Abstract
Aim
The aim of this study was to investigate the direct relationship of sleep schedule and sleep quality variables between healthy preschool children and their parents, focusing on the influence of the difference in bedtime between each other.
Methods
Forty-seven Japanese 5-year-old children and their primary parent were studied. The parents completed questionnaires including the Epworth Sleepiness Scale and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. The children wore an actigraph for one week.
Results
Although sleep patterns of children were generally independent of their parents, late sleep end time and bedtime of children were associated with parents' late sleep end time on weekends. For 87% of children and parents who shared a bedroom, sleep quality was negatively affected by a shorter difference in bedtimes between child and parent, but not by co-sleeping.
Conclusion
Sleep behaviours of parents can influence those of their children. For parents and children who share a bedroom, the timing of bedtime rather than co-sleeping may be a key factor in modulating sleep patterns. Trying to get children asleep and subsequently falling asleep at a similar time may disturb parents' sleep quality, which may subsequently affect that of their children.
Keywords: Actigraphy, Co-sleeping, Parent–child interaction, Preschool children, Sleep pattern
Key notes
Sleep patterns of preschool children can be influenced by sleep behaviours of their parents.
For Japanese parents and children who share a bedroom, the timing of bedtime rather than co-sleeping appeared to be an important determining factor of sleep quality.
Falling asleep at a similar time as their children may disturb parents#x2019; sleep quality, which may subsequently influence that of their children.
Introduction
Sleep patterns and sleep behaviours of children are influenced by a number of factors, including biological, cultural, social and family backgrounds. Despite increasing evidence suggesting the pivotal influence of parental and environmental factors 1,2, only a few studies have specifically examined the direct association between sleep patterns in healthy, typically developing children and their parents. One study that recruited children with a wide age range (3 years of age on average) observed the synchrony of maternal chronotype and children's sleep patterns, suggesting the interplay of sleep habits between mothers and young children 3. In contrast, another study that compared sleep schedule variables between children in their early teens and their parents demonstrated a lack of reciprocal relationships 4. These conflicting findings might be attributed to the difference in children's age and subsequent alteration in sleep arrangements.
Of several environmental factors, the influence of co-sleeping has been widely recognized. Co-sleeping has been linked with a range of sleep problems such as frequent awakenings and difficulty in getting back to sleep thereafter 5–8. However, previous studies have not fully addressed differences in co-sleeping arrangements, such as the type and size of the bed and bedroom or parental proximity and body contact as the children fall asleep. These factors may directly affect the interplay of sleep habits between children and their parents.
In addition to environmental factors, cross-cultural backgrounds deserve consideration when assessing the influence of co-sleeping on sleep quantity and quality. A comparative study of co-sleeping and solitary sleeping in young children in Japan and the United States demonstrated that co-sleeping was associated with overall stressful sleep problems in the United States sample but not in the Japanese sample 9. This suggests the possibility that the adverse influence of co-sleeping appears mainly in conjunction with certain types of sleep arrangements and/or sleep habits. In Asian countries, co-sleeping occurs as a natural extension of bedroom sharing. For example, Japanese traditional beds, namely futons, are arranged on the floor in rows without clear boundaries between family members. Therefore, it is possible that the time when parents fall asleep (i.e. at the same time as their children or at a different time) can be a more important independent variable of their sleep patterns rather than the difference between co-sleeping and mere bedroom sharing.
To investigate the direct relationship of sleep schedule and sleep quality variables between Japanese preschool children and their parents, we performed a study in a cohort of healthy, typically developing Japanese preschool children and their parents, focusing on the influence of the difference in bedtime between the two.
Method
This study was conducted according to the guidance and approval of the ethics committee of Kurume University School of Medicine, and parental informed consent was obtained.
We used the same data set as our previous studies that investigated the sleep habits of healthy five-year-old children using actigraphy 10,11. These studies demonstrated the dependence of sleep patterns of young children on both intrinsic and extrinsic variables, such as gender, childcare and other parental preferences.
Participants
Details of the study population and the data acquisition protocol have been described elsewhere 10–12. In brief, the study population comprised 47 5-year-old children (27 males and 20 females) and their parents, who were recruited from two day-care nurseries in the city of Kurume.
Data collection
Participating children wore an actigraph watch on their wrist from a Friday afternoon to the next Friday afternoon (Ambulatory Monitoring, Ardsley, NY, USA). Primary parents, who spent more time with the child around night time (46 mothers and one father), submitted the child's daily sleep log for the corresponding period for actigraphic validation. The Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) and the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) were used to assess daytime sleepiness and global sleep quality/disturbances for parents, respectively. The ESS scores >10 and PSQI scores >5 were regarded as indicating excessive daytime sleepiness and poor sleep, respectively 13,14. Because the PSQI is likely to reflect the information during weekdays 12, parents were asked to report their sleep–wake patterns on the weekend. Finally, the parents completed brief questionnaires enquiring about their sleep satisfaction, the presence of sleep problems in their child and the sleep arrangement for their child.
Data analysis
Actigraphic data of the children were analysed to provide the bedtime, sleep onset time, sleep end time, sleep period, sleep latency and sleep efficiency (fraction of the sleep period excluding waking after sleep onset relative to the sleep period) using an established algorithm 15. Because sleep variables of young children are affected by nursery attendance and weekday–weekend difference 10, variables were assessed for weekdays and the weekend, respectively, and the weekend was defined as a nursery off day (14 participants attended the nurseries on Saturdays; see reference 12 for detail). Pearson's product–moment correlations and Spearman's rank correlations were used to assess the correlations in sleep variables between the children and parents.
To assess the influence of sleep arrangements on sleep pattern, participants were grouped into four categories according to the sleep arrangement of the children: (i) solitary sleeping (solitary sleeping group), (ii) sharing bedroom with family members other than parents (separate bedroom group), and (iii) sharing bedroom with parents (bedroom sharing group). The last group was further divided into co-sleeping participants (co-sleeping subgroup) and others (non co-sleeping subgroup). Sleep variables were compared between groups using an analysis of variance and Fisher's least significant difference test. Finally, in the bedroom sharing group, the difference in bedtime between children and parents (ΔBT) was calculated as the parent's bedtime deducted from that of the child, which was compared with sleep variables using Pearson's product–moment correlations and Spearman's rank correlations.
Because of the exploratory nature of the study, p-values are shown without correcting for multiple comparisons.
Results
All children completed the actigraphic data collection. The mean age of the parents was 35.4 years (range: 26–46 years), and none was engaged in a night job. Demographic data of sleep variables for the children and their parents are shown in the Table1. Sixty-six percentage of parents were satisfied with their sleep, whereas 13% and 34% showed abnormally high ESS (>10) and PSQI (>5) scores, respectively. Thirty-six percentage of children were considered by their parent to have sleep problems such as long sleep latencies (n = 10), unfixed sleeping places (n = 3), irregular sleep patterns (n = 2) and night awakenings (n = 2).
Table 1.
Sleep variables | Parents | Children | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Weekday | ||||
Bedtime | (h) | 23.1 (1.3) | 21.5 (0.6) | <0.001 |
Sleep end time | (h) | 6.4 (0.9) | 7.1 (0.5) | <0.001 |
Sleep period | (h) | 7.0 (1.2) | 9.5 (0.5) | <0.001 |
Weekend | ||||
Bedtime | (h) | 23.5 (1.3) | 22.0 (0.9)* | <0.001 |
Sleep end time | (h) | 7.6 (1.7)* | 7.4 (0.7)* | ns |
Sleep period | (h) | 8.1 (1.3)* | 9.3 (0.8) | <0.001 |
Sleep latency | (min) | 16.6 (13.8) | 8.8 (4.5) | <0.001 |
Sleep efficiency | (%) | – | 90.3 (5.6) | – |
Sleep problems identified by parents | – | 17 (36)† | – | |
ESS score | 3.9 (5.0) | – | – | |
ESS score >10 | 6 (13)† | – | – | |
PSQI score | 4.6 (2.2) | – | – | |
PSQI score >5 | 16 (34)† | – | – | |
Sleep satisfaction | 31 (66)† | – | – |
For parents, the sleep end time was later and the sleep period was longer on the weekend than on weekdays. Children's sleep schedules were later on the weekend than on weekdays; no difference in the sleep period was noted. Parents had a later bedtime, an earlier sleep end time and a shorter sleep period than their children on weekdays.
p values are from the analysis of variance between parents and children.
Significant difference between weekdays and weekends.
Data are shown as mean (standard deviation) or number of incidences (%).
Sleep variables were obtained differently for the children (actigraphy) and the parents (questionnaires).
ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale. PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. ns, not significant.
Relationship of sleep variables between children and parents
A late sleep end time of parents on the weekend was associated with a late sleep end time on weekdays and the weekend (p < 0.01 and 0.05, respectively) and a late bedtime on weekdays (p < 0.05) of the children (Table2). However, bedtime and the sleep period were not associated between the children and parents during the week. Low sleep efficiency of the children on weekdays was associated with an early bedtime (p < 0.05) and a long sleep period (p < 0.01) of the parents on weekdays. In contrast, abnormally high ESS scores of parents were associated with children's early bedtime on the weekend and high sleep efficiency on weekdays (both p < 0.05).
Table 2.
Child's sleep variable | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bedtime | Sleep end time | Sleep period | Sleep efficiency | ||||||
Parental sleep variables | Weekday | Weekend | Weekday | Weekend | Weekday | Weekend | Weekday | Weekend | |
Bedtime | Weekday | 0.10 | −0.02 | 0.20 | 0.23 | 0.05 | 0.25 | 0.37* | 0.17 |
Weekend | 0.15 | 0.27 | 0.20 | 0.16 | −0.03 | −0.16 | 0.11 | 0.18 | |
Sleep end time | Weekday | 0.15 | 0.10 | 0.39** | 0.18 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 0.03 | −0.01 |
Weekend | 0.34* | 0.28 | 0.48** | 0.38* | −0.02 | 0.03 | 0.15 | 0.07 | |
Sleep period | Weekday | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.03 | −0.18 | 0.00 | −0.23 | −0.40** | −0.35* |
Weekend | 0.28 | 0.07 | 0.41** | 0.30 | 0.02 | 0.19 | 0.08 | −0.09 | |
ESS score >10 | −0.17 | −0.32* | −0.06 | −0.21 | 0.15 | 0.18 | 0.33* | 0.20 | |
PSQI score >5 | −0.11 | −0.10 | −0.12 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.12 | 0.10 | −0.03 | |
Sleep satisfaction | 0.06 | −0.03 | 0.20 | −0.04 | 0.16 | −0.02 | −0.31* | −0.26 |
*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01, from Pearson's product–moment correlations or Spearman's rank order correlations.
ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.
Influence of sleep arrangement on sleep variables
Three children did not have a regular sleeping place and were excluded from further analysis because of their highly heterogeneous backgrounds. No child was assigned to the solitary sleeping group, whereas 3 and 41 children were assigned to the separate bedroom group (all children shared a bedroom with their siblings) and bedroom sharing group, respectively. Twenty-three children in the bedroom sharing group were co-sleeping (co-sleeping subgroup) and 18 were not (non co-sleeping subgroup) (Table3).
Table 3.
Parents | Children | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Separate bedroom group (n = 3) | Bedroom sharing group | Separate bedroom group (n = 3) | Bedroom sharing group | ||||
Sleep variables | Non co-sleeping (n = 18) | Co-sleeping (n = 23) | Non co-sleeping (n = 18) | Co-sleeping (n = 23) | |||
Weekday bedtime | (h) | 25.2 (1.4) | 23.1 (1.3)* | 22.9 (1.1)* | 21.4 (0.8) | 21.4 (0.5) | 21.7 (0.6) |
Weekday sleep end time | (h) | 7.2 (0.8) | 6.1 (1.1) | 6.5 (0.6) | 7.3 (0.9) | 7.0 (0.4) | 7.2 (0.5) |
Weekday sleep period | (h) | 6.3 (0.6) | 6.8 (1.2) | 7.1 (1.0) | 9.9 (0.5) | 9.5 (0.5) | 9.4 (0.5) |
Sleep latency | (min) | 28.4 (12.7) | 17.3 (14.7) | 15.0 (13.2) | 7.6 (2.0) | 8.6 (4.4) | 9.0 (5.0) |
Sleep efficiency | (%) | – | – | – | 94.4 (3.8) | 90.5 (5.7) | 89.8 (5.9) |
Sleep problem† | – | – | – | 1 (33) | 7 (39) | 7 (30) | |
ESS score >10† | 1 (33) | 1 (6) | 4 (17) | – | – | – | |
PSQI score >5† | 2 (67) | 8 (44) | 6 (26) | – | – | – | |
Sleep satisfaction† | 2 (67) | 9 (50) | 18 (78) | – | – | – | |
ΔBT | (h) | 3.8 (1.3) | 1.7 (1.4)* | 1.3 (1.2)** | – | – | – |
Data are shown as mean (standard deviation) or number of incidences (%)†. Sleep variables are obtained differently for the children (actigraphy) and the parents (questionnaires).
*p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01, compared with no bedroom shared with children from unpaired analysis of variance and post hoc with Bonferroni correction.
ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; ΔBT = difference in the bedtime between children and parents.
Bedtime was significantly later for parents in the separate bedroom group compared with the two subgroups of the bedroom sharing group (both p < 0.05). However, there was no other intergroup/subgroup difference in sleep variables for children and parents. For participants in the bedroom sharing group, a shorter ΔBT was associated with a later bedtime and lower sleep efficiency of the children on weekdays (both p < 0.05) and an earlier bedtime, longer sleep period, lower ESS score, higher sleep satisfaction and a paradoxically longer sleep latency for the parents (p < 0.001, 0.01, 0.05, 0.05 and 0.01, respectively) (Table4).
Table 4.
Sleep variables | ΔBT in bedroom sharing group | |
---|---|---|
Parents | Children | |
Weekday bedtime | 0.882*** | −0.383* |
Weekday sleep end time | 0.206 | −0.166 |
Weekday sleep period | −0.493** | 0.295 |
Sleep latency | −0.449** | 0.099 |
Sleep efficiency | – | 0.387* |
Sleep problem | – | 0.141 |
ESS score >10 | 0.321* | – |
PSQI score >5 | −0.033 | – |
Sleep satisfaction | −0.333* | – |
ESS = Epworth Sleepiness Scale; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; ΔBT = difference in the bedtime between children and parents.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.00, from Pearson's product–moment correlations or Spearman's rank order correlations.
Discussion
In healthy Japanese 5-year-old children, the sleep end time of parents appeared to play a key role in regulating children's sleep patterns. For our current cohort, where bedroom sharing was common, co-sleeping was not associated with adverse sleep patterns of children or parents. However, children and parents going to sleep at a similar time were associated with unfavourable sleep patterns in both children and their parents. Further investigations are required to delineate specific characteristics of bedroom sharing and co-sleeping associated with unfavourable sleep patterns of children and parents.
Sleep patterns of the children and parents
We observed that the sleep end time of the parents was linearly associated with the children's sleep variable scores. In our study population, the vast majority of the primary parents were mothers, where interactions of sleep patterns between the children and their parents might be relatively more prominent, because mothers' sleep habits are reported to have a stronger influence than fathers' 16. It is unclear whether children's chronotype mimics that of their parents in later developmental stages, including unfavourable trends such as eveningness. A study of early-teenage school children in Taipei showed that sleep patterns were not correlated between the children and their parents. However, several social factors in Taipei (e.g. schools generally open earlier than parents' work places, and children often have breakfast and walk to school on their own) should be considered 4. Future longitudinal studies should address developmental changes in the dependence of children's sleep behaviours on their parents, taking into account different cultural backgrounds.
Influence of parental sleep disorders on their children's sleep patterns
In our current study, the high PSQI scores of parents did not affect sleep behaviour of the 5-year-old children. Furthermore, parents with a high ESS score appeared to have a paradoxically positive influence on their children's sleep patterns, such as the early bedtime and high sleep efficiency, a finding inconsistent with previous studies suggesting negative effects of parental stress on their children's sleep 1. In addition, a lower ESS score was associated with a shorter ΔBT (see the next paragraph for a detailed discussion). It is possible to explain that the parents with high ESS scores may be sleeping solitarily or may fall asleep much later than their children, potentially improving the sleep quality of the children. Further studies are required to confirm these findings.
Difference in bedtimes between children and parents: influence on sleep patterns
There are marked cross-cultural variations in sleeping arrangements. A large-scale cross-sectional study reported that in Western countries, the most common behaviour for infants occurring at bedtime is falling asleep independently in their own bed (57%), whereas in Asian countries, this rate is substantially lower (4%) 2. Researchers have investigated the influence of co-sleeping on sleep quality and emotional development of children. When a mother and an infant co-sleep, levels of partner-influenced arousal can be significantly higher, resulting in reduced sleep quality 17. In addition, co-sleeping children may have difficulty getting back to sleep on their own 5,8. A study of school-aged children found an association between co-sleeping and tiredness 18. However, there can be emotional security benefits from increased sensory contact and proximity during the night between children and parents, such as reduction in night fears in children and fulfilment of the protective instinct in parents 19,20. Moreover, co-sleeping might contribute to children having significantly higher self-esteem, experiencing less guilt and anxiety, and exhibiting a feeling of satisfaction with life 21,22.
Our current results suggest that, for parents who share a bedroom with their children, falling asleep at a similar time as their children negatively affects their and their children's sleep patterns, which may account for a considerable portion of adverse events attributed to bedroom sharing and co-sleeping. Circumstances in which parents try to get their children to go to sleep and subsequently fall asleep themselves shortly thereafter may be associated with poor sleep quality in the parents and subsequently in their children. Interestingly, in this study, parents who fell asleep at a similar time as their children appeared to be unaware of their and their children's sleep problems, as shown in their high sleep satisfaction and low ESS scores and unelevated rates in their children's sleep problems. It is possible that such parents might secure enough sleep periods to compensate for the poor sleep quality. Interventions to better manage the bedtime of children and parents in co-sleeping families might improve sleep patterns of both children and their parents.
Limitations
We were able to recruit only a limited number of participants for our current study, resulting in a lack of sufficient statistical power in identifying detailed interactions in sleep patterns between the children and their parents. Actigraphy was available only for the children. Future studies need to address simultaneous, objective evaluation of sleep variables in both children and their parents to facilitate direct comparisons between the two. In the vast majority of the participating families, children and parents shared a bedroom, and our findings might not be applicable to families in Western countries. Further research is required to investigate the influence of specific sleep arrangements associated with bedroom sharing and co-sleeping on the sleep patterns of children and their parents.
Conclusions
Parents' sleep behaviour influenced the sleep schedule and sleep quality of the children. Parents' falling asleep at a similar time to their children appeared to be a risk factor for lowered sleep quality in both the children and their parents. To promote optimal sleep habits in young children, understanding parents' sleep patterns along with those of the children would be important. Studies that account for multiple factors, including detailed sleep arrangements and cultural backgrounds, may help improve the sleep quality of individuals who share a bedroom and/or co-sleep with children or parents.
Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge Dr Akiko Iemura and Ms Mizue Iwasaki for their contribution to the data collection process. This work was supported by the Japan Children's Study Group, Japan Science and Technology Agency and the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research C24591533 and B01-24119004, Constructive Developmental Science, Innovative Areas).
References
- Meltzer LJ, Montgomery-Downs HE. Sleep in the family. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2011;58:765–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2011.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindell JA, Sadeh A, Kohyama J, How TH. Parental behaviors and sleep outcomes in infants and toddlers: a cross-cultural comparison. Sleep Med. 2010;11:393–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonhard C, Randler C. In sync with the family: children and partners influence the sleep-wake circadian rhythm and social habits of women. Chronobiol Int. 2009;26:510–25. doi: 10.1080/07420520902821101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gau SS, Merikangas KR. Similarities and differences in sleep-wake patterns among adults and their children. Sleep. 2004;27:299–304. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.2.299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adair R, Bauchner H, Philipp B, Levenson S, Zuckerman B. Night waking during infancy: role of parental presence at bedtime. Pediatrics. 1991;87:500–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortesi F, Giannotti F, Sebastiani T, Vagnoni C. Cosleeping and sleep behavior in Italian school-aged children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2004;25:28–33. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200402000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes MJ, Parker KG, Sallinen B, Davare AA. Bedsharing, temperament, and sleep disturbance in early childhood. Sleep. 2001;24:657–62. doi: 10.1093/sleep/24.6.657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mindell JA, Meltzer LJ, Carskadon MA, Chervin RD. Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: findings from the 2004 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Med. 2009;10:771–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latz S, Wolf AW, Lozoff B. Cosleeping in context: sleep practices and problems in young children in Japan and the United States. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1999;153:339–46. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.4.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwata S, Iwata O, Iemura A, Iwasaki M, Matsuishi T. Determinants of sleep patterns in healthy Japanese 5-year-old children. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2011;29:57–62. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2010.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwata S, Iwata O, Iemura A, Iwasaki M, Matsuishi T. Sleep architecture in healthy 5-year-old preschool children: associations between sleep schedule and quality variables. Acta Paediatr. 2012;101:e110–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02515.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwasaki M, Iwata S, Iemura A, Yamashita N, Tomino Y, Anme T, et al. Utility of subjective sleep assessment tools for healthy preschool children: a comparative study between sleep logs, questionnaires, and actigraphy. J Epidemiol. 2010;20:143–9. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20090054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–5. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doi Y, Minowa M, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Kim K, Shibui K, et al. Psychometric assessment of subjective sleep quality using the Japanese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI-J) in psychiatric disordered and control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2000;97:165–72. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00232-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC. Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep. 1992;15:461–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.5.461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Komada Y, Adachi N, Matsuura N, Mizuno K, Hirose K, Aritomi R, et al. Irregular sleep habits of parents are associated with increased sleep problems and daytime sleepiness of children. Tohoku J Exp Med. 2009;219:85–9. doi: 10.1620/tjem.219.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosko S, Richard C, McKenna J. Infant arousals during mother-infant bed sharing: implications for infant sleep and sudden infant death syndrome research. Pediatrics. 1997;100:841–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.100.5.841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein MA, Mendelsohn J, Obermeyer WH, Amromin J, Benca R. Sleep and behavior problems in school-aged children. Pediatrics. 2001;107:E60. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.4.e60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies L. Babies co-sleeping with parents. Midwives. 1995;108:384–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medoff D, Schaefer CE. Children sharing the parental bed: a review of the advantages and disadvantages of cosleeping. Psychology. 1993;30:1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Lewis RJ, Janda LH. The relationship between adult sexual adjustment and childhood experiences regarding exposure to nudity, sleeping in the parental bed, and parental attitudes toward sexuality. Arch Sex Behav. 1988;17:349–62. doi: 10.1007/BF01541812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okami P, Weisner T, Olmstead R. Outcome correlates of parent-child bedsharing: an eighteen-year longitudinal study. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23:244–53. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200208000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]