Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2014 Dec 9;9(12):e112776. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112776

The Causal Inference of Cortical Neural Networks during Music Improvisations

Xiaogeng Wan 1, Björn Crüts 2, Henrik Jeldtoft Jensen 1,*
Editor: Antonella Gasbarri3
PMCID: PMC4260787  PMID: 25489852

Abstract

We present an EEG study of two music improvisation experiments. Professional musicians with high level of improvisation skills were asked to perform music either according to notes (composed music) or in improvisation. Each piece of music was performed in two different modes: strict mode and “let-go” mode. Synchronized EEG data was measured from both musicians and listeners. We used one of the most reliable causality measures: conditional Mutual Information from Mixed Embedding (MIME), to analyze directed correlations between different EEG channels, which was combined with network theory to construct both intra-brain and cross-brain networks. Differences were identified in intra-brain neural networks between composed music and improvisation and between strict mode and “let-go” mode. Particular brain regions such as frontal, parietal and temporal regions were found to play a key role in differentiating the brain activities between different playing conditions. By comparing the level of degree centralities in intra-brain neural networks, we found a difference between the response of musicians and the listeners when comparing the different playing conditions.

Introduction

Improvisation, an instantaneous creative behavior, is often encountered in different forms of art such as music and dance. In this paper, we study the brain mechanisms of music improvisation. We refer to the performance according to notes as composed music, while the instantaneous creative performance as improvisation. In the performances, each piece of music either composed or improvised was played in either a mechanical manner (i.e. strict mode) or in a more emotionally rich manner (“let-go” mode).

Music improvisation is believed to involve neural-substrates in large brain regions [16] [18] [19] [6] [4] [3] [2] [1] [12] [13]. If these large brain regions are identified, one could use neuro-scientific approaches to improve the quality of music performance. To analyze the brain mechanisms of music improvisation, we use causality measures to analyze the EEG data measured from the experiments and construct generalized neural networks for each experimental condition. We have investigated various kinds of causality measures and found that the conditional Mutual Information from Mixed Embedding (MIME, a time domain direct causality measure developed by I. Vlachos and D. Kugiumtzis [27]) to be the optimal causality measure for our EEG analysis. In this paper, we present our results on intra-brain and cross-brain neural networks, and compare the networks observed for different playing conditions.

Music improvisation has long been studied by neuroscientists and mathematicians using various approaches. Recent research has identified a number of frontal brain regions, including the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) [7] [18], [22] and the dorsal premotor cortex (PMD) [9] [15], to play central roles in more cognitive aspects of movement sequencing and creative generation of music. For a while, scientists have used brain scanning techniques such as fMRI, PET and EEG to study the brain. O. D. Manzano et. al. used fMRI to study the melodic and rhythmic improvisation in a 2×2 factorial experiment [18] [19], where the dorsal premotor cortex (PMD, in frontal cortex, is assumed to be consistently involved in cognitive aspects of planning and selection of spatial motor sequences) was found to be the main region for melodic improvisation, while the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA, showing increased activation during perception, learning and reproduction of temporal sequences) was identified to be related to rhythmic improvisation [18] [19]. A. L. Berkowitz et. al. [3] also used fMRI in a study of expertise-related neural differences between musicians and non-musicians during improvisation. Their results show that musicians have right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ) deactivation during music improvisation, while non-musicians showed no activity change in this region [3]. Moreover, C. Babiloni et al. [1] studied the frequency filtered EEG measured from professional saxophonists during music performances, they found the EEG power density values decreased in the alpha band (8–12 Hz) in the posterior cortex during resting state, while the power values enhanced within narrow high-frequency bands during music performances [1]. Other studies are e.g. EEG phase synchrony analysis [5], fMRI study of jazz improvisation [16], PET studies of melody and sentence generation [6] and the fMRI study of pseudo-random motor and cognitive tasks [2].

Amongst the many mathematical tools used to study music improvisation, we consider the most relevant tools to be measures of correlations, standardized Low Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography (sLORETA, [21]) and analysis of variance (ANOVA, [20], [23]). sLORETA is a method used to localize, identify and visualize EEG point sources in the brain [21]. ANOVA is used to analyze the statistical differences between different experimental conditions [20] [23]. It has been applied to e.g. the music improvisation study on trained pianists [4], which revealed that the dorsal premotor cortex, the rostral cingulate zone of the anterior cingulate cortex and the inferior frontal gyrus are important to both rhythmic and melodic motor sequence creation [4]. D. Dolan et al. undertook a sLORETA analysis of the EEG for music improvisation [10]. They used part of the same data as we have done for the analysis described in this paper, namely EEG measured on members of a trio and two members of the audience. Their sLORETA analysis suggested similar results to those we obtained from the MIME analysis (see below). In both cases the frontal cortex was found to be strongly involved in music improvisation [10]. Other studies investigated e.g. the correlation analysis of EEG [11] [14] and music performance [12] [13], but these investigations did not address the direction of causal influence between EEG channels. In the present paper, we will complement the analysis in [10] by applying MIME and network theory to the analysis of the multi-channel EEG data streams.

For our EEG analysis, we have investigated three popular measures, namely the nonlinear indirect measures: transfer entropy (TE [24]) and MIME [27], and a linear direct measure: partial directed coherence (PDC [25] [26]). TE was found to unsatisfactorily resolve the direction of the causal flow, while PDC returns false or unreliable causalities presumably due to the limitations of the linear autoregressive model fitting. MIME appears to be the best measure for our EEG analysis, among the measures we investigated, and is able to efficiently generate reliable and robust causality results with a satisfactory resolution. Hence, we only report the MIME analysis of the neural intra-brain and inter-brain information flow between large brains regions of musicians and listeners. The technical details of the comparison between different causality measures will be discussed in another publication.

Our study

We consider two experiments regarding the effects of the performance approach for different types of music (composed music and improvisation) and different playing modes (strict mode and “let-go” mode). Our main interest is to identify neural information flow between EEG channels using MIME. It is noteworthy that our emphasis was put on making the experimental environment as close as possible to real concert performances. This emphasis was especially addressed in the second experiment, whose playing environment is more extensive.

Two music improvisation experiments were done at the Guild Hall School of Music and Drama in London separately on 20.06.2010 and 31.03.2012. Synchronized EEG measurements from both the musicians and the listeners were collected by Björn Crüts and his team (BrainMarker Corp.) using CE-certified EEG device (Brainmarker, the Netherlands) during the music performances.

