Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2013 May 6;1288(1):59–69. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12047

The Evolution of Human Handedness

Jeroen B Smaers 1,2, James Steele 3, Charleen R Case 4, Katrin Amunts 5,6
PMCID: PMC4298027  PMID: 23647442

Abstract

There is extensive evidence for an early vertebrate origin of lateralized motor behavior and of related asymmetries in underlying brain systems. We investigate human lateralized motor functioning in a broad comparative context of evolutionary neural reorganization. We quantify evolutionary trends in the fronto-cerebellar system (involved in motor learning) across 46 million years of divergent primate evolution by comparing rates of evolution of prefrontal cortex, frontal motor cortex, and posterior cerebellar hemispheres along individual branches of the primate tree of life. We provide a detailed evolutionary model of the neuroanatomical changes leading to modern human lateralized motor functioning, demonstrating an increased role for the fronto-cerebellar system in the apes dating to their evolutionary divergence from the monkeys (∼30 million years ago (Mya)), and a subsequent shift toward an increased role for prefrontal cortex over frontal motor cortex in the fronto-cerebellar system in the Homo-Pan ancestral lineage (∼10 Mya) and in the human ancestral lineage (∼6 Mya). We discuss these results in the context of cortico-cerebellar functions and their likely role in the evolution of human tool use and speech.

Keywords: laterality, primates, cerebellum, prefrontal cortex, tool use

Introduction

Lateralization in human motor functioning is often considered as a principal factor explaining the exceptional capacity of humans to learn complex motor skills in a wide range of tasks. Lateralization in motor behavior and in its underlying neural systems is, however, not unique to humans, with extensive evidence demonstrating lateralization in primates,13 nonprimate mammals,46 birds,7,8 fish,9,10 reptiles,11,12 and amphibians13,14 (e.g., pawedness in toads,15 footedness in birds,16 and handedness in fish17). Considering the evidence for an early vertebrate origin for lateralized motor behavior and its close links to neural structural asymmetries,18,19 human lateralized motor functioning could be considered in a broad primate evolutionary context of neural organizational patterns with possibly deep evolutionary roots. Here, we aim to elucidate aspects of the neural evolutionary origin for complex motor learning and its lateralization, in the context of millions of years of divergent primate evolution.

We focus on quantifying the evolution of a brain system fundamental to motor control (the fronto-cerebellar system) across 46 million years of divergent evolution in anthropoids. The brain is organized as a distributed system, with different anatomically and functionally connected areas interacting in coordination to produce complex behaviors. The acquisition and adaptation of complex manual motor sequences involves activation of a frontoparietal praxis network involved in hand manipulation skills,20,21 as well as a frontocerebellar–basal ganglia network involved in novel motor sequence learning.2225 Within the fronto-cerebellar network, the lateral hemispheres of the cerebellum receive input exclusively from the cerebral cortex projecting to the frontal motor and prefrontal areas via the dentate nucleus.26 The traditional (and empirically well-supported) theory of cerebellar function is that it encodes and continuously refines input–output relationships between motor commands and their consequences27 in both feed-forward and inverse feedback models of ongoing movements during action execution.28 In relation to its prefrontal projections, the cerebellar cortex simulates the way in which the outputs of prefrontal areas are processed, allowing it to issue feed-forward commands of correction signals back to the frontal lobe circuits.29 This neural system crucially underlies the process of motor learning and allows the development of motor plans that are not coded for limb-specific movements, but for the goal of an action.30 Functional neuroimaging studies of complex forms of motor learning confirm this interpretation of the role of the prefronto-cerebellar system in motor learning, by indicating that in the initial stages of learning, prefrontal processes control complex action execution, but that when the motor sequence is learned as a specialized automatic execution, cerebellar activation increases and prefrontal activation decreases.3133 Considering the function of this prefronto-cerebellar system in the context of human evolution, we can hypothesize that its elaboration under natural selection could explain humans’ exceptional capacity to acquire, and continuously and dynamically adapt, complex forms of motor behavior.

Evidence for laterality in the fronto-cerebellar system primarily comes from human studies. The distributed cortical network involved in complex tool use is functionally left biased,20 and both prefrontal cortex34 and frontal motor areas35,36 have been demonstrated to be structurally lateralized in relation to language processing and handedness (e.g., neural asymmetry in primary motor cortex corresponds to behavioral lateralization in hand preference). Cerebellar directional asymmetry of size has been observed for lobules III and IV (left < right) and VI (left > right),37 possibly also related to handedness.38 Laterality is thus an additional feature of the prefronto-cerebellar system in humans, and may underlie humans’ exceptional capacities in tool use and language.

Despite comparative evidence suggesting increased prefrontal input to the cortico-cerebellar system39,40 and of its lateralization in human evolution, there is only limited information on the evolutionary history of these patterns of brain system connectivity. The main reasons for this are that previous studies have compared humans with a maximum of three nonhuman primate species (without consideration of their phylogenetic relatedness), and that methods to infer detailed evolutionary pathways for all branches in a phylogenetic tree have only recently become available.41,42 Reconstructing the detailed evolutionary history of the prefronto-cerebellar system and of its structural lateralization is of crucial importance: this will both provide more detailed information on the selective pressures that have defined its adaptive role in primate behavior, and enable assessment of the deeper evolutionary history of its structural lateralization.

Our previous work on this aspect of brain system evolution43 demonstrated a selective and correlated expansion of both frontal cortex and the cerebellar hemispheres at the dawn of the ape and great ape radiations. Here, we extend our previous work by differentiating between prefrontal (PF) and frontal motor areas (FM) within the frontal cortex and by delineating the part of the cerebellar hemispheres that has the closest functional association with prefrontal cortex, i.e., the posterior lobe of the cerebellar hemispheres (PCH).4446 We further differentiate between the left and right hemispheres in each case, allowing inference of the evolution of laterality in the fronto-cerebellar system. We have collected information for 16 extant primate species, and quantified evolutionary rates of hemisphere-specific volumetric changes in the PF, FM, and PCH along individual branches of the primate phylogenetic tree. We aim to infer the evolutionary origin of a hypothesized shift from a predominantly frontal motor to a predominantly prefrontal involvement in the cortico-cerebellar system, and to examine the possible association of that shift with increased structural lateralization.

Materials and methods

Brain data

We examined both hemispheres of 29 individuals from 16 anthropoid species (see Table 1). Data consist of serially sectioned brains from the Stephan, Zilles, and Zilles–Amunts collections47 housed at the C. & O. Vogt Institute for Brain Research (University of Düsseldorf, Germany). Volumetric data for frontal motor areas and prefrontal cortex were taken from our previous work,48,49 where they were measured using a delineation protocol involving a bootstrap approach of estimating cumulative volumes at successive slice intervals along the anterio–posterior and posterior–anterior axes of the cytoarchitectonically defined frontal lobe. Data presented here indicate the cumulative volumes up to the 7th section interval of the anterior-posterior axis (PF) and posterior-anterior axis (FM) (Supporting Fig. 1).50

Table 1.

Volumetric data (mL) used in the current analysis

Left hemisphere
Right hemisphere
Species Individual Brain size PCH FM PF PCH FM PF
Homo sapiens (human) 5,694 1,216.00 46.33 51.91 42.56 46.67 38.18 59.12
6,895 1,110.00 51.04 26.02 57.46 50.98 31.81 51.76
1,696 1,622.00 72.22 61.92 69.25 70.09 61.85 71.40
14,686 1,437.00 47.55 58.08 59.01 46.19 49.08 77.04
Pan troglodytes (chimpanzee) 280 444.98 12.62 20.85 17.70 12.37 21.91 16.74
497 378.00 15.17 14.44 11.52 15.17 13.69 12.57
Gorilla gorilla (western gorilla) 375 434.36 20.68 21.79 13.61 20.01 23.56 13.36
8,214 376.00 10.40 13.27 8.07 11.19 14.14 7.83
Hylobates lar (gibbon) 1,203 98.36 3.61 3.78 3.02 3.70 3.61 3.08
397 107.00 3.87 5.04 2.70 3.90 5.23 2.43
Papio anubis (olive baboon) 97 184.36 4.05 6.42 4.72 4.24 6.44 4.69
Cercopithecus mitis (blue monkey) 261 72.39 1.34 2.29 1.50 1.42 2.25 1.79
Cercopithecus ascanius (black-cheecked white-nosed monkey) 219 59.36 1.15 1.85 1.29 1.13 1.82 1.33
Erythrocebus patas (patas monkey) 1,341 93.73 1.55 3.22 1.88 1.60 3.48 1.82
1,545 89.00 1.87 3.24 2.04 1.84 3.32 1.91
Miopithecus talapoin (talapoin monkey) 1,171 39.67 0.69 1.26 1.04 0.66 1.11 1.17
1,201 38.32 0.57 0.98 0.79 0.57 0.97 0.73
Nasalis larvatus (proboscis monkey) 1,365 62.02 1.77 2.50 0.81 1.88 1.86 1.04
Procolobus badius (western red colobus) 213 75.97 2.04 2.61 1.81 2.11 2.69 1.84
Alouatta seniculus (red howler monkey) 1,184 45.17 1.00 1.53 0.88 1.08 1.58 1.60
Ateles geoffroyi (Central American spider monkey) 1,000 102.70 2.65 3.74 2.84 2.63 3.48 3.68
Lagothrix lagotricha (Humboldt's woolly monkey) 1,571 88.16 2.16 3.25 2.17 2.18 2.91 2.26
Pithecia monachus (monk saki) 1,180 32.82 0.69 0.96 0.45 0.71 1.02 0.95
Cebus albifrons (white-fronted capuchin) 1,200 77.03 1.72 2.47 1.74 1.65 2.75 2.01
6,062 68.53 1.80 1.63 1.94 1.78 1.68 2.56

PCH, posterior cerebellar hemispheres; FM, frontal motor areas; PF, prefrontal cortex. Data for PCH were measured for the current analysis; data on FM and PF were taken from our previous work.4850

Comparative cerebellar anatomy

The cerebellum occupies only 10–15% of total brain volume in primates,51 but it contains roughly half of the brain's neurons.52 Although cellular organization is very uniform compared to the cerebral cortex,27 there is a clear differentiation between different parts of the cerebellum in terms of input–output relationships with other brain areas.26 The macro-anatomical subdivisions of the cerebellum across mammals involve 10 lobules5355 (defined as I–X), which have been found to relate to distinct topographical cortical connectivity patterns.45,46 In particular, lobules V, VI, VIIb, and VIIIa have reciprocal connections with frontal motor areas, whereas portions of lobule VI and the entirety of crus 1 and crus 2 (subdivisions of lobule VII that make up an average of 40% of total cerebellar gray matter in humans37) have connections with the prefrontal cortex.44,45

Delineation of the posterior cerebellar hemispheres

We delineated all lobules posterior to the primary fissure. This measurement comprises lobules VI–X, thus including the prefrontal projecting lobules VI–VII that encompass the majority of cerebellar gray matter volume posterior to the primary fissure (up to 64% in humans37). Our present delineation of the posterior cerebellar hemisphere differs from our previous delineation of the overall cerebellar hemispheres43 in that it focuses more specifically on prefrontal projecting lobules VI–VII. All measurements are presented in Table 1 (for example delineations, see Supporting Fig. 2). Volumes were computed using the Cavalieri procedure.56,57 Systematic samples from each brain were taken, the position of the first section was chosen randomly, and subsequent sections were chosen based on a regular sampling interval. Twenty or more sections per brain58 were used and digitized with a flatbed scanner at 800 dpi.

Phylogenetic scaling

In any comparative analysis, raw data will consist of phylogenetically nonindependent data points (e.g., sampled by species); comparisons of such points need to be weighted for phylogenetic distance. Allometric analyses of comparative datasets therefore incorporate phylogenetic trees, to account for differences that are due to phylogenetic relatedness. We use phylogenetically reduced major-axis regressions with a likelihood-fitted lambda model to obtain residuals from regressions of brain structure size on the rest of brain size.59 “Rest of brain” was here defined as total brain size minus size of PCH, FM, and PF. These residuals are used as measures of the relative size of a particular brain structure. 60 Relative sizes of brain structures were then scaled using phylogenetically generalized least squares regressions.61 All analyses were performed in the R software environment. 6264

Evolutionary rates and inferring evolutionary history

We use an adaptive peak model of evolution to infer rates of change for individual branches along the tree of life.4143 This model allows for rates of change to be different for each branch in a phylogenetic tree in response to the wanderings of adaptive peaks through phenotype space. We use the adaptive peak model as formalized in the method of independent evolution41,42 because it allows for the incorporation of more specific models such as Brownian Motion and Ornstein Uhlenbeck as special cases by collapsing its algorithms accordingly under relevant conditions. This formalization has further been shown to accurately estimate fossil values of brain and body size in primates, bats and carnivorans,41,42 supporting its validity in estimating evolutionary trends for brain structure sizes in primates.

Reconstructing rates of the evolution of traits allow identification of branches in the evolutionary tree associated with episodes of selective and correlated trait coevolution, which can be compared with the general scaling patterns revealed by phylogenetic regressions.41 Evolutionary trends on individual branches may align with or diverge from more general evolutionary patterns as different species follow different adaptive directions. Importantly, recognition of general scaling regularities simply identifies average trends across all species in a sample, and does not mean that changes in each branch of the phylogenetic tree necessarily exemplify them. The extent to which changes in particular branches align with the clade-general correlation pattern can be revealed by a comparison of evolutionary rates.41 This approach complements the use of phylogenetic regressions by allowing a more detailed investigation of the evolutionary history of trait coevolution along particular evolutionary branches.

Results

Comparative correlations between PCH, FM, and PF

Phylogenetically generalized least squares analysis with a maximum likelihood-fitted lambda model (Fig. 1) reveals a significant correlation between the relative sizes of PCH and of FM (slope 95% C.I. = 0.04:1.02, R2 = 0.24, P = 0.0527, λ = 1), but not of PCH and of PF (slope 95% C.I. = −0.57:0.27, R2 = 0.04, P = 0.4836, λ = 1). Considering evidence for contralateral cortico-cerebellar connectivity44 we further analyzed contralateral hemispheric correlations. Results demonstrate a significant correlation between right PCH and left FM (slope 95% C.I. = 0.17:1.23, R2 = 0.32, P = 0.0215, λ = 1), but not left PCH and right FM (slope 95% C.I. = −0.10:0.74, R2 = 0.14, P = 0.1582, λ = 1). No significant correlation was found between either right PCH and left PF (slope 95% C.I. = −0.56:0.21, R2 = 0.05, P = 0.3888, λ = 1) or left PCH and right PF (slope 95% C.I. = −0.46:0.26, R2 = 0.02, P = 0.5829, λ = 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Results from a phylogenetically generalized least squares analysis of correlations in the relative size of the posterior cerebellar hemispheres, frontal motor areas, and prefrontal cortex. Hemisphere-specific regressions between frontal and cerebellar structures were performed contralaterally, because evidence suggests the importance of contralateral connections in the fronto-cerebellar system.44

Laterality in PCH, FM, and PF

We investigated structural lateralization in PCH, FM, and PF by scaling hemisphere-specific volumes of each of these structures to rest of brain size (defined as brain size minus PCH, FM, and PF). Lateralization was assessed by comparing scaling coefficients (intercepts and slopes of the regression) for the left and right hemispheres. In this approach, significant results refers to comparisons of the 95% confidence intervals of the scaling coefficients from different analyses; if the scaling coefficient of one analysis lies outside the confidence interval of another analysis, it is considered to be significantly different at P < 0.05. Results are summarized in Table 2. For PCH and FM, there is no asymmetry in general scaling trends in primates. However, and consistent with previous analyses,48 for PF there is significant hyperscaling of the left compared to the right hemisphere, together with a significantly lower intercept.

Table 2.

Results from a phylogenetically generalized least squares analysis of scaling of hemisphere-specific brain structures/areas to rest of brain size. Rest of brain size is here defined as total brain size minus the size of PCH, FM, and PF

Left hemisphere
Right hemisphere
Slope 95% C.I. Slope 95% C.I.
PCH 1.64 1.46 1.82 1.63 1.44 1.82
FM 1.54 1.43 1.64 1.54 1.43 1.66
PF 1.82 1.64 2.00 1.63 1.45 1.82
Intercept 95% C.I.
Intercept 95% C.I.
PCH −1.92 −2.35 −1.48 −1.89 −2.35 −1.43
FM −1.64 −1.89 −1.38 −1.65 −1.93 −1.37
PF −2.38 −2.82 −1.94 −1.90 −2.35 −1.44

The evolutionary history of the fronto-cerebellar system

We compared rates of evolution of PCH, FM, and PF relative to rates of evolution of rest of brain size, to investigate which branches align with or diverge from clade-general coevolutionary trends. We focus here on branches where our methods have reconstructed a disproportionate increase in PCH and either FM or PF, since this identifies branches in which the fronto-cerebellar system plays an unusually strong adaptive role. Disproportionate FM–PCH increase characterizes the ape (∼30 Mya) and great ape (∼20 Mya) ancestral branches, but the trend does not continue in branches leading specifically to Pan and to Homo (Fig. 2A). Disproportionate PF–PCH increase also characterizes the ape and great ape ancestral branches, but that trend continues in the Homo-Pan (∼10 Mya) ancestral branch and in the human (∼6 Mya) ancestral lineage (Fig. 2B).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Comparative analysis of evolutionary rates of the posterior cerebellar hemispheres (PCH), frontal motor areas (FM), and prefrontal cortex (PF). Rates were compared for individual branches of the primate phylogenetic tree, allowing detailed inferences of the evolutionary history of fronto-cerebellar systems across 46 million years of divergent primate evolution.

To infer the evolutionary origin of a hypothesized shift from a predominantly frontal motor to a predominantly prefrontal involvement in the cortico-cerebellar system, we quantified the increase of PF relative to FM in relation to PCH. These results reveal directional selection for an increased PF contribution to the fronto-cerebellar system in branches leading from the Old World anthropoid ancestral lineage through to the human ancestral branch, with the most pronounced trends in the ape ancestral lineage and in the Homo-Pan ancestral lineage (Fig. 2C). Results further indicate that these trends are similar when assessing each contralateral cortico-cerebellar pattern (left PF/FM and right PCH versus right PF/FM and left PCH; Fig. 2 lists results across both hemispheres, and Supporting Figs. 3 and 4 give results for each contralateral pattern).

Discussion

The fronto-cerebellar brain system plays a crucial part in the automatization of learned motor sequences and the incremental acquisition of movements into well-executed behavior.25,27,6567 Reconstructing the evolution of this brain system will advance understanding of the evolution of humans’ exceptional motor capacities. To investigate the evolutionary history of humans’ increased (lateralized) prefrontal input to this brain system, we delineated relevant brain structures for 29 individuals from 16 different primate species, and quantified evolutionary rates on separate branches of the primate phylogenetic tree.

Our results indicate a significant contrast in the scaling of left versus right PF, but not of left versus right FM and PCH (Table 2). The lack of general trends for volumetric asymmetry in PCH across these 16 anthropoid species is consistent with findings of within-species variation in chimpanzees demonstrating “no population-bias in the lateralization of the cerebellum.”68 When considering hemisphere-specific correlations within the fronto-cerebellar system, we find stronger evidence for a left FM–right PCH coupling than for the contralateral pattern (Fig. 1). This is likely to be principally related to a frontal motor praxis system involved in primate hand manipulation skills. PF–PCH coevolutionary coupling at the level of correlations between contralateral hemispheres is not found consistently across the whole primate sample (Fig. 1).

To investigate lineage-specific patterns of brain reorganization,41 we quantified rates of evolution for each branch in the phylogeny. Although our sample included multiple individuals for several key species (including all the ape species represented) and are consistent with findings of the distribution of structural asymmetries in larger samples of specific species by other workers,68 future work should look to expand sample sizes for each species to increase the robustness of lineage-specific inferences. In Figure 2 we highlight branches for which we have reconstructed a coordinated and disproportionate increase in the size of the fronto-cerebellar system. This phylogenetic mapping reveals strong selective investment in both FM–PCH and PF–PCH at the dawn of the ape (∼30 Mya) and great ape (∼20 Mya) radiations, differentiating them from monkeys (Fig. 2A and 2B). A subsequent expansion of PF–PCH, but not FM–PCH, is indicated on the Homo-Pan ancestral branch (∼10 Mya) and in the human lineage (∼6 Mya). When comparing evolutionary rates for PF and FM, the ape ancestral branch, the Homo-Pan clade and the human ancestral lineage all appear to be characterized by shifts toward an increased role for PF in the fronto-cerebellar system (Fig. 2C).

These results, and the finding of left PF hyperscaling as a general trend in our sample, are consistent with observed structural brain asymmetries in humans and chimpanzees, absent in other nonhuman primates, for several relevant frontal and cerebellar areas,3,6873 suggesting that at least part of the neural foundation for human complex motor behavior was present before the ancestral split with the lineage leading to chimpanzees. With increased selection for context- and goal-dependent, dynamic adjustment of learned motor plans (e.g., tool use), the prefrontal input to the cortico-cerebellar system may have become more pronounced and led to selection for increased lateralization. This suggestion is also supported by studies in chimpanzees demonstrating that individual variability in structural asymmetry of the PCH is related to the propensity to perform complex activities such as tool use and aimed throwing, and handedness for a tool-use task (termite fishing).68

Our finding that humans and chimpanzees share a preadaptation for increased prefrontal involvement in the fronto-cerebellar system, which continued in the human lineage but stabilized in the chimpanzee lineage, may shed light on the evolutionary role of the fronto-cerebellar system in tool use and vocal articulatory control, and on the differences in tool use and vocalizing abilities between these two species.

Humans and chimpanzees share the capacity to perceive the affordances of objects as potential tools74,75 and the ability to modify the kinetic energy produced in relation to the affordances of the task constraints.76,77 In both species, however, these capacities involve an experience-based learning process where increased experience results in increased efficiency. In other words, the ability to move from initial action execution of complex motor sequences to specialized automatic execution through experience plays a crucial role in nut-cracking in both humans and chimpanzees.78

Stone flaking, however, a bimanually coordinated task that became a habitual behavior within the hominin radiation, may require greater lateralization of hand function because it involves the two hands working at two different levels of resolution in a coordinated fashion to yield a common functional outcome (the hammering hand needs to be controlled in such a way as to transmit the appropriate amount of kinetic energy at impact with considerable accuracy at the point of percussion, whereas the postural hand has to rotate and adjust the position of a core to prepare for the following hammer strike, and stabilize the core against the shock of the blow78). Stout et al.79 have found increased frontal activation in stone flaking tasks, with site, lateralization and level of activation varying as a function of task complexity and task familiarity, but the extent of any similarities and differences with activation patterns in a nut-cracking task have not yet been studied in humans or in chimpanzees.

Humans are additionally distinguished from chimpanzees in possessing the capacity for articulate speech. Posterior cerebellar activation in language tasks has been found to be right lateralized and focused in lobule VI and crus 1,80 which, as noted above, are prefrontal-projecting areas. Fluent speech requires the serial ordering of phonemes and syllables, and it has been shown that preparation and production of more complex syllables and syllable sequences recruit left hemisphere inferior frontal sulcus, posterior parietal cortex, and bilateral regions at the junction of the anterior insula and frontal operculum, to supplement the more basic cortical and subcortical components of the speech production system.81 Activation patterns in a verbal motoric rehearsal task suggest the existence of a frontal (BA44/46) and superior cerebellar (lobuleVI/crus 1) articulatory control system,82 and there is increased PF activation with increased working memory loads in a speech motor control task.83 Thus, it is plausible that the expansion of the prefrontal system and of prefrontal-projecting cerebellar lobules in humans39 also relates to adaptations for articulate speech.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the UK Natural Environment Research Council, Grant number NE/H022937/1.

Conflicts of interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest.

Supporting Information

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article.

Disclaimer: Supplementary materials have been peer-reviewed but not copyedited.

Figure S1. Delineation examples for the posterior cerebellar hemispheres for Homo sapiens, Hylobates lar, Cercopithecus mitis, and Cebus albifrons.

nyas1288-0059-sd1.pdf (220.4KB, pdf)

Figure S2. Delineation protocol for PF and FM (from Refs. 48, 49, and 50). For each individual, the border between areas 3 and 4 is delineated using cytoarchtitectonic criteria. Cumulative volumes are computed along the anterio–posterior and posterio–anterior axes of the brain, anterior to the border of areas 3 and 4. (a) and (b) indicate the 4th and 17th sections of a human brain, (c) presents the dorsal view of the brain exemplifying the cumulative volumetric approach along the anterio-posterior axis.

nyas1288-0059-sd2.pdf (2.1MB, pdf)

Figure S3. As in Figure 2, results comprise the analyses of left cortical (PF and FM) and right cerebellar (PCH) hemispheres, and of right cortical (PF and FM) and left cerebellar (PCH) hemispheres.

nyas1288-0059-sd3.pdf (85.7KB, pdf)

Figure S4

nyas1288-0059-sd4.pdf (85.7KB, pdf)

References

  • 1.Sun ZY, et al. The effect of handedness on the shape of the central sulcus. Neuroimage. 2012;60:332–339. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nudo RJ, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of hand preference in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J. Neurosci. 1992;12:2918–2947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-08-02918.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bogart SL, et al. Cortical sulci asymmetries in chimpanzees and macaques: a new look at an old idea. Neuroimage. 2012;61:533–541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rogers LJ, Ward JP, Stafford D. Eye dominance int he small-eared bushbaby, Otolemur garnettii. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:257–264. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90011-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Petersen MR, et al. Neural lateralization of species-specific vocalizations by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata. Science. 1978;202:324–327. doi: 10.1126/science.99817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ehret G. Left hemisphere advantage in the mouse brain for recognizing ultrasonic communication calls. Nature. 1987;325:249–251. doi: 10.1038/325249a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Nottebohm F. Neural lateralization of vocal control in a passerine bird. J. Exp. Zool. 1971;177:229–261. doi: 10.1002/jez.1401770210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vallortigara G, Andrew RJ. Differential involvement of right and left hemisphere in individual recognition in the domestic chick. Behav. Process. 1994;33:41–57. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(94)90059-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cantalupo C, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Lateralization of predator-evasion response in a teleost fish (Girardinus falcatus) Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:1637–1646. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00043-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bisazza A, Pignatti R, Vallortigara G. Detour tests reveal task- and stimulus-specific behavioural lateralization in mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki) Behav. Brain Res. 1997;89:237–242. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00061-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Engbretson GA, Reiner A, Brecha N. Habenular asymmetry and the central connections of the parietal eye of the lizard. J. Comp. Neurol. 1981;198:155–165. doi: 10.1002/cne.901980113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hoso M, Asami T, Hori M. Right-handed snakes: convergent evolution of asymmetry for functional specialization. Biol. Lett. 2007;3:169–173. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bauer RH. Lateralization of neural control for vocalization by the frog (Rana pipiens. Psychobiology. 1993;21:243–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Vallortigara G, et al. Complementary right and left hemifield use for predatory and agonistic behaviour in toads. Neuroreport. 1998;9:3341–3344. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bisazza A, et al. Right-pawedness in toads. Nature. 1996;379:408–408. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rogers LJ, Workman L. Footedness in birds. Ani. Behav. 1993;45:409–411. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hori M. Frequency-dependent natural seletion in the handedness of scale-eating Cichlid fish. Science. 1993;260:216–219. doi: 10.1126/science.260.5105.216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bisazza A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G. The origins of cerebral asymmetry: a review of evidence of behavioural and brain lateralization in fishes, reptiles and amphibians. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1998;22:411–426. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(97)00050-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005;28:575–633. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Johnson-Frey SH, Newman-Norlund R, Grafton ST. A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15:681–695. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ramayya AG, Glasser MF, Rilling JK. A DTI investigation of neural substrates supporting tool use. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:507–516. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lewis JW. Cortical networks related to human use of tools. Neuroscientist. 2006;12:211–231. doi: 10.1177/1073858406288327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Obayashi S, et al. Functional brain mapping of monkey tool use. Neuroimage. 2001;14:853–861. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Doyon J, et al. Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:625–641. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00168-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Doyon J, Penhune V, Ungerleider LG. Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:252–262. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00158-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Middleton FA, Strick PL. Cerebellar output channels. Cereb. Cogn. 1997;41:61–82. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(08)60347-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Marr D. A theory of cerebellar cortex. J. Physiol. 1969;202:437–470. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wolpert DM, Kawato M. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Net. 1998;11:1317–1329. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(98)00066-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ramnani N. The primate cortico-cerebellar system: anatomy and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:511–522. doi: 10.1038/nrn1953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Greger B, Norris SA, Thach WT. Spike firing in the lateral cerebellar cortex correlated with movement and motor parameters irrespective of the effector limb. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;91:576–582. doi: 10.1152/jn.00535.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Passingham RE. Attention to action. Philosop. Trans. R. Soc. London Series B, Biol. Sci. 1996;351:1473–1479. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ramnani N, Passingham RE. Changes in the human brain during rhythm learning. J. Cog. Neurosci. 2001;13:952–966. doi: 10.1162/089892901753165863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jueptner M, et al. Anatomy of motor learning: 1. Frontal cortex and attention to action. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;77:1313–1324. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Uylings HBM, et al. Left-right asymmetry in volume and number of neurons in adult Broca's area. Cortex. 2006;42:652–658. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70401-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Amunts K, et al. Asymmetry in the human motor cortex and handedness. Neuroimage. 1996;4:216–222. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Amunts K, et al. Interhemispheric asymmetry of the human motor cortex related to handedness and gender. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:304–312. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00075-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Diedrichsen J, et al. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage. 2009;46:39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Snyder PJ, et al. Cerebellar volume asymmetries are related to handedness: a quantitative MRI study. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:407–419. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00125-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Balsters JH, et al. Evolution of the cerebellar cortex: the selective expansion of prefrontal-projecting cerebellar lobules. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2045–2052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ramnani N, et al. The evolution of prefrontal inputs to the cortico-pontine system: diffusion imaging evidence from macaque monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex. 2006;16:811–818. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Smaers JB, et al. Comparative analyses of evolutionary rates reveal different pathways to encephalization in bats, carnivorans, and primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:18006–18011. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212181109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Smaers JB, Vinicius L. Inferring macro-evolutionary patterns using an adaptive peak model of evolution. Evol. Ecol. Res. 2009;11:991–1015. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Smaers JB, Steele J, Zilles K. Modeling the evolution of cortico-cerebellar systems in primates. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2011;1225:176–190. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06003.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Krienen FM, Buckner RL. Segregated fronto-cerebellar circuits revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex. 2009;19:2485–2497. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Buckner RL, et al. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:2322–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:8432–8444. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-23-08432.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Zilles K, Amunts K, Smaers JB. Three brain collections for comparative neuroanatomy and neuroimaging. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2011;1225(S1):E94–E104. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05978.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Smaers JB, et al. Primate prefrontal cortex evolution: human brains are the extreme of a lateralized ape trend. Brain Behav. Evol. 2011;77:67–78. doi: 10.1159/000323671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Smaers JB, et al. Sexual dimorphism and laterality in the evolution of the primate prefrontal cortex. Brain Behav Evol. 2012;79:205–212. doi: 10.1159/000336115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Smaers JB, et al. Frontal white matter volume is associated with brain enlargement and higher structural connectivity in haplorrhine primates. PLoS One. 2010;5:e9123. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Stephan H, Frahm H, Baron G. New and revised data on volumes of brain structures in insectivores and primates. Folia Primatologica. 1981;35:1–29. doi: 10.1159/000155963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zagon IS, McLaughlin PJ, Smith S. Neural populations in human cerebellum: estimations from isolated cell nuclei. Brain Res. 1977;127:279–282. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90541-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Larsell O, Jansen O. The Comparative Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebellum: The Human Cerebellum, Cerebellar Connections and Cerebellar Cortex. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Larsell O. Comparative Anatomy and Histology of the Cerebellum from Monotremes through Apes. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Schmahmann JD, et al. MRI Atlas of the Human Cerebellum. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Cavalieri B. Geometria Degli Indivisibili. Torino: Unione Tipografico-Editrice Torinese; 1635. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Howard CV, Reed MG. Three-Dimensional Measurement in Microscopy. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1998. Unbiased Stereology. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Zilles K, Schleicher A, Pehlemann FW. How many sections must be measured in order to reconstruct the volume of a structure using serial sections. Microscopica Acta. 1982;86:339–346. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Revell LJ. Phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things) Methods Ecol. Evol. 2012;3:217–223. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Revell LJ. Size-correction and principal components for interspecific comparative studies. Evolution. 2009;63:3258–3268. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00804.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Orme D, et al. 2012. Caper: comparative analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R. R package version 0.5.
  • 62.R Development Core Team. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: 2011. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Paradis E, Claude J, Strimmer K. APE: analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics. 2004;20:289–290. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Harmon L, et al. 2009. pp. 3–1. Geiger: analysis of evolutionary diversification. R package version 1.
  • 65.Glickstein M. The cerebellum and motor learning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1992;2:802–806. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(92)90137-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Ito M. Historical review of the significance of the cerebellum and the role of Purkinje cells in motor learning. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2002;978:273–288. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07574.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar function. Math. Biosci. 1971;10:25–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Cantalupo C, Hopkins W. The cerebellum and its contribution to complex tasks in higher primates: a comparative perspective. Cortex. 2010;46:821–830. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Dadda M, Cantalupo C, Hopkins WD. Further evidence of an association between handedness and neuroanatomical asymmetries in the primary motor cortex of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2582–2586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Hopkins WD, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA. The neural and cognitive correlates of aimed throwing in chimpanzees: a magnetic resonance image and behavioural study on a unique form of social tool use. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 2012;367:37–47. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Lyn H, et al. Planum temporale grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), vervet (Chlorocebus aethiops sabaeus), rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and bonnet (Macaca radiata) monkeys. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2004–2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Hopkins WD, Nir TM. Planum temporale surface area and grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effect of handedness and comparison with findings in humans. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;208:436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Phillips KA, Hopkins WD. Exploring the relationship between cerebellar asymmetry and handedness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and capuchins (Cebus apella) Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2333–2339. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Sakura O, Matsuzawa T. Flexibility of wild chimpanzee nut-cracking behavior using stone hammers and anvils: an experimental analysis. Ethology. 1991;87:237–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bril B. Enacting the perception of the affordances of potential tools. I: the case of children hammering. In: Heft H, Marsh KL, Foucart J, editors. Studies in Perception and Action VIII. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 2005. pp. 3–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Bril B, et al. Tool use as a way to assess cognition: how do captive chimpanzees handle the weight of the hammer when cracking a nut. Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:217–235. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0184-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Bril B, et al. The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2010;36:825–839. doi: 10.1037/a0018171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Bril B, et al. Functional mastery of percussive technology in nut-cracking and stone-flaking actions: experimental comparison and implications for the evolution of the human brain. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biol. Sci. 2012;367:59–74. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Stout D, et al. Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;33:1328–1338. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07619.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. 2009;44:489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Bohland JW, Guenther FH. An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. Neuroimage. 2006;32:821–841. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Chen SHA, Desmond JE. Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. Neuroimage. 2005;24:332–338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Hayter AL, Langdon DW, Ramnani N. Cerebellar contributions to working memory. Neuroimage. 2007;36:943–954. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1. Delineation examples for the posterior cerebellar hemispheres for Homo sapiens, Hylobates lar, Cercopithecus mitis, and Cebus albifrons.

nyas1288-0059-sd1.pdf (220.4KB, pdf)

Figure S2. Delineation protocol for PF and FM (from Refs. 48, 49, and 50). For each individual, the border between areas 3 and 4 is delineated using cytoarchtitectonic criteria. Cumulative volumes are computed along the anterio–posterior and posterio–anterior axes of the brain, anterior to the border of areas 3 and 4. (a) and (b) indicate the 4th and 17th sections of a human brain, (c) presents the dorsal view of the brain exemplifying the cumulative volumetric approach along the anterio-posterior axis.

nyas1288-0059-sd2.pdf (2.1MB, pdf)

Figure S3. As in Figure 2, results comprise the analyses of left cortical (PF and FM) and right cerebellar (PCH) hemispheres, and of right cortical (PF and FM) and left cerebellar (PCH) hemispheres.

nyas1288-0059-sd3.pdf (85.7KB, pdf)

Figure S4

nyas1288-0059-sd4.pdf (85.7KB, pdf)

Articles from Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES