Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jan 2.
Published in final edited form as: Mem Cognit. 2014 Nov;42(8):1211–1224. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0435-9

Deconstructing the effect of self-directed study on episodic memory

Douglas Markant 1, Sarah DuBrow 2, Lila Davachi 3,4, Todd M Gureckis 5
PMCID: PMC4281964  NIHMSID: NIHMS640965  PMID: 24941938

Abstract

Self-directed learning is often associated with better long-term memory retention, however, the mechanisms that underlie this advantage remain poorly understood. This series of experiments was designed to “deconstruct” the notion of self-directed learning in order to better identify the factors most responsible for these improvements to memory. In particular, we isolate the memory advantage that comes from controlling the content of study episodes from the advantage that comes from controlling the timing of those episodes. Across four experiments, self-directed learning significantly enhanced recognition memory relative to passive observation. However, the advantage for self-directed learning was found to be present even under extremely minimal conditions of volitional control (simply pressing a button when ready to advance to the next item). Our results suggest that improvements to memory following self-directed encoding may be related to the ability to coordinate stimulus presentation with the learner’s current preparatory or attentional state, and highlight the need to consider the range of cognitive control processes involved in and influenced by self-directed study.

Keywords: memory, metacognition, self-directed learning, self-regulated learning, volitional control


One way to characterize different learning tasks is along a dimension of volitional control. In a self-directed task learners exert influence over the flow of information, including the order and timing of new study episodes. In contrast, in a fully passive task information flow is determined by the dynamics of the environment in which the learner is simply an observer. Experimental paradigms for studying learning and memory often fall at the passive end of this continuum. In many studies of memory the experimenter determines the sequence and timing of study items, precluding any significant influence by the participant over the flow of events. Yet volitional interaction with the environment—deciding what to learn about and when—is a ubiquitous feature of human learning and may have consequences for basic learning processes (Gureckis & Markant, 2012; Kornell & Bjork, 2007; Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006).

Recent studies have shown that self-directed study leads to better episodic memory than passive observation in a variety of tasks, including face recognition (Liu, Ward, & Markall, 2007), object recognition (Harman, Humphrey, & Goodale, 1999; Voss, Galvan, & Gonsalves, 2011; Voss, Gonsalves, Federmeier, Tranel, & Cohen, 2011; Voss, Warren, et al., 2011), and spatial learning (Meijer & Van der Lubbe, 2011; Plancher, et al., 2012). Memory improvements have also been found in tasks where learners choose which items to study in preparation for future cued recall tests (Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006). At the same time, people often have incorrect beliefs about how their own memory works, leading them to pursue inefficient study strategies (Bjork, Dunlosky, & Kornell, 2013). For example, students often believe that massed practice (e.g., cramming for a test) will benefit memory more than distributing practice over time (Kornell & Bjork, 2007; Simon & Bjork, 2001), which runs counter to robust evidence that spacing study sessions improves memory (Dempster, 1988).

A challenge in understanding the effect of self-directed learning is determining what constitutes an appropriate control condition in which such decisions are not present. Kornell and Metcalfe (2006) compared performance between items based on whether learners’ study decisions were honored or not (e.g., in the dishonor condition, only items that were not chosen by a participant would appear during restudy). However, under this method the kind of items studied in the honor and dishonor conditions are not matched. Indeed, people tended to choose items for restudy that were easier, raising the possibility that differences between conditions result from studying different kinds of items, rather than from the process of making self-directed study decisions.

One solution to this concern is an experimental design in which the information selected by a self-directed learner is replayed to a “yoked" partner, thereby holding the learning experience constant and providing a direct test of self-directed decision-making on learning (Gureckis & Markant, 2012; Markant & Gureckis, 2014). In this design, a self-directed learner has greater control over the stimulus presentation than their yoked partner. Of course, given that a yoked observer always has some measure of control (e.g., where to direct attention), it is the level of control that defines self-directed behavior in a given context. The key question is how such control can lead to different outcomes even when the content of the learning experience appears to be matched.

Self-directed advantages for memory: Selecting content vs. coordinating attention

Self-directed study is not a unitary behavior, as it involves a range of decision-making and control processes that are not present during passive observation. As a result, the cognitive basis for memory enhancements following self-directed study remains unclear. For example, one possible benefit of self-directed learning is that it allows a person to select information based on their own uncertainty or existing memory. One activity in which this figures prominently is making decisions about what material to study for a future test. Given a finite amount of study time, a person should allocate effort so as to maximize the amount of material that will be recalled in the future. For example, they should not waste time on items that can already be recalled with ease, nor should they attempt to learn items that are too difficult to be acquired within the allotted time (Metcalfe, 2002, 2009; Nelson & Narens, 1994; Son & Metcalfe, 2000). Making adaptive encoding decisions thus relies on relating external sources of information to internal judgments of learning (Metcalfe & Finn, 2008; Metcalfe & Kornell, 2003). To the extent that a person can make these decisions accurately, self-directed learning can improve performance by structuring the content of study to the individual’s needs (which may be different from those of a yoked partner).

At the same time, the control associated with self-directed learning may allow a person to optimize their experience with respect to short-term fluctuations in their own motivational or attentional state. Waiting to access a source of information until one is prepared to learn or prolonging a stimulus presentation (e.g., due to a lapse in attention) reflects a qualitatively different kind of adaptive control that enhances the coordination between ongoing experience and learning. In contrast to the metacognitive processes described above, this coordination may facilitate the learning of material independently of its actual content or a person’s preexisting memory of it. As with decisions about the content of study, however, the experience that results from control over the timing of new information may be mismatched to the needs of a yoked partner.

Self-directed study is thus associated with at least two kinds of adaptive choice: 1) selecting content that is useful with respect to the learner’s goals and existing memory, and 2) coordinating stimulus presentation with their attentional or motivational state. These two levels of control have frequently been confounded in existing studies that have found differences in memory between self-directed and yoked study. In the present study we deconstruct a recent example of a self-directed memory enhancement in order to assess how these different levels of control impact performance.

Enhanced memory through self-directed exploration

A recent set of studies (Voss, Galvan, et al., 2011; Voss, Gonsalves, et al. 2011; Voss, Warren, et al., 2011) found a robust benefit for self-directed memory encoding in a spatial exploration task in which participants memorized the identity and location of icons presented in a series of 5×5 grids (see Figure 1). At any given time, a single item in the grid was visible through a “window" that moved throughout the display. Each participant alternated between self-directed blocks, in which they controlled the movement of the window, and yoked blocks, in which they followed the window movements that had been executed by a previous participant. After studying six grids of items, participants were tested for their recognition memory and their ability to recall the spatial location in which items had appeared during study. The results from Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) showed an advantage for self-directed encoding for both recognition and spatial recall. A consistent pattern of results was found in a separate follow-up experiment, showing that the benefit from self-directed encoding was still evident a week after the initial study session (Voss, Galvan, et al., 2011).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A graphical summary of the experiment design used in all four experiments. Top: In each study block, 25 objects were “hidden" inside a grid and the participant could only view a single object at a time through a moving window. Each participant completed six study blocks, alternating between self-directed and yoked study. During self-directed blocks the participant controlled the movement of the window. During yoked blocks the sequence of movements was identical to that of a previous participant. Bottom: During the test phase, the participant made a recognition decision for every item that was studied (as well as 150 new objects). If they responded “OLD", they were then given a spatial recall test in which they had to place the item onto the study grid based on their memory for where it appeared. If they could not recall its studied location, they could “opt-out" of the spatial response by clicking on the question mark to the side of the grid.

Interestingly, the benefit for self-directed encoding was related to how people studied different items. Voss, Gonslaves, et al. (2011) found better memory for items studied for longer durations specifically in the self-directed condition, whereas, surprisingly, memory for items studied in the yoked condition was equivalent for brief and long durations. Voss, Warren, et al. (2011), in a later analysis of the same data, showed that there was a similar selective benefit for items that were “revisited” within a short period of time. Although the authors ascribed this advantage to decision-making processes present during self-directed encoding, the lack of a simple relationship between study time and later memory under yoked conditions suggests this condition could be disadvantaged at a more basic level.

Overview of the current study

Although prior studies have revealed a robust advantage for self-directed encoding, this condition has involved control over multiple aspects of the learner’s environment that are absent in passive, observational learning. The goal of the present study was to determine whether control over the content of study is necessary for producing an advantage for self-directed learning, or if simpler forms of control over the timing of study are sufficient to produce differences between self-directed and yoked study. We modified the spatial exploration paradigm across a series of experiments, beginning with a replication of Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) to establish a self-directed memory benefit (Experiment 1) and incrementally removing potential sources of the self-directed advantage (Experiments 2–4, see Figure 2 for an overview).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Design manipulations for Experiments 2-4. In Experiments 3 and 4, the window followed a fixed, “snaking” path through the array, an example of which is shown by the dotted line (but which was not visible to participants).

Experiment 1: Replicating the self-directed memory advantage

The first experiment aimed to replicate the memory advantage for self-directed study in the spatial exploration paradigm employed by Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011). We made three minor modifications to the previous design. First, we used a fully opaque (rather than semi-transparent) mask such that stimuli were only visible when revealed by the moving window. We reasoned that this would provide more accurate data on the items being attended during study. Second, Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) allowed the window to move freely through the display based on mouse movements, enabling rapid transitions between items in different regions of the display. In our design participants could only move the window in cardinal directions. Finally, instead of using separate sets of items for the recognition and spatial memory tests, we employed a two-step procedure in which all items that were recognized were subsequently tested for spatial recall. In addition, we added an “opt-out” response during spatial recall to minimize the influence of random guessing. We predicted that these changes to the testing procedure would deconfound recognition and spatial memory and provide a more sensitive measure of each.

Participants

Thirty-one members of the NYU community participated either for course credit or a $10 payment. One person was a “seed" whose data was used for yoking the subsequent participant but was not included in the analysis.

Materials

The experiment was run in a single session on Macintosh computers.

The stimuli were the same as those used in Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) and included 300 line drawings of commonplace items. For the seed participant, half of these stimuli were randomly sampled and assigned to one of six encoding blocks and a location in the grid. For all subsequent participants, the stimuli experienced during yoked blocks were the same in identity and location as in the previous participant’s self-directed blocks. Items were then randomly sampled from the set of remaining stimuli and assigned to the participant’s self-directed blocks. Following random assignment of items to study blocks, the 150 remaining stimuli were retained for the test phase to be used as foils.

Procedure

Study phase

The study phase was comprised of six encoding blocks. In each block the goal was to memorize the identity and spatial location of 25 objects that were hidden in a 5×5 grid. The duration of each study block was 1 minute and each block was preceded by a 20 second break.

At any point in time, a single object was visible through a “window" which moved in four cardinal directions. Each participant alternated between self-directed and yoked encoding blocks, with the type of the first block counterbalanced across participants. Participants were instructed that there would be six study blocks of two types (“MOVE" and “FOLLOW") that differed in whether they would move the window themselves. The instructions excluded any mention of “active" or “self-directed" learning or differences in “control" other than through the terms “move" and “follow". The instructions included a practice self-directed round in which the participant moved the window around the grid for 1 minute (only a single example item was present at every location and they were not trying to memorize items).

Self-directed encoding

During self-directed blocks, the participant moved the window by pressing one of the arrow keys on the keyboard, causing it to “slide" in one of the four directions. Once the window had transitioned to the chosen location it revealed the object at that position. The participant could initiate the next movement at any time.

Yoked encoding

During yoked blocks, the self-directed blocks of the previous participant determined the movement of the window. Between the two participants, the stimuli, sequence of positions, and the durations of item presentation were the same.

Test phase

The test phase was preceded by a two-minute break. During the test, items were presented in a pseudo-random order such that every 12 items included six new objects and one object from each of the six study blocks. On each test trial, a fixation cue (500ms) preceded the presentation of the item. Once the item was displayed the participant had three seconds to respond, with the options 1) “Definitely OLD", 2) “Probably OLD", 3) “Probably NEW" and 4) “Definitely NEW".

When a participant responded “OLD", the recognition test was followed by a spatial recall test for the same item. For each spatial recall trial, the study grid was presented with small black circles marking the 25 positions. The current stimulus appeared in a random location and the participant used the mouse to reposition it according to their memory of its location. Participants were not required to place the stimulus directly above one of the 25 locations, but could indicate uncertainty about its location by placing it between positions. The participant then clicked the mouse button to enter their response (with a response deadline of 10 s). Alternatively, they could “opt-out" from making a spatial response by clicking a button on the edge of the screen. Participants were instructed to make “opt-out" responses when they were completely uncertain about where an item had appeared during the study phase.

Results

Study sequences

Two participants failed to visit a small number of items during study (nine and six items, respectively), and as a result the same items were not studied by the following participants during their yoked blocks. All remaining participants viewed every training item at least once. Items were visited an average of 3.5 times (SD = 0.94) for an average median study duration of 473 ms per visit (SD = 210 ms). The average cumulative study time per item (summed across separate episodes of studying each item and averaged for each subject) was 1948 ms (SD = 122 ms).

Recognition

The results of the recognition test are summarized in Table 1 for all experiments. Analysis of recognition was limited those items that were visited at least once. A greater proportion of studied items were recognized from self-directed blocks than from yoked blocks, with a significant advantage for self-directed study (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Recognition test results

FA rate Self-
directed
hit rate
Yoked
hit rate
Difference in hit rate (self-directed – yoked)

Experiment N M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) t p 95% CI Cohen’s d
Exp 1
(Replication)
30 .11 (.08) .71 (.19) .61 (.16) .10 (.10) 5.67 < .001 .06–.13 1.03
Exp 2
(Attentional cueing)
30 .13 (.10) .71 (.16) .64 (.18) .07 (.13) 2.70 < .01 .02–.11 0.49
Exp 3
(Follow a fixed path)
32 .12 (.09) .71 (.19) .64 (.22) .06 (.12) 2.89 < .01 .02–.10 0.51
Exp 4
(Press to reveal)
30 .09 (.07) .71 (.16) .64 (.20) .07 (.13) 2.71 .01 .02–.12 0.50

Our next goal was to determine how features of an item’s study experience were related to whether it was recognized during the test. We used mixed effects logistic regression on the recognition response for studied items (HIT/MISS, combining low and high confidence items since there were relatively few low confidence responses). Items were grouped by participant (random effect), with the following variables modeled as fixed effects: encoding condition (self-directed/yoked), number of visits, block index (1–6), and recency (measured using the minimum reversed serial position across presentations of an item; e.g., an item visited last in the sequence was assigned position 1, regardless of whether it was visited earlier in the sequence). The purpose of this analysis was to assess whether an effect of encoding condition was still present after controlling for the frequency and recency of study episodes, and to assess whether these factors interacted with encoding condition.

The results of the best fitting model are shown in Table 3 for all experiments. Notably, in addition to effects of recency and the number of visits, there was a significant effect of encoding condition such that yoked study decreased the likelihood of an item being recognized, consistent with the result of the paired t-test reported above. Based on the results of Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) and Voss, Warren, et al. (2011), we expected that there would be an interaction between encoding condition and the number of visits, such that a greater number of visits would lead to a larger advantage under self-directed encoding than yoked encoding. However, including this interaction did not significantly improve the fit of the model (χ2(1) = 0.62, p = .43).1

Table 3.

Results of mixed effects models.

Recognition of studied items
Experiment 1 Experiment 2 Experiment 3 Experiment 4

Variable β SE Wald z p β SE Wald z p β SE Wald z p β SE Wald z p

Intercept 1.29 0.18 7.1 < .001 .557 .184 3.0 .002 1.34 .255 5.2 < .001 −.734 .368 −2.0 .05
Condition
(yoked study)
−.512 .068 −7.5 < .001 −.342 .071 −4.8 < .001 −.367 .071 −5.2 < .001 −.343 .070 −5.0 < .001
Number of
visits
.068 .022 3.1 .002 .473 .040 11.8 < .001 .238 .080 2.9 .003 1.19 .167 7.1 < .001
Reversed
serial position
−.009 .002 −4.1 < .001 0. .002 −.04 .97 −.015 .004 −3.9 < .001 0. .003 .16 .87
Block index −.144 .021 −7.0 < .001 −.182 .021 −8.5 < .001 −.164 .024 −6.8 < .001 −.223 .021 −10.8 < .001

Spatial recall – Placement error
Experiment 1 Experiment 2 Experiment 3 Experiment 4

Variable β SE Wald z p β SE Wald z p β SE Wald z p β SE Wald z p

Intercept 1.29 .080 16.1 < .001 1.39 .117 11.9 < .001 1.11 .106 10.5 < .001 1.57 .263 6.0 < .001
Condition
(yoked study)
.144 .040 3.6 < .001 .053 .058 .90 .37 .103 .039 2.6 < .01 .040 .040 1.0 .30
Number of
visits
−.040 .013 −3.0 .003 −.009 .032 −.27 .78 .002 .046 .05 .95 −.154 .129 −1.2 .24
Reversed
serial position
.001 .001 1.1 .27 −.001 .002 −.28 .78 .012 .002 5.4 < .001 .002 .002 1.4 .16
Block index .008 .012 .65 .52 −.006 .017 −.36 .72 −.014 .013 −1.1 .29 −.027 .011 −2.3 .02

Spatial recall - Opt-outs

Analysis of spatial recall was limited to items that were studied at least once and correctly recognized by the participant. There was no difference between the proportion of opt-out responses for items from self-directed and yoked blocks (self-directed: M = .10, SD = .11; yoked: M = .12, SD = .15; signed rank test, W = 79, p = .21).

Spatial recall - Placement error

Comparisons of spatial error are summarized in Table 2 for all experiments. Spatial error was measured as the Euclidean distance between the studied location and the participant’s response, which was normalized such that an error of 1.0 corresponded to a single “grid unit" (the distance between two adjacent locations in any cardinal direction). Using this measure, the expected error from random guessing is approximately 2.5 grid units after averaging across possible locations where an item was actually studied. Note that random guessing will lead to smaller errors on average when the item appeared in the center of the grid (M = 1.8 grid units) than at the edges (e.g., M = 3.2 grid units at the corners).

Table 2.

Spatial recall results

Self-directed
placement error
(# grid units)
Yoked placement
error (# grid units)
Difference in spatial placement error
(self-directed – yoked)

Experiment N M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) t p 95% CI Cohen’s d
Exp 1
(Replication)
30 1.21 (.33) 1.33 (.29) −.13 (.25) −2.76 .01 −.22 (−.03) 0.50
Exp 2
(Attentional cueing)
30 1.35 (.49) 1.36 (.46) −.01 (.29) −0.13 .90 –– ––
Exp 3
(Follow a fixed path)
32 1.26 (.34) 1.35 (.41) −.09 (.29) −1.78 .09 –– ––
Exp 4
(Press to reveal)
30 1.24 (.37) 1.27 (.30) −.03 (.29) −0.65 .52 –– ––

Average error was relatively low for both self-directed (M = 1.21 grid units, SD = .33) and yoked (M = 1.33 grid units, SD = .29) items across subjects, and in both types of blocks error was lower than expected from random guessing (active: ℓ(29) = −22.5, p < 0.001; yoked: ℓ(29) = −22.6, p < 0.001). In addition, spatial error was significantly lower for items from self-directed blocks than yoked blocks (paired t-test, see Table 2). A linear mixed effects model was used to assess the impact of the number of visits on spatial error (the results are shown in Table 3 for all experiments. In addition to the effect of training condition there was a significant effect of the number of visits (with more visits leading to lower spatial error), but no effects of recency (measured by reversed serial position or block index). Including the interaction between condition and number of visits did not improve the fit of the model (χ2(1) = 1.9, p = .17).2

Revisitation

Our final analysis was an attempt to replicate the finding from Voss, Warren, et al. (2011) of a specific benefit of quickly revisiting items under self-directed study. We divided studied items according to whether they were part of a “revisitation” sequence of up to six items (with the longest possible sequence A-B-C-D-E-F-E-D-C-B-A, with all items considered “revisited” except for F). Note that this category does not include all items that are seen more than once, but only those that were part of a short sequence that was "doubled back" on. An average of 49% (SD = 20%) of items were part of at least one revisitation sequence (compared to an average proportion of 31% reported by Voss, Warren, et al., 2011).

We performed two-way ANOVAs with condition and revisitation as within-subjects factors. For the recognition test, in addition to the main effect of encoding condition (F(1,07) = 9.85, MSE = .19, p = .002), there was a main effect of revisitation (F(1,07) = 4.76, MSE = .09, p = 0.003), with revisited items more likely to be recognized during test. There was no condition by revisitation interaction (F(1,07) = 2.49, MSE = .05, p = .12). For the spatial recall test, there was a significant main effect of condition (F(1,06) = 7.18, MSE = .00, p = .008) but no effect of revisitation (F(1,86) = 1.26, MSE = .14, p = .26), and no interaction (F(1,86) = 1.26, MSE = .14, p = .26).

Voss, Warren, et al. (2011) reported that the effects of revisitation could not be explained by longer viewing duration or number of visits because they found both to be decreased for “revisited” items. This was not the case for our data. Average total viewing duration was significantly higher for revisited items (M = 2252 ms, SD = 332) than other items (M = 1775 ms, SD = 189; paired ℓ(29) = −7.74, p < .001). Similarly, the overall number of visits was significantly higher (M = 4.3, SD = .72) than for non-revisited items (M = 2.8, SD = .66; paired ℓ(29) = −11.1, p < .001).

Discussion

In the first experiment we successfully replicated the finding that self-directed exploration is associated with a memory advantage for both item recognition and spatial recall (Voss, Galvan, et al., 2011; Voss, Gonsalves, et al., 2011). Importantly, we found lower spatial error for items from self-directed blocks despite the changes to the procedure that made the spatial recall test contingent on successful item recognition, which may have reduced the influence of random guessing on the spatial error measure. In addition, we found that increased study was beneficial regardless of the encoding condition. Importantly, we did not find that self-directed study was disproportionately advantageous for items that were studied more or that were a part of “revisitation" sequences, suggesting that the interactions reported by Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) and Voss, Warren, et al. (2011) depended on features of the task design that were altered here (e.g., restricting the window movement to cardinal directions rather than unconstrained movement throughout the display).

Despite some differences, the finding that self-directed learning enhanced recognition memory and spatial memory appears robust even with the minor changes in procedure we introduced. However, these results still leave the open question of why self-directed study is better than yoked study. Having successfully established the viability of the modified spatial exploration paradigm in Experiment 1, in each of the following experiments we introduce a manipulation that reduces the gap between self-directed and yoked study conditions.

Experiment 2: Cueing to guide attention

Since self-directed study allowed people to choose the timing and location of each study opportunity in Experiment 1, they may have been better at coordinating their attention with new episodes than during yoked study. For example, by deciding where to move the window next, self-directed learners may be at an advantage in allocating endogenous attention to each new stimulus (Carrasco, 2011). In contrast, during yoked study there is uncertainty about when and where the window will move. If the yoked learner incorrectly predicts the next study location or duration of the current episode, this may incur a cost to re-allocating spatial attention to the next item.

The goal of Experiment 2 was to reduce the difference between conditions in the ability to coordinate attention with the movement of the window, while maintaining a similar level of self-directed control over the study sequence. We modified the procedure such that, before the window moved to a new location, that position was indicated with a visual cue which lasted 600 ms. (for comparison, it takes about 250–300 ms to deploy endogenous attention to a cued target, Carrasco, 2011; Posner, 1980). Cues were present in both self-directed and yoked study. Whereas participants likely found the cue redundant during self-directed blocks, during yoked blocks participants could use it to guide their attention to the upcoming stimulus. Since the cue was deterministically related to the movement of the window, it also removed some of the uncertainty associated with the yoked condition. The key question was whether memory for items studied under self-directed conditions would still be better than yoked items given this attentional aid.

Participants

Thirty-one NYU undergraduates participated for course credit or a $10 payment, including one seed participant whose data were not analyzed.

Procedure

Most aspects of the procedure were identical to that of Experiment 1. The only change was the addition of a cue (a black outline) that appeared when the window was moved to a new location, for both self-directed and yoked blocks. As in Experiment 1, each item presentation ended when the participant decided to move the window (self-directed blocks) or the duration matched that of the previous participant (yoked blocks). At that point, the window momentarily disappeared and a black outline was displayed around the next study location for 600 ms, followed by the appearance of the window in that new location (revealing the item hidden there). This cueing procedure was identical for both self-directed and yoked blocks.

Results

Study sequences

Since the duration of each study block was kept to 1 minute, the inclusion of a 600 ms cue before each item led to a smaller proportion of time in which a stimulus was present. As a result, we expected that participants would view each item a smaller number of times than in Experiment 1 and that a greater number of items would never be visited. An average of 2.6 self-directed items (out of 75) were never visited (SD = 5.2). For items that were seen at least once, the average number of visits was 1.9 (SD = 0.29) for an average median study duration of 484 ms per visit (SD = 169) and an average cumulative study time per item of 1179 ms (SD = 184). Thus, people in this experiment tended to dwell on individual items for a similar length of time as in Experiment 1 but made fewer visits overall.

Recognition

A greater proportion of studied items were recognized from self-directed blocks than from yoked blocks (see Table 1). The average within-subject difference in hit rates between self-directed and yoked blocks was .07 (SD = .13), which was not significantly different from the average difference in Experiment 1 (ℓ(58) = 1.2, p = .26).

We repeated the logistic regression analysis described in Experiment 1, which indicated significant effects of encoding condition, number of visits, and block index (but not reversed serial position). There was a marginal improvement to the model when including the interaction between encoding condition and the number of visits (χ2(1) = .2, p = .07), but it was such that more frequent visits had a larger positive effect on the probability of recognizing items from yoked blocks compared to self-directed blocks.

Spatial recall - Opt-outs

Participants chose to opt-out of the spatial recall for the same proportion of self-directed and yoked items (self-directed: M = .51, SD = .32; yoked: M = .52, SD = .29; signed rank test, W = 192, p = .59).

Spatial recall - Placement error

There was no difference in placement error between conditions (see Table 2). The regression analysis revealed no significant effects of encoding condition, number of visits, or recency on placement error.

Discussion

Since a self-directed learner is aware of the “next step" in the study sequence as soon as they have decided what to view next, we hypothesized that they can allocate their attention to the next item more efficiently than a yoked observer of the same material. In Experiment 2 we introduced a cueing procedure in order to minimize this attentional lag, giving yoked learners ample opportunity to direct their attention to the next location.

The results show that cueing did not eliminate the advantage for self-directed study in terms of recognition memory. Note that an important consequence of the cueing procedure was that participants had less study time overall than in Experiment 1. As a result, a slightly greater proportion of items were never visited during study and the cumulative amount of time devoted to each item was lower than in Experiment 1. Nevertheless, recognition performance on studied items was comparable to that of Experiment 1 (M = .71 in both experiments), with yoked hit rates slightly increased (M = .64 compared to M = .61 in Exp 1).

Unlike Experiment 1, we did not find evidence for an advantage in spatial recall for self-directed items, which is notable given that self-directed study still required making exploratory decisions about where to move the window. For those items where participants made a spatial response, overall spatial error was similar to that of Experiment 1. However, participants chose to opt-out of making a spatial response with much higher frequency, suggesting that the cueing manipulation did impair spatial encoding. This pattern of results suggests that the spatial memory benefit for self-directed study may be driven by differences in the ability to orient to relevant locations in space, while another possibility is that cueing may have made spatial position less relevant by reducing the uncertainty associated with the window movements. However, the self-directed memory advantage for item recognition advantage was robust to attentional cueing, indicating that a simple attentional account does not fully explain the benefit of self-directed study.

Experiment 3: Following a fixed path

The spatial exploration paradigm used by Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) was designed to parallel research on exploration and learning in rodents (Ellen, et al., 1982; O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978) and humans (Doeller, Barry, & Burgess, 2010; Doeller & Burgess, 2008). For example, rats placed in a novel environment will tend to explore vigorously, but this behavior will decline over time as the environment becomes more familiar (Save, et al., 1992). In the same way, self-directed learners may devote more effort to items that are unfamiliar, adaptively allocating attention in order to maximize learning (Metcalfe, 2002; Renner, 1990).

Self-directed study in Experiment 1 and 2 provided control over both the content of each encoding episode (i.e., what item to look at next) as well as its timing (i.e., how long to study the current item). In the next experiment, we restricted the self-directed condition by removing the ability to decide what items to look at, while preserving control over the duration of each study episode. If the self-directed advantage depends on adaptively selecting items to study, removing this ability should abolish any such memory advantage.

In this experiment, rather than choosing which item to study, self-directed participants simply decided when to move the window in a fixed “snaking" path across the grid. This task is similar to the “manual deterministic" control condition that Voss, Gonslaves, et al. (2011) reported (Exp. 2) but differs in a key way: in our experiment, while participants could not decide which items to visit, they could decide how long to study a item and when to move to the next item (rather than being externally cued). Thus, they retained some control over the content of study by deciding how long to view each item.

Participants

Thirty-three NYU undergraduates participated for course credit, including one seed participant whose data were not analyzed. Due to an error during data collection, two participants were yoked to the same previous participant.

Procedure

The sole difference from the previous experiment was that during self-directed encoding the participant could not control the sequence of items. The window followed a fixed, “snaking” path through the grid. During self-directed encoding, the participant simply pressed the spacebar to initiate movement to the next location. As a result, they could control how long to dwell on each item but not where to move next. When all items in a grid were visited, the study sequence was reversed such that the window “double-backed" on previously visited items.

Results

Study behavior

Since participants did not choose which items to visit, study opportunities were more evenly distributed in this experiment, with most items visited 1–2 times (and no items receiving more than 3 visits). Twenty-two participants visited every item at least once during self-directed blocks. Within the remaining 10 people, an average of 14.7 items were never visited across all blocks (SD = 9.7). For items that were seen at least once, the average number of visits was 1.7 (SD = 0.34) for an average median study duration of 839 ms (SD = 644) and an average cumulative study time per item of 1582 ms (SD = 553).

Recognition

The false alarm rate was similar to that of the previous experiments (M = .12, SD = .09). Of those items that were visited at least once, a greater proportion of items were recognized from self-directed blocks (see Table 1). The results of the logistic regression were similar to that of Experiment 1, with significant effects of encoding condition, number of visits, block index, and reversed serial position. The interaction between condition and number of visits was not significant (χ2(1) = 0.29, p = .59).

Spatial recall - Opt-outs

There was no difference in the proportion of items for which participants opted out of the spatial test between self-directed and yoked blocks (self-directed: M = .11, SD = .14; yoked: M = .12, SD = .15; signed rank test, W = 100, p = .16).

Spatial recall - Placement error

As in the previous experiments, spatial error was relatively low for both self-directed (M = 1.26, SD = .34) and yoked (M = 1.35, SD = .41) items. There was no difference in overall error for self-directed items relative to yoked items (see Table 2). A linear mixed effects model revealed a significant effect of condition in the same direction as well as a significant effect of recency as measured by reversed serial position.

Discussion

Removing control over the order of the study sequence did not disrupt the memory advantage in the self-directed condition, suggesting that, at least in this paradigm, the benefit of self-direction does not depend on an adaptive selection process through which items are chosen based on the learner’s existing memory. In this experiment the sequence of items was determined independently of how well each item had been learned, as people only controlled the length of time to study each item during self-directed blocks. Of course, the ability to select information based on one’s uncertainty undoubtedly benefits performance in a variety of other learning situations (Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006; Markant & Gureckis, 2014; Metcalfe, 2002, 2009). However, our results so far point to a distinct and additional benefit from controlling the timing of study.

It is interesting to compare the results of this experiment to the “deterministic" control condition reported by Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011, Exp. 2). Like the present design, Voss, Gonsalves, et al. removed the ability of participants to select which items to view. However, they also removed their ability to decide how long to study each item by using an auditory cue to schedule when to advance to the next item. Their results showed no self-directed advantage for either test measure. Compared to that design, participants in our experiment still had control over at least two factors: how long to study each item, and when to begin studying the next item. Thus, in the Experiment 4 we went one step further by removing study duration as an element of adaptive control.

Experiment 4: Controlling the timing of study episodes

In this experiment we sought to create a “minimal" self-directed condition in which the learner could only control when to reveal an item without controlling which item to look at or how long to dwell on the item. As in the previous experiment, the window followed a fixed path through the grid, but now each stimulus appeared the same number of times and for a fixed duration. During self-directed blocks, the participant only determined when to start viewing the next item (i.e., the duration of the interval between study episodes). As a result, the content of each study episode, including the time spent studying it, was entirely independent of the learner’s current memory.

Participants

Thirty-two NYU undergraduates participated for course credit, including 1 seed participant whose data were not analyzed.

Procedure

Most details of the procedure were identical to the previous experiment. The number of item presentations was fixed such that each block consisted of two runs through the “snaking" pattern (after one run was complete, the sequence was then visited in the reverse order). Each stimulus was presented for a fixed duration of 750 ms. As a result, item exposures were matched across all participants for number of visits and study duration.

During self-directed blocks the participant controlled the onset of the next item. On each trial, a cue appeared in the new study location. The participant then pressed the spacebar to advance the trial, after which the cue changed color and remained for 300 ms, followed by the item presentation. During yoked blocks, the inter-stimulus intervals from the previous participant’s self-directed blocks were used.

Results

One participant was excluded from analysis because they failed to respond during the test phase.

Encoding response times

During self-directed blocks, the delay before the onset of each item was determined by the participant’s response time. The average median response time was 572 ms (SD = 445 ms). Since the number of visits was fixed in this experiment, the overall duration of each study block depended on participants’ response times. The overall study duration was somewhat longer than in the previous experiments (M = 92 s, SD = 27 s) but the total amount of time that items were viewed was similar to the previous experiments.

Recognition

A greater proportion of old items were recognized from self-directed study blocks than from yoked blocks (see Table 1). Regression analysis revealed effects of condition, number of visits, and block index (see Table 3) similar to those of previous experiments.3

Spatial recall

Participants opted out of a spatial recall response on a similar proportion of recognized items between conditions (self-directed: M = .15, SD = .12; yoked: M = .16, SD = .13; signed rank test W = 183, p = .46). For those items that participants made a spatial response there was no difference in spatial error across conditions (see Table 2), a result which was confirmed by mixed effects modeling (Table 3).

Pre- and post-item ISIs

Control over the timing of new study episodes could be associated with at least two processes. First, it might allow participants to rehearse the previous item before studying the next item. If so, the duration of the ISI following an item (post-ISI) should be related to successful memory, with longer durations allowing for more rehearsal. Second, control over the ISI might allow learners to postpone the presentation of the next item until they are prepared to study. In this case, the duration of the ISI preceding an item (pre-ISI) might predict successful memory.

We tested whether pre- or post-ISIs were related to later recognition through mixed effects logistic regression. For each studied item we measured the total duration (summed across presentations) of the pre- and post-ISI (the last item of each sequence was excluded for the purposes of this analysis because it did not have a post-ISI). In addition to the set of predictors tested earlier (see Table 3), we tested whether the addition of pre-ISI or post-ISI as fixed effects improved the fit of the model. There was a significant effect of total pre-ISI (χ2(1) = 6.8, p < .01), with longer pre-ISIs associated with a higher likelihood of correctly recognizing the item. However, there was no effect of post-ISI (χ2(1) = 3.6, p = .06), indicating that the amount of time following an item was not associated with it being successfully recognized. There was no interaction between condition and pre-ISI (χ2(1) = 1.1, p = 30), and there were no effects of either ISI measure on spatial error.

Discussion

The results from this experiment were remarkably consistent with the previous two experiments, despite the minimal control afforded during self-directed study and the procedural change in the number of visits. Participants could only control when to reveal the next item, without controlling its on-screen duration or content. This suggests that the ability to coordinate the onset of new study episodes with one’s own preparatory or motivational state is sufficient to produce a self-directed advantage for recognition memory. Our analysis of pre- and post-item intervals showed that the amount of time following an item did not affect whether it was recognized, suggesting that delaying the next study episode was not related to rehearsing the previous item. Instead, increased time to prepare for the next item presentation seems to be related to later recognition, consistent with an important role for attentional coordination in this paradigm.

General Discussion

In the present paper we “deconstructed” the self-directed memory advantage and found that simply being able to control the timing of study led to recognition memory advantages. After replicating the basic advantage in Experiment 1, we found that controlling for attentional differences between conditions slightly improved recognition of items from yoked study but eliminated any differences in spatial memory (Experiment 2). In subsequent experiments, the self-directed advantage was preserved after removing learners’ ability to decide which items to study (Experiment 3) and how long to study each item (Experiment 4).

Across all four experiments, we found surprisingly consistent evidence of a recognition memory advantage for self-directed study. Similar patterns have recently been observed in a range of tasks including object identification (Craddock, Martinovic, & Lawson, 2011), memory for 3D faces or objects (Harman et al., 1999; Liu et al., 2007; Meijer & Van der Lubbe, 2011), and spatial learning (Chrastil & Warren, 2012; Luursema & Verwey, 2011; Plancher et al., 2012). However, the mechanism behind these effects has been unclear because self-directed conditions generally differ from passive observation in a variety of ways, each of which could potentially influence memory. Like other examples of goal-directed behavior, self-directed study entails a hierarchy of cognitive control processes (Botvinick, 2008). The present study highlighted the distinction between decisions about the content of study (a higher-level process) and coordination of the study experience with attention (a lower-level process). At the higher level, people may select information so as to maximize the number of items that can be memorized (e.g., preferring to study easy items first; Metcalfe, 2002) or to focus on items for which their existing memory is poor (Metcalfe & Finn, 2008). At the lower level, each study episode may require decisions about its onset and duration, and memory is tied to attentional and motivational processes involved in the execution of those decisions (Chun & Turk-Browne, 2007). Our results suggest that this kind of low-level control is sufficient to enhance recognition memory for visual objects.

Although we found a consistent advantage in terms of recognition memory, the effect on spatial recall was much less clear. In Experiment 1 we found improved spatial memory from self-directed study (replicating the finding from Voss, Gonsalves, et al., 2011). In subsequent experiments in which study sequences were more constrained (due to precuing or fixed search paths) and thus the orientation of attention was better matched between conditions, the same effect was either absent or inconsistent across different analyses. It is possible that reducing self-directed learners’ control over window movements led to less processing of spatial information; however, it is important to note that the need to make spatial decisions in Experiment 2 did not lead to any differences in spatial recall. Overall, our results add to evidence that a self-directed advantage for spatial memory is relatively inconsistent across different tasks (Chrastil & Warren, 2012), and suggest that it may closely depend on the nature of exploration in a given environment.

Our results also differed from those of Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) and Voss, Warren, et al. (2011) in that we did not find a larger self-directed advantage for items studied more often (Exp. 1–3) or that were quickly revisited (Exp. 1). Instead, additional study led to better memory regardless of encoding condition. One explanation for this discrepancy is that it was easier for yoked learners in our task to coordinate attention with the study sequence, and as a result they also benefited from more exposure to an item. A selective benefit for self-directed study might occur in situations where cues are not available to help yoked observers in the same way, but our findings suggest that such a pattern results from differences in coordination of attention rather than arising from a decision-making process that is only present during self-directed study.

What causes the recognition advantage for self-directed study?

We found that the self-directed advantage was present when control was limited to choosing when to reveal the next item, suggesting that during self-directed blocks participants were better able to coordinate new presentations with their own preparatory state. There is increasing evidence that pre-stimulus neural activity can predict subsequent memory (Guderian, Schott, Richardson-Klavehn, & Düzel, 2009; Otten, Quayle, Akram, Ditewig, & Rugg, 2006; Yoo et al., 2011), and that this activity is modulated by motivational factors including the anticipation of reward (Gruber & Otten, 2010). There are a number of ways in which self-directed learning might interact with these processes. One possibility is that the learners monitor ongoing fluctuations in their internal state (e.g., due to distraction or mind-wandering) and can schedule new episodes in an adaptive manner. Alternatively, decisions to begin a new study episode might play a causal role in initiating those attentional or mnemonic processes.

One objection may be that during yoked blocks participants were not required to make a response, raising the possibility that the advantages were related to executing motor responses. However, we think it is unlikely that the motor component can account for the memory enhancement. Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011, Experiment 2) compared yoked observation with a “manual” condition that required key presses in response to an external cue in order to move the window along a predefined path. They found no advantage for this condition, with recognition performance similar to that of participants in our yoked condition. Other comparisons of self-directed and yoked study that included a secondary task in order to control attention and motor activity across conditions have also found advantages for active control (Liu et al., 2007; Meijer & Van der Luube, 2011).

It would be hard to argue against the hypothesis that selecting content during learning can influence later memory. For example, in richer contexts where materials vary more in their difficulty (e.g., studying a textbook), an advantage from strategic selection of information would be expected to play a larger role. It is likely that participants in Experiment 1 and 2 were engaged in strategic decision-making about how to navigate the array or making judgments about which items required further study. Collectively, however, our experiments showed that the self-directed advantage was maintained after removing elements of control that would allow the learner to make memory-based decisions about what to study and for how long.

Deconstructing self-directed study using yoked designs

The purpose of “yoked" experimental designs is to equate the content of study while isolating the impact of decision-making on performance. As we have demonstrated, comparing active exploration and passive observation with this method requires careful consideration of the many ways in which these conditions differ, particularly when the outcome of interest is subsequent memory. Although our experiments were based on the design of Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011), there are a number of other examples of yoked designs that suffer from the same confound that we have described. For example, Meijer & Van der Luube (2011) used a task where the goal was to memorize a set of 3D objects. During self-directed study the learner interacted with an object by rotating it with a mouse, whereas in yoked study they passively observed the interaction of another participant. The results showed a consistent benefit for self-directed study in terms of recognition, but it is unclear if this advantage is related to low-level processes related to the interaction or higher-level metacognitive control (e.g., exploring parts of the object that were poorly encoded).

Rather than treating self-directed study as a unitary process, a more productive approach may be to decompose it into a hierarchy of control processes. A yoked design may be useful for testing the effects of individual decision-making processes while ensuring that the “passive" condition also experiences the outcomes of those decisions, but it is important that the influence of other forms of adaptive control is controlled for in the design. For example, the effect of high-level decision-making about the content of study may be best studied in paradigms where self-directed learners cannot control the dynamics of individual study episodes (for example, the honor/dishonor paradigm, in which the decision to study something is separated from the actual study opportunity; Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006).

Of course, comparing fully self-directed study with passive observation can reveal the magnitude of an advantage in a given learning problem, which may be especially relevant for educational contexts in which the most common format may be passive observation (e.g., viewing lectures). Our results add further insight to these comparisons, however, by revealing the extent to which different forms of “active learning" lead to differences in performance. Whereas people may be biased in how they make high-level decisions about how to sequence study episodes (Bjork et al., 2013), our results show that simply allowing people to control the temporal dynamics of study episodes may have widespread benefits for learning and memory.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Patricia Chan, Hao Wang, and Devin Domingo for their help collecting data. We also thank Joel Voss for sharing the stimuli used in the experiments.

Footnotes

1

All model comparisons were performed using likelihood-ratio tests.

2

A measure that is similar to the total number of visits is the total amount of time spent studying an item, which Voss, Gonsalves, et al. (2011) found interacted with encoding condition such that items studied for longer durations led to a specific benefit under self-directed conditions. The same analysis of our data revealed an effect of duration for both recognition and spatial recall, but no such interactions with encoding condition. Since study duration is closely related to number of visits this analysis is not reported here, but the results are available on request.

3

Note that in Exp. 4 the number of visits was not equal to 2 for all items studied, since items at the midpoint of each sequence (at which point the window doubled-back) were only visited once. The effect of number of visits is thus dependent on a relatively small number of items; importantly, removing this explanatory variable from the model does not alter any conclusions from this analysis.

Contributor Information

Douglas Markant, Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin.

Sarah DuBrow, Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY.

Lila Davachi, Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY; Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY.

Todd M. Gureckis, Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY

References

  1. Bjork RA, Dunlosky J, Kornell N. Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology. 2013;64:417–444. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Botvinick MM. Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12(5):201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Carrasco M. Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research. 2011;51(13):1484–1525. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cepeda N, Pashler H, Vul E, Wixted J, Rohrer D. Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:354–380. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chrastil E, Warren W. Active and passive contributions to spatial learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2012;19:1–23. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0182-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chun MM, Turk-Browne NB. Interactions between attention and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17(2):177–184. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Craddock M, Martinovic J, Lawson R. An advantage for active versus passive aperture-viewing in visual object recognition. Perception. 2011;40:1154–1163. doi: 10.1068/p6974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dempster F. The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist. 1988;43:627–634. [Google Scholar]
  9. Doeller C, Barry C, Burgess N. Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature. 2010;463:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nature08704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Doeller C, Burgess N. Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105:5909–5914. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711433105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ellen P, Parko E, Wages C, Doherty D, Herrmann T. Spatial problems solving by rats: Exploration and cognitive maps. Learning and Motivation. 1982;13:81–94. [Google Scholar]
  12. Gruber M, Otten L. Voluntary control over prestimulus activity related to encoding. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(29):9793–9800. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0915-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Guderian S, Schott B, Richardson-Klavehn A, Düzel E. Medial temporal theta state before an event predicts episodic encoding success in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106(13):5365. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900289106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gureckis T, Markant D. Self-directed learning: A cognitive and computational perspective. Perspectives in Psychological Science. 2012;7:464–481. doi: 10.1177/1745691612454304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Harman KL, Keith Humphrey G, Goodale MA. Active manual control of object views facilitates visual recognition. Current Biology. 1999;9(22):1315–1318. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(00)80053-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kornell N, Bjork R. The promise and perils of self-regulated study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2007;14(2):219–224. doi: 10.3758/bf03194055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kornell N, Metcalfe J. Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2006;32(3):609–622. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Liu CH, Ward J, Markall H. The role of active exploration of 3D face stimuli on recognition memory of facial information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2007;33(4):895. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Luursema JM, Verwey WB. The contribution of dynamic exploration to virtual anatomical learning. Advances in Human-Computer Interaction. 2011;2011(7):1–6. [Google Scholar]
  20. Markant D, Gureckis TM. Is it better to select or to receive? Learning via active and passive hypothesis testing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2014;143(1):94–122. doi: 10.1037/a0032108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Meijer F, Van der Lubbe RH. Active exploration improves perceptual sensitivity for virtual 3D objects in visual memory. Vision Research. 2011;51:2431–2439. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Metcalfe J. Is study time allocated selectively to a region of proximal learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2002;131(3):349–363. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.131.3.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Metcalfe J. Metacognitive judgments and control of study. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2009;18(3):159–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01628.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Metcalfe J, Finn B. Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15(1):174–179. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Metcalfe J, Kornell N. The dynamics of learning and allocation of study time to a region of proximal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2003;132(4):530–542. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Nelson T, Narens L. Why investigate metacognition? In: Metcalfe J, Shimamura AP, editors. Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. MIT Press; 1994. pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  27. O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  28. Otten LJ, Quayle AH, Akram S, Ditewig TA, Rugg MD. Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9(4):489–491. doi: 10.1038/nn1663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Plancher G, Barra J, Orriols E, Piolino P. The influence of action on episodic memory: A virtual reality study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2013;66(5):895–909. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.722657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Posner M. Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1980;32:3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Renner M. Neglected aspects of exploratory and investigatory behavior. Psychobiology. 1990;18(1):16–22. [Google Scholar]
  32. Rhodes MG, Jacoby LL. On the dynamic nature of response criterion in recognition memory: Effects of base rate, awareness, and feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2007;33(2):305–320. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Save E, Buhot M, Foreman N, Thinus-Blanc C. Exploratory activity and response to a spatial change in rats with hippocampal or posterior parietal cortical lesions. Behavioural Brain Research. 1992;47:113–127. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80118-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Simon D, Bjork R. Metacognition in motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27(4):907–912. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Son L, Metcalfe J. Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2000;26(1):204. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.1.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Voss J, Galvan A, Gonsalves B. Cortical regions recruited for complex active-learning strategies and action planning exhibit rapid reactivation during memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 49. 2011:3956–3966. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Voss J, Gonsalves B, Federmeier K, Tranel D, Cohen N. Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitional exploratory behavior enhances learning. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14(1):115–120. doi: 10.1038/nn.2693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Voss J, Warren D, Gonsalves B, Federmeier K, Tranel D, Cohen N. Spontaneous revisitation during visual exploration as a link among strategic behavior, learning, and the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(31):E402–E409. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100225108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Yoo J, Hinds O, Ofen N, Thompson T, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Triantafyllou C, Gabrieli J. When the brain is prepared to learn: Enhancing human learning using real-time fMRI. Neuroimage. 2011;59:846–852. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES