Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jan 19.
Published in final edited form as: J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014 Jul;202(7):530–538. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000154

Psychiatrists’ views of the genetic bases of mental disorders and behavioral traits and their utilization of genetic tests

Kristopher J Abbate 1, Wendy Chung 1, Karen Marder 1, Ruth Ottman 1, Katherine Johansen Taber 2, Cheng-Shiun Leu 3, Paul S Appelbaum 1,4
PMCID: PMC4298352  NIHMSID: NIHMS580314  PMID: 24933415

Abstract

We examined how 372 psychiatrists view genetic aspects of mental disorders and behaviors, and use genetic tests (GTs). Most thought the genetic contribution was moderate/high for several disorders (e.g. bipolar, schizophrenia, depression, Alzheimer’s, intelligence, creativity, anxiety, suicidality). In the past 6 months, 14.1% ordered GTs, 18.3% discussed prenatal testing with patients, 36.0% initiated discussions about other GTs, 41.6% had patients ask about GTs, and 5.3% excluded GT results from patient records. Many thought that GTs were available for schizophrenia (24.3%) and major depression (19.6%). Women were more likely to report that patients asked about GTs; and were less certain about the degree of genetic contribution to several disorders. Psychiatrists perceive strong genetic bases for numerous disorders and traits; and many have discussed and ordered tests for GTs; but have relatively little knowledge about available tests. These data suggest possible gender differences in psychiatrist’s beliefs about genetic contributions to disorders; and have implications for future research, education, policy, and care.

Keywords: Genomics, epigenetics, genetic testing, decision making, etiology

Introduction

Numerous studies continue to probe possible genetic contributions to a wide range of mental disorders and behavioral traits, but many questions remain concerning how psychiatrists themselves view these issues. With the identification of genetic markers associated with diagnoses and responses to treatment, and decreasing costs of whole genome and exome sequencing, psychiatrists may well be called upon increasingly to incorporate genetic information into clinical practice (Laegsgaard and Mors, 2008; Hoge and Appelbaum, 2012). Hence, the views and approaches of these providers towards genetic testing can potentially shape future diagnosis, treatment, and disease prevention (Farmer et al., 2007; Hoop et al., 2008; Mitchell et al., 2010).

To date, susceptibility genes have been associated to varying degrees with several neuropsychiatric disorders including Huntington’s disease (HD), Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Selkoe, 1996; Selkoe and Podlisny, 2002) and epilepsy (Ottman et al., 2010). Emerging evidence suggests that copy number variants and single gene mutations may be found in relatively small proportions of patients with other psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia (Walsh et al. 2008, Tam et al., 2009; Shi et al., 2009), ADHD (Thapar et al. 1999; Stergiakouli and Thapar, 2010), major depression (Caspi et al., 2003; Wilhelm et al., 2009), and autism (Glessner et al., 2009). In 2012, researchers claimed to have developed a genetic test with clinical utility for autism spectrum disorders (Skafidas et al.), but that claim has since been refuted (Robinson et al., 2013). Many of the genes are shared among these disorders, providing evidence for similar genetic risks to a wide range of diagnoses with varying severity (Sullivan et al., 2012).

Clinically significant behavioral traits, such as aggression and antisocial behavior, have been associated with a variant of the MAOA gene, in combination with a history of abuse (Caspi et al., 2002). Clearly, tests vary in penetrance and predictiveness. While HD is a Mendelian dominant disorder, and the mutation is fully penetrant and predictive, other genetic markers mentioned above vary widely in the degrees to which they contribute to disease in various patients. Psychiatrists could also use other genetic tests for medical conditions that can cause psychiatric symptoms, such as mitochondrial disorders, porphyria and other monogenic disorders (Dimauro and Schon, 2008; Simon and Herkes 2011). Genetic markers associated with psychiatric pharmacogenomics are also being sought, identified, and employed. Indeed, there may be wider understanding and use of pharmacogenomics among psychiatrists than tests for markers directly associated with diseases (Mrazek, 2010). How widely pharmacogenomics will be used is uncertain, but they may potentially improve treatment of depression and anxiety, and identification of past inappropriate prescription of medication (Winner et al., 2013).

Direct-to-consumer marketing (DTC) of genetic tests, which often includes variants associated with psychiatric disorders, has also been increasing, though recently questioned by the FDA (Klitzman, 2013). Yet many internists have been found to have significant deficits in understanding genetic tests (Klitzman et al., 2013), and there is reason to be concerned that this may be true of many psychiatrists as well.

Only a few studies have examined psychiatrists’ attitudes and practices concerning genetic tests. Most psychiatrists believe that they are the most appropriate mental health professionals to counsel patients about the possible impact of genetics on patients’ diagnoses (Hoop et al., 2008) and see discussing genetic information as clinically relevant and part of psychiatrists’ role (Hoop et al., 2008; Finn et al., 2005). In 2006, A large majority of 45 U.S. psychiatrists thought genetic testing would have high utility for determining a patient’s optimal dose of medication (73%), and risk of serious side effects from psychiatric medication (82%), for predicting severity of mental illness (85%), and assessing risk of an asymptomatic person developing mental illness (84%) (Hoop et al., 2008). Of 64 researchers and clinicians who worked with patients with schizophrenia, 72% indicated that they would test all patients with initial diagnoses of schizophrenia, even if a test with only limited diagnostic power were available (DeLisi and Bertisch, 2006). Of 352 psychiatrists surveyed in 2005, 45% said they would use genetic tests for schizophrenia, if available, to test asymptomatic adults with a family history (Finn et al., 2005). In 2006, 9 of 41 (20.9%) psychiatrists surveyed had ordered a genetic test in the previous five years (Hoop et al., 2008). Yet, in another study, only 23% of psychiatrists felt competent to talk with patients about genetic information, 15% felt adequately trained to do so, and only 1% could answer ten genetics questions correctly (Finn et al., 2005). Psychiatrists would welcome additional education in genetics (Lawrence and Appelbaum, 2011).

As genetic research continues to advance rapidly, it is important to understand whether these views and practices may have changed over time, and what variables may affect them (e.g., age and gender of the provider, understandings of genetic contributions to psychiatric disorders, knowledge of the availability of genetic tests). Indeed, prior research by one of us (RK) found that among psychiatrists, women were more likely than men to have psychotherapeutic rather than biological orientations toward treatment of psychiatric disorders (Bodkin, Klitzman, Pope, 1995), which may also impact their attitudes toward and use of genetic tests. To address these issues, we thus examined the views of genetic influences and use of genetic tests among a large sample of psychiatrists.

Methods

In conjunction with the American Medical Association (AMA), we sent e-mails describing the study and inviting participation from psychiatrists on the AMA Master Lists who had provided e-mail addresses and had agreed to receive survey invitations. We contacted 5,316 psychiatrists in September and October 2011. We did not follow-up through direct mail or phone calls, nor compensate respondents. We received 372 responses (response rate: 7.0%). The New York State Psychiatric Institute IRB approved the study. The survey instrument was developed on the basis of our prior, related study of internists (Klitzman et al., 2013), a literature review, drawing on items reflecting attitudes and behaviors in two prior studies of psychiatrists by other investigators (Hoop and Finn) and our clinical experience, and used the Survey Monkey online survey system (www.surveymonkey.com). Domains probed included: 1) demographics of respondents and their patients; 2) respondents’ self-reported knowledge of genetics and genetics testing; 3) practices regarding genetic testing and privacy of genetic information; 4) barriers and facilitators to use of genetic tests; 4) attitudes toward tests and diagnoses; and 5) perceived needs for education. The survey included an information sheet that described the study, and indicated that participants’ consent would be presumed by their completing survey questions. Before distribution, the survey was piloted with three psychiatrists, who provided feedback, and was revised accordingly.

Statistical analyses included chi-squared tests for examination of categorical variables, simple binary logistic regressions, and a multiple logistic regression to explore independent variables associated with differences in respondents’ characteristics and beliefs by age and gender. We first assessed associations with age (<49 vs. >49 or older) and gender separately, and then entered both age and gender into a multiple logistic regression model. As this was an initial, exploratory study of these rapidly emerging areas, we did not test the internal validity of the scale; however, the items here appear to have a certain face validity.

Results

Sociodemographic characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1. As shown on Table 2, in the past six months 14.1% of psychiatrists had ordered a genetic test, 18.3% had discussed with patients prenatal genetic testing for non-psychiatric conditions, 51.9% had discussed genetic testing with patients (36.0% had initiated discussions with patients about genetic testing, and 41.6% had patients ask about genetic testing) and 5.6% had excluded genetic information from patient records.

Table 1.

Socio-demographic Characteristics of Respondents

Total % (N*)
100% (372)

Gender
Male 61.8% (162)
Female 38.2% (100)

Age
<49 years 65.6% (172)
>49 years 34.4% (90)

Race
White 70.4% (183)
Asian 13.8% (36)
Prefer not to answer 9.6% (25)
Other 4.6% (12)
Black or African American 1.2% (3)
Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander 0.4% (1)

Religion
Protestant 19.1% (50)
Jewish 17.2% (45)
Prefer not to answer 13.7% (36)
Catholic 12.6% (33)
Other 37.4% (98)

Primary Practice Setting
Private Practice 57.3% (150)
Other 42.7% (112)

Approximately, what % of patient population is:
White
<75% 64.5% (160)
>75% 35.5% (88)

Covered by No Insurance
<25% 60.1% (134)
>25% 39.9% (89)

Table 2.

Behavior of Respondents with respect to Genetic Tests

Total Gender Binary Logistic (Controlling for Age)

% (N) Male Female AOR1 p value 2 CI 3

Total 100% (372)

Have you discussed prenatal GT for non-psychiatric condition?
Yes 18.3% (48) 74.4% (32) 75.0% (30)
No 81.7% (214) 25.6% (11) 25.0% (10)

In past 6 months, have patients asked about GT? 1.72 0.038 1.03–2.86
Yes 41.6% (129) 37.7% (61) 51.0% (51)
No 58.4% (181) 62.3% (101) 49.0% (49)

In past 6 months, have you ordered a GT?
Yes 14.1% (37) 14.2% (23) 14.0% (14)
No 85.9% (225) 85.8% (139) 86.0% (86)

Have you ever excluded genetic information from the patient record?
Yes 5.3% (14) 5.6% (9) 5.0% (5)
No 94.7% (248) 94.4% (153) 95.0% (95)
1

Adjusted Odds Ratio

2

P value shown if <0.1

3

Confidence Interval

As shown on Table 3, of respondents, 61.7% knew that a genetic test was available for Alzheimer’s disease risk, yet many also mistakenly thought that comparable tests were available for major depression (19.6%), obsessive-compulsive disorder (9.9%), suicidality (6.6%), intelligence (7.6%), and social anxiety disorder (4.7%).

Table 3.

Respondent’s Knowledge and Attitudes about Genetic Tests

Total Gender Binary Logistic (Controlling for Age)

% (N) Male Female AOR a p value b CI c

Total 100% (372)

To best of your understanding, is there a GT available for:
Alzheimer’s
Yes d 61.7% (171) 60.0% (96) 65.7% (65)
No 38.3% (106) 40.0% (64) 34.3% (34)

Autism
Yes 19.6% (54) 18.1% (29) 21.2% (21)
No e 80.4% (221) 81.9% (131) 78.8% (78)

Borderline Personality Disorder 0.14 0.068 0.02–1.16
Yes 4.0% (11) 6.3% (10) 1.0% (1)
No f 96.0% (264) 93.7% (149) 99.0% (98)

Intelligence
Yes 7.6% (21) 8.8% (14) 7.1% (7)
Nof 92.4% (254) 91.2% (145) 92.9% (92)

Major Depression
Yes 19.6% (54) 18.2% (29) 22.0% (22)
Nof 80.4% (222) 81.8% (130) 78.0% (78)

Obsessive Compulsive Disorder
Yes 9.9% (27) 11.4% (18) 8.1% (8)
Nof 90.1% (247) 88.6% (140) 91.9% (91)

Schizophrenia 0.61 0.088 0.34–1.07
Yes 25.0% (65) 21.2% (34) 31.0% (31)
No g 75.0% (195) 78.8% (126) 69.0% (69)

Social Anxiety Disorder 0.13 0.049 0.02–0.99
Yes 4.7% (13) 7.5% (12) 1.0% (1)
Nof 95.3% (261) 92.5% (147) 99.0% (97)

Suicidality
Yes 6.6% (18) 8.2% (13) 4.1% (4)
Nof 93.4% (255) 91.8% (145) 95.9% (94)

In the future, will GT have a role in diagnosing:
Alzheimer’s
Yes 94.2% (243) 94.3% (150) 93.9% (93)
No 5.8%(15) 5.7% (9) 6.1% (6)

Autism
Yes 83.7% (216) 83.0% (132) 84.8% (84)
No 16.3% (42) 17.0% (27) 15.2% (15)

Bipolar Disorder 0.56 0.06 0.30–1.02
Yes 79.2% (205) 83% (132) 73% (73)
No 20.8% (54) 17% (27) 27% (27)

Major Depression 0.63 0.094 0.37–1.08
Yes 68.3% (177) 72.3% (115) 62% (62)
No 31.7% (82) 27.7% (44) 38% (38)

Schizophrenia 0.49 0.03 0.26–0.93
Yes 81.2% (211) 85% (136) 75% (75)
No 18.8% (49) 15% (24) 25% (25)
a

Adjusted Odds Ratio

b

P value shown if <0.1

c

Confidence Interval

d

A genetic test exists and is used clinically, though not widely.

e

At time of survey, recent studies had indicated a viable test had been developed. This has since been refuted.

f

No clinically useful genetic test exists.

g

Genetic markers have been identified for a subset of patients, though a clinically useful test does not exist.

Most psychiatrists thought that the extent of genetic contribution (Table 4) was moderate or high for bipolar disorder (88.7%), schizophrenia (85.2%), major depression (81.7%), Alzheimer’s (79.3%), intelligence (76.6%), panic disorder (67.6%), creativity (60.9%), social anxiety disorder (57.3%), dysthymia, (55.2%), and suicidality (52.9%). Most also believed there was low or no genetic contribution for anorexia (50.7%), homosexuality (50.9%), and pedophilia (64.1%).

Table 4.

Respondents’ Understanding of Genetic Contribution to Psychiatric Disorders

Total Gender Binary Logistic(Controlling for Age)

% (N) Male Female AOR a p value b CI c

Total 100% (372)

Extent of genetic contribution to:
Alzheimer’s
Unsure 8.3% (24) 4.4% (7) 12.0% (12) 3.12 0.023 1.07–8.33
Moderate/High 79.3% (230) 81.1% (130) 78.0% (78)
None/Very Low/Low 12.4% (36) 13.8% (22) 10.0% (10)

Anorexia
Unsure 12.8% (37) 6.3% (10) 21.2% (21) 4.17 0.001 1.84–9.43
Moderate/High 36.6% (106) 43.8% (70) 29.3% (29)
None/Very Low/Low 50.7% (147) 50.0% (80) 49.5% (49)

Antisocial Personality Disorder
Unsure 13.4% (39) 7.5% (12) 21.0% (21) 3.75 0.001 1.71–8.13
Moderate/High 45.4% (132) 48.8% (78) 43.0% (43)
None/Very Low/Low 41.2% (120) 43.8% (70) 36.0% (36)

Autism
Unsure 10.8% (31) 6.3% (10) 16.2% (16) 2.84 0.016 1.22–6.62
Moderate/High 56.6% (163) 58.9% (93) 55.6% (55)
None/Very Low/Low 32.6% (94) 34.8% (55) 28.3% (28)

Bipolar Disorder
Unsure 5.2% (15) 2.5% (4) 7.0% (7) 3.68 0.045 1.03–13.16
Moderate/High 88.7% (258) 91.9% (147) 88.0% (88)
None/Very Low/Low 6.2% (16) 5.6% (9) 5.0% (5)

Borderline Personality Disorder
Unsure 11.3% (33) 5.0% (8) 19.0% (19) 4.70 0.001 1.94–11.36
Moderate/High 39.9% (116) 43.8% (70) 35.0% (35)
None/Very Low/Low 48.8% (142) 51.3% (82) 36.0% (46)

Creativity
Unsure 12.5% (36) 7.5% (12) 17.0% (17) 2.65 0.017 1.19–5.88
Moderate/High 60.9% (176) 59.7% (95) 63.0% (63) 1.88 0.046 1.01–3.50
None/Very Low/Low 26.6% (77) 32.7% (52) 20.0% (20)

Dysthymia
Unsure 9.0% (26) 5.0% (8) 11.0% (11) 2.66 0.047 1.01–6.99
Moderate/High 55.2% (160) 56.0% (89) 59.0% (59)
None/Very Low/Low 35.9% (104) 39.0% (62) 30.0% (30)

Homosexuality
Unsure 14.8% (43) 10.6% (17) 21.0% (21) 2.19 0.029 1.08–4.42
Moderate/High 34.4% (100) 35.0% (56) 31.0% (31)
None/Very Low/Low 50.9% (148) 54.4% (87) 48.0% (48)

Major Depression
Unsure 5.5% (16) 1.9% (3) 8.1% (8) 5.44 0.015 1.38–21.3
Moderate/High 81.7% (237) 85.0% (136) 81.8% (81)
None/Very Low/Low 12.8% (37) 13.1% (21) 10.1% (10)

Panic Disorder
Unsure 8.3% (24) 4.4% (7) 12.0% (12) 3.12 0.023 1.17–8.33
Moderate/High 67.6% (196) 69.8% (111) 67.0% (67)
None/Very Low/Low 24.1% (70) 25.8% (41) 21.0% (21)

Pathological Gambling
Unsure 15.3% (44) 8.2% (13) 27.3% (27) 4.27 0.001 2.06–8.85
Moderate/High 36.8% (106) 40.3% (64) 36.4% (36)
None/Very Low/Low 47.9% (138) 51.6% (82) 36.4% (36)

Pedophilia
Unsure 23.8% (69) 16.4% (26) 35.0% (35) 2.79 0.001 1.54–5.05
Moderate/High 12.1% (35) 13.2% (21) 9.0% (9)
None/Very Low/Low 64.1% (186) 70.4% (112) 56.0% (56)

Schizophrenia
Unsure 4.8% (14) 1.9% (3) 6.0% (6) 3.77 0.068 0.91–15.6
Moderate/High 85.2% (248) 86.9% (139) 86.0% (86)
None/Very Low/Low 10.0% (29) 11.3% (18) 8.0% (8)

Social Anxiety Disorder
Unsure 10.4% (30) 7.5% (12) 13.1% (13)
Moderate/High 57.3% (165) 54.1% (86) 60.6% (60) 1.67 0.083 0.94–2.97
None/Very Low/Low 32.3% (93) 38.4% (61) 26.3% (26)

Suicidality
Unsure 11.7% (34) 6.9% (11) 17.0% (17) 2.74 0.015 1.21–6.21
Moderate/High 52.9% (154) 56.3% (90) 50.0% (50)
None/Very Low/Low 35.4% (103) 36.9% (59) 33.0% (33)
a

Adjusted Odds Ratio

b

P value shown if <0.1

c

Confidence Interval

Younger respondents were more likely to be women (44.0% vs. 28.4%, p<.010), and as a trend, were less likely to believe there is a genetic test for intelligence (OR: 0.29, p<0.054). When controlling for gender younger respondents were, as trends, less likely to be uncertain about the genetic contribution to bipolar (AOR: 0.14, CI: 0.02–1.16, p<0.07), or to believe a genetic test existed for intelligence (AOR: 0.30, CI: 0.08–1.04, p<0.059).

We then explored gender, controlling for age, and found that women were more likely than men to have a patient ask about genetic tests (51.0% vs. 37.7%, AOR=1.719, CI: 1.03–2.86, p<0.038), and be unsure about the degree of genetic contribution to many psychiatric disorders and behavioral traits (Table 4). Women were less likely to think that genetic tests would have a role in diagnosing schizophrenia.

Discussion

These data suggest that most psychiatrists are discussing genetic tests with patients, either on their own initiative or in response to patients’ inquiries. However, only 14.1% had ordered a genetic test in the preceding 6 months. Most perceived strong genetic bases for certain psychiatric disorders and behavioral traits, but a sizable minority appeared misinformed about the availability of genetic tests for several disorders (e.g., 38.3% did not know that genetic testing is currently available for AD). In addition, gender differences may exist in some of these views. A small but notable percentage of psychiatrists (5.2%) reported having excluded genetic information from medical records in response to concerns about patients’ genetic privacy. Whether patients or providers initiate this practice is unclear. Our past research suggests that patient groups, particularly those confronting HD, may request such exclusions,. We found a similar rate among internists (4.5%) (Klitzman et al., 2013). Clearly the practice deserves further study, to understand when and why it occurs and with what possible implications.

As suggested in Tables 4 and 5, over time, psychiatrists’ perceptions of genetic contributions to various disorders and behavioral traits appear to have increased. The two prior studies conducted by other researchers (Finn et al. 2005; Hoop et al. 2008) used different metrics, which makes direct comparisons difficult, though overall trends emerge. Specifically, Finn et al. reported the means of respondents’ estimates of the percent risk of developing each condition due to genetic factors (ascertained by filling in a blank); while Hoop reported the proportion of respondents who chose each of the 4 forced choices of qualitative descriptions of the influence of genetics (scaled 1= none, 2 = weak, 3 = moderate, 4 = strong). Nonetheless, comparisons suggest possible patterns.

Table 5.

Views and Extents of Heritability of Psychiatric Disorders and Behavioral Conditions

Condition Finn et al. (2005)
Median % perceived risk of developing condition due to heritable genetic factors
Hoop et al. (2008)
% of psychiatrists who thought the extent of genetic contribution in developing condition is moderate/high
Klitzman et al. (2013)
% of psychiatrists who thought the extent of genetic contribution in developing condition is moderate/high
Actual Extent of Contribution
Alzheimer’s 20% 94% 79.3% 0.58–0.79 (Gatz et al., 2006)
Anorexia 36.6% 56%–84% (Klump et al., 2001)
Antisocial Personality Disorder 20% (in males) 52% 45.4% .65 (aggressive rule breaking)
.48 (non-aggressive rule breaking) (Burt, 2009)
Autism 10% 63% 56.6% 0.70–0.90 (Geschwind, 2009)
Bipolar Disorder 40% 95% 88.7% 0.71 (Edvardsen et al., 2009)
Borderline Personality Disorder 39.9% .51 (Distel et al., 2010)
Creativity 78% 60.9% N/A
Dysthymia 55.2% .29 (Savitz et al., 2008)
Homosexuality 34.4% .34–.39 (Langstrom et al., 2008)
Intelligence 76.6% 0.50–0.90 (Devlin et al., 1997)
Major Depression 30% 89% 81.7% 0.40 (females)
0.30 (males) (Kendler et al., 2006)
Obsessive Compulsive Disorder 78.3% 0.27 (Van Grootheest et al., 2005)
Panic Disorder 25% 78% 67.6% .40–.50 (Spatola et al., 2010)
Pathological Gambling 36.8% .27–.61 (Slutske et al., 2010)
Pedophilia 12.1% 14.6% (Alanko et al., 2013)
Schizophrenia 30% 93% 85.2% 0.41–0.87 (Cannon et al., 1998)
Social Anxiety Disorder 57.3% 0.30–0.50 (Kendler et al., 1999)
Suicidality 52.9% 43% (McGuffin et al, 2001)

In 2005, Finn et al. reported that a sample of psychiatrists thought the extent of genetic contribution was relatively low. The percent risks were estimated as follows (as shown in parentheses): schizophrenia (30%), autism (10%), Alzheimer’s disease (20%), ADHD (25%), alcoholism (30%), panic disorder (25%), and antisocial personality disorder (20%). In 2008, Hoop et al. found that a different group of psychiatrists reported beliefs that the influence of genetics was moderate to strong for bipolar disorder (3.53), schizophrenia (3.37), Alzheimer’s disease (3.30), ADHD (3.28), major depression (3.24), panic disorder (2.86), autism (2.86), and antisocial personality disorder (2.62). Since these studies were conducted, genetic research has continued to grow enormously, examining a wide range of psychiatric, neurologic, and other disorders, and clarifying their genetic contributions including copy number variation (Malhotra and Sebat, 2012) and the role of de novo genetic events in the absence of a family history of the disorder (Kong et al., 2012). Still, additional research using larger experimental sample sizes is needed (Visscher et al., 2012).

Our data suggest that psychiatrists now perceive more genetic contribution to certain psychiatric disorders, reflecting growing research and new findings over time on the genetic bases of mental health disorders. Yet, many psychiatrists remain unsure of the genetic contribution to various disorders, even for conditions where the degree of heritability has been clearly established.

Compared with available scientific data on heritability of various traits and psychiatric conditions, our respondents were relatively accurate, but with some important exceptions. As shown on Table 5, most psychiatrists correctly thought that the genetic contribution was moderate or high for several disorders and characteristics for which the heritability (shown in Table 5 and in parentheses below) is above 0.5: autism (0.70–0.90) (Geschwind, 2009), bipolar disorder (0.71) (Edvardsen et al., 2008), intelligence (0.50–0.90) (Devlin et al., 1997), schizophrenia (0.41–0.87) (Cannon et al., 1998), and Alzheimer’s disease (0.58–0.79) (Gatz et al., 2006). However, for several disorders, most respondents overestimated the genetic contribution, thinking that it was high when it is estimated to be less than 0.5: social anxiety disorder (0.30–0.50) (Kendler et al., 1999), major depression (0.40 for women, 0.30 for men) (Kendler et al., 2006), and obsessive compulsive disorder (0.27–0.47) (Van Grootheest et al., 2005). For anorexia, most respondents underestimated the genetic contribution, thinking it was low, though scientists estimate heritability to be 56% to 84% (Klump et al., 2001). The fact that psychiatrists overestimate the genetic contribution for major depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and social anxiety disorder, and underestimate it for anorexia is noteworthy, as these beliefs may reflect patterns of diagnosis for these disorders. These data highlight important needs for enhanced education of psychiatrists through both training programs and CME.

Although Hoop reported in 2008 that 20.9% of psychiatrists had ordered genetic tests in the prior 5 years, we found that 14.1% of respondents had done so in the previous six months, suggesting a higher rate per year now than in the past. No previous studies have examined whether and how often psychiatrists or patients initiate discussions about genetic tests. Of note, a sizable proportion of our respondents (41.6%) reported that patients had asked about genetic tests, slightly more than the percentage of respondents who had themselves initiated these discussions (35%). DTC testing may contribute to patients’ questions, highlighting the importance of psychiatrists being aware of such services, and able to address issues that they may raise for patients.

These data also suggest possible gender differences with respect to how psychiatrists view genetics. More women than men reported that they were uncertain about genetic contributions to several psychiatric disorders, which may reflect either greater uncertainty or more willingness to admit uncertainty. Uncertainty about particular issues may not reflect knowledge deficits, but rather unwillingness to endorse categorical statements, given that many disorders result from a complex interplay of genetic and environmental influences. Interestingly tolerance for ambiguity has been found to be higher among female than male medical students, especially among those who identify with a more “humanistic” approach to treatment methods (Geller et al., 1990), though other studies show more mixed results (Geller et al., 1993). The fact that women were less likely than men to think that in the future genetic tests will have a role in diagnosing schizophrenia and, as trends, bipolar disorder and major depression, suggests that women may be warier of emphasizing genetics in understanding psychiatric etiology. This finding is in line with prior research that indicated that among psychiatrists, women were less likely than men to have a biological orientation in treatment of psychiatric disorders (Bodkin et al., 1995). Of note, women psychiatrists’ reduced certainty about genetic contributions to psychiatric disorders and unlikelihood to think genetic tests will have a future role in diagnosing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depression also reflect, anecdotally, the views of many psychiatric geneticists, as suggested by the range of heritability estimates in Table 5. Clearly, additional research is needed to explore these gender differences more fully.

Compared to the results of our prior survey of internists concerning genetic testing, we found psychiatrists were less likely to have ordered tests (14.1% vs. 44.0%) but only slightly less likely to have patients ask about tests (41.6% vs. 50.6%). Both groups only rarely did not include test results in patients’ medical records (5.2% vs. 4.5%). These differences not only suggest that internists have more experience ordering genetic tests, but that patients seen by both sets of doctors have relatively high levels of interest in genetic testing. Additional research is also needed to assess the differences between physicians in these and other specialties (e.g. pediatrics, obstetricians/gynecologists, etc.)

These data may have important implications for professional education. Psychiatrists’ knowledge of genetics and of the roles of genetic testing are clearly imperfect. Genetics and genomics have been playing increasing roles in the medical school curriculum, suggesting that recent medical school graduates may be more knowledgeable about genetics (Korf, 2002). However, only 47% of U.S. and Canadian medical schools report incorporating genetics into the third and fourth years (Thurston et al., 2007). More genetics may be entering the curriculum (Dhar et al., 2012), yet the absence of an age effect in our data indicates a general need for more education of psychiatrists across all stages of their careers, regarding the genetic bases of disorders and the uses of genetic testing. To handle growing amounts of genetics research on psychiatric disorders, the incorporation of additional genetics training during psychiatric residency appears to be critical to prepare future psychiatrists. In a 2010 survey, 39% of psychiatric residency program educators and 55% of trainees reported little or no genetics instruction in their curricula. Only 20% and 16% respectively reported moderate or a great deal of emphasis. Almost half of educators indicated they had few or no faculty with sufficient expertise in instructing trainees about genetics (Hoop et al., 2010). CME activities should also emphasize understanding of genetic associations that are being discovered, and their limitations. On-going research over time is also needed to assess knowledge of genetics and needs for education among these groups.

This study has several limitations. We had a relatively low response rate from those invited to participate; however, we did not follow-up via direct mail or phone calls, and did not compensate respondents – all of which might have increased the response rate. Nonetheless, this sample is the largest to date of psychiatrists concerning their views and behaviors in relation to genetics, and the first to examine several critical issues. Moreover, our sample did not differ significantly from a national sample of psychiatrists (based on data obtained from the American Psychiatric Association) in gender, race (white vs. non-white), or proportion in solo practice. Furthermore, in recent years response rates in studies generally have been declining steeply (Huber et al., 2011), especially among physicians (Galea et al., 2007; Cull et al., 2005), yet research has suggested that low response rates do not necessarily result in non-response bias (Galea et al., 2007). Such bias in any case may be less of a concern in surveys of doctors, compared with surveys of the general public (Asch et al., 2000; Kellerman and Herold, 2001; Guadagnoli and Cunningham, 1989; Hovland et al., 1980), since there is more consistency in knowledge, training and behavior among physicians than within the population at large. We also relied on self-reports, with the usual uncertain effects on validity of responses. Although there is no reason to suspect that the effects of social desirability played a role in the answers of our respondents, recall of events may vary in accuracy. Finally, due to the exploratory nature of this study, we did not adjust our statistical analyses for multiple comparisons.

Conclusions

In sum, these data, the first to explore several key aspects of how psychiatrists view and respond to genetic information, reflect growing levels of genetic knowledge, possible gender differences in perceptions of these issues, and needs for further research into these issues and for enhanced professional education. The extent of inaccurate information about the availability of certain genetic tests is of concern, and highlights needs for educational outreach and distribution of educational materials. More referrals of psychiatric patients to genetic counselors or other knowledgeable professionals may also be appropriate. Especially given advances in genetic research, use of DTC testing, and rapidly decreasing costs of genome sequencing, these issues are of increasing importance.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Matthew Wynia, MD and Patricia Contino, MFA and Jennifer Teitcher, BA for their assistance with this manuscript.

Funding: This work was supported by NHGRI grant #1P20HG005535-01 and #1P50HG007257-01 (Paul Appelbaum, PI).

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest

The authors had no affiliations, nor did they perform actions, that might be regarded as potential conflicts of interest during the grant funding, data collection, or manuscript preparation stages of their work.

References

  1. Alanko K, Salo B, Mokros A, Santtila P. Evidence for heritability of adult men’s sexual interest in youth under age 16 from a population-based extended twin design. J Sex Med. 2013;10:1090–1099. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Asch S, Connor SE, Hamilton EG, Fox SA. Problems in recruiting community-based physicians for health services research. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15(8):591–599. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2000.02329.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bodkin JA, Klitzman RL, Pope HG. Treatment in orientation and associate characteristics of North American Academic psychiatrists. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1995;183(12):729–735. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199512000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Burt SA. Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29:163–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cannon TD, Kaprio J, Lönnqvist J, Huttunen M, Koskenvuo M. The genetic epidemiology of schizophrenia in a Finnish twin cohort: A population-based modeling study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:67–74. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caspi A, McClay J, Moffit TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IW, Taylor A, Poulton R. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science. 2002;297:851–853. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington HL, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J, Braithwaite A, Poulton R. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cull W, O’Connor KG, Sharp S, Tang SS. Response rates and response bias for 50 surveys of pediatricians. Health Serv Res. 2005;40(1):213–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2005.00350.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. DeLisi L, Bertisch H. A preliminary comparison of the hopes of researchers, clinicians, and families for the future ethical use of genetic findings on schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;141B (1):110–115. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dhar SU, Alford RL, Nelson EA, Potocki L. Enhancing exposure to genetics and genomics through an innovative medical school curriculum. Genet Med. 2012;14(1):163–167. doi: 10.1038/gim.0b013e31822dd7d4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dimauro S, Schon EA. Mitochondrial disorders in the nervous system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2008;31:91–123. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Distel MA, Willemsen G, Ligthart L, Derom CA, Martin NG, Neale MC, Trull TJ, Boomsma DI. Genetic covariance structure of the four main features of borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2010;24(4):427–444. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.4.427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Devlin B, Daniels M, Roeder K. The heritability of IQ. Nature. 1997;388(6641):468–471. doi: 10.1038/41319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Edvardsen J, Torgersen S, Røysamb E, Lygren S, Skre I, Onstad S, Øien PA. Heritability of bipolar spectrum disorders. Unity or heterogeneity? J Affect Disord. 2008;106(3):229–240. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Farmer A, Elkin A, McGuffin P. The genetics of bipolar affective disorder. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2007;20(1):8–12. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280117722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Finn CT, Wilcox MA, Korf BR, Blacker D, Racette SR, Sklar P, Smoller JW. Psychiatric genetics: A survey of psychiatrists’ knowledge, opinions, and practice patterns. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66:821–830. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Galea S, Tracy M. Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Ann Epidemiol. 2007;17(9):643–653. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gatz M, Reynolds CA, Fratiglioni L, Johansson B, Mortimer JA, Berg S. Role of genes and environments for explaining Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(2):168–174. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.2.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Geller G, Faden RR, Levine DM. Tolerance for ambiguity among medical students: Implications for their selection, training and practice. Soc Sci Med. 1990;31:619. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(90)90098-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Geller G, Tambor ES, Chase GA, Holtzman NA. Measuring physicians’ tolerance for ambiguity and its relationship to their reported practices regarding genetic testing. Medical Care. 1993;31(11):989–1001. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199311000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Geschwind DH. Advances in autism. Annu Rev Med. 2009;60:367–380. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.60.053107.121225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Glessner JT, Wang K, Cai G, Korvatska O, Kim CE, Wood S, Hakonarson H. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature. 2009;459:569–573. doi: 10.1038/nature07953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Guadagnoli E, Cunningham S. The effects of non-response and late response on a survey of physician attitudes. Eval Health Prof. 1989;12:318–328. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hoge SK, Applebaum PS. Ethics and neuropsychiatric genetics: A review of major issues. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;15:1547–1557. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711001982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hoop JG, Roberts L, Hammond K, Cox NJ. Psychiatrists’ attitudes regarding genetic testing and patient safeguards: A preliminary study. Genet Test. 2008;12:245–252. doi: 10.1089/gte.2007.0097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hoop JG, Weiss Roberts L, Green Hammond KA, Cox NJ. Psychiatrists’ attitudes, knowledge, and experience regarding genetics: A preliminary study. Genet Med. 2008;10(6):439–449. doi: 10.1097/GIM.0b013e318177014b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hoop JG, Savla G, Weiss Roberts L, Zisook S, Dunn LB. The current state of genetics training in psychiatric residency: Views of 235 U.S. educators and trainees. Acad Psychiatr. 2010;34:109–114. doi: 10.1176/appi.ap.34.2.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hovland EJ, Romberg E, Moreland EF. Nonresponse bias to mail survey questionnaires within a professional population. J Dent Educ. 1980;44(5):270–274. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Huber S, Vorhaus D. [Accessed June 10, 2013];Genetic Bill of Rights Proposed in Massachusetts. 2011 Available from: http://www.genomicslawreport.com/wp-content/uploads/2011/02/MA-GBR-Text.pdf.
  30. Kellerman S, Herold J. Physician response to surveys: A review of the literature. Am J Prev Med. 2001;20(1):61–67. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(00)00258-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kendler K, Karkowski L, Prescott C. Fears and phobias: Reliability and heritability. Psychol Med. 1999;29(3):539–553. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799008429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kendler K, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL. A Swedish national twin study of lifetime major depression. Ame J Psych. 2006;163(1):109–114. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Klitzman R. Why genetic testing could endanger your health. Chicago Tribune. 2013 Dec 4; Retreived from: http://articles.chicagotribune.com/2013-12-04/site/ct-genetics-testing-diabeties-alcoholism-23andme-p-20131204-2_1_mutations-test-kit-craig-venter.
  34. Klitzman R, Chung W, Marder K, Shanmugham A, Chin LJ, Stark M, Leu CS, Appelbaum PS. Attitudes and practices among internists concerning genetic testing. J Genet Couns. 2013;22:90–100. doi: 10.1007/s10897-012-9504-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Klump KL, Miller KB, Keel PK, McGue M, Iacono WG. Genetic and environmental influences on anorexia nervosa syndromes in a population-based twin sample. Psychol Med. 2001;31(4):737–740. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701003725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kong A, Frigge ML, Masson G, Besenbacher S, Sulem P, Magnusson G, Gudjonsson SA, Stefansson K. Rate of de novo mutations and the importance of father’s age to disease risk. Nature. 2012;488:471–475. doi: 10.1038/nature11396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Korf BR. Integration of genetics into clinical teaching in medical school education. Genet Med. 2002;4 (6S):33S–38S. doi: 10.1097/00125817-200211001-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Laegsgaard MM, Mors O. Psychiatric genetic testing: Attitudes and intentions among future users and providers. Am J Med Genet. 2008;147(Part B):375–384. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Langstrom N, Rahman Q, Carlstrom E, Lichtenstein P. Genetic and environmental effects on same-sex sexual behavior: A population study of twins in Sweden. Arch Sex Behav. 2010;39:75–80. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9386-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lapham EV, Kozma C, Weiss JO, Benkendorf JL, Wilson MA. The gap between practice and genetic education of health professionals: HuGEM survey results. Genet Med. 2000;2(4):226–231. doi: 10.1097/00125817-200007000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lawrence R, Appelbaum P. Genetic testing in psychiatry: A review of attitudes and beliefs. Psychiatry. 2011;74(4):315–331. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2011.74.4.315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Malhotra D, Sebat J. CNVs: Harbingers of a rare variant revolution in psychiatric genetics. Cell. 2012;148:1223–1241. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.02.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McGuffin P, Marusic A, Farmer A. What can psychiatric genetics offer suicidology? Crisis. 2001;22:61–65. doi: 10.1027//0227-5910.22.2.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mitchell PB, Meiser B, Wilde A, Fullerton J, Donald J, Wilhelm K, Schofield PR. Predictive and diagnostic genetic testing in psychiatry. Clin Lab Sci. 2010;30:829. doi: 10.1016/j.cll.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Mrazek DA. Psychiatric pharmacogenomic testing in clinical practice. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2010;12:69–76. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2010.12.1/dmrazek. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ottman R, Hirose S, Jain S, Lerche H, Lopes-Cendes I, Noebels JL, Serratosa J, Zara F, Scheffer IE. Genetic testing in the epilepsies – Report of the ILAE Genetics Commission. Epilepsia. 2010;51:655–670. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02429.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Robinson EB, Howrigan D, Yang J, Ripke S, Antilla V, Duncan LE, Jostins L, Barret JC, Medland SE, MacArthur DG, Breen G, O’Donovan MC, Wray NR, Devlin B, Daly MJ, Visscher PM, Sullivan PF, Neale BM. Response to ‘Predicting the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder using gene pathway analysis.’. Mol Psychiatr. 2013 doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.125. Epub: 1–3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Savitz J, Van der Merwe L, Ramesar R. Personality endophenotypes for bipolar affective disorder: A family-based genetic association analysis. GenesBrain Behav. 2008;7:869–876. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00426.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Selkoe DJ. Amyloid β-protein and the genetics of Alzheimer’s disease. J Biol Chem. 1996;271:18295–18298. doi: 10.1074/jbc.271.31.18295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Selkoe DJ, Podlisny MB. Deciphering the genetic basis of Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2002;3:67–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genom.3.022502.103022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shi J, Levinson DF, Duan J, Sanders AR, Zheng Y, Pe’er I, Gejman PV. Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia. Nature. 2009;460(7256):753–757. doi: 10.1038/nature08192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Simon NG, Herkes GK. The neurological manifestations of the acute porphyrias. J Clin Neurosci. 2011;18:1147–1153. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2011.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Skafidas E, Testa R, Zantomio D, Chana G, Everall IP, Pantelis C. Predicting the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder using gene pathway analysis. Mol Psychiatr. 2012 doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.126. Epub: 1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Slutske WS, Zhu G, Meier MH, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental influences on disordered gambling in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(6):624–630. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Spatola CAM, Scaini S, Pesenty-Gritti P, Medland SE, Moruzzi S, Ogliari A, Tambs K, Battaglia M. Gene-environment interactions in panic disorder and CO2 sensitivity: Effects of events occurring early in life. Am J Med Genet Part B. 2010;156:79–88. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stergiakouli E, Thapar A. Fitting the pieces together: Current research on the genetic basis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2010;6:551–560. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S11322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sullivan PF, Daly MJ, O’Donovan M. Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications. Nat Rev Genet. 2012;13:537–551. doi: 10.1038/nrg3240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tam GWC, Redon R, Carter NP, Grant SGN. The role of DNA copy number variation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:1005–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Thapar A, Holmes J, Poulton K, Harrington R. Genetic basis of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;174:105–111. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.2.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Thurston VC, Wales PS, Bell MA, Torbeck L, Brokaw JJ. The current status of medical genetics instruction in U.S. and Canadian medical schools. Acad Med. 2007;82 (5):441–445. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e31803e86c5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Van Grootheest DS, Cath DC, Beekman AT, Boomsma DI. Twin studies on obsessive compulsive disorder: A review. Twin Res Hum Genetics. 2005;8(5):450–448. doi: 10.1375/183242705774310060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Visscher PM, Goddard ME, Derks EM, Wray NR. Evidence-based psychiatric genetics, AKA the false dichotomy between common and rare variant hypotheses. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:474–485. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Walsh T, McClellan JM, McCarthy SE, Addington AM, Pierce SB, Cooper GM, Sebat J. Rare structural variants disrupt multiple genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in schizophrenia. Science. 2008;320:539–543. doi: 10.1126/science.1155174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wilhelm K, Meiser B, Mitchell PB, Finch AW, Siegel JE, Parker G, Schofield PR. Issues concerning feedback about genetic testing and risk of depression. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;194:404–410. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.047514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Winner J, Allen JD, Anthony Altar C, Spahic-Mihajlovic A. Psychiatric pharmacogenomics predicts health resource utilization of outpatients with anxiety and depression. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e242. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES