Abstract
Human societies are characterized by more cultural diversity than chimpanzee communities. However, it is currently unclear what mechanism might be driving this difference. Because reliance on social information is a pivotal characteristic of culture, we investigated individual and social information reliance in children and chimpanzees. We repeatedly presented subjects with a reward-retrieval task on which they had collected conflicting individual and social information of equal accuracy in counterbalanced order. While both species relied mostly on their individual information, children but not chimpanzees searched for the reward at the socially demonstrated location more than at a random location. Moreover, only children used social information adaptively when individual knowledge on the location of the reward had not yet been obtained. Social information usage determines information transmission and in conjunction with mechanisms that create cultural variants, such as innovation, it facilitates diversity. Our results may help explain why humans are more culturally diversified than chimpanzees.
Keywords: culture, social learning, chimpanzees, children, decision-making
1. Introduction
Culture, pivotally defined by socially transmitted information, is a more pronounced characteristic of human societies than of the societies of our closest living relatives [1]. Even in the most studied of our extant relatives, the chimpanzee, although culture has been identified [2,3], its magnitude does not compare to the cultural richness of the human species [4,5]. The reason for this cultural gap has been speculated upon. For instance, scholars have emphasized chimpanzees' conservative nature [6,7] and conjectured this predisposition to be impeding their cultural diversification (see [4]). However, direct comparisons with human's conservative tendencies have remained scarce and where the two species have been compared, chimpanzees did not prove to be more conservative than humans ([8], also see [9,10]). Others have asserted that humans, but not chimpanzees, are able to imitate each other and that this capacity may be the driving force behind humans' relatively vast cultural proliferation ([11,12]; also see [13,14]). While this may be true, empirical and theoretical work indicates that culture could also thrive without imitation [15–17]. Hence, these explanations provide insufficient accounts of the cultural gap [4]. A relatively unexplored explanation for the cultural gap is that humans might place more value on social information than chimpanzees do and thus integrate more observed behaviours in their repertoires, which is the hallmark of cultural transmission [1]. Notably, ‘culture’ does not refer exclusively to cumulative information, which currently seems to be the locus of comparative investigations with respect to cultural capacities of great apes [4,7,8,10], but more broadly to socially obtained behavioural patterns, which can be quantified in their own right [5].
Therefore, we investigated whether children and chimpanzees place different value on social information. We explored this question with a simple reward-retrieval task in order to mimic natural contexts (i.e. foraging) and boost subjects' motivation. Moreover, we tested subjects' social information reliance in both the presence and absence of equally informative individual information, thereby allowing for investigation of information reliance in two different contexts. With regard to the intrinsic information preferences of humans and chimpanzees, we predicted that both species prefer individual over social information, because individual information is more accurate across a broad range of conditions [18]. In the light of the documented cultural gap [4,5], however, we additionally predicted that humans give more weight to social information than chimpanzees do.
2. Material and methods
We tested 23 German pre-school children (11 boys, 12 girls; Mage = 3.7 years, range = 3.0–4.6 years) at their kindergartens and 14 chimpanzees at the Wolfgang Kohler Primate Research Center in Leipzig, Germany (five males, nine females; Mage = 22.1 years, range = 7.0–36.3 years). Subjects were presented with a task in which they had to select the correct location out of three options in order to obtain a reward (which was hidden under one of three covers). Prior to this task, they had obtained conflicting information regarding the correct location through individual exploration and demonstrations by one conspecific (figure 1). Individual exploration comprised 10 successful trials (mean number of unsuccessful trials = 1.85). Demonstrations were given by one conspecific stooge (different individual for each subject; also see the electronic supplementary material) and similarly comprised 10 successful trials (mean number of unsuccessful trials = 1.28) to balance the individual and social learning phase in terms of informational value (see the electronic supplementary material). Subjects' information reliance was investigated after a 2 min and 24 h delay (henceforth ‘condition’), where the order of information acquisition was counterbalanced across and within subjects (table 1). This test phase comprised 10 trials per individual per condition, where each of the three covers was baited. Individuals' first responses were modelled to preclude the influence of positive reinforcement across trials (generalized linear mixed model, GLMM with binomial error structure and logit link function; see the electronic supplementary material). Subsequently, to enable inspection of means and errors, all 10 test trials were included in the analysis (both analyses yielded the same results; see the electronic supplementary material). First inspection focused on subjects' information reliance after being exposed to both individual and social information (figure 2a). Second, we investigated subjects' first location-choice in their individual exploration phase after they had been exposed to social information only, in order to find out to what extent subjects would use social information in the absence of individual knowledge (figure 2b).
Table 1.
procedure test 1 | procedure test 2 | children (n) | chimpanzees (n) |
---|---|---|---|
1. Individual information | 1. Social information | 6 | 4 |
2. Social information | 2. Individual information | ||
3. Preference test after 2 min | 3. Preference test after 24 h | ||
1. Social information | 1. Individual information | 5a | 3 |
2. Individual information | 2. Social information | ||
3. Preference test after 2 min | 3. Preference test after 24 h | ||
1. Individual information | 1. Social information | 6 | 4 |
2. Social information | 2. Individual information | ||
3. Preference test after 24 h | 3. Preference test after 2 min | ||
1. Social information | 1. Individual information | 6 | 3 |
2. Individual information | 2. Social information | ||
3. Preference test after 24 h | 3. Preference test after 2 min |
aOne dropout because of random stooge behaviour.
3. Results
(a). Information reliance after individual and social information acquisition
Both children and chimpanzees relied significantly more on individually obtained information than on socially obtained information and the third (non-experienced) alternative combined (intercept test, Wald estimate ± s.e. = 1.16 ± 0.38, p = 0.002 and 1.10 ± 0.46, p = 0.017, respectively; figure 2a). We detected a trend towards an interaction between the factors species and condition, when modelling social information reliance (null–full model comparison: χ2 = 6.88, d.f. = 3, p = 0.076; interaction test, model comparison: χ2 = 4.70, d.f. = 1, p = 0.029). Closer inspection revealed that the chimpanzees progressed from minimal social information reliance in the 2 min condition (mean ± s.d. = 5.0 ± 16.1%) to a substantial reliance on social information in the 24 h condition (mean ± s.d. = 28.6 ± 36.8%). Notably, this increase in social information reliance was due to a choice pattern in the 24 h condition not being different from a random response (Pearson's chi-squared test: χ2 = 2.00, p = 0.421). The children maintained a rather equal social information reliance across the two conditions (mean ± s.d. = 20.4 ± 36.7% and 17.0 ± 33.4%; figure 2a). Lastly, whereas the children preferred social information over the third, non-experienced alternative in the 2 min condition (one-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test: V = 45.5, p = 0.032), the chimpanzees chose the random option more than the socially demonstrated one (figure 2a; n.s.).
(b). Social information reliance when only social information was obtained
Throughout the information acquisition phases, half the time social learning was followed by individual learning (table 1). Strikingly, whereas children used the social information in their first subsequent individual exploration (binomial test for probability being different from 0.33: 21/23 subjects, p < 0.001), chimpanzees did not (5/13 subjects, p = 0.77). Accordingly, in the absence of individual knowledge, the children were more inclined to use social information than the chimpanzees were (Fisher exact test: p < 0.002, odds-ratio = 15.1; figure 2b).
4. Discussion
Children and chimpanzees relied more on their individual information than on equally accurate information presented to them by conspecifics, which is consistent with theoretical predictions on information usage in social animals [18,19]. The children, however, searched for the reward at the socially demonstrated location more than at a random location (in the 2 min condition), whereas the chimpanzees never did. Moreover, only the children used social information to guide their subsequent individual exploration, which suggests that children considered social information more readily than chimpanzees.
Finding that children rely more on social information than chimpanzees do may provide a novel addition to explanations for the cultural gap between humans and chimpanzees [4,5]; i.e. humans' heightened inclination to assimilate observed behaviours could facilitate the emergence of within-group homogeneity, which is the hallmark of culture [1]. Note that the difference in social information reliance between children and chimpanzees was especially pronounced when subjects had not yet obtained individual information. Learning models predict that animals would incorporate social information when they are ‘uncertain’ [19], but based on our study, it could be hypothesized that where humans rely on their conspecifics, chimpanzees may prefer to overcome their uncertainty through additional individual exploration (also see [20]).
Importantly, the reward-retrieval task employed did not require sophisticated capacities to be solved; mechanisms such as local and stimulus enhancement could have allowed the subjects to learn socially. As such, the current comparison between children and chimpanzees seems to expose motivational rather than cognitive differences. Accordingly, in line with our postulated hypothesis and a recent study showing that chimpanzees recognize but refrain from imitating successful actions [21], it might be that chimpanzees do not lack the capacity to understand the relevance of social information, but are less motivated to use it than humans are (also see [22]).
Consistent with theoretical predictions [18], we conclude that even culturally rich species such as humans and chimpanzees may prefer individual information over information obtained from conspecifics. The children's responses paralleled adults' reliance on individual information in the presence of valuable social information across choice contexts [23–26], rather than children's over-imitation tendencies reported in studies on imitation of motor patterns [20,27]. Minimally, this finding highlights the fact that children do not favour social over individual information indiscriminately, which provides a new impetus for future research. Nevertheless, in the absence of individual information, children seemed substantially more inclined to rely on social information than chimpanzees. Given that our study used a simple task in which humans' and chimpanzees' social information reliance were compared directly, our findings provide a novel and empirically grounded perspective on the striking difference between human and chimpanzee culture.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Sandy Kennert, Ronja Buechner, Felix Haiduk and the WKPRC staff for their invaluable assistance, Katherine Cronin for helpful discussions, Roger Mundry for statistical advice and three anonymous reviewers for constructive comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.
Ethics statement
The study was approved by the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (Leipzig).
Data accessibility
Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.q2hn2.
Funding statement
This research was supported by the Max Planck Institute.
References
- 1.Richerson PJ, Boyd R. 2005. Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Luncz LV, Mundry R, Boesch C. 2012. Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Curr. Biol. 22, 922–926. ( 10.1016/J.Cub.2012.03.031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.van Leeuwen EJC, Cronin KAC, Haun DBM, Mundry R, Bodamer MD. 2012. Neighbouring chimpanzee communities show different preferences in social grooming behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 4362–4367. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.1543) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mesoudi A. 2011. Cultural evolution: how Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. London, UK: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Whiten A, van Schaik CP. 2007. The evolution of animal 'cultures’ and social intelligence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 603–620. ( 10.1098/rstb.2006.1998) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hrubesch C, Preuschoft S, van Schaik C. 2009. Skill mastery inhibits adoption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim. Cogn. 12, 209–216. ( 10.1007/s10071-008-0183-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Marshall-Pescini S, Whiten A. 2008. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach. Anim. Cogn. 11, 449–456. ( 10.1007/s10071-007-0135-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dean LG, Kendal RL, Schapiro SJ, Thierry B, Laland KN. 2012. Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 335, 1114–1118. ( 10.1126/Science.1213969) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.van Leeuwen EJC, Cronin KA, Schütte S, Call J, Haun DBM. 2013. Chimpanzees flexibly adjust their behaviour in order to maximize payoffs, not to conform to majorities. PLoS ONE 8, e80945 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0080945) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yamamoto S, Humle T, Tanaka M. 2013. Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique. PLoS ONE 8, e55768 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0055768) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Galef BG. 1992. The question of animal culture. Hum. Nat. 3, 157–178. ( 10.1007/BF02692251) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tomasello M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hill K. 2010. Experimental studies of animal social learning in the wild: trying to untangle the mystery of human culture. Learn. Behav. 38, 319–328. ( 10.3758/LB.38.3.319) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M. 2009. Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 2405–2415. ( 10.1098/rstb.2009.0052) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Caldwell CA, Millen AE. 2009. Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: is imitation necessary? Psychol. Sci. 20, 1478–1483. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02469.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Heyes CM. 1993. Imitation, culture and cognition. Anim. Behav. 46, 999–1010. ( 10.1006/anbe.1993.1281) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Whiten A, Horner V, Marshall-Pescini S. 2003. Cultural panthropology. Evol. Anthropol. 12, 92–105. ( 10.1002/evan.10107) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Boyd R, Richerson P. 1985. Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago press. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Laland KN. 2004. Social learning strategies. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14. ( 10.3758/BF03196002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Horner V, Whiten A. 2005. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim. Cogn. 8, 164–181. ( 10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M. 2013. Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, recognize successful actions, but fail to imitate them. Anim. Behav. 86, 755–761. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Over H, Carpenter M. 2011. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 182–192. ( 10.1037/a0024555) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Efferson C, Lalive R, Richerson PJ, McElreath R, Lubell M. 2008. Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 56–64. ( 10.1016/J.Evolhumbehav.2007.08.003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Eriksson K, Strimling P. 2009. Biases for acquiring information individually rather than socially. J. Evol. Psychol. 7, 309–329. ( 10.1556/JEP.7.2009.4.4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mesoudi A. 2011. An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 334–342. ( 10.1016/J.Evolhumbehav.2010.12.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Morgan TJH, Rendell L, Ehn W, Hoppitt W, Laland K. 2011. The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 653–662. ( 10.1098/rspb.2011.1172) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC. 2007. The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 50, 19 751–19 756. (doi:10.1073_pnas.0704452104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.q2hn2.