Abstract
Graph analysis is a promising tool to quantify brain connectivity. However, an essential requirement is that the graph measures are reproducible and robust. We have studied the reproducibility and robustness of various graph measures in group based and in individual binary and weighted networks derived from a task fMRI experiment during explicit associative-semantic processing of words and pictures. The nodes of the network were defined using an independent study and the connectivity was based on the partial correlation of the time series between any pair of nodes. The results showed that in case of binary networks, global graph measures exhibit a good reproducibility and robustness for networks which are not too sparse and these figures of merit depend on the graph measure and on the density of the network. Furthermore, group based binary networks should be derived from groups of sufficient size and the lower the density the more subjects are required to obtain robust values. Local graph measures are very variable in terms of reproducibility and should be interpreted with care. For weighted networks, we found good reproducibility (average test-retest variability <5% and ICC values >0.4) when using subject specific networks and this will allow us to relate network properties to individual subject information.
Introduction
There is a surge of interest in mapping and modelling the complicated networks within the brain. Functional connectivity analyses of neuroimaging data are based on the concept of synchrony between the signal responses in spatially distinct brain regions [1]. Analysing networks extracted from functional imaging data has proven to be a promising tool to investigate the complex functional structure of the human brain that influences the dynamics underlying cognition [2]–[4].
A promising tool to rigorously study the problem is graph analysis. This provides a framework to characterize and to quantify networks [5]–[9]. Many non-trivial graph characteristics, such as small-worldness, modularity and highly connected hubs, have been observed in human brain networks. Differences in graph properties have been found in people with Alzheimer's disease [10] and schizophrenia [6], [11], [12], and also in association with age [7], [13]. Changes in graph measures were also found during motor learning [14] and taking nicotine [15]. All these studies suggest that graph analysis can be a promising tool in clinical and basic research to characterize brain connectivity in a way that is both biologically meaningful and related to normal and abnormal function. However, an essential requirement when using this type of quantification is that the different measures are reproducible and robust.
The reproducibility of graph measures has already been investigated in a number of studies looking at binarized networks derived from structural MRI [16], [17], diffusion-weighted MRI [18]–[21], resting state fMRI [10], [22], [23], MEG [24] and resting-state functional near infrared spectroscopy [25]. Only one study has looked at the reproducibility of graph measures when using task fMRI [23]. Graph measures using task fMRI is expected to be different compared to resting state fMRI since functional connectivity between two nodes depend on the context (i.e. resting state versus a specific task context). Furthermore, the reproducibility of weighted graph measures has received very little attention: only two studies are available which addressed this problem and both were using graphs derived from diffusion-weighted MRI [26], [27]. Therefore, we investigated the reproducibility and the robustness of graph measures of weighted and binarized networks derived from a task fMRI during explicit associative-semantic processing of words and pictures. This task activates a distributed set of brain areas that has been replicated across a wide range of studies [28]–[38]. Previously, we have applied graph analysis to examine the structure of this network [39].
Materials and Methods
Participants
A group of 54 healthy elderly participants (age (mean std): 65.2 5.6 yrs; 31 male) [40] performed an associative-semantic judgement task. Twenty-eight subjects were scanned on a 3T Philips Intera system equipped with an 8-channel receive-only head coil (Philips SENSitivity Encoding head coil). Twenty-six subjects could not undergo the fMRI in the Intera system due to space limitation in the scanner lumen in combination with the screen. These subjects were scanned on a 3T Philips Achieva system equipped with a 32-channel receive-only head coil (Philips 10 SENSitivity Encoding head coil) which used a screen placed behind the individual's head for the projection.
The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee University Hospitals Leuven (EudraCT: 2009-014475-45) and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Experimental design
Stimuli were projected onto a screen (resolution of 1024×768 pixels, refresh rate 60 Hz) using Presentation 14.8 (NeuroBehavioural Systems, Albany, CA, USA). The design of the fMRI experiment was factorial [28]–[31]. The first factor, task, had two levels: associative-semantic versus visuoperceptual judgement. The second factor, input modality, also had two levels: pictures versus printed words. During a trial of the associative-semantic condition, a triplet of stimuli was presented for 5250 ms, one stimulus on top (the sample stimulus) and one in each lower quadrant (the test stimuli), at 4.6° eccentricity, followed by a 1500 ms interstimulus interval. Subjects had to press a left- or right-hand key depending on which of the two test stimuli matched the sample stimulus more closely in meaning. A given triplet was presented either as pictures or as words and this was counterbalanced across subjects. In the visuoperceptual control condition, a picture or word stimulus was presented in three different sizes (mean picture size was 3.7° and mean letter size 1.2°). Subjects had to press a left- or right-hand key depending on which of the two test stimuli matched the sample stimulus more closely in size on the screen. An epoch, i.e. a block of trials belonging to the same condition, consisted of four trials (total duration 27 s). The fifth condition consisted of a resting baseline condition during which a fixation point was presented in the centre of the screen. During each fMRI run (5 runs in total), a series of the 5 epoch types, was replicated 3 times. The order of conditions was pseudorandom and differed across runs of the same subject. Subjects received a practice session before entering the scanner. In this session we determined which size difference (9%, 6%, 3%, or 1%) for the visuoperceptual conditions was needed for each individual subject to obtain comparable accuracies as for the associative-semantic conditions.
Preprocessing of the data
Image analysis was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK. http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Functional images of each subject were realigned to correct for small head motion during each run. The anatomical -weighted image was coregistered to the average of the realigned functional volumes and non-linearly normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using the unified segmentation approach [41] and the resulting transformation was used to spatially normalize the functional images. The voxel size of the images in MNI space was 3×3×3 mm3. Images were smoothed using a 6×6×6 mm3 Full Width at Half Maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. We also applied a temporal high-pass filter (cutoff 270s) and a low-pass filter consisting of the canonical hemodynamic response function. The epoch-related response was modelled by a canonical hemodynamic response function convolved with a boxcar.
Network construction
Volumes of interests (VOI) were taken from a previously published study on the associative-semantic network [39], namely fifty-seven spheres (radius 6 mm) located at least 20 mm apart. The spheres were centred on group-specific activation maxima (from the main effect of task) determined from this previous study. Note that the position of the VOIs was identical as in the previous study, i.e. the functional information in the current study was not used to position the VOIs. We have previously shown that the nodes of the associative semantic network have a low anatomical inter-subject variability [42].
For each subject, we applied each of these VOIs to the current dataset and we extracted the time series after whitening, filtering and removing effects of no interest (session specific effects) using code from statistical parametric mapping software (SPM8; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Finally, the average time series in the VOI was calculated as the mean of the time series over all voxels in the VOI. Time series of different runs were concatenated. It is important to note that we used the whole time series, i.e. it includes all the different conditions as well as the null condition.
Based on the average time series, partial correlation coefficients between volumes of interest were calculated. Partial correlation was used to obtain the degree of association between regions, with the effect of other regions removed [43], [44]. Among the methods evaluating functional interdependencies between functional MRI time courses in different regions, partial correlations have a high sensitivity to network connection detection [44]. An association matrix was defined in which each element represents the association strength between two regions. The association strength is defined as the absolute value of the z-score which is calculated from the partial correlation using the Fisher r-to-z transform [45]:
(1) |
in which is the partial correlation, the number of data points in the time series and the number of nodes.
The great majority of network analysis in systems neuroscience relies on the construction of undirected and unweighted graphs through thresholding of the association matrix [3], [4]. The thresholded association matrix represents a binary adjacency matrix with 1 indicating the presence and 0 indicating the absence of an edge (connection) between two vertices (nodes/regions). A possible approach to define the threshold is to fix the network's edge density (also referred to as wiring cost), i.e. the number of existing edges divided by the number of possible edges. In order to investigate changes in the network topology as a function of network density, we thresholded the association matrix at network densities ranging from 5% to 45%, in steps of 5%. Densities below 5% are considered too sparse and densities above 50% are less likely to be biological [46], [47]. Furthermore, we also included a density of 7.8% corresponding to the density of the associative-semantic network in the previously published study [39].
The binarization of connections has one major drawback: it enhances scale contrast by underrating (overrating) connections because connections around the threshold may vary considerably between subjects. To avoid this problem, weighted graph analysis [11], [48]–[51], which preserves all the edge information, is also used. To obtain weights with , we applied a nonlinear mapping of z score to weight:
(2) |
where is the cumulative distribution function of the standard normal distribution.
Graph-theoretical analysis
Local and global graph measures were calculated for the binary network (at different densities) as well as for the weighted network using the brain connectivity toolbox version 2013_12_25 (https://sites.google.com/a/brain-connectivity-toolbox.net/bct/Home; [4]). We calculated the following local graph measures for node : node degree , average path length , local clustering coefficient , local efficiency , efficiency and betweenness centrality . Global measures included characteristic path length , mean clustering coefficient , mean local efficiency , global efficiency and mean betweenness centrality . For the definition of these network measures, we refer to [4]. It should be noted that isolated nodes can be present when the density of the network is very low. In that case, these nodes were not taking into account when calculating the network measures.
The network itself was either defined at the individual level (for every subject separately) or at the group level (after averaging the association matrices across the subjects belonging to that group).
Reproducibility at the individual level
To look at the intra-subject reproducibility, two groups were constructed by evenly splitting each subject's time series into two parts by randomly assigning four of the five runs to one of the two even parts. This corresponds to the situation in which each subject is measured twice under the same conditions. In this way we constructed two groups of paired subjects. We refer to this situation as the split-half case. A partial correlation based network and corresponding graph measures were obtained for each subject in each group across a range of densities or using the weights of the network.
The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was used to analyse reproducibility of the network [52]. More specifically, values were merged into a 2×54 matrix (number of measurements x subjects). The total variance was split into the between-subject (BMS) and the residual (EMS) variance. ICC values were calculated according to the equation [53]:
(3) |
By randomly switching (100 random realizations) time series parts between the two measurements, we can calculate the mean and variance of the ICC for each graph measure. ICC is usually considered as a cut-off for a fairly reliable measure [54].
We also calculated the test-retest value between the two measurements of the same subject and averaged this over all subjects to obtain the average test-retest value. The test-retest was calculated as:
(4) |
where denotes the absolute value and and are the values of the graph measure under investigation obtained in the first and second measurement respectively.
Reproducibility for group-based graph measures
Averaging the association matrices across subjects summarizes the overall characteristics of the group [55], [56]. To study the reproducibility for group-based graph measures, we looked at the test-retest calculated as equation 4 except that and represent the measures obtained for the first and second group. Mean and variance of each graph measure were calculated based upon the 100 random realizations of switching time series parts.
However, a more challenging situation occurs when we want to compare two independent groups which are scanned on MR scanners with different field strength and which differ slightly in the fMRI paradigm. This is the case when we want to compare the results of the current group with the results of our previous study [39]. In that study we used the same paradigm (except that we did not include a null condition in our measurement). Furthermore, the data of this group () were acquired on a 1.5 T Siemens Sonata. Age (67.2 8.5 years) and gender (19 M/14 F) were similar to the current study. We refer in the remaining of the paper to the comparison of these two studies as the between-independent groups case. The test-retest values were calculated according to equation 4.
Hubs and community structure
We also assessed the reproducibility of the community structure and the identification of hubs for binary and weighted networks at the individual and the group level.
The identification of hubs was based on a hub score [2], [11], [14], [39], [57]–[59], which is the sum of dummy values for four criteria. We gave a score of 1 or 0 depending on whether or not the node belongs to the top 20% of nodes with 1) the highest node degree, 2) the highest betweenness centrality, 3) the lowest local cluster coefficient (limited to nodes with a degree ), and 4) the lowest average path length. Nodes with a hub score were considered hubs.
We evaluated the consistency of hubs by measuring the co-occurrence of hubs () across networks. If is a list of hubs in network A and in network B, the co-occurrence is calculated as
(5) |
where denotes the cardinality of the set. A value of 1 corresponds to a perfect agreement of hubs while 0 reflects no agreement at all.
To determine the community structure of the network, we used the algorithm of Newman [60] as implemented in the Brain Connectivity Toolbox. The algorithm starts from a random order of the nodes which lead to a slightly different community structure each time the algorithm is applied. To generate a consensus assignment of nodes to communities, we used a two step procedure [61]. In the first step, we computed a co-assignment matrix represented as an matrix, where cell was equal to 1 if node i and node j were assigned to the same community. During the second step, a probabilistic co-assignment matrix is then obtained by averaging 100 realizations of co-assignment matrices.
The consistency of the community structure of the network was assessed by probabilistic scaled inclusivity (pSI), a metric quantifying the consistency of communities across multiple networks and which is an extension of the scaled inclusivity SI [62], [63]. SI is calculated by measuring the overlap of modules across multiple networks while penalizing for disjunction of modules. For example, a node is part of module in network A and module in network B. Then SI for node , denoted as , is calculated as
(6) |
where and denote sets of nodes in modules and and denotes the cardinality of a set. If the two modules and consist of an identical set of nodes, then . The SI value between two networks is a value between 0 and 1; if at a particular node, it means that the node is in the same module with exactly the same set of nodes in the two networks. A problem with this way of calculating the SI value is that it requires a final assignment of nodes to a community. This can be done based upon the probabilistic co-assignment matrix but may lead to different results depending on the algorithm to assign the final community to each node. An alternative is to use the probabilistic co-assignment matrix directly to calculate a probabilistic SI value. This is done as follows:
(7) |
in which and are the probability co-assignment between nodes and in network and network .
To further characterize the consistent parts of the communities, we averaged the co-assignment matrices for the two groups in the between-independent groups case or across every possible pair in the split-half case.
There's no established rule to define which and are sufficiently high to ensure consistency between networks but we can determine if it is significantly different from the value obtained under random conditions by comparing it to the distribution of values obtained from null networks, i.e. networks with the same number of nodes and connections which were generated by randomly (1000 realizations) rewiring the observed network [64]. The weighted null network is obtained by randomly rewiring connections with the same distribution of weights.
Robustness of the data
Group size effect
We applied a bootstrapping procedure (100 realizations) to calculate graph measures as a function of group size. More specifically, we created random subsamples from our 54 subjects, each time creating a subgroup with a certain number of subjects, and we repeated this for subgroup sizes ranging from 10 to 53. Results were calculated as the relative change (in %) taking the values of the complete group of 54 subjects as the reference.
Network robustness
When we identify the nodes of the network, we may not have captured all nodes. The question then arises, in how far is this affecting the quantification of the network. To address this question, we assume that the 57 nodes represent all nodes of the underlying network and we removed nodes from this network to investigate the impact when nodes were not captured. The procedure is similar to network robustness analysis against random failures and targeted attacks [5], [17], [65] although the interpretation is clearly different. Since it is more likely to miss the least significant nodes, we conducted our analysis by removing nodes based on their significance in the main effect of task in the fMRI study starting by removing the least significant ones. The degree of tolerance will be expressed as the relative change of the graph measures compared to values of the network with all 57 nodes.
Statistics
To evaluate if ICC values were significantly higher than 0.4, we performed a one-sample t-test. The same test was used to evaluate if the test-retest variability was 5% or 10%. The comparison between subject-specific graph measures obtained in two independent groups was assessed by a two-sample t-test. To test the relation between test-retest variability of global graph measures and density, we first log-transformed the test-retest values and performed a linear regression.
The statistical threshold to reach significance was set to . We corrected for the number of densities under investigation in case of global graph measures and additionally for the number of nodes in case of local graph measures.
Results
In table 1 a summary of the main findings is given.
Table 1. Summary of the main findings.
Subject-specific networks |
Binary networks |
• Global efficiency, characteristic path length and mean betweenness centrality are reproducible only when the network density is high. |
• The intra subject split-half test-retest values of global graph measures decreases with the increase of density. |
Weighted networks |
• Global graph measures are reproducible for all the measures investigated. |
• The intra subject split-half test-retest values of global graph measures were very low. |
• The test-retest values of the mean of the global graph measures derived from subject-specific weighted networks for two independent groups varied between 7 and 17%. |
• Communities are consistent for both intra-subject (in the split-half case) and inter-subject comparisons. |
• The average global graph measures are not critically depending on the group size. |
• The average global graph measures show robustness against missing nodes. |
Group-based networks |
Binary and weighted networks |
• In the split-half case, all graph measures show test-retest variability |
• Hubs show a significant high consistency in the split-half case compared to the values obtained from random null networks. |
• Communities show consistency for both the split-half case and when comparing independent groups |
• A sufficiently large group size is required to obtain reliable results. |
• Global efficiency and characteristic path length are more robust for the group size. |
• Global efficiency, characteristic path length, mean local efficiency and clustering coefficient are more robust against missing nodes compared to the mean betweenness centrality |
Binary networks |
• Test-retest variability of global graph measures decreases as the network becomes more dense. |
Weighted networks |
• The global efficiency and the characteristic path length have the smallest overall test-retest variability. |
Reproducibility at the individual level (subject-specific networks)
Averaged global ICC across all randomizations are shown in Fig. 1 for binary (over a range of densities) and weighted networks. For binary networks, global efficiency , characteristic path length and mean betweenness centrality show significant () reproducibility () when the network density is high (40%). This is not the case for the mean cluster coefficient and the mean local efficiency. Weighted global graph measures show significant () reproducibility () for all the measures investigated (Fig. 1).
As can be expected, reproducibility at the nodal level exhibits heterogeneity across graph measures and nodes (see S1 Table). Efficiency and average path length are the most reproducible nodal graph measures (although only in 2 nodes a significant () ICC value 0.4 was found) in contrast to the betweenness centrality which is the least reproducible one. In case of weighted local graph measures, we observed the folowing range of averaged (across subjects) ICC values: ; ; ; and .
The intra subject split-half test-retest values (TRT) for global graph measures are shown for binary and weighted networks (Fig. 2). The test-retest values of global graph measures decreases with the increase of density in case of binary networks (for all global graph measures under investigation ). For weighted networks, the test-retest values were excellent ( 5%): :1.12% ();: 1.11% (); : 4.09% (); : 1.93% () and : 2.40% ().
In table 2, we show the comparison of the global graph measures derived from subject-specific weighted networks for the two independent groups. The values in the current group are significantly different from those obtained in our previous study [39] and the test-retest values of the mean of the global graph measures derived from subject-specific weighted networks for two independent groups varied between 7 and 17% (table 2).
Table 2. Comparison of global graph measures derived from subject-specific weighted networks between two independent groups.
Current study mean std | Previous study [39] mean std | TRT of the mean % | |
mean clustering coefficient | 0.588 0.020 | 0.520 0.022 | 12 |
characteristic path length | 1.444 0.028 | 1.544 0.034 | 7 |
global efficiency | 0.739 0.014 | 0.690 0.016 | 7 |
mean local efficiency | 0.629 0.018 | 0.566 0.020 | 11 |
mean betweenness centrality | 0.0057 0.0003 | 0.0067 0.0004 | 17 |
TRT: test-retest variability.
Reproducibility for group-based graph measures
Test-retest variability for the different global graph measures are shown for the split-half case (Fig. 3) and the between-independent groups case (Fig. 4) for binary and weighted networks.
In the split-half case (Fig. 3), all graph measures show a significantly () small test-retest variability () for binary (with densities ) and weighted networks. The global graph measures showing the smallest overall test-retest variability are the global efficiency and the characteristic path length. For binary networks, test-retest variability of global graph measures decreases as the network becomes more dense (for all global graph measures under investigation: ). When looking at the local graph measures (table 3), we observe that the efficiency and average path length in the majority of nodes show a significantly ( 0.05) small test-retest variability () for both binary (with densities ) and weighted networks. On the other hand, betweenness centrality has the largest test-retest variability.
Table 3. The split-half case: the test-retest variability of local graph measures across densities.
% of nodes with a significant ( 0.05) test-retest variability | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
node degree | 28 | 9 | 7 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 2 | 0 | 0 | 98 |
cluster coefficient | 9 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 4 | 12 | 37 | 75 | 98 |
average path length | 0 | 53 | 91 | 95 | 98 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 |
efficiency | 23 | 65 | 89 | 95 | 96 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 | 100 |
local efficiency | 7 | 2 | 2 | 2 | 21 | 46 | 79 | 95 | 100 | 100 | 98 |
betweenness centrality | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
median test-retest variability (%) | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
node degree | 15 | 18 | 18 | 16 | 16 | 15 | 14 | 13 | 12 | 11 | 5 |
cluster coefficient | 56 | 41 | 31 | 24 | 20 | 16 | 13 | 10 | 9 | 7 | 6 |
average path length | 15 | 8 | 6 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 |
efficiency | 11 | 7 | 6 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 | 3 |
local efficiency | 58 | 46 | 34 | 23 | 14 | 8 | 5 | 4 | 3 | 2 | 6 |
betweenness centrality | 90 | 69 | 58 | 52 | 49 | 44 | 40 | 37 | 33 | 29 | 42 |
In the between-independent groups situation, there was a trend that the test-retest variability of global graph measures in case of binary networks (Fig. 4) decreases as the network becomes more dense (for all global graph measures under investigation: uncorrected ). Furthermore, the test-retest variability for graph measures of weighted networks in this situation are mostly above . When looking at the local graph measures (table 4), we observe that the efficiency and average path length in the majority of nodes show small test-retest variability () for binary networks with densities .
Table 4. Between independent groups: the test-retest variability of local graph measures across densities.
% of nodes with a test-retest variability | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
node degree | 26 | 18 | 25 | 19 | 16 | 16 | 23 | 21 | 26 | 35 | 25 |
cluster coefficient | 7 | 7 | 12 | 14 | 19 | 19 | 32 | 53 | 53 | 75 | 26 |
average path length | 23 | 47 | 46 | 60 | 65 | 72 | 79 | 84 | 81 | 75 | 30 |
efficiency | 30 | 44 | 53 | 60 | 65 | 68 | 72 | 77 | 72 | 75 | 35 |
local efficiency | 4 | 7 | 16 | 26 | 40 | 53 | 70 | 88 | 98 | 100 | 30 |
betweenness centrality | 7 | 5 | 2 | 4 | 7 | 7 | 11 | 12 | 16 | 16 | 14 |
median test-retest variability (%) | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
node degree | 40 | 40 | 29 | 25 | 27 | 29 | 24 | 20 | 18 | 16 | 18 |
cluster coefficient | 129 | 73 | 57 | 26 | 26 | 20 | 13 | 9 | 9 | 6 | 19 |
average path length | 18 | 11 | 11 | 8 | 7 | 7 | 5 | 4 | 4 | 5 | 16 |
efficiency | 17 | 11 | 9 | 8 | 8 | 6 | 6 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 15 |
local efficiency | 133 | 79 | 53 | 22 | 17 | 8 | 5 | 3 | 3 | 2 | 18 |
betweenness centrality | 116 | 118 | 105 | 91 | 60 | 62 | 58 | 52 | 44 | 37 | 67 |
Hubs and communities
We analysed the hubs and community structure for binary and weighted networks at the individual and group-averaged level.
At the group-averaged level, table 5 shows the co-occurrence of hubs in the split-half case and in the comparison between independent groups. For binary and weighted networks, hubs show a significant () high consistency in the split-half case compared to the values obtained from random null networks. However, when comparing two independent groups the consistency of the hubs is clearly reduced.
Table 5. Co-occurrence of hubs .
Split-half case | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
Hc | 0.62 | 0.70 | 0.78 | 0.75 | 0.73 | 0.70 | 0.68 | 0.66 | 0.64 | 0.62 | 0.79 |
Between-independent groups | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
Hc | 0.43 | 0.32 | 0.52 | 0.31 | 0.31 | 0.30 | 0.41 | 0.30 | 0.34 | 0.40 | 0.43 |
Bold: Values which are significantly () different from the value obtained from null networks (see text). Italic: .
The results for the consistency of the community structure are given in table 6 as the mean pSI for both binary and weighted networks. Communities show consistency for both the split-half case and when comparing independent groups, i.e. the pSI values are significantly different from those obtained from random null networks. When we look at groups of nodes which are consistently assigned (average value in the co-assignment matrix 0.95) to the same module between the two independent groups at the density , we find the following groups: 1) nodes in the dorsomedial prefrontal gyrus, the left superior frontal gyrus, the left supplementary motor area, the left anterior and the right cingulate gyrus; 2) nodes in the left inferior frontal gyrus pars orbitalis, the left posterior middle temporal gyrus, the left anterior and posterior superior temporal sulcus and the left supramarginal gyrus; 3) nodes in the left lingual gyrus, the left intraparietal sulcus and the left middle occipital gyrus; 4) nodes in the left superior frontal gyrus and the left medial frontal; 5) nodes in the right caudate, the left anterior thalamus and posterior thalamus; 6) nodes in the right inferior and middle occipital gyrus.
Table 6. Mean pSI.
Split-half case | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
0.31 | 0.35 | 0.40 | 0.42 | 0.42 | 0.47 | 0.50 | 0.56 | 0.54 | 0.49 | 0.50 | |
Between-independent groups | |||||||||||
density% | 5 | 7.8 | 10 | 15 | 20 | 25 | 30 | 35 | 40 | 45 | w |
0.14 | 0.13 | 0.15 | 0.18 | 0.14 | 0.29 | 0.23 | 0.35 | 0.33 | 0.37 | 0.44 |
Bold: Values which are significantly () different from the value obtained from null networks (see text). Italic: .
At the individual level for weigthed networks in the split-half case, the mean intra-subject co-occurrence of hubs across all 54 subjects and 100 randomization is () while the mean inter-subject across all pairs of subjects is (). The mean intra-subject consistency of the community structure across all 54 subjects and 100 randomization is () while the mean inter-subject across all pairs of subjects is ().
At the individual level for weigthed networks in the comparison of two independent groups, the mean inter-subject across all pairs of subjects was for the current study and this was not significantly different form obtained from the data of the previous study [39]. However, the mean inter-subject across all pairs of subjects was significantly () different: for the current study versus for the previous study [39].
Group size effect
For group based networks, the relative change of global graph measures as a function of group size are shown in Fig. 5 for networks with a density of 5%, 20%, 45% as well as for the weighted network. Overall, the relative difference in graph measures when taken the complete group (54 subjects) as the reference, decreases when the number of participants and/or the density increase. Furthermore, some binary graph measures are more robust for the group size: the absolute value of the relative error of global efficiency and characteristic path length are significantly () smaller than 10% even for smaller group sizes ( subjects) for all densities . The mean betweenness centrality, the mean local efficiency and the mean clustering coefficient show a similar behaviour but for minimum group sizes of respectively 17, 38 and 44 subjects for a binary network at a density of . At a density of , the minimum group size to obtain relative errors significantly () smaller than 10% in absolute value, could only be determined for , , and and this size was respectively 42, 44, 44 and 49 subjects. For the weighted network, the minimum group size is 13, 14, 19, 20 and 23 for , , , and respectively.
At the individual level for weigthed networks, all the average global graph measures showed a small relative error ( in absolute value, ) as a result of the smaller group size even for groups as small as 10 subjects (Fig. 6).
Network robustness
For group based networks, the robustness of graph measures in case we missed the least significant nodes is shown in Fig. 7 for binary (with an initial density of 5%, 20%, 45%) and weighted networks. The robustness depends on the network measure under investigation. Global efficiency, characteristic path length, mean local efficiency and clustering coefficient are more robust compared to the mean betweenness centrality.
At the individual level for weigthed networks, the robustness to missing nodes remains within 10% error () up to removal of the 8 least significant nodes (Fig. 8).
Discussion
In this work, reproducibility and robustness of the functional connectivity network associated with an associative-semantic task was examined by studying local and global graph measures, hubs and the community structure. The nodes of the associative-semantic network were taken from a previous study [39]. The paradigm that we used gives a highly consistent activation pattern using univariate analyses and this is replicated in our and other centers [28]–[38]. Furthermore, we have shown previously that the nodes have a low anatomical inter-subject variability [42]. This consistency and reproducibility at the nodal level is essential when looking at the network measures.
Choice of connectivity measure
A network is dependent on the choice of the measure of connectivity between different brain regions. Many groups investigating functional networks in the human brain use the Pearson correlation coefficient [8], [50], [66]. Other similarity measures have also been used, including the correlation between wavelet components [5], [10], [67] and mutual information [44]. However, constructing a network by correlation or mutual information does not necessarily imply that the functional connection between two nodes is direct. The distinction between direct and indirect functional relationships between areas is very important in terms of correctly estimating the network. Hence, partial correlation became a hot topic in recent years (e.g.[43], [68], [69]). Partial correlation provides a convenient summary of conditional independence and turns out to be an effective way to model the connectivity [44]. In our work, partial correlations are used to remove mutual dependencies on common influences from other brain areas. By conditioning the dependencies between two nodes on other nodes, the functional connectivity (i.e. partial correlation) reflects a quantity that is more closely related to direct interaction, taking the analysis of functional connectivity closer to the characterization of functional interactions in terms of effective connectivity. It is data-driven in the sense that, unlike existing methods such as structural equation modelling (SEM) and dynamic causal modelling (DCM), it does not require any prior information regarding functional interactions.
Split-half variability versus comparison of two independent groups
In this work, we have studied two situations which reflect two complete different situations when looking at the variability of the measurement. The first situation is the one in which we have split the timeseries in two even parts to assess the split-half variability. This corresponds to a situation in which subjects are measured twice under almost similar conditions (i.e. exactly the same scanner, the same sequence, the same paradigm) within one session and assuming no time effects. The limitation of this approach is that we have violated the temporal order of the runs and that the number of runs in the newly composed parts is small. The other situation corresponds to the measurement of two independent groups on different scanners using a slightly different paradigm. As can be expected, the test-retest variability in the latter case is larger than the split-half variability in case of weighted networks and for most densities. The test-retest values of the mean of the global graph measures derived from subject-specific weighted networks for two independent groups varied between 7 and 17%. However, the values were significantly different between both groups most likely due to the inclusion of a null condition in the current study.
Reproducibility
For binary group based networks, we observed that the reproducibility improves when the density of the network increases. Networks with high density, weighted group based networks and weigthed individual networks show all a very good reprodubility for the global graph measures. Only when we compared two independent groups, the weighted group based network showed a weaker reproducibility. Networks with low density (e.g. 5%), showed weak reproducibility and this was depending on the graph measure itself.
Local graph measures showed weak reproducibility in almost all situations for most nodes and therefore quantification of local graph properties needs to be interpreted with care.
Hubs and community structure
A node playing a pivotal role in the flow of information is called a hub but the operational definition of a hub differs between studies. In the current study, we have taken a similar approach as [11]. We observe a high co-occurrence in the split-half case but a low co-occurrence for the comparison between independent groups. We also observe that the co-occurrence is relatively stable over the different densities and this is also the case for the weighted graph measures. The average inter-subject and intra-subject co-occurrence of hubs in case of individual weighted networks is somewhat lower and this is probably due to the higher variability which one can expect in individual networks compared to group based networks.
The community structure represents how nodes are separated into interacting (integrated) but distinct (segregated) functional modules. A major challenge in examining network module organization is the reproducibility of modules and how to measure this reproducibility. Several studies compared modularity Q and number of communities to achieve this goal [49], [50], [70]. However, the value of Q only gives a sense of the network strength in dividing itself into modules. One could easily have two networks which may be considerably different, yet sharing the same number of communities and similar Q values. A more appropriate measure is to use scaled inclusivity which is a measure for the overlap of modules across networks while penalizing for disjunction of modules [62], [63]. The calculation requires a final assignment of nodes to a community. In this work, we have extended this formula so that it is now directly based on the probabilistic co-assignment matrix without the need to assign each node to a community. In case the co-assignment matrix is binarized (which is similar to assigning each node to a community) it reduces to the original formula. We found that the reproducibility of the community structure of weighted group based networks was similar or even better compared to the values for dense binarized networks. When looking at the average intra- and intersubject probabilistic scaled inclusivity, we observed smaller values most likely again due to the higher variability which you can expect in individual networks compared to group based networks.
Group size effect
An important issue relates to the number of subjects required to obtain robust graph measures. In a recent study [71], it was emphasized that a small sample size undermines the reproducibility of neuroscience. We found that in low density networks, a large number of subjects is required to obtain robust values and this depends on the graph measure under investigation (global efficiency and characteristic path being the most stable measures). The use of weighted graph measures leads to robust values. This is also true for the averaged graph measures in case of individual weighted networks.
Robustness against missing nodes
If we have not captured all nodes of the network, the question is in how far graph measures will be influenced. Indeed, some nodes are only weakly activitated in an fMRI experiment and the choice of selection of inclusion of nodes can be based on a statistical criterion. We found that all graph measures, except the mean betweenness centrality, are robust even if we didn't include several of these weaker nodes.
Binary versus weighted networks
The popularity of binary network analysis may arise from the fact that in most cases it is simpler to characterize[4]. In our previous study [39], we have binarized the network based on a statistical criterion (significant association values). If we would have applied the same criterion in our current study, we would have found much more significant connections (and therefore a higher density) because of the larger cohort size, the inclusion of a null condition and the use of an MRI scanner with a higher magnetic field. Networks with different densities can have different properties [51]. To overcome this problem, we used an equi-density thresholding [67], [72] and we have studied the networks at different densities like most other studies [13], [50], [66], [67]. Another problem with the use of a threshold to binarize the network is that a small change in the association (connectivity) strength can lead to a change in connectivity (just below versus just above a threshold).
To overcome these problems, one can define a weighted network. The question is then how to define these weights. Some groups take the connection with the highest connectivity value and set this to one and scale the remaining connections accordingly [11]. This approach is straightforward and simple but it is also more sensitive to noise. We propose an approach which is based on the fact that the partial correlations were transformed to a Z-score using a Fisher r-to-Z transform and by applying a non-linear transform based on the cumulative distribution of the standard normal distribution. This approach is less sensitive to noise (or to outliers) in connection strength and it leads in a natural way to positive weights between 0 and 1.
Subject specific versus group based networks
When studying brain networks in a group of subjects we have two possible approaches: 1) define the network for each subject, calculate the variables of interest (e.g. network measures) and use these values for further analysis or 2) define the network based upon the group itself by e.g. averaging the association matrices across the subjects of the group and by calculating the network (and the corresponding measures) based upon this averaged association matrix. The first approach gives information about the individual variability and we can relate directly graph measures to other subject specific information. This is important especially in the light of comparing and quantifying networks in normals and patients. However, the disadvantage is that the results are more sensitive to noise in the measurements. On the other hand, the notion that averaging connection strengths across subjects summarizes the overall characteristics of the group is widely accepted [55], [56].
Based on the ICC values and on the intra-subject test-retest results, weighted individual networks can be realiably determined. The fact that the identification of hubs and communities is more variable compared to the group based networks is less important since it may also be a result of subject specific behaviour and in that sense it may capture the underlying biological variability.
Conclusions
We have studied the reproducibility and robustness of various graph measures in group based and in individual binary and weighted networks derived from an fMRI experiment using an associative-semantic paradigm. We have shown that global graph measures exhibit a good reproducibility and robustness but the results depend on the graph measure itself and on the density in case of binary networks. Group based binary networks should be derived from groups of sufficient size and the lower the density the more subjects are required to obtain robust values. Local graph measures are very variable in terms of reproducibility and should be interpreted with care. For weighted networks, we found good reproducibility when using subject specific networks and this will allow us to relate network properties to individual subject information.
Supporting Information
Data Availability
The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are available as matlab.mat files and matlab.m scripts and can be found at DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.12500.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) [G0660.09 & G0A0913N to RV and PD], KU Leuven [OT/12/097 to RV and PD], Federaal Wetenschapsbeleid belspo [IAP-VII P7/11] and Stichting voor Alzheimer Onderzoek (SAO11020 & 13007). YW has a grant from the Chinese Scholarship Council, KA is a doctoral fellow of the FWO and RV is a senior clinical investigator of the FWO. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Niu H, He Y (2013) Resting-State Functional Brain Connectivity Lessons from Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy. The Neuroscientist 20:173–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2012) The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:336–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2009) Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:186–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Rubinov M, Sporns O (2010) Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage 52:1059–1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E (2006) A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. J Neurosci 26:63–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Micheloyannis S, Pachou E, Stam CJ, Breakspear M, Bitsios P, et al. (2006) Small-world networks and disturbed functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 87:60–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Micheloyannis S, Vourkas M, Tsirka V, Karakonstantaki E, Kanatsouli K, et al. (2009) The influence of ageing on complex brain networks: a graph theoretical analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 30:200–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Van den Heuvel MP, Stam CJ, Boersma M, Hulshoff Pol HE (2008) Small-world and scale-free organization of voxel-based resting-state functional connectivity in the human brain. NeuroImage 43:528–539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. He Y, Chen Z, Evans A (2007) Small-world anatomical networks in the human brain revealed by cortical thickness from MRI. Cereb Cortex 17:2407–2419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Wang J, Zuo X, Dai Z, Xia M, Zhao Z, et al. (2013) Disrupted functional brain connectome in individuals at risk for alzheimer's disease. Biological psychiatry 73:472–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Van den Heuvel MP, Mandl RC, Stam CJ, Kahn RS, Pol HEH (2010) Aberrant frontal and temporal complex network structure in schizophrenia: a graph theoretical analysis. J Neurosci 30:15915–15926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Ma S, Calhoun VD, Eichele T, Du W, Adal T (2012) Modulations of functional connectivity in the healthy and schizophrenia groups during task and rest. NeuroImage 62:1694–1704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Wang L, Li Y, Metzak P, He Y, Woodward T (2010) Age-related changes in topological patterns of large-scale brain functional networks during memory encoding and recognition. NeuroImage 50:862–872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Heitger M, Ronsse R, Dhollander T, Dupont P, Caeyenberghs K, et al. (2012) Motor learning-induced changes in functional brain connectivity as revealed by means of graph-theoretical network analysis. NeuroImage 61:633–650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Wylie KP, Rojas DC, Tanabe J, Martin LF, Tregellas JR (2012) Nicotine increases brain functional network efficiency. NeuroImage 63:73–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Tijms BM, Seriès P, Willshaw DJ, Lawrie SM (2012) Similarity-based extraction of individual networks from gray matter MRI scans. Cereb Cortex 22:1530–1541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Bernhardt B, Chen Z, He Y, Evans A, Bernasconi N (2011) Graph-theoretical analysis reveals disrupted small-world organization of cortical thickness correlation networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Cereb Cortex 21:2147–2157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Vaessen MJ, Hofman PA, Tijssen HN, Aldenkamp AP, Jansen JF, et al. (2010) The effect and reproducibility of different clinical DTI gradient sets on small world brain connectivity measures. NeuroImage 51:1106–1116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Li L, Rilling JK, Preuss TM, Glasser MF, Damen FW, et al. (2012) Quantitative assessment of a framework for creating anatomical brain networks via global tractography. NeuroImage 61:1017–1030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Liu J, Zhao L, Nan J, Li G, Xiong S, et al. (2013) The trade-off between wiring cost and network topology in white matter structural networks in health and migraine. Exp Neurol 248:196–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bassett DS, Brown JA, Deshpande V, Carlson JM, Grafton ST (2011) Conserved and variable architecture of human white matter connectivity. NeuroImage 54:1262–1279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Telesford QK, Morgan AR, Hayasaka S, Simpson SL, Barret W, et al. (2010) Reproducibility of graph metrics in fMRI networks. Front Neuroinform 4:117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Weber MJ, Detre JA, Thompson-Schill SL, Avants BB (2013) Reproducibility of functional network metrics and network structure: a comparison of task-related BOLD, resting ASL with BOLD contrast, and resting cerebral blood flow. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 13:627–640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Deuker L, Bullmore ET, Smith M, Christensen S, Nathan PJ, et al. (2009) Reproducibility of graph metrics of human brain functional networks. NeuroImage 47:1460–1468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Niu H, Li Z, Liao X, Wang J, Zhao T, et al. (2013) Test-retest reliability of graph metrics in functional brain networks: a resting-state fNIRS study. PLoS One 8:e72425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Owen JP, Ziv E, Bukshpun P, Pojman N, Wakahiro M, et al. (2013) Test-retest reliability of computational network measurements derived from the structural connectome of the human brain. Brain Connect 3:160–176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Buchanan CR, Pernet CR, Gorgolewski KJ, Storkey AJ, Bastin ME (2014) Test-retest reliability of structural brain networks from diffusion MRI. NeuroImage 86:231–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Vandenberghe R, Price C, Wise R, Josephs O, Frackowiak RS (1996) Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature 383:254–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Vandenbulcke M, Peeters R, Dupont P, Van Hecke P, Vandenberghe R (2007) Word reading and posterior temporal dysfunction in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Cereb Cortex 17:542–551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Nelissen N, Vandenbulcke M, Fannes K, Verbruggen A, Peeters R, et al. (2007) Aβ amyloid deposition in the language system and how the brain responds. Brain 130:2055–2069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Nelissen N, Dupont P, Vandenbulcke M, Tousseyn T, Peeters R, et al. (2011) Right hemisphere recruitment during language processing in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and alzheimer's disease. J Mol Neurosci 45:637–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Schacter DL, Rosen BR (2000) Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming. Brain 123:620–640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Van Doren L, Dupont P, De Grauwe S, Peeters R, Vandenberghe R (2010) The amodal system for conscious word and picture identification in the absence of a semantic task. NeuroImage 49:3295–3307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Seghier ML, Price CJ (2011) Explaining left lateralization for words in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex. J Neurosci 31:14745–14753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Whitney C, Kirk M, O′Sullivan J, Lambon Ralph MA, Jefferies E (2011) The neural organization of semantic control: Tms evidence for a distributed network in left inferior frontal and posterior middle temporal gyrus. Cereb Cortex 21:1066–1075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Goldberg RF, Perfetti CA, Fiez JA, Schneider W (2007) Selective retrieval of abstract semantic knowledge in left prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 27:3790–3798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL (2009) Where is the semantic system? a critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 19:2767–2796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Wagner A, Desmond J, Demb J, Glover G, Gabrieli J (1997) Semantic repetition priming for verbal and pictorial knowledge: A functional MRI study of left inferior prefrontal cortex. J Cognitive Neurosci 9:714–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Vandenberghe R, Wang Y, Nelissen N, Vandenbulcke M, Dhollander T, et al. (2013) The associative-semantic network for words and pictures: Effective connectivity and graph analysis. Brain Lang 127(2):264–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Adamczuk K, De Weer AS, Nelissen N, Chen K, Sleegers K, et al. 2013) Polymorphism of brain derived neurotrophic factor influences amyloid load in cognitively intact apolipoprotein e 4 carriers. NeuroImage: Clinical 2:512–520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Ashburner J, Friston KJ (2005) Unified segmentation. NeuroImage 26:839–851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dupont P, Nelissen N, Vandenbulcke M, Vandenberghe R (2008) The anatomical variability of the associative-semantic network. Program No. 868.18. 2008 Neuroscience Meeting Planner. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience, 2008. Online.
- 43. Marrelec G, Krainik A, Duffau H, Plgrini-Issac M, Lehricy S, et al. (2006) Partial correlation for functional brain interactivity investigation in functional MRI. NeuroImage 32:228–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Smith SM, Miller KL, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Webster M, Beckmann CF, et al. (2011) Network modelling methods for fMRI. NeuroImage 54:875–891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Finn JD (1974) A general model for multivariate analysis. Holt, Rinehart & Winston 182. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC (2006) Nonoptimal component placement, but short processing paths, due to long-distance projections in neural systems. PloS Computational Biology 2:e95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Hosseini S, Black JM, Soriano T, Bugescu N, Martinez R, et al. (2013) Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks in children with familial risk for reading difficulties. NeuroImage 71:260–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Li H, Xue Z, Ellmore TM, Frye RE, Wong ST (2014) Network-based analysis reveals stronger local diffusion-based connectivity and different correlations with oral language skills in brains of children with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. Hum Brain Mapp 35:396–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Wang JH, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Milham MP, Biswal BB, et al. (2011) Graph theoretical analysis of functional brain networks: test-retest evaluation on short- and long-term resting-state functional MRI data. PLoS One 6:e21976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Schwarz AJ, McGonigle J (2011) Negative edges and soft thresholding in complex network analysis of resting state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage 55:1132–1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Van Wijk B, Stam C, Daffertshofer A (2010) Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS One 5:e13701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Telesford QK, Burdette JH, Laurienti PJ (2013) An exploration of graph metric reproducibility in complex brain networks. Frontiers in neuroscience 7:67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Shrout P, Fleiss J (1979) Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological bulletin 86:420–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Faria A, Joel S, Zhang Y, Oishi K, van Zjil P, et al. (2012) Atlas-based analysis of resting-state functional connectivity: Evaluation for reproducibility and multi-modal anatomy-function correlation studies. NeuroImage 61:613–621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Rubinov M, Sporns O (2011) Weight-conserving characterization of complex functional brain networks. NeuroImage 56:2068–2079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Power J, Cohen A, Nelson S, Wig G, Barnes K, et al. (2011) Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72:665–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Wu G, Stramaglia S, Chen H, Liao W, Marinazzo M (2013) Mapping the voxel-wise effective connectome in resting state fMRI. PloS one 8:e73670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Shi L, Wang D, Chu W, Liu S, Xiong Y, et al. (2013) Abnormal organization of white matter network in patients with no dementia after ischemic stroke. PloS one 8:e81388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Fornito A, Bullmore E (2015) Reconciling abnormalities of brain network structure and function in schizophrenia. Current opinion in neurobiology 30:44–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Newman M (2006) Finding community structure in networks using the eigenvectors of matrices. Physical review E 74:036104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Barnes K, Nelson S, Cohen A, Power J, Coalson R, et al. (2012) Parcellation in left lateral parietal cortex is similar in adults and children. Cerebral Cortex 22:1148–1158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Steen M, Hayasaka S, Joyce K, Laurienti P (2011) Assessing the consistency of community structure in complex networks. Physical Review E 84:016111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Moussa MN, Steen MR, Laurienti PJ, Hayasaka S (2012) Consistency of network modules in resting-state fMRI connectome data. PLoS ONE 7:e44428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore E (2012) On the use of correlation as a measure of network connectivity. NeuroImage 60:2096–2106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Albert R, Barabási AL (2002) Statistical mechanics of complex networks. Reviews of modern physics 74:47–97. [Google Scholar]
- 66. Hayasaka S, Laurienti PJ (2010) Comparison of characteristics between region-and voxel-based network analyses in resting-state fMRI data. NeuroImage 50:499–508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Achard S, Bullmore E (2007) Efficiency and cost of economical brain functional networks. PLoS computational biology 3:e17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Soranzo G, Bianconi N, Altafini C (2007) Comparing association network algorithms for reverse engineering of large-scale gene regulatory networks: synthetic versus real data. Bioinformatics 23:1640–1647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Stifanelli PF, Creanza TM, Anglani R, Liuzzi VC, Mukherjee S, et al. (2013) A comparative study of covariance selection models for the inference of gene regulatory networks. Journal of biomedical informatics 46:894–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Braun U, Plichta MM, Esslinger C, Sauer C, Haddad L, et al. (2012) Test–retest reliability of resting-state connectivity network characteristics using fMRI and graph theoretical measures. NeuroImage 59:1404–1412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, et al. (2013) Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:365–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. He Y, Chen Z, Evans A (2008) Structural insights into aberrant topological patterns of large-scale cortical networks in alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci 28:4756–4766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are available as matlab.mat files and matlab.m scripts and can be found at DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.12500.