In the first experiment the international concert pianist David Dolan solo performed four pieces of music:

Test 1: Schubert-Impromptu in G flat major Op. 90 No. 3, neutral mode, uninvolved

Test 2: Schubert-Impromptu in G flat major Op. 90 No. 3, fully involved

Test 3: Improvisation, polyphonic, intellectual exercise

Test 4: Improvisation, polyphonic, emotional letting go.

The audience consisted of one listener. Both participants were connected to synchronized EEG amplifiers (250 Hz sampling frequency) with 8 electrodes (P4, T8, C4, F4, F3, C3, T7, P3). The electrodes are labeled by the initial of the corresponding cortices, P: parietal cortex (perception, multi-sensory integration), T: temporal cortex (processing of language and sounds), C: central cortex (sensory and motor function), F: frontal cortex (attention and executive control). The odd numbers stand for locations on the left brain, while even numbers represent locations on the right brain. The electrodes are all localized according to the international 10–20 system (Jasper, 1958). A reference electrode (Cz) at the central location on the top of the head was used, so that each EEG signal was mono-polar referenced to this central site and activity levels of the eight sites could be compared relative to each other.

In the second experiment, the music was performed by the Trio Anima (three highly acclaimed musicians: Drew Balch (violist), Matthew Featherstone (flutist) and Anneke Hodnett (harpist)) in the following order:

  1. Ibert [duration: 3′ 30′′]: 1. strict & 2. “let-go”

  2. Telemann [duration: 2′]: 1. “let-go” & 2. strict

  3. Improvisation: 1. “let-go” & 2. strict

  4. Ravel [duration: 2′ 50′′]: 1. strict & 2. “let-go”

  5. Improvisation: 1. strict & 2. “let-go”

The audience consisted of 14 listeners. Synchronized EEG data was measured from all the musicians and from only two of the listeners, this was due to technical limitations on the number of EEG machines. However, the data from one listener had to be excluded from the cross-brain analysis, due to a technical issue concerning the synchronization with the other EEG machines. The EEG data (100 Hz sampling frequency) was measured from 10 electrodes: P4, T8, C4, F4, F3, C3, T7, P3, O1 and O2 (O: occipital cortex, visual processing center).

In this experiment, pieces A, B and D are composed music (music performances according to a written score) and as such are similar to the first two tests pieces in the first experiment. Pieces C and E were entirely improvised (instantaneous creative performance of music) by the trio and therefore similar in performance approach to the last two pieces of the first experiment. Both the composed music and the improvised were played in the strict mode and the “let-go” mode. Similar to the mode played in the test 2 and test 4 of the first experiment, the “let-go” is a music playing mode with full emotional expression, whereas the strict mode is a mechanical rendition of music, which is similar to the neutral mode in test 1 of the first experiment. However, the intellectual exercise (test 3) of the first experiment consists of the musician improvising a technically correct piece of music without any emotional content. This mode wasn't used in the second experiment. Our aim is to identify the neural differences between the different modes of performance.

For the EEG measurements, standard EEG caps (BraiNet, Jordan Neuroscience) were used to standardize the electrode locations by using anatomical reference points. This ensures that measurements within and between subjects could be compared. Ag/AgCl electrodes with carbon shielded wires (Temec, the Netherlands) and conductive electrode gel (Ten20, D.O. Weaver & Co) were used to minimize movement artifacts. Data acquisition was carried out with a sample frequency of 250 Hz in the first experiment and 100 Hz in the second experiment. Data filtering was executed using a first order 0.16 Hz high pass filter and 59 Hz fourth order low pass filter. The amplifiers were time-synchronized using a purpose build external trigger. Before the measurement, the skin was cleaned using abrasive gel (NuPrep, D.O. Weaver & Co.) to ensure low skin impedance (5 k Ω) and high signal quality.

The reason for the use of 8 channel EEG recordings, is that we focus on the activities of large cortical brain regions, such as the primary motor or temporal cortex. In addition, we aim to choose an experimental set-up of minimal discomfort for the musicians, but still apply enough electrodes to distinguish the activity from different large brain regions. Similar approaches have been used in other patient studies e.g. studies of autism, where the motor cortex activity was measured. Due to the machine set-up (active shielding mechanism), movement artifacts were minimized. Since prefrontal poles were not measured, eye movement artifacts were excluded. Similar considerations hold for other muscle activity, which most frequently originate in the prefrontal cortex, this region was excluded from our measurement. Concerning muscle activity originating in the temporal regions (T), we note that that such activity is limited to high frequencies and that these frequency components (>32 Hz) were filtered out via Fourier transforms.

According to Dr. David Dolan, the redering of composed and improvised music in our experiments are mainly distinguished by the overall manner of the music performances [10]. Improvisation contains more coherent and long-term structural lines, shared by all members of the ensemble. The short-term beats are freer and uneven, but the deep, longer-term pulse is extremely stable in improvisation. In performances of composed music, the gestures seemed to be shorter and more rigid (even in quick repetitive phrases). There is less room for spontaneity and the audience finds themselves less surprised. This is perhaps the reason behind the results of psychological tests, which showed that the audiences found improvisation to be more emotionally engaging and musically interesting [10]. Extra notes were added spontaneously by the freer distribution of time over gestures, which leans more significantly on structural key moments. Another important characteristic of improvisation, is that the risk-taking and mutual support are provided spontaneously by the members of the ensembles. This is probably a consequence of the higher level of active listening that is needed during the improvisation. Hence, one may expect that when improvising, musicians are prevented from entering into an ‘autopilot mode’ since the improvisation forces them to listen very attentively to the music while being prepared for the unexpected to happen at any instance.

Previous research has identified a number of the frontal regions, including the pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA) [7] [18] [22] and the dorsal premotor cortex (PMD) [9] [15] to play central roles in more cognitive aspects of the movement sequencing and creative generation of music. We hypothesize that the musicians may trigger more wide-distributed neural networks when improvising than when performing composed music, and that the frontal regions (attention and executive control) play an important role in the improvisation process.

Given the above differences between different music types and playing modes, we aim to investigate their neural substrates based on the network structure of the neural information flow. Previous music improvisation studies considered either unique point sources of the EEG [10] or symmetric correlations between brain regions [7] [22] [9] [15]. In this paper, we present a causality analysis of the EEG data recorded during music improvisation, which aims to identify neural differences between experimental conditions. We use the MIME causality measure to analyze the EEG data, and construct both intra-brain and cross-brain networks from the MIME causalities. We mention one limitation of MIME. Namely that since MIME is a bivariate causality measure (in contrast to multivariate measures) it is unable to distinguish between direct and indirect causalities. (By this we mean the following. Consider three time series Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic. Assume Inline graphic depends on Inline graphic and Inline graphic depends on Inline graphic, but that Inline graphic doesn't depend directly on Inline graphic. MIME would however return the following dependencies: Inline graphic on Inline graphic and Inline graphic on Inline graphic and also Inline graphic on Inline graphic, because it is a bivariate measure, it is unable to determine that the dependence of Inline graphic on Inline graphic is only through Inline graphic. We stress that although MIME is not a direct measure, in the sense just mentioned, it is certainly a directed measure, in the sense that it can determine whether Inline graphic causes Inline graphic or Inline graphic causes Inline graphic.)

As has been addressed earlier, the reason for using MIME among the many other measures is that we have found it to be the most reliable and useful measure for our experimental data analysis. To verify the directionality of MIME in cross-brain analysis, two reading experiments (See subsection Causality verification of MIME) were analyzed, where MIME was found to be able to identify the correct direction of cross-brain interaction from the reader to the listener. We are convinced from this analysis that MIME does not generate false causalities and is reliable for the analysis of music experiments. Here we especially use the MIME causalities to construct networks that allow us to investigate the differences between experimental conditions.

Results

We have used the causality measure MIME to analyze EEG data from music experiments, from these results we construct both intra-brain and cross-brain neural networks connecting large-brain regions of the musicians and listeners.

Intra-brain neural information flow

In our analysis, each brain is considered as a neural network composed of large cortical brain regions connected by the neural information flow. A link is drawn in the intra-brain neural networks if the causality value is positive and significant according to a significance thresholding test (i.e. surpass the significance threshold, for details see the subsection Dependence on Thresholding), this way residual information flow [17] is avoided. We use time averages of the MIME causalities to determine the links between brain regions.

In the first experiment, the intra-brain neural networks for the pianist and the listener are shown in Figures 1 (pianist) and 2 (listener), respectively. The significance thresholds are taken as Inline graphic and Inline graphic for the pianist and the listener, respectively. The Inline graphic and the Inline graphic are the maximum causality values (averaged over time windows) for the pianist and the listener, respectively. The specific choice of the values 0.2 and 0.1 were found to produce the most clear difference between experimental conditions. Robustness of the results with respect to the choice of these parameters are discussed in subsection Dependence on Thresholding.

Figure 1. Pianist's intra-brain neural networks for the first experiment.

Figure 1

The two panels show the pianist's intra-brain neural networks separately for composed music (left) and improvisation (right). The large brain regions are labeled by the 8 electrodes: F3, F4, C3, C4, T7, T8, P3, P4. The red links indicate the direction of neural information flow between large brain regions, where the thickness of the links represent the magnitudes of the causalities.

Figure 2. Listener's intra-brain neural networks for the first experiment.

Figure 2

The two panels show the listener's intra-brain neural networks separately for composed music (left) and improvisation (right). The large brain regions are labeled by the 8 electrodes: F3, F4, C3, C4, T7, T8, P3, P4. The red links indicate the direction of neural information flow between large brain regions, where the thickness of the links represents the magnitudes of the causalities.

The intra-brain neural information flow networks differ between composed music and improvisation. For the pianist (Figure 1), the information flow is confined to the back of the brain during composed music, whereas during improvisation the flow expands to the entire brain. A similarly expansion was observed for the listener (Figure 2), although in this case the expansion is from the right part of brain to the entire brain when comparing composed music to improvised music.

In the second experiment, an extra pair of conditions: strict mode and “let-go” mode, was added to the experiment. To compare the differences between experimental conditions, we study the contrasts, computed as the difference, between the MIME causalities for the pairwise conditions, e.g. composed music versus improvisation and between “let-go” and strict mode. The contrast causality values were averaged over time windows separately for the musicians and the listeners. Again we used a significance thresholding test to decide the significance of the difference between the causalities. For each pair of conditions, e.g. the composed music vs improvisation (Figure 3), we define a radius Inline graphic as half of the difference between the global maximum and the global minimum contrasts causality values, one radius for the musicians and one for the listeners. The significance threshold was defined as half of the radius Inline graphic, the contrast values outside the interval Inline graphic were deemed significant, otherwise insignificant. This definition of threshold is empirically reasonable, because a lower threshold will lead to a sharp increase in the number of detected information flow, while a higher threshold will prevent reasonable direction of information flow to be registered. For instance, the Inline graphic lattice site in the left panel (musicians) of Figure 3, which has the maximum contrast values 0.1, indicates significant information flow from Inline graphic, i.e. from the left central region to the left temporal region. For more details on the dependence on the specific value of the threshold see subsection Dependence on Thresholding.

Figure 3. Color-map of the contrast causality matrix between composed music and improvisation.

Figure 3

In this figure, the two Inline graphic lattice plot indicates the contrast causality matrices between composed music and improvisation separately for musicians (left) and listeners (right). The direction of information flow is from the row channel to the column channel for each lattice. The color of the lattice indicates the strength of the causality contrasts, of difference, between composed music and improvisation, which is scaled between Inline graphic and Inline graphic. The correspondence between the color and the causality strength is shown in the color-bar.

When composed music is compared to improvisation, we find that the composed music has overall stronger intra-brain causalities than the improvised, which is seen as more links (red) for “composed music > improvisation” than the links (green) for “composed music < improvisation” in the contrast intra-brain neural networks (Figure 4). This result does not contradict the observed expansion of neural information flow when composed music is changed to improvisation, this only indicates a difference in the strength of the causality values between the two conditions and corresponds to stronger values for the composed music.

Figure 4. The contrast intra-brain neural networks between composed music and improvisation for the second experiment.

Figure 4

The contrast neural networks were drawn from Figure 3. The causality contrasts were obtained by taking the differences of the MIME causalities between composed music and improvisation. A link is drawn in this network if the causality contrast is significant according to a thresholding test. The red links indicate the information flow that are significantly stronger (causality values) in composed music than in improvisation, while the green links indicate the information flow that are significantly stronger in improvisation than in composed music.

For musicians (the left panel in Figure 4) the significant information flow of composed music are from both the left and right central regions to the left temporal region ([C3,C4]Inline graphicT7) and from the right frontal region to the right occipital region (F4Inline graphicO2). For listeners (the right panel in Figure 4), information flow that are significant in composed music are from the left frontal and left parietal regions break into two branches, one is to the left temporal region ([F3,P3]Inline graphicT7), the other is to the right frontal region via the right central ([F3,P3]Inline graphicF4, or [F3, P3]Inline graphicC4Inline graphicF4) and right temporal regions (P3Inline graphicT8Inline graphicF4, or [F3, P3]Inline graphicC4Inline graphicT8Inline graphicF4). The left frontal (F3) and left parietal (P3) regions act as the main sources of information flow, while the left temporal (T7) and right frontal (F4) regions are the main sinks, and the right central (C4) and right temporal (T8) regions serve as transit hubs. The listeners also have significant information flow during improvisation (green links in the right panel of Figure 4): from the right frontal region to the left frontal (F4Inline graphicF3) and right temporal regions (F4Inline graphicT8) and from the left central to the left frontal region (C3Inline graphicF3). The information flow that are significant during improvisation (red links) have directions opposite those found during composed music (green links). The more red links than green links in the figure of the contrast intra-brain neural networks are not in contradiction to the expansion in the distributions of information flow, when composed music is changed to improvisation. The dominance of red links only implies that those directions have stronger causality values during composed music than during improvisation. In other words, this analysis highlights the difference in causality values between the different music types. Namely, when the flow occurs with significant different causal weights for different mode of performance, it will be detected in Figure 4, while if the information flow occurs with more or less comparable and significant causality values for the two conditions no contrast will be registered in this figure. In the subsection Dependence on Thresholding, we explain that the observed contrast structure doesn't depend in any important way on the choice of thresholds.

The network structures are more complicated for strict mode and “let-go” mode (Figure 5). For musicians (left panel) information flow that are stronger in strict mode (i.e. “strictInline graphiclet-go”) are from the left frontal region to the left and right central regions (F3Inline graphicC3, C4) and to the left occipital (F3Inline graphicO1) and the right temporal (F3Inline graphicT8) regions, from the right frontal and left central regions to the left temporal region (F4, C3Inline graphicT7) and from the right parietal region to the left central (P4Inline graphicC3), right occipital (P4Inline graphicO2) and right temporal (P4Inline graphicT8) regions. Here we see that the left frontal region (F3) and the right parietal (P4) region are key to musicians playing in strict mode (“strictInline graphiclet-go”). However, in the same intra-brain neural network for musicians, information flow that are stronger in “let-go” (i.e. “let-goInline graphicstrict”) mode are from the right frontal region to left occipital region (F4Inline graphicO1) and from the right parietal region to left temporal region (P4Inline graphicT7). For listeners, there is also a clear difference in the distribution of neural information flow. The information flow stronger in strict mode (i.e. “strict modeInline graphiclet-go mode”) are from the left parietal to the left frontal (P3Inline graphicF3) and left temporal regions (P3Inline graphicT7), from the right temporal region to the left temporal region (T8Inline graphicT7) and from the right central region to the right frontal region (C4Inline graphicF4), whereas information flow stronger in “let-go” mode (i.e “strict mode < let-go mode”) are from the left and right frontal regions to the right central (F3,F4Inline graphicC4) and right temporal regions (F3,F4Inline graphicT8) and from the left central region via the left temporal region to the right central region (C3Inline graphicT7Inline graphicC4). In strict mode flow tend to be from the back to the front of the brain, whilst “let-go” mode tend to exhibit the inverse direction from the front to the back of the brain. We explain in the subsection Dependence on Thresholding that the observed contrast structure doesn't change when we change the thresholds values by small amounts.

Figure 5. The contrast intra-brain neural networks between strict mode and “let-go” mode for the second experiment.

Figure 5

The contrast neural networks were drawn from Figure 3. The causality contrasts were obtained by taking the differences of the MIME causalities between strict mode and “let-go” mode. A link is drawn in this network if the causality contrast is significant according to a thresholding test. The red links indicate the information flow that are significantly stronger (causality values) in strict mode than in “let-go” mode, while the green links indicate the information flow that are significantly stronger in “let-go” mode than in strict mode.

Since the intra-brain analysis studied the difference between different experimental conditions, we do not have enough statistics to discuss reliably person specific instantaneous flow patterns, but have concentrated on average trends of information flow as well as the sink and source activities of large brain regions. These results are obtained by averaging over time windows and over experimental conditions.

Degree centrality analysis

To identify the difference between experimental conditions in terms of the importance of large brain regions, we carried out an analysis of the degree centrality of the intra-brain neural networks. Since the intra-brain neural networks are directed we count the number of in-going and out-going links to a node separately and thereby calculate the in-degree and out-degree for each node (i.e. large brain region). The degree centralities were averaged over time windows and experimental conditions, results show that the musicians typically have opposite trends to the listeners when composed music is compared to improvised and the strict mode is compared to the “let-go” mode.

The differences between the experimental conditions were compared by subtracting the degree centralities found under one condition from those found under another condition and thereby focus on the contrast between experimental conditions. In Figure 6, we show that musicians were found to have larger in- and out-degrees during improvisation than during composed music, while the listeners exhibit the opposite trend. When strict mode was compared with “let-go” mode (Figure 7), we find that musicians have larger in- and out-degrees in strict mode than in “let-go” mode, while listeners again exhibit the opposite results. In this analysis, a larger in- and out-degree indicates a larger amount of information flow in and out of the coresponding brain region and hence one would expect this to imply that the region is more functionally involved with the other regions in the network.

Figure 6. Degree centrality contrasts, or difference, between composed music and improvisation in the second experiment.

Figure 6

In this figure, the red stems and the blue stems indicate the in and out degree centrality contrasts between composed music and improvisation, respectively. The horizontal axis has 9 channels represent the 8 electrodes: P4, T8, C4, F4, F3, C3, T7, P3 and the overall average over the 8 electrodes, while the vertical axis gives the magnitudes of the degree centrality contrasts between composed music and improvisation.

Figure 7. Degree centrality contrasts between strict mode and “let-go” mode in the second experiment.

Figure 7

In this figure, the red stems and the blue stems indicate the in and out degree centrality contrasts between strict mode and “let-go” mode, respectively. The horizontal axis has 9 channels represent the 8 electrodes: P4, T8, C4, F4, F3, C3, T7, P3 and the overall average over the 8 electrodes, while the vertical axis gives the magnitudes of the degree centrality contrasts between strict mode and “let-go” mode.

Cross-brain networks

P. Vuust reported in [28] and [29] a study of jazz performances, where the jazz musicians were found to communicate with each other by modulating their individual rhythm during ensemble performances. In our experiments, we study both the musicians and the listeners, and we try to investigate the way the musicians coordinate with each other in terms of information flow. In Figure 8, we show e.g. the information flow within and between the brains of the flutist and the harpist. We also investigate how the musicians interact with the listeners during the music performances. In our study, to analyze the pattern of coordination, we monitor the average cross-brain causalities which results in a single nonnegative real number for each direction (i.e. from one brain to another). A cross-brain link is drawn if the average causality value i.e. the cross-brain weight, is significantly higher in one direction than in the opposite direction. For instance, in the second experiment, the cross-brain weight was significantly higher for harpistInline graphic listener, but almost vanished for listenerInline graphicharpist, hence the cross-brain interaction was from the harpist to the listener during the music performances. We do not need to define a specific threshold, since the cross-brain weight is positive with high values in one direction and almost vanishing cross-brain weight in the opposite direction between each pair of brains. For instance, in Figure 9, the cross-brain weight for flutistInline graphiclistener is clearly higher than the cross-brain weight for listenerInline graphicflutist, which implies a directed link from the flutist to the listener.

Figure 8. Color-map of cross-brain causality matrix between flutist and harpist in the second experiment.

Figure 8

This graph shows a color-map (scaled between 0 and 1) of the cross-brain causality matrix for the flutist and harpist at the time window 15 (140.01s-150.00s) during the performance of piece A: Ibert (strict mode). The two Inline graphic diagonal blocks indicate the intra-brain causalities for the flutist (upper-left) and the harpist (lower-right), respectively, while the two Inline graphic off-diagonal blocks indicate the cross-brain causalities for flutistInline graphicharpist (upper-right) and for harpistInline graphicflutist (lower-left). The correspondence between the color and the causality values is shown in the color-bar.

Figure 9. The cross-brain weights between flutist and listener in the second experiment.

Figure 9

This figure plots the cross-brain causalities between flutist and listener against time windows for piece A: Ibert, strict mode. The red curve indicates flutistInline graphiclistener, the blue curve represents listenerInline graphicflutist, while the black curve is the significance threshold.

In the first experiment (the left graph of Figure 10), the cross-brain interaction is from the pianist to the listener (average weights: Inline graphic), while in the second experiment (the right graph of Figure 10), the cross-brain interactions are from the three musicians to the listener: [flutist, harpist, violinist]Inline graphiclistener (average weights: Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic) and from the harpist to the flutist and violinist: harpistInline graphic[flutist, violinist] (average weights: Inline graphic and Inline graphic). The flutist ping-pongs with the violinist: flutistInline graphicviolinist (Inline graphic), the average values are high in both directions, but the dominance of the cross-brain weights swaps between the two when the time window moves). This network structure is robust for all performances in the second experiment.

Figure 10. Cross-brain networks for the two music improvisation experiments.

Figure 10

The left graph is for the first experiment, while the right graph is for the second experiment. The red links represent the direction of cross-brain information flow, while the thickness of the links is proportional to the strength of the cross-brain weights (i.e. the average cross-brain causalities).

To verify the directionality of MIME in the detection of cross-brain interactions, we conducted two reading experiments. The reading experiments consist of a reader and a listener, where the reader read to the listener to establish a natural driver-responder system during the reading processes. Each reading experiment has two tests, where the reader and the listener swap their roles for the different tests. The EEG data of the reading experiments were analyzed by MIME to obtain the cross-brain information flow. The outcome is a causal direction pointing from the reader to the listener. We mention that in one test of each experiment the cross-brain weights for the flow between the reader and the listener were equivalent, but, importantly, both weights were insignificant, in which case no cross-brain interaction is detected. This is of course a limitation of the MIME analysis and shows that MIME may miss causal relations. On the other hand, the analysis of the reading experiment suggests that MIME is unlikely to produce causalities that do not exist. In other words, we believe that MIME is unlikely to produce false positives. For more details see subsection Causality verification of MIME.

Discussion and Summary

In this paper, we constructed intra-brain and cross-brain networks for both musicians and listeners during music performances. The differences between the composed music and improvised music and between the strict mode and the “let-go” mode can be identified in terms of the direction of neural information flow, the number of in-going and out-going connections (i.e. the in-degree and out-degree centralities) between large brain regions, as well as the sink and source activities in the frontal, parietal and temporal regions. The latter are similar to the results obtained from the sLORETA on the same data set [10], [8].

In the intra-brain neural network analysis, the improvisation was found to trigger a more widely distributed network structure than the composed music did. When composed music is changed to improvisation, the distribution of intra-brain neural information flow expands from the back of the brain to the entire brain (for musicians), the frontal (attention and executive control) and central (motor cortex) regions become activated when musicians improvise. This may be because performing or listening to improvisations demands more widespread functional coordinations between large brain regions. When composed music is compared to improvisation, the intra-brain causality values are found to be greater in composed music than during improvisation, particularly for the listeners. We find that the neural information flow starts and terminates separately in left frontal and right frontal regions, the neural information flow reverses directions when composed music is changed to improvisation and strict mode is changed to “let-go” mode. These results agree with earlier studies [7] [22]: the frontal regions (a more general area that covers the dorsal prefrontal regions), especially the right frontal region, play an important role in free improvisation of melodies and rhythms, which is the key regions that distinguish the brain activities between composed music and improvisation and between strict mode and “let-go” mode. Moreover, the central regions tend to act as transit hubs for the neural information flow for all experimental conditions. This is in contrast to what we find for the temporal and parietal regions, which behave differently under different experimental conditions.

The identification of the importance of the frontal regions is similar to the findings of a previous fMRI study of pianist improvisation [2], where the dorsal prefrontal cortex (part of the frontal regions) and rostral premotor regions (located within the frontal regions) were found to be involved in the free-response selection. This study shows an activation of the cortical association areas, especially the prefrontal cortex, during divergent thinking, where the right prefrontal cortex appears to be particularly involved. The high level of involvement of the frontal and central regions and the source activity of the right frontal region during improvisation also agree with the cortical source analysis (sLORETA) on the EEG data we have studied in this paper (see [10]). Dolan et al. found that a clear increase in the activation of the frontal region, acting as the EEG point sources of the brain activities, when composed music was changed to improvisation [10]. A similar studies on cortical regions of music improvisation used fMRI on a improvising pianist [4]. The study found the dorsal prefrontal and rostral cingulate regions to play a key role in melodic and rhythmic improvisation [4].

In the study of intra-brain neural networks we used the degree centrality to analyze the level of connections between large brain regions. Since the intra-brain neural networks are directed and the degree centrality measure (i.e. the number of links connected to the nodes) is very simple to use and is a very suitable centrality measure for directed networks. In this analysis musicians were found to have opposite trends to the listeners. The musicians tend to have overall larger (in and out) degree centralities in improvisation than in composed music, which may be because the improvisation demands more intra-brain communication for the musicians to be able to instantaneously create melodies and rhythms. They also have larger degree centralities in strict mode than in “let-go” mode, which may be because musicians need more brain attention to perform in strict mode. In contrast, the listeners have larger degree centralities in composed music than in improvisation, which may be because the listeners found the music performed according to a score to be more familiar than the instantaneous creation of music during improvisation. The listeners also have larger degree centralities in “let-go” mode than in strict mode. It is interesting to mention that a questionnaire answered by the listeners showed that music performed with free emotional expression, i.e. the “let-go” mode, is considered more beautiful than the mechanical rendition of music, i.e. the strict mode, see [10] for details.

The cross-brain network structure provides a sensible view of the pattern of coordination between musicians and interactions between musicians and listeners, either during solo, or ensembles, performances. In the cross-brain networks, the musicians are pointing to the listeners, which of course seems to confirm the fact that the musicians are communicating to the listeners during the music performances. The harpist was frequently found to lead the flutist and the violinist, this may be because the harp provides the chord structure, which is then responded to by the flutist and the violinist during the trio's improvisation.

We want to point out a limitation and a strength of our study. Our EEG recording has only 8 or 10 electrodes, which are quite few compared to other studies. This enables us to analyze only general brain activities. However, since we do not focus on specific task-related brain activities, this experimental set-up is sufficient for us to be able to distinguish the brain activities from different cortical brain regions and at the same time the low number of electrodes minimize the discomfort to the participants during the experiments. Nevertheless, the results of our study imply that the neural differences in the brain of the subjects (e.g. the musicians and the listeners) under different experimental conditions (e.g. composed music and improvisation) can be detected by the network analysis generated from the MIME causality measures. This analysis provides a potential tool to study the intra-brain and cross-brain information flow and therefore is a very promising tool for the analysis of group behavior in other situations similar to ensemble performances of music. The method of analysis can potentially be applied to financial and more general neuroscience data sets.

Methods

The experiment on musicians has already been published in [10] and received ethical approval from Guildhall School of Music and Drama.

The reading experiment was performed on employers from Brainmarker. Before agreeing to participate, the participants were informed in detail about the experiment and signed the informed consent form; although they were allowed to withdraw at any moment. The reading experiment simply consists of the same kind of check-of-method as carried out routinely by, and on, Brainmaker's employees, hence no institutional approval was obtained. We have obtained confirmation from the Head of the Imperial College Mathematics Department that prior approval was not required.

We use causality measures to analyze the intra-brain connectivities between large brain regions and cross-brain interactions between musicians and listeners. We have tried three frequently used causality measures, namely the partial directed coherence (PDC [25] [26]), transfer entropy (TE [24]) and conditional mutual information from mixed embedding (MIME [27]), in order to compare the efficiency and practicality in EEG analysis. From the analysis, we found that PDC gives an unrealistic large number of cross-brain causalities from listener to the pianist in the first experiment although listener and pianist were facing away from each other. TE has poor directionality as it gives similar strength for causalities between pairs of links with opposite directions. Only MIME presents clear directionality and robust results with larger average causalities from musicians to listeners than from listeners to musicians. Therefore, we use MIME as our core causality measure for the EEG analysis.

The MIME software package developed by I. Vlachos and D. Kugiumtzis, et al. [27] was used to calculate the causalities between EEG data channels. MIME is a time domain bivariate method, used to analyze nonlinear indirect information flow. It uses a progressive scheme to select mixed embedding vectors that maximizes the conditional mutual information rate between future and past embedding vectors [27]. For a K-dimensional stationary vector process Inline graphic, the causality from Inline graphic to Inline graphic is calculated by defining a future vector Inline graphic containing the future of the driven variable (Inline graphic), a uniform state-space embedding vector

graphic file with name pone.0112776.e095.jpg

consists of the lagged values from both driving (Inline graphic) and driven (Inline graphic) variables and an empty vector Inline graphic as an initial selected non-uniform state-space embedding vector, Inline graphic is the time horizon (prediction step) of Inline graphic and Inline graphic are the maximum time lags for Inline graphic and Inline graphic, respectively. In each iterative cycle Inline graphic, the progressive scheme seeks element in Inline graphic that satisfies the maximum criterion

graphic file with name pone.0112776.e106.jpg (1)

which element will be add to Inline graphic to form a new selected vector Inline graphic. The progressive scheme stops at an Inline graphic-th iterative circle and uses Inline graphic as the final embedding vector if the stopping criterion

graphic file with name pone.0112776.e111.jpg (2)

is satisfied. Here, Inline graphic is a threshold close to 1. I. Vlachos and D. Kugiumtzis, et al. [27] found empirically that Inline graphic (default in MIME software) gives the best causality detection.

When the progressive scheme terminates, MIME measures the causal effect from Inline graphic to Inline graphic (Inline graphic, Inline graphic) by evaluating the ratio between the conditional mutual information rates

graphic file with name pone.0112776.e118.jpg (3)

where Inline graphic is the final selected non-uniform state-space embedding vector when the progressive scheme terminates where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are the Inline graphic th and Inline graphic th components of Inline graphic, respectively.

Here, MIME was applied on the standardized EEG voltages. MIME is an information based measure entirely determined by the probability distributions of the signals and therefore independent of the amplitude of the measured signal. Hence no normalisation is necessary. To analyze cross-brain information flow, synchronized EEG data measured from each combination of two different brains was put together to form an augmented data matrix, e.g. the EEG data of the pianist and the listener. These augmented data matrices were analyzed by moving time windows with window size Inline graphics (Inline graphicHz) for the first experiment and Inline graphics (Inline graphicHz) for the second experiment. These time windowed data files were used as input to the MIME software.

The MIME software outputs sequences of causality matrices, which contain both intra-brain and cross-brain causalities. These matrices are of size Inline graphic (Inline graphic) for the first (second) experiment, which consists of two Inline graphic (Inline graphic) diagonal sub-matrices for intra-brain causalities and two Inline graphic (Inline graphic) off-diagonal sub-matrices for cross-brain causalities. The diagonal sub-matrices (intra-brain) were averaged over time windows to construct intra-brain neural networks. The intra-brain causality matrices were also discretized into binary matrices, which after matrix transposition become the directed adjacency matrix for the intra-brain neural networks, the directed adjacency matrices were then used to compute the degree centralities. The off-diagonal sub-matrices (cross-brain) were used to construct cross-brain networks by taking averages of the cross-brain causalities over electrodes and comparing the magnitudes of the causality averages with the opposite cross-brain direction. A cross-brain link is drawn from one brain to another, if the average cross-brain causality is significantly larger from one brain to the other than measured in the opposite direction. If the average values are equivalent in both directions, the cross-brain causality cancel each other and one will not draw a link between this pair of brains.

A strength of MIME is that it doesn't rely on any computationally costly significance tests. This is due to the stopping criterion and the progressive scheme. However, we do have to use significant thresholding tests on the MIME causalities in order to identify the important differences between experimental conditions and filter out any residual flow arising from numerical inaccuracies [17]. We made use of different values for the thresholds for the different types of analysis reported. In all cases we chose the threshold with the aim to best identify neural difference between experimental conditions. It is important to point out that small changes to the value of the thresholds do not have any serious influence on the results. Details of the investigation of the significance threshold are presented in subsection: Dependence on Thresholding.

Causality verification of MIME

We use MIME for our EEG analysis because it is found to be more reliable than TE [24] and PDC [25], [26]. Several simulation tests find that MIME is able to detect correctly the causality structure [27]. Moreover, reasonable directional interdependencies are reported for experimental time series such as EEG for epilepsy patient [27]. To our knowledge, no paper has used MIME to study music improvisation studies yet. To verify the directionality of MIME, we designed two reading experiments with the aim to check the cross-brain directional inference of MIME.

The reading experiments include one reader and one listener, both of which are healthy normal people. The reader is to read a short story to the listener, while the listener is to listen to the story carefully and try to imagine the scene described by the story. When the first story is finished the reader and the listener swap their roles after a short break, to repeat the reading process on another story. The stories were new to both the reader and the listener. The reader and listener did not face each other during the tests in order to avoid visual influences. Synchronized EEG data was measured from the reader and the listener on 10 electrodes (P4, O2, T8, C4, F4, F3, C3, T7, P3, O1) during the reading processes with 100Hz sampling frequency. The whole experiment was repeated once on another pair of healthy normal subjects to avoid fortuity.

The MIME analysis (time window analysis, window size: Inline graphics) shows that the dominant cross-brain information flow is from the reader to the listener. In the first reading experiment, the average (cross-brain) causalities are Inline graphic and Inline graphic in one test, while Inline graphic and Inline graphic in the other test. In the second reading experiment, Inline graphic and Inline graphic in one case, while Inline graphic and Inline graphic in the other test. For both experiments, a link can be drawn from the reader to the listener, rather than the opposite direction, because the overall causality average is significantly greater for readerInline graphiclistener than for listenerInline graphicreader. This is according to a significance thresholding test with instantaneous threshold Inline graphic above the mean value between Inline graphic and Inline graphic at each time window. For the cases with equivalent causality strength between the reader and the listener, non of the two causal directions surpass the significance threshold, in which case it was deemed that no significant causal influence occurred between the reader and the listener. Nevertheless, the overall average for the four tests (two experiments) gives dominant cross-brain causalities from the reader to the listener.

We have varied the parameters of MIME, e.g. the time horizon (prediction step) Inline graphic and the maximum embedding dimension (time lags) Inline graphic under restriction that Inline graphic [27]. In spite of these variations the directional results were unchanged. This implies that the directionality of MIME doesn't depend strongly on the parameter choice. Our conclusion from the reading experiments is that MIME may fail to pickup up causal links (e.g. no significant causal influence between the reader and the listener), but it never predicts an unreliable causalities. This means, once MIME picks up a causal direction, one has good reason to believe in the directional results.

Dependence on Thresholding

The network exhibiting the information theoretic causality flow between brain regions relies on defining thresholds. If the strength of the causal weight in a certain direction exceeds the threshold, a directed link is included in the network. We now study systematically for each of the extracted networks, how the resulting structure depends on the value of the chosen threshold.

The First experiment

In Figures 1 and 2 different thresholds were used for the pianist (Inline graphic) and for the listener (Inline graphic). This choice of thresholds correspond to the most clear difference between experimental conditions. In order to access the stability of our results we altered the thresholds to intervals with boundaries 10% below and above the original thresholds, namely, Inline graphic and Inline graphic or Inline graphic and Inline graphic. For this new set of parameters the intra-brain neural network structures for both the pianist and the listener remain unchanged. We also checked what happens when we changed the thresholds by 100% below and above the original thresholds. In this case changes do occur to the network structures but only involving the weaker links, the stronger links i.e. the most dominant links still remain unchanged. Thus the conclusion concerning the main differences between the different modes of performance remain unchanged

The Second experiment

We again check how the 10% changes to the thresholds influence the network structures. See Figure 4 for the contrast intra-brain neural networks between composed and improvised music. When the significance thresholds increase by 10%, i.e. a new interval Inline graphic (Inline graphic denotes the original threshold), the contrast network structures remain unchanged for both the musicians and the listeners. When the significance thresholds decrease by 10%, i.e. a new interval Inline graphic, the contrast network structure is unchanged for the musicians, while for the listeners, all the old links remain, with new red link (causalities Composed musicInline graphicimprovised music) F3Inline graphicT8 and green links (causalities Composed musicInline graphicimprovised music) F4Inline graphicC4 and T7Inline graphicC3 added to the network. However, these small changes to the listeners' network, follow the same pattern as the old links do, i.e. the trends of the neural difference between the composed music and the improvisation remain unchanged.

For the contrast between the strict mode and the “let-go” mode in Figure 5, again the networks for both musicians and listeners stay unchanged when the significance threshold increases by 10%, i.e. Inline graphic. However, when the significance threshold decreases by 10%, new red links (causalities Strict modeInline graphic“let-go” mode) T8Inline graphicO2 and P3Inline graphic[C4,T7] and new green links C4Inline graphicT7 and C3Inline graphicF3 are added to the musicians contrast network, whereas new red links T8Inline graphicF4 and P3Inline graphicF3 and new green links F4Inline graphic[T8,C4] are added to the listeners contrast networks. Nevertheless, the new links follow the same pattern as the original links do and hence no essential change occurred to the observed neural differences between the strict mode and the “let-go” mode.

From this analysis we conclude that small changes to the significance thresholds do not have any essential influence on the neural network structures, which implies that the observed neural differences between experimental conditions are robust.

As to the cross-brain networks, the significant cross-brain links are drawn in the dominant directions (i.e. the direction with stronger weight between a pair of opposite directions). Because the cross-brain weights are significantly positive in one direction and almost vanished in the opposite direction, the cross-brain networks are robust and do not necessitate the use of any significance thresholds.

Selection of causality measures

There are a number of reasons for us to use MIME in our EEG analysis. Firstly, we have compared our EEG analysis using three popular causality measures: MIME [27], PDC [25] [26] and TE [24], in which MIME produces the most reliable results among the three measures. PDC is a linear method which relies crucially on linear autoregressive models. For real EEG analysis, PDC presents a large number of, presumably false, causalities from the listeners to the musicians. The PDC causality flow from listeners to musicians was found even when the two groups couldn't watch each other and since only the musicians made any sound we would expect causality to entirely flow from musicians to listeners.

TE is a nonlinear method, which is supposed to work better than PDC in nonlinear time series analysis. However, due to computational restrictions on embedding dimensions, TE cannot use large enough embedding dimensions and is for this reason unable to produce satisfactory directional results. The TE's analysis generates similar causalities between every pair of brains, so no cross-brain network structure is detected. A small increment in the embedding dimension will cause a dramatic increase in the computation time.

Both the linearity and computational short comings of the PDC and TE were overcome by MIME, which can produce reliable causality results efficiently [27]. As has been tested on various data, MIME presents all correct directional results for model data and reasonable causalities for experimental time series [27]. Furthermore, we also did tests on a natural driving-driven respond system, namely the reading experiments to test the reliability of MIME in cross-brain analysis (See section Causality verification of MIME), from which analysis we concluded that MIME does not present false or unreasonable causalities when analyzing directed interactions between experimental time series.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge Prof. D. Kugiumitzis (Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece) for the contribution of MIME and its software package, international concert pianist Dr. D. Dolan (Guildhall School of Music and Drama) and the trio Anima for music improvisation performances. We also express our gratitude to K. Mamani, J. Clough, and I. Norman for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Funding Statement

These authors have no support or funding to report.

References

  • 1. Babiloni C, Vecchio F, Infarinato F, Buffo P, Marzano N, Spada D, Rossi S, Bruni I, Rossini PM, Perani D (2011) Simultaneous recording of electroencephalographic data in musicians playing in ensemble. Science Direct, Cortex 47:1082–1090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Bengtsson SL, Csikszentmihalyi M, Ullen F (2007) Cortical Regions Involved in the Generation of Musical Structures during Improvisation in Pianists. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Massachusetts Institute of Technology 19:830–842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Berkowitz AL, Ansari D (2010) Expertise-related deactivation of the right temporoparietal junction during music improvisation. NeuroImage 49:712–719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Berkowitz AL, Ansari D (2008) Generation of novel motor sequences: The neural correlates of musical improvisation. NeuroImage 41:535–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Bhattacharya J, Petsche H (2005) Phase Synchrony Analysis of EEG During Music Perception Reveals Changes In Functional Connectivity Due To Musical Expertise. Signal Processing 85:21612177. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Brown S, Martinez MJ, Parsons LM (2006) Music and language side by side in the brain: a PET study of the generation of melodies and sentences. European Journal of Neuroscience 23:2791–2803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Beudel M, Jong BMD (2009) Overlap and segregation in predorsal premotor cortex activations related to free selection of self-referenced and target-based finger movements. Cerebral Cortex 19:23612371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Cruts B (2012) Report of statistical analysis of EEG measurements for music improvisation experiments on 30th, Mar. 2012. Private Communication [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Deiber MP, Passingham RE, Colebatch JG, Friston KJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RSJ (1991) Cortical areas and the selection of movement: a study with positron emission tomography. Experimental Brain Research 84 393?02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Dolan D, Sloboda J, Jensen HJ, Crutz B, Feygelson E (2013) The improvisatory approach to classical music performance: an empirical investigation into its characteristics and impact. Music Performance Research 6:1–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Dumas G, Nadel J, Soussignan R, Martinerie J, Garnero L (2010) Inter-Brain Synchronization during Social Interaction. PLOS ONE 5 8:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Hennig H, Fleischmann R, Fredebohm A, Hagmayer Y, Nagler J, Witt A, Theis FJ, Geisel T (2011) The Nature and Perception of Fluctuations in Human Musical Rhythms. PLOS ONE 6:10, 1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Hennig H, Fleischmann R, Geisel T (2012) Musical rhythms: The science of being slightly off. Physics Today 65 7:64–65. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Kawasaki M, Yamada Y, Ushiku Ye, Miyauchi E, Yamaguchi Y (2013) Inter-brain synchronization during coordination of speech rhythm in human-to-human social interaction. Scientific Reports 3:1692, 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Lau HC, Rogers RD, Ramnani N, Passingham RE (2004) Willed action and attention to the selection of action. NeuroImage 21:14071415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Limb CJ, Braun AR (2008) Neural Subtrates of Spontaneous Musical Performance: An fMRI Study of Jazz Improvisation. PLoS ONE e1679, 3.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Lungarella M, Olaf S (2006) Mapping information flow in sensorimotor networks. PLoS Computational Biology 2 10:e144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Manzano OD, Ullen F (2012) Activation and connectivity patterns of the presupplementary and dorsal premotor areas during free improvisation of melodies and rhythms. NeuroImage 63:272–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Manzano OD, Ullen F (2012) Goal-independent mechanisms for free response generation: Creative and preudo-random performance share neural substrates. NeuroImage 59:772–780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Martin G, Larson SD (2008) Analysis of Variance. Circulation 117:115–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Pascual-Marqui D (2002) Standardized Low Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography (sLORETA): Technical Details. Methods and Findings In Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology 24D:5–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Pesaran B, Nelson MJ, Andersen RA (2008) Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature 453:406409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Shapiro SS, Wilk MB (1965) An Analysis of Variance Test for Normality. Biometrika 52:591–611. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Schreiber T (2000) Measuring Information Transfer. Phys. Rev. Letters 85 (2):461–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Takahashi DY, Baccala LA, Sameshima K (2010) Frequency Domain Connectivity: an Information Theoretic Perspective. 32nd Annual International Conference of the IEEE EMBS, Buenos Aires, Argentina [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Takahashi DY, Baccala LA, Sameshima K (2010) Information Theoretical interpretation of frequency domain connectivity measures. Biol Cybern 103:463–469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Vlachos I, Kugiumtzis D (2010) Nonuniform state-space reconstruction and coupling detection. Physical Review, E 82:016207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Vuust P (2000) Polyrytmik og -metrik i moderne jazz: En studie af Miles Davis' kvintet fra 1960erne. Det Jyske Musikkonservatorium [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Vuust P, Roepstorff A (2008) Listen up! Polyrhythms in brain and music. Cognitive Semiotics 3:134–158. [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES