 fair wide 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 217–324
Model 4 20:3 poor high 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 325–432
Model 5 20:3 poor low 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 433–540
Model 6 20:3 poor wide 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 541–648
Model 7 20:3 very good high 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 649–756
Model 8 20:3 very good low 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 757–864
Model 9 20:3 very good wide 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+7 exact thru poor 865–972
Model 10 10:3 fair high 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 973–1020
Model 11 10:3 fair low 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1021–1068
Model 12 10:3 fair wide 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1069–1116
Model 13 10:3 poor high 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1117–1164
Model 14 10:3 poor low 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1165–1212
Model 15 10:3 poor wide 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1213–1260
Model 16 10:3 very good high 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1261–1308
Model 17 10:3 very good low 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1309–1356
Model 18 10:3 very good wide 100, 300, 500 r1 thru r+2 exact thru poor 1357–1404

Note. p = number of observed variables; r = number of factors.

Overdetermination of Λ (p:r)

Two conditions were specified for p:r: highly overdetermined (i.e., p:r = 20:3) or weakly overdetermined (i.e., p:r = 10:3). The highly overdetermined condition was consistent with EFA models in practice where the median p is ~20 and median r is ~3.00 (Henson & Roberts, 2006). The weakly overdetermined condition (i.e., relatively small p:r) was consistent the questionable, but often observed, practice of discarding complex, yet substantively important, items (Marsh, Hau, Balla, & Grayson, 1998).

Model error (me)

Three conditions were specified for me: low (i.e., population RMSEA, ε, = .025) moderate (ε = .065), or high (ε, = .090), These values were equal to those reported in MacCallum et al. (2001) and were conceptualized in the same way: very good fit, fair fit, and poor fit, respectively (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Steiger, 1989). Model error conditions were consistent with levels commonly observed in practice (Jackson et al., 2009).

Model error was integrated into the simulation of the current study through the use of minor factors (n=180) as described in Hong (1999).2 For each of the 18 population models a pattern matrix for the minor factors, Λm, was generated. Each Λm was initially generated by using multivariate random normal deviates. The standard deviation of the first minor factor was equal to 1. The standard deviation of each successive minor factor was .9 times the standard deviation of the preceding minor factor. After generating random pattern values for the minor factors the rows of Λm were rescaled to achieve a specific amount of variance accounted by the minor factors. The specific amount of total variance accounted by the minor factors ranged from 12.5% (i.e., ε = .025 and communality equaled high) to 60.6% (i.e., ε = .090 and communality equaled low) and was manipulated so that the desired level of me (i.e., ε, = .025 or .065 or .090) was integrated into each of the 18 population correlation matrices. The population correlation matrices were formed from Λm, Λ and unique variances and were used for data generation.

Communality (h)

Three conditions were specified for h: high (i.e., hi = .60 to .80; high = .70 ), wide (i.e., hi = .20 to .80; wide = .50 ), or low (i.e., hi = .20 to .40; low = .30 ).3 These values were consistent with those used in MacCallum et al. (2001). Henson and Roberts (2006) reported that for EFA models in practice, the average total variance explained by the extracted factors (R2 ) ≈ 52% (R2min ≈ 17% and R2max ≈ 87%).

Sample size (N)

Three conditions were specified for N: 100, 300, or 500. Henson and Roberts (2006) reported that the distribution of sample size for EFA models in practice is positively skewed (minimum = 42, median = 267, M = 436, maximum = 3113).

Number of targets per factor (t)

Two sets of conditions were specified for t based on level of p:r (see Table 3). For all 18 population models, t was increased systematically from a minimum number necessary for rotation identification, r1 per column, up to 50% of targeted elements in each column of B (i.e., r + 7 when p:r = 20:3 and r + 2 when p:r = 10:3). Fifty percent of targeted elements in each column of B was selected as the maximum to reflect the incomplete measurement theory commonly observed in practice (Myers et al., 2013).

Table 3.

Target Conditions by p:r

t p:r = 20:3
p:r = 10:3
t per r (%) nt per r (%) nt t per r (%) nt per r (%) nt
r1 2 (10%) 18 (90%) 54 2 (20%) 8 (80%) 24
r 3 (15%) 17 (85%) 51 3 (30%) 7 (70%) 21
r + 1 4 (20%) 16 (80%) 48 4 (40%) 6 (60%) 18
r + 2 5 (25%) 15 (75%) 45 5 (50%) 5 (50%) 15
r + 3 6 (30%) 14 (70%) 42
r + 4 7 (35%) 13 (65%) 39
r + 5 8 (40%) 12 (60%) 36
r + 6 9 (45%) 11 (55%) 33
r + 7 10 (50%) 10 (50%) 30

Note. p = number of observed variables; t = number of targets per factor; r = number of factors; nt = number of non-targets.

A total of 117 unique B (holding target error = 0) were specified across all 18 population models (i.e., 81 B in p:r = 20:3 case and 36 B in p:r = 10:3 case). For all B, the first t condition (i.e., r1) included two small pattern coefficients in each column (M = .00, SD = .08). From there the smallest non-targeted pattern coefficient in each column was specified as a target as t increased (e.g., from r1 to r).4 See Figure 1 for an example of a B where 50% of the elements were targeted. Specifying many small valued pattern coefficients as targets was consistent with previous research (Browne, 1972a, 1972b; Gruvaeus, 1970). A conceptual rationale for targeting only small valued pattern coefficients is that for any particular indicator the targeted value(s) may frequently be more accurate if the column(s) that the indicator is(are) not intended to measure is(are) targeted (e.g., values near zero) than if the column(s) that the indicator is(are) intended to measure is(are) targeted.

Target error (te)

Four conditions were specified for te: exact, Bexact (bij = λij), close, Bclose, (bij = λij or bij = λij ± 1SE), approximate, Bapprox (bij = λij or bij = λij ± 1SE or bij = λij ± 2SE), or poor, Bpoor (bij = λij ± 2SE). Within each B condition, the assignment of each bij to a particular value was systematic and continuous (from factor 1 to factor 3) as t increased. For example, when B = Bapprox and t = r1, the first target in the first column was specified as bi1 = λi1, the second target in the first column was specified as bi1 = λi1 + 1SE, the first target in the second column was specified as bi2 = λi2 − 1SE,..., the second target in the third column was specified as bi3 = λi3. Continuing with t = r for B = Bapprox, the additional target in the first column was specified as bi1 = λi1 + 1SE,..., and so forth.

Empirical standard errors for each λij within each of the 18 Λ were estimated in 18 Monte Carlo studies (i.e., one for each population model) where, N = 300, the number of replications, NR, = 10,000, and the rotation criterion was direct quartimin. A modest N was selected to reflect sample sizes in practice, while a relatively large NR was selected to increase the accuracy of the empirical standard errors (Hoogland & Boomsma, 1998). For each λij, the average of the estimated standard error over the replications was used as the empirical standard error. Figure 2 provides the four te conditions for the Λ previously depicted in Figure 1. A total of 468 unique B were specified (i.e., 117 for each of the four te conditions).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Four target error conditions for the Λ depicted in Figure 1. Non-targeted elements were denoted with a ? symbol.

Rotation Identification

Some known conditions for rotation identification in factor analysis exist (e.g., Algina, 1980; Hayashi & Marcoulides, 2006). Under oblique rotation H* is a non-symmetric square matrix that results in r2 indeterminacies. Imposing r2 constraints on Λ and Ψ is a necessary condition for rotation identification. Fixing the variance for each of the common factor to 1 provided r constraints. A set of sufficient conditions for imposing the remaining r(r – 1) constraints included: (a) each column of Λ has r – 1 elements specified as zeros, and (b) each submatrix Λs, where s = 1,…,r, of Λ composed of the rows of Λ that have fixed zeros in the rth column must have rank r – 1. For each t specification, it was confirmed that both conditions were met with regard to the population values.5

Population-level rotation

In each of the 18 population models the parameter values represented one possible rotated solution. In order to operate under the assumption that the particular set of parameter values in Λ was the simplest in each case, population-level rotation, Λ*, was obtained from a Monte Carlo study where N =1,000,000 and NR, = 1 (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009). This was done for each t condition (because each t condition represented a unique rotation criterion) in each of the 18 population models when B = Bexact. Across all 117 Monte Carlo studies the average difference between each element within the relevant Λ* and the relevant Λ was −.02 with SD = .04.

Data Generation

The experiment was performed within the exploratory structural equation modeling framework (ESEM; Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009) as implemented in Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012). Data were drawn from a multivariate normal distribution (0, Σ). Number of iterations was set to 10000 for both the unrotated and the rotated solution. Parameter values were provided in the model statement to define the alignment criteria in order to avoid factor order switching and reduce the likelihood of negative factors (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009).

Within each cell NR = 100 for each initial run though it was expected that non-convergence and/or inadmissible solutions would frequently be observed in some cells (e.g., Velicer & Fava, 1998). Consistent with the goal of maximizing the representativeness of the results to practice, replications that did not converge to a (rotated) solution or that converged to an improper solution were discarded (Bandalos, 2006). Each replication was screened because the program retained all completed replications – even if the solution was improper. Taking this approach was a primary reason that NR was relatively small. Replications were added as necessary to achieve 100 acceptable solutions in each cell.

Outcomes

To investigate the similarity between Λ and Λ̂* mean bias, δ, was calculated for each replication:

δ=i=1pj=1r(λij-λ^ij)pr, (7)

where

  • λ^ij is the observed rotated pattern coefficient for variable i on factor j, and

  • λij is the true pattern coefficient for variable i on factor j.

Bias was used to assess the accuracy of Λ̂*.

To investigate the stability of Λ̂* a measure of variability for the entire matrix, V, was calculated for each replication:

V=[Trace[(Λ^-Λ^¯)(Λ^-Λ^¯)]pr]1/2, (8)

where

  • Λ^¯ = was the mean observed rotated solution over 100 replications in a given cell. V was used to assess the stability of Λ̂*.

Testing Hypotheses 1 through Hypothesis 3

A six-way ANOVA modeled δ and then V by p:r, me, h, N, t, and te. All main and interaction effects were modeled and α was set .05 from this point forward. Effect size was emphasized over statistical significance and was categorized as small, .01 < ω̂2 < .05, medium, .05 ≤ ω̂2 < .13, and large, ω̂2 ≥ .13, consistent with general guidelines from Cohen (1988).

Study 2

Study 2 tested Hypothesis 4 under some of the conditions imposed in Study 1. The manipulated factor t was re-conceptualized as rotation criterion, rc, and geomin rotation was added as a condition (where ε = .001; the default value in Mplus for three factors). Target error was reduced from four conditions to one condition, Bpoor, for textual parsimony and to potentially provide a more conservative test with regard to target rotation. All other manipulated factors, data generation procedures, and outcomes were identical to Study 1.

Testing Hypothesis 4

A five-way ANOVA modeled δ and then V by p:r, me, h, N, and rc. All main and interaction effects were modeled but the focus was on post hoc comparisons within rc factor (i.e., geomin versus levels of t) by h. Thus, there was a total of 54 post hoc comparisons (27 for each outcome). Effect size was categorized as small, .1 ≤ |d| < .5, medium, .5 ≤ |d| < .8, and large, |d| ≥ .8, consistent with general guidelines from Cohen (1988). A somewhat liberal definition of small was considered reasonable given that both target and geomin rotation are somewhat flexible in that neither f (Λ) was confined to search for a perfect simple structure, and, that rotation occurs after estimation of the unrotated solution.

Results

The number of acceptable solutions from the first 100 replications in each cell was consistent with results from Velicer and Fava (1998). The percentage of acceptable solutions when p:r ≥ 20:3 was high (i.e., ≥ 83% in each of these cells; M = 98% across these cells), while the percentage of acceptable solutions when p:r = 10:3 was more variable (i.e.,≥ 45% in each of these cells; M = 86% across these cells). A total of 152,056 datasets were generated in order to achieve 140,400 datasets that yielded an acceptable solution.

Study 1

Bias (δ) summary

Average (across the 100 replications within each of the 1404 cells of the design) mean bias, δ̄, was small: M = −0.02, SD = 0.03. The small amount of δ observed in the current study was consistent with result reported in MacCallum et al. (2001). Given the small amount of δ and its location around zero, the absolute value of δ (i.e., unsigned bias) was calculated for each replication because the magnitude of δ was viewed as more important than the direction of δ, and, to avoid cancelling due to δ of opposite signs in subsequent analyses.

Effects from the six-way ANOVA that were greater than small and not directly related to the hypotheses in the current study were: h (ω̂2 = .16 ), p:r (ω̂2 = .10), p:r*h (ω̂ 2 = .05 ).6 These main and interaction effects were consistent with MacCallum et al. (1999) and are not discussed further. The major patterns (e.g., statistically significant but small effects) found in MacCallum et al. (2001) with regard to the main and interaction effects of me were replicated and are not discussed further in the main text. The smaller main and interaction effects of N in the current study (as compared to MacCallum et al. 2001) was likely attributable to the omission of a very small N (e.g., = 60 as in MacCallum et al. 2001) condition in the current study.

Variability (V) summary

Average (across replications within each of the 1404 cells of the design) mean variability, , was: M = 0.31, SD = 0.14. Effects from the six-way ANOVA that were greater than small and not directly related to the hypotheses in the current study were: p:r (ω̂ 2 = .09) and h (ω̂ 2 = .07 ). Both of these main effects, along with the statistically significant but small main and interaction effects for me and N, were consistent with MacCallum et al. (2001) and are not discussed further. Hypotheses unique to this study are discussed below.

Hypothesis 1

Effect size for the main effect of te, and all interaction effects involving te, on both δ and V was ω̂ 2 ≤ .01 and in the vast majority (58 out of 64) cases ω̂ 2 = .00. Thus, there was support for Hypothesis 1 in that target rotation was relatively robust to the target error conditions studied.

Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 3

There was a small main effect for t on both δ (i.e., ω̂ 2 = .01) and V (i.e., ω̂ 2 = .04 ) but in both cases the magnitude of this effect depended somewhat on level of h (i.e., ω̂2 = .01 and ω̂ 2 = .01, respectively). For example, as can be viewed in Figure 3, the negative effect of t on V was strongest/weakest when h was high/low. For example, as can be viewed in Figure 4, the negative effect of t on δ was weakest/strongest when h was high/low. The negative effect of t on δ was weaker and likely due to a floor effect previously described in the bias summary. Thus, there was some support for both Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 3 in that the negative effect of t on both δ and V depended somewhat on level of h.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The effect of rotation criterion, where t = the number of targets per factor and implies target rotation, on variability by communality (h) when target error was poor. Symbols represent an average variability while error bars represent the 95% confidence interval around this average.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

The effect of rotation criterion, where t = the number of targets per factor and implies target rotation, on mean absolute value of bias by communality (h) when target error was poor. Symbols represent an average absolute bias value while error bars represent the 95% confidence interval around this average.

Study 2

Hypothesis 4

Target rotation outperformed geomin rotation in each of the 27 post hoc comparisons within levels of rc by h in regard to δ (see Figure 3). The magnitude of the effect tended to differ somewhat by level of h and t. When h = high, d ranged from 0.21 to 0.98 and was consistently large (i.e., d ≥ .84) when tr + 3. When h = wide, d was consistently large and ranged from 1.10 to 1.76. When h = low, d ranged from 0.17 to 0.99 and was consistently moderate to large (i.e., d ≥ .74) when tr + 3. For all comparisons, the magnitude of d was heavily influenced by the by the precision of the pooled SD as the largest mean difference for any comparison was 0.08. Similarly, and as can be viewed in Figure 4, both rotation criteria tended to produce solutions of relatively good accuracy as δ̄ ≤ .10.

Geomin rotation outperformed target rotation in 20 of the 27 post hoc comparisons within levels of rc by h in regard to V (see Figure 4). The magnitude of the effect tended to differ somewhat by level of h and t. When h = high and tr +2, d ranged from 0.13 to 0.86 in favor of geomin rotation and this effect tended to shrink as t increased. When h = high and t > r +2, d ranged from 1.01 to 1.25 in favor of target rotation. When h = wide and tr +5, d ranged from 0.17 to 1.19 in favor of geomin rotation and this effect tended to shrink as t increased. When h = wide and t > r + 5, d ranged from 0.16 to 0.29 in favor of target rotation. When h = low, geomin rotation always outperformed target rotation and d ranged from 0.74 (t = r + 7) to 1.58 (t = r). An a priori hypothesis regarding the performance of target rotation versus geomin rotation with regard to stability was not put forth due to a lack of directly related research; results from these comparisons will be tentatively discussed in relation to the partially specified target matrix.

Discussion

The purpose of the current Monte Carlo study is to explore the influence of the number of targets specified on the quality of EFA solutions with a complex underlying structure and incomplete substantive measurement theory. Study 1 extends previous research by exploring this phenomenon in situations in which specification errors are present in both the common factor model (i.e., model error) and the targeted pattern matrix (i.e., target error). Study 2 extends previous research by comparing the performance of target rotation to geomin rotation under some of the conditions imposed in Study 1. Established sources of error in factor analysis manipulated in both studies include overdetermination of the pattern matrix, communality, and sample size. Outcomes are a measure of accuracy and a measure of stability with regard to the rotated pattern matrix. Findings are discussed with respect to the hypotheses tested with an emphasis on potential implications for applied research.

There is support for Hypothesis 1 in that the magnitude of the main effect of target error, and all interactive effects involving target error, on both the accuracy and stability of the rotated solution is negligible. This finding is consistent with long held methodological speculation that target rotation may be relatively robust to such misspecification due to the inherent flexibility of the least squares fit to the targeted pattern matrix. This finding also may have important implications for practice given that the true value of any particular pattern coefficient, let alone the true value for each of the several pattern coefficients necessary for rotation identification, is likely to be unknown in any particular application. More broadly, the fact that target rotation may be relatively unaffected by both model and target error suggests that this rotation criterion may be useful under conditions frequently observed in practice: important substantive measurement theory is available yet it is both incomplete and less than perfectly accurate.

There is some support for both Hypothesis 2 and Hypothesis 3 in that the magnitude of the effect of the number of targets specified on both the accuracy and stability of the rotated solution depends somewhat on level of communality (but not on target error). Like in the Myers et al. (2013) study, these findings can be viewed as analogous to the interactive effect of communality and degree of overdetermination of the pattern matrix (substituting the number of targets per factor for p:r) predicted and found in both MacCallum et al. (1999) and MacCallum et al. (2001). In general, when communality is high, the effect of the number of targets on increasing accuracy is smaller (likely due to a floor effect) than when communality is not high. From an applied perspective, these findings make clear the potential importance of using extant, even if incomplete and somewhat inaccurate, substantive measurement theory to specify the target matrix to the full extent possible – particularly when the observed variables are not uniformly highly reliable indicators of the underlying factors.

There is some support for Hypothesis 4 in that target rotation outperforms geomin rotation in regard to accuracy. While the size of the effect generally is at least medium it should be noted that both rotation criteria tend to produce solutions of relatively good accuracy thus the effect size was largely achieved by the precision of the pooled SD. From a methodological perspective the accuracy findings are consistent with the fact that neither rotation criterion is restricted to search for a perfect simple structure, and, that target rotation can be specified in such a way as to search for a complex underlying structure. From an applied perspective, the potential importance of using extant, even if incomplete and somewhat inaccurate, substantive measurement theory to inform the rotation criterion in a non-mechanical way may be worthwhile in general with regard to accuracy. Most simply, the additional demand placed on the user under target rotation versus geomin rotation may be worthwhile with regard to accuracy.

Geomin rotation outperforms target rotation in most conditions of this study in regard to stability. As can be viewed in Figure 1, the target matrix typically is only partially specified and thus the complexity function that is minimized is based on partial information. For this reason, target rotation may yield relatively less stable results as compared to rotation criterion whose complexity function is based on fuller information, particularly when only a small percentage of possible targets are specified. As can be viewed in Figure 3, instability tended to decrease as the number of targets increased but the magnitude of this decrease tended to depend on level of communality. From an applied perspective, when the observed variables are less than uniformly reliable indicators of the underlying factors, geomin rotation may be expected to outperform target rotation. When, however, the observed variables are uniformly reliable indicators of the underlying factors and several targets are specified, target rotation may be expected to outperform geomin rotation. In summary, using incomplete substantive measurement theory to inform the rotation criterion in a non-mechanical way may be worthwhile only in limited circumstances, as compared to a fuller information mechanical approach, with regard to stability.

The ability to broadly generalize results from this study is constrained by the conditions imposed. Specific conditions in this study that are restrictive include the exclusion of: large pattern matrices, structural coefficients in the latent variable model, and egregiously incorrect targeted values. Subsequent studies that examine the performance of the number of targets per factor for large pattern matrices (e.g., several factors) under conditions commonly observed in practice (e.g., model and target error), and compare this performance to mechanical rotation criteria, could advance the literature. Subsequent studies also could expand the form of the structural equation to include structural coefficients in the latent variable portion of the model, while examining the performance of target rotation as compared to other rotation criteria. Subsequent studies that examine the performance of the number of targets per factor when the targets are more incorrect than in the current study could advance the literature. Finally, the correct number of factors always is specified in the current study, which is a limitation consistent with known limits of factor analysis (e.g., Fava & Velicer, 1996). The potential studies proposed in this paragraph may be timely as the recently proposed ESEM framework (and by extension potential specification of a rotation criterion) appears to offer a viable alternative to CFA in some instances in theory (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009) and in practice (e.g., Marsh et al., 2009; Morin & Maïano, 2011; Myers, Chase, Pierce, & Martin, 2011).

Acknowledgments

The project described was supported by Grant Number 1UL1TR000460, Miami Clinical and Translational Science Institute, from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences and the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

1

Population parameter values for Λ20x3 were retrieved at the website of Robert C. MacCallum (http://www.unc.edu/~rcm/article_material.htm). Occasionally a value within a Λ20x3 was altered slightly in order to achieve the desired level of either h or model error via minor factors.

2

The R code written for this is available upon request.

3

The expression communality is used for simplicity of presentation in the current study as in MacCallum et al. (2001). The variance accounted for by the major factors was denoted b12 in Tucker et al. (1969) to more clearly distinguish this variance from the variance accounted for by the minor factors. While hb12 for each observed variable, h varied with b12 and the values of b12 used in this study were consistent with Tucker et al. (1969) and MacCallum et al.

4

This procedure was altered slightly occasionally due to problems with rotation identification. For example, sometimes specifying the smallest non-targeted pattern coefficient as a target in each column led to a poorly identified rotated solution (e.g., insufficient rank of a submatrix, Λs – see pp. 409-411 of Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009). To remedy this problem, one of the previously targeted coefficients was re-specified as a non-target and another small pattern coefficient in that column was identified as a suitable replacement. Each B is available upon request.

5

Consistent with Asparouhov and Muthén (2009), small values (in this case, ≤ |.08|) were counted as approximately zero.

6

The full six-way ANOVA table for each outcome is available upon request to the first author.

Contributor Information

Nicholas D. Myers, Department of Educational and Psychological Studies, University of Miami

Ying Jin, Department of Psychology, Middle Tennessee State University.

Soyeon Ahn, Department of Educational and Psychological Studies, University of Miami.

Seniz Celimli, Department of Educational and Psychological Studies, University of Miami.

Cengiz Zopluoglu, Department of Educational and Psychological Studies, University of Miami.

References

  1. Algina J. A note on identification in the oblique and orthogonal factor-analysis models. Psychometrika. 1980;3:393–396. doi: 10.1007/BF02293911. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling. 2009;16:397–438. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008204. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bandalos DL. The use of Monte Carlo studies in structural equation modeling research. In: Serlin RC, Hancock GR, Mueller RO, editors. Structural equation modeling: A second course. Greenwich, CT: Information Age; 2006. pp. 385–462. [Google Scholar]
  4. Browne MW. Orthogonal rotation to a partially specified target. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 1972a;25:115–120. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1972.tb00482.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Browne MW. Oblique rotation to a partially specified target. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 1972b;25:207–212. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1972.tb00492.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Browne MW. An overview of analytic rotation in exploratory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2001;36:111–150. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3601_05. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 136–162. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cohen J. Statistical power for the behavioral sciences. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  9. Comrey AL, Lee HB. A first course in factor analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cudeck R, Henly SJ. Model selection in covariance structure analysis and the “problem” of sample size: A clarification. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;115:512–519. doi: 10.1038//0033-2909.109.3.512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. DiStefano C, Hess B. Using confirmatory factor analysis for construct validation: An empirical review. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment. 2005;23:225–241. doi: 10.1177/073428290502300303. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fabrigar LR, Wegener DT, MacCallum RC, Strahan EJ. Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods. 1999;4:272–299. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.4.3.272. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fava JL, Velicer WF. The effects of underextraction on factor and component analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 1996;56:907–929. doi: 10.1177/0013164496056006001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gorsuch RL. Factor analysis. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gruvaeus GT. A general approach to Procrustes pattern rotation. Psychometrika. 1970;35:493–505. doi: 10.1007/BF02291822. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hayashi K, Marcoulides GA. Examining identification issues in factor analysis. Structural Equation Modeling. 2006;13:631–645. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem1304_7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Henson RK, Roberts JK. Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2006;66:393–416. doi: 10.1177/0013164405282485. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hong S. Generating correlation matrices with model error for simulation studies in factor analysis: A combination of the Tucker-Koopman-Linn model and Wijsman’s algorithm. Behavior Research Methods: Instruments & Computers. 1999;31(4):727–730. doi: 10.3758/bf03200754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hoogland JJ, Boomsma A. Robustness studies in covariance structure modeling: An overview and a meta-analysis. Sociological Methods & Research. 1998;26:329–367. doi: 10.1177/0049124198026003003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Horst P. A non-graphical method for transforming an arbitrary factor matrix into a simple structure factor matrix. Psychometrika. 1941;6:79–99. doi:10.1007%2FBF02292176. [Google Scholar]
  21. Jackson DL, Gillaspy JA, Purc-Stephenson R. Reporting practices in confirmatory factor analysis: An overview and some recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2009;14:6–23. doi: 10.1037/a0014694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jennrich RI. Rotation methods, algorithms, and standard errors. In: MacCallum RC, Cudeck R, editors. Factor analysis at 100: Historical developments and future directions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2007. pp. 315–335. [Google Scholar]
  23. Jennrich RI, Sampson PF. Rotation to simple loadings. Psychometrika. 1966;31:313–323. doi: 10.1007/BF02289465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Korth B, Tucker LR. The distribution of chance congruence coefficients from simulated data. Psychometrika. 1975;40:361–372. doi:10.1007%2FBF02294704. [Google Scholar]
  25. MacCallum R. Working with imperfect models. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2003;38:113–139. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3801_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. MacCallum RC, Roznowski M, Necowitz LB. Model modifications in covariance structure analysis: The problem of capitalization on chance. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;111:490–504. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. MacCallum R, Tucker LR. Representing sources of error in the common factor model: Implications for theory and practice. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;109:502–511. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.3.502. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. MacCallum R, Widaman KF, Preacher KJ, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis: The role of model error. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2001;36:611–637. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3604_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. MacCallum R, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods. 1999;4:84–99. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.4.1.84. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Marsh HW, Hau KT, Balla JR, Grayson D. Is more ever too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1998;33:181–220. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Marsh HW, Muthén BO, Asparouhov T, Lüdtke O, Robitzsch A, Morin AJ, Trautwein U. Exploratory structural equation modeling, integrating CFA and EFA: Application to students’ evaluations of university teaching. Structural Equation Modeling. 2009;16:439–476. doi: 10.1080/10705510903008220. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. McDonald RP. Semiconfirmatory factor analysis: The example of anxiety and depression. Structural Equation Modeling. 2005;12:163–172. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem1201_9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Morin AJS, Maïano C. Cross-Validation of the Short Form of the Physical Self- Inventory (PSI-18) using Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling (ESEM) Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2011;12:540–554. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mulaik SA. The foundations of factor analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  35. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 7. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2012. [Google Scholar]
  36. Myers ND, Ahn S, Jin Y. Rotation to a partially specified target matrix in exploratory factor analysis: How many targets? Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2013;20:131–147. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2013.742399. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Myers ND, Chase MA, Pierce SW, Martin E. Coaching efficacy and exploratory structural equation modeling: A substantive-methodological synergy. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology. 2011;33:779–806. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.6.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nunally JC. Psychometric theory. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  39. Saris WE, Aalberts C. Different explanations for correlated disturbance terms in MTMM studies. Structural Equation Modeling. 2003;10:193–213. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1002_2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Sass DA, Schmitt TA. A comparative investigation of rotation criteria within exploratory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2010;45:73–103. doi: 10.1080/00273170903504810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Steiger JH. Causal modeling: A supplementary module for SYSTAT and SYGRAPH. Evanston, IL: SYSTAT; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  42. Thurstone LL. Multiple factor analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1947. [Google Scholar]
  43. Tucker LR. A semi-analytical method of factorial rotation to simple structure. Psychometrika. 1944;9:777–792. doi:10.1007%2FBF02288713. [Google Scholar]
  44. Tucker LR, Koopman RF, Linn RL. Evaluation of factor analytic research procedures by means of simulated correlation matrices. Psychometrika. 1969;34:421–459. doi:10.1007%2FBF02290601. [Google Scholar]
  45. Velicer WF, Fava JL. Effects of variable and subject sampling on factor pattern recovery. Psychological Methods. 1998;3:231–251. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.2.231. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Yates A. Multivariate exploratory data analysis: A perspective on exploratory factor analysis. Albany: State University of New York Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES