Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Dec 16.
Published in final edited form as: Nat Commun. 2012;3:1079. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2086

Genetic architecture supports mosaic brain evolution and independent brain–body size regulation

Reinmar Hager 1,#, Lu Lu 2,3,#, Glenn D Rosen 4, Robert W Williams 2
PMCID: PMC4267555  NIHMSID: NIHMS425630  PMID: 23011133

Abstract

The mammalian brain consists of distinct parts that fulfil different functions. Finlay and Darlington have argued that evolution of the mammalian brain is constrained by developmental programs, suggesting that different brain parts are not free to respond individually to selection and evolve independent of other parts or overall brain size. However, comparisons among mammals with matched brain weights often reveal greater differences in brain part size, arguing against strong developmental constraints. Here, we test these hypotheses using a quantitative genetic approach involving over 10,000 mice. We identify independent loci for size variation in seven key parts of the brain, and observe that brain parts show low or no phenotypic correlation, as is predicted by a mosaic scenario. We also demonstrate that variation in brain size is independently regulated from body size. The allometric relations seen at higher phylogenetic levels are thus unlikely to be the product of strong developmental constraints.

Introduction

The variation seen in overall brain size within and among species in relation to the size of different brain parts and of body size has been the focus of a large research effort because these fundamental relations reveal much about the constraints, adaptations, and even candidate mechanisms that lead to micro- and macro- evolutionary change1-4. Several hypotheses based on comparative data have been developed to answer two fundamental questions about brain evolution4-9. The first is the degree to which the different functional brain systems evolve independently of each other and the brain as a whole. Under the mosaic evolutionary hypothesis, the size of different systems evolves independently due to differential selective pressures associated with different tasks4,6. In contrast, under a concerted evolutionary paradigm, promoted by Finlay and Darlington1, different regions are constrained or canalized by developmental factors and thus evolve predominantly as a whole. The distinction between these two hypotheses is central to our understanding of brain evolution and our interpretation of the underlying genetic, developmental and ecological mechanisms on which selection is presumed to operate. Comparative analyses of brain size have provided evidence for both hypotheses1,6,10. For example, the seminal data set collated and first analysed by Stephan and colleagues (e.g.11) on brain and brain part sizes in primates and insectivores has provided the basis for much of the comparative analyses2. However, few studies, in particular in mammals, have investigated the genetic architecture of the brain to address the microevolutionary and genetic underpinnings of brain evolution12.

Mosaic evolution of brain regions predicts that heritable variation in the size of different brain parts should be modulated by independent genetic loci and gene variants, and that phenotypic correlations among different brain parts may be low or absent. In contrast, under concerted evolution overlapping sets of loci should modulate the size of multiple parts of the brain with high levels of positive covariation. Quantitative genetic studies in the cave fish have demonstrated independent loci regulating the evolution of different eye phenotypes and argue in favour of a mosaic model12. A quantitative genetic approach offers the further advantage that the degree of mosaic versus concerted evolution may be inferred by comparing the level of variation explained by loci that are shared across many brain parts versus the level of variation explained by unique loci specific to brain parts. Although macroevolutionary patterns (e.g. phylogenetic patterns seen above the species level including the occurrence of higher taxa) arise from microevolutionary mechanisms, i.e. changes occurring within species13, it remains unclear whether specific brain parts can respond individually to selective pressure or are constrained in their response imposed by other brain parts or overall brain size.

A second fundamental question is how phylogenetic differences in brain:body ratios have evolved. What are the mechanisms underlying the strong allometry between brain and body? Overall, brain size scales with body size due to linked processes very early during development14. After this initial phase, however, body size increases while brain size remains relatively constant14. Evolutionarily, the large relative brain to body size ratio, especially seen in many vertebrates, could in principle be due to changes in overall brain size or be secondary to changes in body size. It is evident that macroevolutionary trends among major vertebrate taxa have often involved a genuine increase in relative brain size at a constant body size, and that this has been made possible due to major changes in bioenergetics and life history5. However at a microevolutionary level within species, brain size may be free to change independently of body size, with different genetic loci accounting for variation in the two traits and low phenotypic correlation between them.

Here, we address these two key questions and present results of a 15-year research effort into the genetic architecture of brain and body size using a massive neuro-morphometric data set for ~10,000 mice belonging to a large set of recombinant inbred strains. The BXD family consists of approximately 100 lines derived from parental strains that differ at ~5 million single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), indels, transposons, and copy-number variants15. This model system harbours naturally occurring genetic variation at a level approximating that of human populations. Our study utilizes a high density linkage analysis16,17 to map loci modulating phenotypic variation in overall brain size, body size, and the size of seven major brain parts: the neocortex, cerebellum, striatum, olfactory bulb, hippocampus, lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) and basolateral complex of the amygdala (BLA). We scanned the entire genome, except the Y chromosome, using interval mapping as implemented in GeneNetwork (www.genenetwork.org17). Mapping relies on a set of 3800 fully informative SNPs and microsatellite markers. In a second analysis, we scan the genome for all two-way epistatic interactions between loci.

Results

Regulation of brain part size

Our results show that distinct loci modulate the size of the brain, different brain regions, and body size (Table 1, morphometric data are given in Table 2). Three loci on chromosomes 12, 15 and 19 influence variation in overall brain size (Brain12a, Brain15a, Brain19a; Fig. 1), and 15 loci modulate weights and volumes of specific brain regions. With one exception, no locus was shared among the seven brain parts nor with overall brain size. The sole exception involved a locus on mouse chromosome 6 that may jointly modulate the volume of both the striatum and neocortex (Str6a and Cx6a). We detected two epistatic interactions—one between Brw15a and Brw19a for overall brain weight and one between Str6a and Str17a for striatum. With these two exceptions, we found no evidence for positive or negative gene-by-gene interaction between loci affecting overall brain size and those affecting the size of different brain parts. Because no sets of loci were shared among brain parts or with overall brain size we did not calculate the proportion of phenotypic variation accounted for by shared versus non-shared loci.

Table 1.

Loci for brain and body size traits.

Trait QTL LRS Chromosome Marker Location [mb]
Overall brain weight Brw12a 13.98 12 103.98
Brw19a 24.70 19 10.79
Brw15a × Brw19a(1 43.07, 13.46 15, 19 62.05, 10.83
Body weight Bw11a 16.44 11 73.92
Cerebellum Cere1a 15.57 1 174.16
Cere8a 23.15 8 98.23
Hippocampus Hip1a 20.50 1 170.99
Hip5a 12.80 5 72.42
Hip7a 13.41 7 95.85
Neocortex(2 Neo6a 14.64 6 90.31
Neo11a 14.67 11 36.56
Olfactory bulb Olf17a 13.50 17 9.57
Striatum(2 Str1a 12.63 1 181.05
Str6a 20.05 6 91.63
Str6a × Str17a(1 36.21, 10.66 6, 17 91.96
35.19
LGN(2 LGN15a 16.24 15 95.00
BLA(2 BLA5a 11.23 5 27.33
BLA8a 14.28 8 75.07

The traits are followed by the quantitative trait locus, QTL (significant or highest suggestive locus), the chromosome number and the location of the marker with the highest linkage in megabases (mouse build 36, mm9). Brw = brain weight, Bw = body weight, Cere = cerebellum, Hip = hippocampus, Neo = neocortex, Olf = olfactory bulb, Str = striatum, LGN = lateral geniculate nucleus; BLA = basolateral complex. QTL = quantitative trait locus; LRS refers to likelihood ratio statistic.

(1

epistasis

(2

volume.

For interactions, first the overall full model LRS is given, followed by the interaction LRS.

Table 2.

Morphometric data.

Trait Overall Brainweight Body weight Cerebellum Hippocampus Neocortex Olfactory bulb Striatum Lateral geniculate nulceus Basolateral complex
unit grams grams grams grams mm3 grams mm3 mm3 mm3
Average
Value
434.87 25.79 55.09 25.32 102.53 20.71 22.19 0.27 1.48
Standard
Error
0.49 0.09 0.21 0.12 0.89 0.12 0.16 0.00 0.02
N 5379 5297 780 634 223 782 342 182 195

The table gives average trait value, standard error and total number of samples for each of the traits in the study.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Mouse brain sketch highlighting the analyzed brain parts. (A) For each of the brain parts and body weight we show the corresponding genome scan with peaks identifying main loci, their location and chromosome number. Were individual peaks are shown, the red lines denote genome-wide significance, the grey line is the suggestive threshold. (B) The QTL cluster maps provide a global whole-genome summary of mapping results for all key traits in the form of colour-coded horizontal bands—one per trait, extending from proximal chromosome 1 to distal chromosome X. Regions of more intense color correspond to linkage peaks and the colours also encode whether the B allele (blue) or the D allele (red) contributes to large weight and volume. CX = cortex; HP = hippocampus; Brain = overall brain weight; Body = body weight; CB = cerebellum; LGN = lateral geniculate nucleus; STR = striatum; BLA = basolateral complex; OB = olfactory bulb.

These results provide comparatively strong support for mosaic brain evolution. We can demonstrate that variation in the size of different brain parts is modulated by loci independent of each other and overall brain size. While the different loci that control the size of brain parts may respond to selection in a similar fashion, selection for specific tasks associated with individual brain parts renders micro-evolutionary change of the size of specific brain parts possible.

We can further investigate the degree of independent regulation by analyzing covariation between overall brain size and brain part size (both corrected for sex and age) and among the seven different brain phenotypes (corrected for overall brain size). Surprisingly, we found absent or low levels of correlation, ranging from -0.22 (Pearson's r) between olfactory bulb and basolateral geniculate nucleus to 0.29 between olfactory bulb and cerebellum (Table 3). Moreover, while the correlations between overall brain size and brain part size are always significant, they are intermediate and the associated r2 values range from 0.12 to 0.55. For individual brain parts, these results suggest very little shared underlying genetic control. Indeed, the result of the quantitative genetic analysis showing independent regulation of overall brain size and brain part size, and among brain parts, is borne out by the phenotypic covariance analysis.

Table 3.

Partial correlation for relative size of brain parts.

Cortex Striatum Olfactory bulb Hippocampus Cerebellum Lateral geniculate nucleus Basolateral complex
Cortex 1 r=0.23
p<0.001
r=0.09
p=0.164
r=−0.08
p=0.279
r=0.09
p=0.198
r=−0.01
p=0.888
r=−0.01
p=0.831
Striatum 1 r=−0.03
p=0.535
r<0.01
p=0.945
r=0.03
p=0.658
r=−0.04
p=0.627
r=−0.01
p=0.182
Olfactory bulb 1 r=0.06
p=0.127
r=0.29
p<0.001
r=−0.06
p=0.422
r=−0.22
p=0.002
Hippocampus 1 r=−0.02
p=0.698
r=−0.08
p=0.339
r=−0.11
p=0.164
Cerebellum 1 r=−0.09
p=0.242
r=−0.08
p=0.305
Lateral geniculate nucleus 1 r=0.08
p=0.265
Basolateral complex 1
Total Brain weight r=0.53
p<0.001
r=0.58
p<0.001
r=0.49
p<0.001
r=0.56
p<0.001
r=0.74
p<0.001
r=0.74
p<0.001
r=0.34
p<0.001

Partial correlation (Pearson's r) between brain Darts and between brain Dart size and total brain weight calculated at the individual level. Correlations between brain parts are controlled for total brain minus brain part.

Brain and body size regulation

We next addressed the second fundamental question of how the large relative brain size in mammals may have evolved by scanning the genome for loci that affect variation in body size to compare with those for overall brain size. We found evidence for independent loci governing brain and body size, the variation in which was affected by loci on different chromosomes, and no positive or negative epistatic interaction (indicative of potential constraints) was detected (Table 1). Indeed, the phenotypic correlation between body size and overall brain size is very low, with only 3.3% of variation in brain size accounted for by variation in body size. These results suggest that variation in overall brain size is regulated by different underlying genes than is body size, with few genetic constraints on independent evolution of brain and body size. This conclusion is in agreement with results from comparative studies. Weston and Lister could demonstrate that in dwarf hippopotami brain size reduction is much greater than body size reduction, which suggests that selective pressures on brain and body size can operate independently and cause deviations from traditional scaling models8. Our study demonstrates the necessary independent genetic regulation of brain and body size that may underlie the findings of Weston and Lister8. Further, a recent comprehensive systematic analysis in primates has shown that brain size decreased in a number of primate lineages18 and found evidence for a dissociation between brain and body size at a macroevolutionary level. Our study demonstrates the necessary independent genetic regulation of brain and body size that may underlie the findings of these studies.

Discussion

In sum, our dissection of within-species variation provides compelling evidence for a mosaic control of brain region size as predicted by ecological and selection theory19. We do not suggest that the strong allometric patterns described by Finlay and Darlington cannot be explained by developmental/ontological processes and scaling functions at a macroevolutionary level. Indeed, Thompson20 developed numerous functions that elegantly describe the relation between functionally very different structures. However, macroevolutionary patterns have microevolutionary origins21-25 and at this level our results do not support strong developmental constraints. Specifically, we argue that apparent constraints seen at the macroevolutionary level do not mean that brain parts are necessarily constrained in their response to selection, nor that some parts may be spandrels (i.e. a by-product of the evolution of other parts adapted later to specific tasks2,26). This argument does not preclude the possibility that brain development may influence the direction of evolution on a larger scale by changing selectable phenotypic variation (e.g.27,28) but it is clear that at a micro-evolutionary level brain part size can respond to selection in a largely independent way. The apparent constraints seen at higher phylogenetic levels are thus unlikely to be the product of strong developmental constraints and appear to mask discrete and mosaic selection.

Methods

Subjects and Phenotypes

For our study we used over 10,000 individuals from the BXD panel of recombinant inbred lines. This family of mouse strains is segregating for ~5 million single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), indels, and copy-number variations (CNVs); a level of common sequence variation that matches or exceeds that in large human families and populations17. Over the past 15 years we have measured overall brain weight, cerebellum, hippocampus, olfactory bulb weights and neocortex, basolateral complex, lateral geniculate nucleus and striatum volume, and body weight from a total of 91 of these BXD lines29-31. In this work we have adjusted brain phenotypes for age, sex, epoch, and body weight.

All histologic data for this study were obtained from The Mouse Brain Library (MBL)—a physical and internet resource that contains high-resolution images of histologically processed slides from over 3200 adult mouse brains (www.mbl.org) with roughly balanced numbers of male and female specimens. The ages ranged from 21-694 days of age (mean ± SEM = 103 ± 5), with most of the cases ranging from 50—120 days. Mice were obtained from either the Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) or the University of Tennessee Health Science Center (UTHSC). All procedures were approved by institutional animal care and use committees and conform to NIH guidelines for humane treatment of animals. Mice were deeply anesthetized with Avertin (0.8 ml i.p.) and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline, followed by fixative (1.25% glutaraldehyde/1.0% paraformaldehyde or 4% paraformaldeyde), and their brains removed and weighed. After variable post-fixation times, the brains were embedded in 12% celloidin and sliced in either a coronal or horizontal plane at a width of approximately 30 μm. Actual section thickness was determined by direct examination of 10 sections for each brain using an x100 oil immersion objective and a z-axis micrometer.

Estimation of regional volume

The volume of brain regions was estimated using a computer controlled microscope (Nikon E800, Nikon, Inc., Melville, NY) and Stereo Investigator (MBF Biosciences, Williston, VT). Brain regions were parcellated by the criteria of the Allen Brain Atlas (mouse.brain-map.org). Volume was estimated by point counting using Cavalieri's method32. In cases where there were missing or damaged sections, a piece-wise parabolic estimation was used33. Final volume estimates were individually corrected for histological shrinkage by determining the previously computed ratio between the brain volume at fixation (brain weight) and that after processing.

Genetic Analysis

The quantitative genetic analysis was performed using interval mapping and a set of 3795 informative SNP markers across all chromosomes, except for the Y chromosome. We used the WebQTL mapping module in GeneNetwork (www.genenetwork.org17,34). Loci were identified by computation of likelihood ratio statistic scores and significance was determined using 2000 permutations of the phenotype data35. We further investigated two-locus epistatic interactions by searching for all possible interactions between pairs of loci in the genome. Phenotypes were then randomly permuted 500 times and reanalyzed using the pair-scan algorithm. We extracted the single highest LRS (likelihood ratio statistic; a measurement of the association between differences in phenotypes and differences in DNA sequence) for the full model for each of these permuted data sets. If the full model exceeded the permutation-based significance threshold, then different models for those locations can be tested by conventional chi-square tests at p<0.01. For both the interval mapping and epistasis analysis we calculated thresholds at the suggestive (p=0.63) and significant associations (p=0.05 and p=0.01).

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the help of David Airey, Anand Kulkarni, Pengchong Liu, and Hongtao Zhai with data collection and thank Casey Bergman and Andrew Loudon for helpful comments on previous drafts of the manuscript. This work was supported by NIH grant U01CA105417, U24 RR021760, U01AA13499, U24AA13513, P20-DA 21131, CITG to R. W.; NIH grant U01AA014425 to L. L.; NIH grant NS052397 to G. D. R. R. H. was supported by a NERC Research Fellowship, NE/F013418/1. The data reported in this paper are publicly accessible in genenetwork.org.

Footnotes

Author contributions

R. H., L. L. and R. W. W. conceived the analysis, analysed data and wrote the paper. L. L., G.D.R, and R. W. W. provided phenotypes and wrote the paper.

Competing financial interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  • 1.Finlay BL, Darlington RB. Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science. 1995;268:1578–1584. doi: 10.1126/science.7777856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Finlay BL, Darlington RB, Nicastor N. Developmental structure in brain evolution. Behav. Brain Sci. 2001;24:263–308. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Blinkov SM, Glezer II. The Human brain in figures and tables: A Quantitative Handbook. Plenum Press; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Barton RA. In: Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Dunbar RIM, Barrett LE, editors. Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 129–144. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Striedter GF. Principles of brain evolution. Sinauer; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Barton RA, Harvey PH. Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals. Nature. 2000;405:1055–1058. doi: 10.1038/35016580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Clark DA, Mitra PP, Wang SS. Scalable architecture in mammalian brains. Nature. 2001;411:189–193. doi: 10.1038/35075564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Weston EM, Lister AM. Insular dwarfism in hippos and a model for brain size reduction in Homo floresiensis. Nature. 2009;459:85–88. doi: 10.1038/nature07922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Navarrete A, van Schaik CP, Isler K. Energetics and the evolution of human brain size. Nature. 2011;480:91–94. doi: 10.1038/nature10629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Yopak KE, et al. A conserved pattern of brain scaling from sharks to primates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:12946–12951. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002195107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stephan H, Frahm H, Baron G. New and revised data on volumes of brain structures in insectivores and primates. Folia Primatol. 1981;35:1–29. doi: 10.1159/000155963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Protas M, Conrad M, Gross JB, Tabin C, Borowsky R. Regressive Evolution in the Mexican Cave Tetra, Astyanax Mexicanus. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:452–454. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mayr E. Animal species and evolution. Harvard University Press; 1963. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Jerison HJ. Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Curr. Anthropol. 1975;16:403–426. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang J, Williams RW, Manly KF. WebQTL: Web-based complex trait analysis. Neuroinformatics. 2003;1:299–308. doi: 10.1385/NI:1:4:299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lander ES, Botstein D. Mapping Mendelian factors underlying quantitative traits using RFLP linkage maps. Genetics. 1989;121:185–199. doi: 10.1093/genetics/121.1.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Chesler EJ, et al. Complex trait analysis of gene expression uncovers polygenic and pleiotropic networks that modulate nervous system function. Nat. Genet. 2005;37:233–242. doi: 10.1038/ng1518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Montgomery SH, Capellini I, Barton RA, Mundy NI. Reconstructing the ups and downs of primate brain evolution: implications for adaptive hypotheses and Homo floresiensis. BMC Biology. 2010;8:9. doi: 10.1186/1741-7007-8-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Harvey PH, Krebs JR. Comparing brains. Science. 1990;249:140–146. doi: 10.1126/science.2196673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Thompson DW. On growth and form. Cambridge Univ. Press; 1917. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Huxley J. Evolution: the modern synthesis. Allen Unwin; 1942. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mayr E. The growth of biological thought. Belknap; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kutschera U, Niklas KJ. The modern theory of biological evolution: an expanded synthesis. Naturwissenschaften. 2004;91:255–276. doi: 10.1007/s00114-004-0515-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Schwander T, Leimar O. Genes as leaders and followers in evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2011;26:143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2010.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pigliucci M, Müller GB. Eds Evolution: the extended synthesis. MIT Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gould SJ, Lewontin SJ. The spandrels of San Marco and the panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc. Roy. Soc. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 1979;205:581–598. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1979.0086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.West-Eberhard MJ. Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford Univ. Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Laland KN, Sterelny K, Odling-Smee J, Hoppitt W, Uller T. Cause and effect in biology revisited: Is Mayr's proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science. 2011;334:1512–1516. doi: 10.1126/science.1210879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mozhui K, Hamre KM, Holmes A, Lu L, Williams RW. Genetic and structural analysis of the basolateral amygdala complex in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Behav. Genet. 2007;37:223–243. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9122-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gaglani SM, Lu L, Williams RW, Rosen GD. The genetic control of neocortex volume and covariation with neocortical gene expression in mice. BMC Neurosci. 2009;9:10–44. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Rosen GD, et al. Genetic modulation of striatal volume by loci on Chrs 6 and 17 in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:296–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00473.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gundersen HJG, Jensen EB. The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology and its prediction. J. Microscop. 1987;147:229–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2818.1987.tb02837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rosen GD, Harry JD. Brain volume estimation from serial section measurements: a comparison of methodologies. J. Neurosci. Methods. 1990;35:115–124. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(90)90101-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Shifman S, et al. A high-resolution single nucleotide polymorphism genetic map of the mouse genome. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Churchill GA, Doerge RW. Empirical threshold values for quantitative trait mapping. Genetics. 1994;138:963–971. doi: 10.1093/genetics/138.3.963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES