Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2014 Dec 19;3:e04617. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04617

Differential effects of light and feeding on circadian organization of peripheral clocks in a forebrain Bmal1 mutant

Mariko Izumo 1,, Martina Pejchal 2,, Andrew C Schook 2,3, Ryan P Lange 2, Jacqueline A Walisser 4, Takashi R Sato 5,6, Xiaozhong Wang 7, Christopher A Bradfield 4, Joseph S Takahashi 1,8,*
Editor: Louis Ptáček9
PMCID: PMC4298698  PMID: 25525750

Abstract

In order to assess the contribution of a central clock in the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) to circadian behavior and the organization of peripheral clocks, we generated forebrain/SCN-specific Bmal1 knockout mice by using floxed Bmal1 and pan-neuronal Cre lines. The forebrain knockout mice showed >90% deletion of BMAL1 in the SCN and exhibited an immediate and complete loss of circadian behavior in constant conditions. Circadian rhythms in peripheral tissues persisted but became desynchronized and damped in constant darkness. The loss of synchrony was rescued by light/dark cycles and partially by restricted feeding (only in the liver and kidney but not in the other tissues) in a distinct manner. These results suggest that the forebrain/SCN is essential for internal temporal order of robust circadian programs in peripheral clocks, and that individual peripheral clocks are affected differently by light and feeding in the absence of a functional oscillator in the forebrain.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.001

Research organism: mouse

eLife digest

Jet lag is a common experience when flying long distance. This disorientating phenomenon occurs when our internal ‘body clock’ remains set to the time zone where the plane departed and fails to reset to the new local time.

Our internal clock actually consists of a series of clocks—each of which is based upon groups of genes that are switched on and off at different times of the day and night. There is a master clock in our brain and a series of peripheral clocks in our other organs and tissues. The master clock is thought to coordinate the peripheral clocks, which in turn control the fluctuating activity of a specific organ in response to the time of day.

To further investigate the master clock, a typical approach has been made to disable it by deleting the genes for its components. But some of these deletions can cause abnormalities in mice and some are lethal. To get around these problems, Izumo, Pejchal et al. have devised a way to delete a molecular component of the master clock only in the mouse's brain.

Izumo, Pejchal et al. used this approach to specifically disable the mouse's master clock and, unlike mice that completely lack the Bmal1 gene, mice with the brain-specific deletion were as healthy and lived as long as normal mice. A molecular probe was used to monitor the peripheral clocks in different organs and tissues of these mutant mice, and revealed that, without a working master clock, the peripheral clocks were no longer synchronized. Izumo, Pejchal et al. found that the lost synchrony could be partially restored by training the mice to adapt to cycles of light and dark and feeding schedules.

Following on from the work of Izumo, Pejchal et al., one of the next challenges is to understand how the master clock communicates with the peripheral clocks in different organs and tissues around the body.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.002

Introduction

The suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus is the primary regulator of daily rhythms of behavior and physiology in mammals (Meijer and Rietveld, 1989; Ralph et al., 1990; Sujino et al., 2003; Welsh et al., 2010), yet the majority of tissues in the body possess autonomous cellular oscillators (Yamazaki et al., 2000; Abe et al., 2002; Nagoshi et al., 2004; Welsh et al., 2004; Yoo et al., 2004; Abraham et al., 2005). The circadian clock in the SCN as well as in other mammalian cells is composed of interacting positive- and negative-transcriptional and post-translational feedback loops involving Clock and Bmal1 transcription factors and their target genes, Period (Per1, 2, and 3) and Cryptochrome (Cry1 and 2) (King et al., 1997; Gekakis et al., 1998; Hogenesch et al., 1998; Kume et al., 1999; Lee et al., 2001; Lowrey and Takahashi, 2004, 2011; Huang et al., 2012; Mohawk et al., 2012). Extensive evidence suggests that the SCN controls not only behavioral rhythms but also the circadian programs of peripheral tissues (Yoo et al., 2004; Guo et al., 2005, 2006; Saini et al., 2013; Yamaguchi et al., 2013). However, much remains unclear regarding the relationship between the central clock and peripheral oscillators, which can be entrained by many different stimuli (Stokkan et al., 2001; Buhr et al., 2010; Saini et al., 2012). To clarify the contribution of a central clock to the circadian rhythms of peripheral tissues, we sought to find a way to disable the molecular oscillators in the brain without affecting the genetic components in the rest of the body.

Traditionally, a functional role of a gene has been investigated by a gene targeting strategy, which eliminates the gene from embryonic stem cells so that it is inactivated ubiquitously in mice. While this approach provides a powerful method to study the function of genes in vivo, it cannot be applied to genes that affect developmental or metabolic processes crucial for survival. Moreover, even if the knockout mice survive to adulthood, some lines suffer from systemic conditions and hence require special handling in experiments. For instance, although a global knock out of Bmal1 (Bmal1−/−) completely eliminates circadian rhythms (Bunger et al., 2000), the animals suffer from morbid conditions including arthropathy (Bunger et al., 2005), sterility (Alvarez et al., 2008; Boden et al., 2010), defects in glucose homeostasis (Rudic et al., 2004; Lamia et al., 2008; Marcheva et al., 2010), premature aging, and decreased lifespan [their average lifespan is 37 weeks (Kondratov et al., 2006; Sun et al., 2006)]. Possibly due to the pleiotropic effects of Bmal1, results of studies such as food entrainment behavior with Bmal1−/− mice have been controversial (Fuller et al., 2008; Mistlberger et al., 2008; Fuller et al., 2009; Pendergast et al., 2009; Storch and Weitz, 2009; Mistlberger et al., 2009a, 2009b; Mieda and Sakurai, 2011). To circumvent these problems, conditional knockout techniques utilizing Cre recombinase and loxP sequences have been applied widely in the nervous system (Gavériaux-Ruff and Kieffer, 2007; Taniguchi et al., 2011). However, until recently, Cre drivers that can efficiently recombine a floxed allele in the majority of SCN neurons have not been identified (Mieda and Sakurai, 2011; Musiek et al., 2013 but see Husse et al., 2011).

In this study, we examined the ability of a forebrain-specific Cre driver to excise floxed alleles of Bmal1, a critical component of the molecular oscillator, specifically from brain regions including the SCN. The resulting knockout mice were devoid of BMAL1 expression in the forebrain/SCN but not in other peripheral tissues. Unlike Bmal1 global knockout mice, Bmal1 forebrain knockouts did not display detectable defects in reproduction, activity, body weight, or life span, thereby providing an ideal model to study circadian physiology in the absence of a central clock. These genetically engineered mice are better experimentally controlled than mice that receive SCN lesions, which is a classic method to induce arrhythmic behavior in rodents (Meijer and Rietveld, 1989). With SCN lesions, precise removal of the SCN tissue varies from mouse to mouse and requires laborious confirmation of the excision. In addition, ablation of the SCN destroys neuronal networks and communication pathways, rendering the network responses and mechanisms incapacitated.

With forebrain-specific Bmal1 knockout mice, we examined the impact of defunct brain clocks on the organization of peripheral oscillators as well as their response to environmental signals such as light and food. Cre-mediated excision of Bmal1 in the forebrain/SCN confers complete arrhythmicity to circadian behavior. In contrast to the abolition of rhythms in the SCN, all peripheral tissues in these mice sustained circadian rhythmicity; however, they lacked phase coordination and normal amplitude in constant darkness. The synchrony and the oscillatory amplitude of peripheral rhythms in these mice were completely rescued by exposure to light dark cycles but not fully in time-restricted feeding experiments. The feeding cues appear to control the phase expression of the peripheral clocks in a tissue-specific way and in a distinct manner from light. These results demonstrate that an intact central clock plays an essential role in driving normal circadian behavior as well as synchronized and robust circadian programs in peripheral clocks and that light and feeding act on individual peripheral clocks differently in the absence of a functional oscillator in the SCN.

Results

Forebrain/SCN-specific deletion of Bmal1

Bmal1 (also known as Arntl, Mop3) is one of the essential components of the molecular oscillator, unique in that the single knockout of Bmal1 can confer arrhythmicity both at the behavioral and molecular levels (Bunger et al., 2000). To generate a forebrain-specific knockout of circadian rhythms, we crossed floxed Bmal1 (Bmal1fx/fx) mice (Johnson et al., 2014) to Camk2a::iCreBAC mice (CamiCre or Cre) (Casanova et al., 2001) and produced CamiCre+; Bmal1fx/fx (that we denote as ‘Brain’ Knockout, BKO, defined as a forebrain-specific knockout) as well as Bmal1fx/fx (Fx/Fx), CamiCre+ (Cre), and CamiCre+; Bmal1fx/+ (Het) control mice. Camk2a::iCreBAC mice were used because their Cre expression is faithful to the endogenous Camk2a gene expression pattern: it is neuron-specific, forebrain-enriched (Casanova et al., 2001), and expressed highly in the SCN as observed in Cre-dependent fluorescence reporter mice, CamiCre+; tdTom+ (Figure 1A–E). Although there are more than 37 different Camk2a::Cre transgenic lines reported in Mouse Genome Informatics database (http://www.informatics.jax.org/searchtool/Search.do?query=Camk2a-cre&page=featureList), the majority do not appear to express well in the SCN (e.g., http://connectivity.brain-map.org/transgenic/experiment/81162458), likely because of shorter promoter regulatory sequences and position effects. The other advantage of this Cre driver is that its Cre expression is post-natal and starts at P3 (Casanova et al., 2001), thus reducing potential developmental effects. Bmal1fx/fx mice were generated by inserting loxP sites into the introns surrounding exon 4, which codes for the DNA-binding bHLH domain (Johnson et al., 2014). Upon Cre recombination, exon 4 is deleted in BKO brain tissues that express Camk2a::iCre. The predicted BMAL1 sequence encounters a STOP codon after a stretch of 18 non-native residues following native coding exon 3. The resulting protein is 91 amino acids long (vs 626 amino acids of native BMAL1) and is almost identical to the truncated BMAL1 protein of Bmal1−/− mice (Bunger et al., 2000); both proteins contain no known functional domains and do not accumulate to a detectable level.

Figure 1. CamiCre+; Bmal1fx/fx mice are forebrain/SCN-specific Bmal1 knockouts.

(A) Dorsal view of a CamiCre-; tdTom+ control brain. Bright field. Scale bar: 1 mm. (B) Dorsal view of a CamiCre+; tdTom+ brain. Bright field. (C) Ventral view of (B). (D) Fluorescence image of a coronal brain section containing the SCN from a CamiCre+; tdTom+ mouse. (E) Confocal image of the SCN showing tdTomato expression in the CamiCre+; tdTom+ mice. Scale bar: 50 µm. (F, G) Coronal brain sections at the level of the SCN, prepared from mice sacrificed every 4 hr starting at CT2 following 2 days of DD, were hybridized in situ to examine Bmal1 (F) and Per2 (G) mRNA levels in CamiCre+; Bmal1fx/fx (BKO) mice (n = 3, except n = 4 for CT10, 14, 18) and Bmal1fx/fx (Fx/Fx) control littermates (same as BKO, except n = 1 for CT18 Bmal1 and n = 3 for CT18 Per2). Below each coronal brain section is a close-up of the SCN. (H, I) Optical density graphs of the in situ time course data displayed in (F) and (G). Shown are mean ± SEM, with significant effect of genotype in (H) [F1,46 = 40.7, p < 0.0001] and (I) [F1,44 = 1148, p < 0.0001] by GLM ANOVA. Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests (p ≤ 0.05) showed that in contrast to Fx/Fx littermates, neither Bmal1 nor Per2 mRNA is rhythmic in BKOs. (J) Immunohistochemistry for BMAL1 on SCN-containing coronal sections of Fx/Fx (n = 3), BKO (n = 3), and Bmal1−/− (KO, n = 1) mice sacrificed at ZT16. Captured with an 20× objective. (K, L) Western blot analysis of BMAL1 in BKOs sacrificed at ZT16. (K) Western blot of various tissues in Fx/Fx (n = 2), BKO littermate (n = 2), and KO (n = 1) mice. (L) Western blot of frontal cortex, cerebellum, and liver in Cre (n = 2), Fx/Fx (n = 2), BKO (n = 3), and KO (n = 2) mice. F. CTX: frontal cortex, Cer: cerebellum.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.003

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Weights of Bmal1 brain knockout mice are similar to controls at 2 months and 4–6 months of age.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

All body weights were measured in live, wheel-naïve animals. Numbers of male mice weighed at 2 months (= 54- to 62-day old) and 4–6 months (= 120- to 195-day old), respectively: Cre (n = 25, 35), Het (n = 13, 15), Fx/Fx (n = 98, 37), and BKO (n = 55, 12). Numbers of female mice weighed at 2 months and 4–6 months, respectively: Cre (n = 28, 39), Het (n = 23, 35), Fx/Fx (n = 84, 54), and BKO (n = 54, 29). Shown are the mean weight ±SD. A significant effect of genotype was found using GLM ANOVA, followed by Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests. The difference between Cre and Fx/Fx mice (*p ≤ 0.05) is likely due to the difference in the genetic background (CamiCre+ is generated in FVB background and floxed Bmal1 in 129sv). Importantly, there was no significant difference between BKO males and control males at 4–6 months, and BKO females did not weigh less than any control group at either time point.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Additional in situ analysis of Bmal1 brain knockout mice.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A, B) Coronal brain sections at the level of the SCN. Below each coronal brain section is a close-up of the SCN. (A) Mice were sacrificed at CT18 and CT6 following 2 days of DD. Sections were hybridized in situ to examine Bmal1 mRNA levels in Cre (n = 5 at CT6 and 4 at CT18), Fx/Fx (n = 7 at CT6 and 7 at CT18), Het (n = 6 at CT6 and 6 at CT18), BKO (n = 4 at CT6 and 4 at CT18), and Bmal1−/− mice (KO, n = 1). (B) Mice were sacrificed at CT10 and CT22 following 2 days of DD. Sections were hybridized in situ to examine Per2 mRNA levels in Cre (n = 7 at CT10 and 6 at CT22), Fx/Fx (n = 10 at CT10 and 8 at CT22), Het (n = 7 at CT10 and 7 at CT22), and BKO (n = 9 at CT10 and 9 at CT22) mice. (C, D) Optical density of Bmal1 (C) and Per2 (D) mRNA in the SCN. Shown are mean ± SEM, with significant effect of genotype in (C) [F3,42 = 217.91, p < 0.0001] and (D) [F3,62 = 28.00, p < 0.0001] by GLM ANOVA. (C) Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests (**p ≤ 0.05) showed that BKO mice had significantly lower Bmal1 mRNA levels than all three control groups at both CT6 and CT18. At both time points Het mice had significantly less Bmal1 mRNA than the other two controls but more mRNA than BKOs (*p ≤ 0.05). (D) BKO mice had significantly lower levels of Per2 mRNA than all three control groups at CT10 but not at CT22 (*p ≤ 0.05).

In contrast to global Bmal1 knockouts, the reproduction, longevity, and health of BKO mice were as robust as that of wild-type mice. The fertility of both males and females was indistinguishable from wild-type. At 2 months and 4–6 months of age, the body weights of BKO were found to be nearly identical to Fx/Fx, Cre, and Het mice (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

In order to assess the deletion of Bmal1 in the SCN, we examined the circadian mRNA expression pattern of Bmal1 and Per2, a key Bmal1 target gene, by in situ hybridization after 2 days of constant darkness (DD). At all seven time points taken over a period of 24 hr, BKOs showed a low level of Bmal1 and Per2 mRNA in the SCN as well as in the entire coronal brain section (Figure 1F,G). In more detailed analysis, control mice (Cre, Fx/Fx) showed intense mRNA expression of Bmal1 in the SCN at its peak time (CT18) as well as circadian fluctuation (CT18 vs CT6) (Figure 1F,H; Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Accordingly, Per2 mRNA was also robustly expressed in the SCN in a circadian manner (Figure 1G,I; Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Het mice showed reduction in the Bmal1 mRNA abundance, yet cycling of Per2 mRNA was retained in the SCN (Figure 1—figure supplement 2), indicating that 50% wild-type Bmal1 mRNA is sufficient to sustain Per2 expression in the SCN. In contrast, in BKO mice, Bmal1 mRNA was reduced ∼90% in the SCN (Figure 1F,H; Figure 1—figure supplement 2), and Per2 mRNA cycling was significantly attenuated (∼20% WT levels at CT10, see Figure 1G,I).

To confirm that no BMAL1 protein was expressed in the SCN of BKOs, we performed immunohistochemistry in the SCN at Zeitgeber Time (ZT) 16. In contrast to the intense BMAL1 staining in the SCN of Fx/Fx controls, BMAL1 staining in the BKO and global Bmal1 knockout SCN was reduced to background levels (Figure 1J). To further characterize the tissue specificity of the BKOs, we used Western blot analyses to measure BMAL1 protein levels in various brain and body tissues at ZT16 (Figure 1K,L). In contrast to Fx/Fx and Cre controls, BMAL1 (∼69 kD) was detected only faintly in BKO frontal cortex and olfactory bulb (Figure 1K), likely due to glial and non-neuronal expression of BMAL1 since Camk2a is not expressed in glial cells (Lin et al., 1987). Levels of BMAL1 in the cerebellum of BKOs were similar to Fx/Fx and Cre control mice (Figure 1L). Outside of the brain, like Fx/Fx mice but unlike global Bmal1−/− mice, BKOs showed equivalent BMAL1 expression levels in the liver, spleen, kidney, heart, and lung (Figure 1K). Taken together, these results confirm the forebrain specificity of our Bmal1 conditional knockouts.

Lack of circadian rhythms in BKOs

To assess whether forebrain/SCN Bmal1 is necessary for normal circadian behavior, we examined circadian rhythms of locomotor activity in Cre, Fx/Fx, Het, and BKO mice in a 12 hr:12 hr light:dark (LD) cycle followed by DD and constant light (LL) conditions (Figure 2). In both DD and LL, control mice (Cre, Fx/Fx, and Het) expressed robust circadian rhythms of activity (Figure 2A,C,D). The locomotor activity of Het mice was indistinguishable from that of the other controls. In contrast to controls, Bmal1 brain knockouts exhibited no circadian rhythm of activity in DD or in LL (Figure 2B–D). Out of 31 BKO mice, none had a period in the circadian (18–30 hr) range in DD. In LL, only 2 out of 21 mice had a period in the circadian range (24.1 and 20.9 hr). However, they showed extremely low amplitudes of circadian activity (0.025 and 0.019, respectively, fraction of power in the circadian range, where 1.0 represents the total power in the normalized power spectrum), and spectral analysis using Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) did not detect periodicity in the circadian range in any of the BKOs under either DD or LL conditions (Figure 2D).

Figure 2. Complete loss of circadian rhythmicity of Bmal1 brain knockout mice in constant darkness and constant light.

Figure 2.

(A, B) Representative actograms of daily wheel-running activity of Cre, Fx/Fx, Het, and BKO mice. Activity records were double plotted, with each day being represented beneath and also to the right of the preceding day. Horizontal black and white bars at the top of each actogram represent lights off and on, respectively. Mice were housed in LD, released into DD for 4 weeks, returned to LD for 2 weeks, released into LL for 4 weeks, and then returned to LD. GLM ANOVA and Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests were used to compare genotypes tested. BKOs showed no significant periodicity in DD or LL. (CE) Period (C), amplitude of circadian rhythm (D), and activity levels (E) in Cre mice (n = 28 for DD, 12 for LD, 12 for LL), Fx/Fx mice (n = 31 for DD, 18 for LD, 18 for LL), Het mice (n = 31 for DD, 13 for LD, 15 for LL), and BKO mice (n = 31 for DD, 16 for LD, 21 for LL). Bar graphs show mean ± SEM. (C) Free-running period was determined using χ2 periodogram for days 1–28 of DD or LL. A significant effect of genotype on period was found for both DD [F3,120 = 164,319, p < 0.0001] and LL [F3,65 = 20,429, p < 0.0001]. In DD, Cre mice were found to have a slightly longer period than the other two control groups (*p ≤ 0.05). In LL, Het mice were found to have a shorter period than the other two control groups (*p ≤ 0.05). Nevertheless, all three control groups showed a free-running period in both DD and LL similar to that reported for WT mice. (D) DD and LL amplitude of circadian rhythm represented by FFT in the circadian range. A significant effect of genotype on amplitude was found for both DD [F3,120 = 150, p < 0.0001] and LL [F3,65 = 23.72, p < 0.0001], with BKO mice having significantly lower amplitude (*p ≤ 0.05) in both DD and LL. (E) Total daily DD, LD, and LL activity levels, in wheel revolutions per 24 hr. BKO activity levels were found to be significantly higher than those of controls in DD [F3,119 = 9.00, p < 0.0001] (*p ≤ 0.05) and LL [F3,65 = 5.84, p = 0.0014] (*p ≤ 0.05) but not in LD.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.006

Unexpectedly, total daily activity of BKOs was significantly higher than that of control mice both in DD and LL (Figure 2E). Previous studies have found that the locomotor activity of Bmal1−/− mice is decreased dramatically below wild-type levels (Bunger et al., 2000) and that transgenic BMAL1 expression in skeletal muscle rescues this diminished activity (McDearmon et al., 2006). Taken together, these results suggest that a peripheral defect in BMAL1 is responsible for the low activity levels that had been reported in global Bmal1−/− mice. In BKOs (which do not have this peripheral defect so that locomotion is normal), the absence of a central circadian gating mechanism, which normally suppresses activity during the subjective daytime (Davis and Menaker, 1980), likely contributes to an increase in overall levels of activity.

Entrainment to light cycles and masking

One of the notable differences between BKO and SCN-lesioned mice is that BKOs display apparent entrainment to light/dark cycles (probably because BKOs are spared from surgical interruption of retinal inputs to the SCN which usually occurs in lesion experiments) (Figure 2). However, the activity onset of BKOs in LD was variable and on average earlier than controls. As a result, daytime activity level was higher in BKOs (15.58 ± 1.8% of total daily activity) compared to the three control groups (3.81 ± 0.96%, average of the three control groups ± SEM) (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Yet it was unclear whether the pre-dark activity of Bmal1 brain knockouts around the light/dark transition was due to poor entrainment of a residual circadian pacemaker or to a defect in masking.

In order to test whether Bmal1 brain knockouts entrain to light, we used a skeleton photoperiod (LDLD 1:10:1:12, Figure 3A–E). The activity patterns of Cre, Fx/Fx, and Het mice during the skeleton photoperiod were virtually indistinguishable from the activity patterns in LD 12:12 (Figure 3A). In contrast, none of the 10 BKO mice used in this study entrained normally to the skeleton photoperiod (Figure 3B): 3 BKO mice showed activity that resembled their activity in DD (Figure 3B, left), 2 BKO mice displayed a bimodal activity pattern (Figure 3B, right), and 5 BKO mice exhibited activity that was intermediate between these two phenotypes (Figure 3B, middle). Unlike control mice, BKO mice distributed their running activity equally between the subjective day and the subjective night of the skeleton photoperiod (Figure 3C,D). In fact, controls spent 97.9% of their activity during the skeleton night, compared to only 52.5% for BKO mice (Figure 3E). The amount of activity that BKOs spent during skeleton day vs night was not significantly different. This type of abnormal entrainment to skeleton photoperiods is reminiscent of the entrainment behavior of pinealectomized sparrows, which behave as a population of circadian oscillators that entrain with two opposite phase relationships to the skeleton photoperiod (Takahashi and Menaker, 1982). Therefore, we conclude that entrainment in Bmal1 brain knockout mice is abnormal under skeleton photoperiods, consistent with the idea that the coherence of phase during entrainment may be compromised. Overall this suggests that the periodicity seen in LD 12:12 in BKOs arises from entrainment of a population of driven oscillators in which phase control is labile.

Figure 3. Both entrainment and masking are abnormal in Bmal1 brain knockout mice.

(A, B) Representative double-plotted actograms of daily wheel-running activity of Cre, Fx/Fx, Het, and BKO mice. Mice were housed in LD, released into DD for 4 weeks, returned to LD for 2 weeks, introduced to a skeleton photoperiod (LDLD 1:10:1:12) for 4 weeks, returned to LD for 2 weeks, and then switched to an ultradian cycle (LD 3.5:3.5) for 3 weeks. Shown are days 1–24 of the skeleton photoperiod and days 3–15 of LD 3.5:3.5. (C, D) Average activity profiles of Fx/Fx (n = 12) and BKO littermates (n = 10) during LDLD 1:10:1:12. (C) Average profiles for individual mice; (D) the ensemble average for the entire group. Each data point represents counts per minute averaged across a 6-min bin (±SEM for D). (E) Total activity counts (mean ± SEM) during 10 hr of subjective day (Sk. Day) and 10 hr of subjective night (Sk. Night) of LDLD 1:10:1:12. In contrast to Fx/Fx littermates, BKOs are equally active during skeleton day vs skeleton night. *p < 0.0001 by t-test. (F) Masking of BKO (n = 14) and Fx/Fx littermates (n = 9) during specific 0.5 hr light bins of LD 3.5:3.5. Total activity (mean ± SEM) during each 30 min light bin is divided by the total of that bin plus the corresponding dark bin. Thus the first data point for each genotype represents the distribution of activity between the first 30 min of light and the first 30 min of dark. There was a significant interaction between the genotype and time using Repeated Measures GLM ANOVA [F1,160 = 22.3, p < 0.0001]. Specifically, days 1, 6, and 7 were found to be significantly different by Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests (*p ≤ 0.05). (G) Representative double-plotted actograms of daily wheel-running activity of Fx/Fx (left) and BKO (right) littermates. These are the same mice as those shown in (A, middle panel) and (B, middle panel), respectively. Light phases are indicated in yellow to show the structure of the LD 3.5:3.5 cycle as well as to help visualize the occurrence of wheel-running activity under this schedule. Note that, after 1 week of this cycle, initial phase relationships are regained. (H) Representative amplitude power spectra of Fx/Fx and BKO mice, from FFT analyses performed on the same activity records shown in (G). The highest peaks for both genotypes were in the 7-hr range, corresponding to the LD 3.5:3.5 schedule. The second-highest peak for the controls was in the circadian (18–30 hr) range (left). None of the BKOs was found to have a period in the circadian range (right). (I) Group averages of amplitudes in the 7-hr range and in the circadian range for the same Fx/Fx and BKO mice as in (F). Compared to controls, BKOs had significantly higher amplitude in the 7-hr range (**p < 0.0001 by t-test) and significantly lower amplitude in the circadian range (*p < 0.0060 by t-test).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.007

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Masking in Bmal1 brain knockout mice.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Daytime activity levels during LD, as percentage of total daily activity, in Cre (n = 12), Fx/Fx (n = 18), Het (n = 13), and BKO (n = 16) mice. A significant effect of genotype on daytime activity was found by GLM ANOVA [F3,58 = 12.43, p < 0.0001], with BKO mice having significantly higher daytime activity by Tukey–Kramer multiple comparisons post-test (*p ≤ 0.05). Combined, controls allocate an average of 3.81 ± 0.96% of their total activity to running during the daytime. In contrast, 15.58 ± 1.8% of BKO wheel-running activity occurs during the light phase. (BE) Shown are days 3–15 of a 21-day LD 3.5:3.5 cycle; these are the same days as those shown in Figure 3A,B. (B, C) Representative single-plotted actograms of daily wheel-running activity of (B) Fx/Fx and (C) BKO littermates. These are the same mice as those shown in Figure 3A (middle panel), 3B (middle panel), and 3G. Actograms are plotted modulo tau (25.2 hr for B) to help visualize the circadian ‘beating’, that is the gaps in activity in controls (B) caused by the suppressive effects of the light phases of the LD 3.5:3.5 cycle on running. A circadian component of wheel-running activity is clearly evident in controls (B) but absent in BKOs (C). (D, E) Representative actograms of wheel-running activity of Fx/Fx (D) and BKO littermates (E), plotted on a 7 hr time scale (x axis). Y-axis shows successive 7 hr spans for days 3–15 of this cycle. The gaps in activity in some of the dark periods in Fx/Fx controls represent times when the circadian cycle is specifying rest even though lights are off (see Figure 3G, left panel). To the right of each actogram is an activity profile for that individual mouse, averaged over 13 days. Each data point represents counts per minute averaged for each mouse across a 6-min bin. The boxed area corresponds to the first one-third of all the light phases of the LD 3.5:3.5 schedule. Substantial BKO wheel-running activity is seen during this interval, especially the first 30 min, during which BKOs fail to mask altogether (see Figure 3F). Horizontal black and white bars represent lights off and on, respectively.

Since the skeleton photoperiod and LD photoperiod results both indicated some impairment of masking in BKOs, we next tested whether negative masking (the ability of light to acutely suppress locomotor activity) in Bmal1 brain knockouts was defective. To assess masking in BKO mice, we used an LD 3.5:3.5-hr light–dark cycle (Redlin et al., 2005) (Figure 3B,F,G). Mice are unable to entrain to LD 3.5:3.5 cycles, thus permitting the masking and the entraining effects of light to be distinguished. Maintaining this schedule for a week ensures the testing of all phases of a circadian cycle (Figure 3G). On average, BKOs exhibited significantly less activity during the light portions of LD 3.5:3.5 (4.68 ± 0.71% of total activity vs 9.56 ± 1.83% for Fx/Fx littermates, p < 0.032 by t-test). Fx/Fx mice display more activity during the light portions of LD 3.5:3.5 because the circadian clock is promoting activity despite the presence of light (Figure 3G, left). While Fx/Fx mice have an underlying circadian component of activity under LD 3.5:3.5, BKOs have none (Figure 3G–I; Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Aside from this difference, when we examined the time course of the masking response, it became apparent that the masking of BKOs is delayed compared to controls (Figure 3F; a score of 50% indicates failure to mask). Thus BKOs failed to mask during the first 30 min of the light phases. Plotting actograms on a 7-hr time scale aids in visualization of this masking impairment in BKOs (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). These entrainment and masking studies led us to conclude that BKOs lack an endogenous clock but also exhibit some impaired masking.

Phase synchrony and desynchrony in peripheral tissues of BKOs

To analyze the temporal organization of peripheral oscillators at an organismal level, we crossed the BKOs to PER2::LUC mice (Yoo et al., 2004) and monitored the bioluminescence signals from the SCN and other tissues in the body. Compared with the robust oscillation of the control SCN, the SCN of BKOs exhibited blunted expression patterns with low-amplitude but detectable rhythms, which were close to 24 hr (23.54 ± 0.29 hr in Fx/Fx control SCN vs 22.99 ± 2.36 hr in BKO SCN for mean period length ± SD, Figure 4A). These rhythms are likely to be derived from glial cells and/or residual neurons in which Cre-mediated excision of floxed Bmal1 was incomplete (see Figure 1J) or possibly from residual stochastic oscillations that we have observed in the SCN of global Bmal1 knockouts (Ko et al., 2010). Rhythmic expression in the dorsomedial hypothalamus (DMH) in BKOs was also significantly attenuated. In contrast to the SCN and DMH, peripheral tissues retained persistent circadian rhythms (Figure 4A), consistent with the normal expression of BMAL1 in peripheral tissues (Figure 1K).

Figure 4. Real-time reporting of circadian expression of PER2::LUC in the forebrain/SCN knockout mice.

Figure 4.

(A) Representative records of bioluminescence reporting of circadian expression from various tissues in Fx/Fx and BKO mice. Tissues were prepared from mice in LD. PMT counts are plotted against the LD cycle of day 1. Shown are 10 days of continuous recording after explant preparation, except for the SCN for which medium was changed on day 12. (B, C) Period plots of various tissues harvested from Fx/Fx control (open circle) and BKO (dark circle) mice in LD (B) and DD (C). The sample size is indicated on right. Shown are mean period ± SD. **p < 0.01, ****p < 0.0001 by t-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.009

These observations prompted us to use the forebrain-specific knockout mice to re-investigate the relationship between the brain's central pacemaker and circadian oscillations of peripheral tissues. Previous studies have addressed a similar question by lesioning the SCN (Yoo et al., 2004; Tahara et al., 2012; Saini et al., 2013). Unlike SCN-lesioned mice, however, BKOs preserve intact neural networks and show normal or enhanced levels of behavioral activity in LD and in constant conditions. This new analysis allows us to determine whether deleting BMAL1 (thus inactivating the molecular clock) in the forebrain, without destroying the structure of the SCN, is sufficient to affect the organization of peripheral rhythms.

Age-matched pairs of mice (Fx/Fx littermate control and BKO) were maintained in DD for more than 30 days (30–44 days) before harvesting eight different tissues for real-time reporting of circadian gene expression. As a light synchronized control group, tissues were also collected from mice in LD 12:12. Consistent with previous studies (Yoo et al., 2004; Tahara et al., 2012), mean period length was variable yet characteristic from tissue to tissue (Figure 4B,C). The difference of the mean periods between the Fx/Fx control and BKO mice, however, was not significant either in LD or DD, except for the SCN and DMH (Figure 4B,C).

To analyze the temporal organization of the rhythms of each tissue, we constructed a phase map by plotting the peak on the second day in the luminometry recording. Fx/Fx control mice displayed tightly clustered phases in peripheral rhythms in both LD and DD (Figure 5A,B,E). On the other hand, while BKOs exhibited relatively coherent rhythms in peripheral tissues in LD (Figure 5C,E; Figure 5—source data 1A), their phases were significantly dispersed among animals in DD (Figure 5D,E; Figure 5—source data 1A). In order to measure the degree of phase coherence of peripheral clocks within individual animals, the circular variance of peak bioluminescence of all tissues in each mouse was compared (Figure 5—source data 1B,C). Statistical analysis (Figure 5F,G) showed that BKO cultures from the DD condition have a significantly wider phase distribution among organs (Figure 5G, p = 0.0008 by Mann–Whitney test). This result indicates that a loss of phase coordination occurred in peripheral clocks within individual BKO animals when placed in DD. In contrast, we found that BKOs displayed well-phased rhythms from tissue to tissue when maintained in LD (Figure 5F, N.S. by Mann–Whitney test), suggesting that the internal phase synchrony is still maintained even in the absence of a functional master pacemaker when the animals are exposed to LD cycles.

Figure 5. Phase analysis of circadian expression of various peripheral tissues from the forebrain/SCN knockout mice.

Figure 5.

(A) Phase map of circadian rhythms of various peripheral tissues from Fx/Fx control mice in LD (n = 15, except for pituitary n = 14). The peak phases (or averaged peak phases where more than two pieces were prepared from the same tissue) in the second cycle were plotted against the LD cycle of the day when explant cultures were prepared. Tissues from the same animal are connected by colored lines with matched symbols. (B) Phase map of Fx/Fx control mice in DD (n = 15). The peak phases (or averaged peak phases) in the second cycle were plotted against the predicted onset of activity (=CT12) of the day when the explant cultures were prepared. (C) Phase map of BKOs in LD (n = 15, except for pituitary n = 14). (D) Phase map of BKOs in DD (n = 15). The phases were mapped against the predicted onset of activity (= CT12) of paired Fx/Fx control mice. (E) Circular plots of peak bioluminescence rhythms in the SCN and peripheral tissues presented in (AD). A circle represents a 24-hr clock, and peak phases of bioluminescence rhythms of individual tissues were calculated as angles and plotted as colored symbols outside the circle. Each tissue is denoted by the same color and symbol scheme. The direction of the arrow indicates mean phase angle, with the length of the arrow expressing the strength of phase clustering. The sample size, mean phase angle, and circular variance for each dataset are summarized in Figure 5—source data 1A. (F, G) Scattered plot of circular variance in each individual mouse. Degree of variance among the peak phase values of pituitary, liver, kidney, heart, lung, and spleen in each individual mouse was calculated and expressed as circular variance (see Figure 5—source data 1B,C) and compared between Fx/Fx controls and BKOs in LD (F) and DD (G). The bar is ±SD. A significant effect was found between Fx/Fx control and BKO mice in DD (p = 0.0008 by Mann–Whitney test) but not in LD (p = 0.0627 by Mann–Whitney test). N.S. = not significant.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.010

Figure 5—source data 1. (A) Summary of statistical analysis of circular plots presented in Figure 5. (B) Statistical analysis of circular variance for peak bioluminescence in individual mice in LD. (C) Statistical analysis of circular variance for peak bioluminescence in individual mice in DD.
elife04617s001.docx (45.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.04617.011

Decreased oscillatory amplitude in peripheral tissues of BKOs

In addition to period and phase, we also estimated amplitude because there was a trend for oscillatory amplitudes in peripheral tissues from BKOs to be lower than that of controls (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). To compare across different experiments, all time-series data were adjusted for PMT background counts and PMT gain, before applying FFT-NLLS computation as developed previously (Izumo et al., 2006). Only normalized amplitudes were compared in this study. For the SCN and DMH in which Bmal1 is deleted from the majority of neurons, only residual amplitude was detected (Figure 6A,B). Likewise, the amplitude of the pituitary was lower in BKOs from LD, which was likely due to the partial deletion of BMAL1 (see Figure 1K). This decrease was further pronounced when BKO pituitary was harvested from DD (Figure 6A,B). For all other peripheral tissues, the relative amplitude was significantly decreased when a light-driven signal was removed from BKOs in DD, but the severe reduction was not observed in BKOs from LD (Figure 6A,B, except for liver). These damped rhythms, however, regained oscillation after receiving a stimulatory medium change (Figure 6—figure supplement 1), which suggests the possibility that phase desynchrony is taking place within individual tissues of BKOs.

Figure 6. Decreased amplitude in peripheral tissues of BKOs.

(A, B) Normalized amplitude of bioluminescence rhythms from Fx/Fx control (open bar) vs BKO (dark bar) in LD (A) and DD (B). Mean relative amplitude ±SD are shown. The sample size is the same as in Figure 4B,C. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ****p < 0.0001 by t-test. (C) Representative records of real-time monitoring of circadian expression of heart tissues in Fx/Fx control (upper) and BKO (lower) mice maintained in DD. (D) Representative frames of bioluminescence imaging with grids over each heart tissue. (E) Heat maps of the brightest 200 time-series data beginning with the strongest signals in the grids shown in (D). (F, G) Linear traces of the top 50 of time-series data from the Fx/Fx (F) and BKO (G) heart tissue shown in (E). The Y-axis was expanded for the BKO sample in the most right graph. (H) Normalized amplitude quantified from the top 50 of time-series data shown in (F) and (G). ****p < 0.0001 by t-test. (I) Circular plots of peak phase values of the top 50 time-series data shown in (F) for Fx/Fx and (G) for BKO tissues. Degree of variance was compared between the two samples by bootstrapping simulation (****p < 0.0001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.012

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Real-time monitoring of circadian expression in forebrain/SCN knockout mice from DD.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Representative records of bioluminescence reporting of circadian expression from Fx/Fx and BKO mice maintained in DD. Tissues were harvested before the predicted onset of activity (=CT12) of Fx/Fx control mice. Shown are 10 days of continuous recordings followed by medium change. Quantification of period, phase, and relative amplitude of these samples are shown in Figures 4C,5,6B.

To distinguish whether the decreased amplitude of BKO tissues is due to phase desynchrony between the cells or to lowered amplitude within the cells, we performed bioluminescence imaging on a peripheral BKO tissue from DD. Heart tissue was chosen for this experiment, because the heart showed both significant phase dispersion and severe amplitude reduction in DD (Figure 5D; Figure 6B,C). In this imaging, bioluminescence signals from heart tissue were insufficient to identify single cells in the field of view. Therefore, we developed a grid method to allow extraction of time-series data in a narrow area of the target-imaging sample (Figure 6D). The size of the grid was narrowed down to 40 µm × 40 µm. Circadian rhythms in the brightest 200 grids were aligned in the order of expression intensity in a heat map (Figure 6E). Consistent with the organotypic recording by LumiCycle luminometry (Figure 6C), the heart cells from a wild-type mouse exhibited robust oscillation, while those from a BKO displayed damped rhythms with broader phase distribution. The damped rhythms were also clear in a linear graph in which the top 50 time-series were plotted (Figure 6F,G). Amplitude analysis of these 50 time-series data showed that the oscillatory amplitude was significantly reduced within the grids in the BKO heart sample (Figure 6H). Furthermore, their phases were significantly dispersed (Figure 6I). These results suggest that the amplitude decrease in the BKO tissue is caused by a combined effect of amplitude reduction and phase desynchrony at the cellular level.

Intact food-anticipatory activity in brain Bmal1 knockouts

Because nutrient signals can provide a potent entraining cue for peripheral circadian oscillators (Green et al., 2008; Bass and Takahashi, 2010; Mohawk et al., 2012), we investigated whether scheduled food restriction (FR) could restore circadian temporal organization of peripheral oscillators in forebrain-specific Bmal1 knockouts. First, to test the effect of forebrain Bmal1 on food-anticipatory activity (FAA), we subjected BKOs to a schedule of FR under both LD and DD conditions (Figure 7A).

Figure 7. Intact FAA in Bmal1 brain knockout mice.

(A) Schedule of a gradual temporal FR protocol in LD (upper) or DD (lower). Horizontal black and white bars represent lights off and on, respectively. Gray bars represent food availability. Since abrupt shifts to FR can have a high morbidity in mice, a gentle temporal FR paradigm was used (FR ramp), decreasing the duration of daily food availability from constant to a final 4 hr window over the course of 5 days. Because free-running in DD can obscure FAA in control mice, we obtained DD baseline activity and then switched mice to 14 days of LD (not shown). We then transferred our mice directly from LD into DD FR following an LD food ramp (lower). It should be noted that once gradual FR began, the time of onset of food availability was the same each day. (B, F) Representative double-plotted actograms of daily wheel-running activity of 3 Fx/Fx control littermates and 3 BKO mice during ad lib feeding and under subsequent FR during LD (B) or DD (F). The boxed area toward the left side of each actogram indicates the daily interval of food availability under FR and yellow areas indicate time of lights-on during LD 12:12. After 5 days of gradually decreasing food availability, the final food availability window was ZT/CT6–10. For clarity, the 5-day FR ramp is not included in the boxed area (B). (C, G) Mean locomotor activity profiles of Fx/Fx littermates (n = 7) and BKO mice (n = 14) under ad lib feeding (left) and during FR days 8–14 (right) under LD (C) or DD (G). Each data point represents counts per minute averaged for each genotype across a 6-min bin (±SEM). The dashed boxed area (left) indicates, for comparison, the daily interval corresponding to subsequent food availability. The solid boxed area (right) indicates the daily interval of food availability under FR. (D, H) Time course of the development of FAA in Fx/Fx controls (n = 7) and BKO mice (n = 14) during LD FR (D) or DD FR (H). FAA is plotted as the total number of activity counts (mean ± SEM) allocated to a 6-hr time interval prior to mealtime, ZT/CT0–6 (D, H). (E) Number of hours by which FAA preceded daily meal times in Fx/Fx (n = 7) and BKO mice (n = 14) during LD FR. Wheel-running activity profiles were averaged for each individual during stable FR (as in C), and the average time of onset of FAA was determined as the time before food availability at which FAA rose to its half-maximum value (mean ± SEM). (I) Quantification of FAA under DD conditions in BKO mice (n = 14). Plotted is fold-change of wheel-running activity (counts per minute, mean ± SEM) in each mouse for CT0–6 (window of FAA) compared with CT10–24 (the rest of the day except for the window of food availability). During FR, increased locomotor activity during CT0–6 compared with CT10–24 was highly significant (*p = 0.0001 by paired t-test).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.014

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Lack of Bmal1 expression in the DMH of Bmal1 brain knockout mice.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) Food consumed during FR was significantly lower in BKOs compared to controls using Repeated Measures GLM ANOVA for both LD conditions [F1,291 = 43.40, p < 0.0001] (A) and DD conditions [F1,293 = 15.47, p = 0.0009] (B). Food consumed on days 12–14 of LD FR (A) represents the average food consumed over those 3 days and is plotted in triplicate to enable comparison between LD and DD food consumed. (C, D) Coronal brain sections at the level of the DMH, prepared from mice under ad lib feeding at CT6 and CT18 following 2 days of DD, were hybridized in situ to examine Bmal1 exon 4 (C) and Per2 (D) mRNA levels in Fx/Fx control littermates (n = 6 for CT6, n = 7 for CT18) and BKO mice (n = 6 for CT6, n = 8 for CT18). On the right of each coronal brain section is a close-up of the DMH. (E, F) Optical density of Bmal1 (E) and Per2 (F) mRNA in the DMC. Shown are mean ± SEM, * significant effect of genotype in (E) [F1,26 = 208.31, p < 0.0001] but not in (F) using GLM ANOVA.

Both qualitatively and quantitatively, mice lacking forebrain Bmal1 exhibited clear food-anticipatory behavior in LD that was similar to that of control mice (Figure 7B–E). Notably, the mean FAA profiles of BKOs were enhanced in DD during temporal food restriction (Figure 7C,G), and the total daily activity levels in BKOs during FR were significantly higher (almost two-fold higher) than those of controls, despite the observation that BKOs exhibited a trend to eat less during FR (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Surprisingly, FAA profiles showed that, during FR in DD, BKOs had extended activity for up to 15 hr before food availability, while controls allocated most of their activity to the 6-hr window of FAA (Figure 7F,G). Another study has found similarly longer FAA in Bmal1-deficient mice (Takasu et al., 2012). To further characterize the food-anticipatory behavior, we measured the time course over which FAA emerged in BKOs and controls after the start of food restriction. We found FAA development to be similar in the two genotypes under both LD and DD conditions. FAA appeared after about 2 days and reached a plateau after about 4 days in both genotypes (Figure 7D,H). The mean time by which FAA anticipated the daily onset of food (phase angle with respect to food presentation) was not significantly different between the two genotypes during FR in LD (Figure 7E). During FR in DD, the random activity in BKOs partially obscured their FAA; however, FAA was still recognizable above the baseline both in individual records (Figure 7F) and in the population activity profile (Figure 7G). This activity change was statistically significant (Figure 7I). Therefore, behavioral responses to temporal food restriction in BKOs were enhanced in DD.

Previously, the DMH was thought to be a possible candidate site of a food-entrainable oscillator (FEO) that regulates feeding behavior [(Gooley et al., 2006; Mieda et al., 2006) but see (Landry et al., 2006, 2007; Moriya et al., 2009; Acosta-Galvan et al., 2011; Landry et al., 2011)]. To examine Bmal1 deletion and Per2 gene expression at this anatomical site, we conducted in situ analysis on the DMH. We collected brains at CT6 and CT18 on the third day of ad lib feeding in DD. Controls showed high expression levels of Bmal1 mRNA in the DMH, albeit with no circadian fluctuation. On the other hand, BKOs had no significant Bmal1 mRNA in the DMH at either time point (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Despite a complete lack of Bmal1 in the DMH, BKOs exhibited normal FAA, indicating that Bmal1 in the DMH is not necessary for food entrainment. Surprisingly, BKOs showed Per2 expression levels comparable to those of controls (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Both genotypes displayed intense staining in the ventromedial portion of the compact nucleus of the DMH (DMC, Figure 7—figure supplement 1), and the difference between peak and trough Per2 mRNA was not significant for either genotype. This result implies that transcriptional regulation of Per2 in the DMH involves activation pathways (e.g., cAMP response element (CRE) and serum response element (SRE) mediated pathways) in addition to CLOCK:BMAL1 as seen previously (Travnickova-Bendova et al., 2002; Gerber et al., 2013).

Restricted feeding selectively resynchronizes peripheral tissues

It has been shown that rhythms of peripheral tissues can be dissociated from the control of the central clock in the SCN and be re-entrained to a new phase by temporal feeding restriction (Damiola et al., 2000; Stokkan et al., 2001). The ability of the peripheral tissues to respond to feeding signals was further demonstrated in vivo (Tahara et al., 2012; Saini et al., 2013), with an observation that phase changes take place faster in the liver when the SCN is surgically lesioned. To assess the impact of feeding cues on the synchrony of peripheral rhythms, we subjected BKO; PER2::LUC mice to a restricted feeding schedule in the absence of light signals (Figure 8A). The duration of constant darkness was the same as the desynchronization experiment (Figure 5). During these >30 days, mice were fed ad libitum for the first 2 weeks, and after 5 days of a food ramp, temporal food restriction was performed for 2 weeks before harvesting the tissues for real-time gene expression recording.

Figure 8. Effects of restricted feeding on circadian rhythms of peripheral tissues.

Figure 8.

(A) Schedule of a temporal FR protocol in DD for real-time circadian reporting assay. Horizontal black and white bars representing lights off and on for LD 12:12 are shown above as a reference for a daily feeding schedule. Gray bars represent food availability. After entrainment to LD 12:12, mice were released into and maintained in DD for a total of >30 days. In DD, mice were fed ad libitum for the initial 2 weeks and underwent a 5-day FR ramp of gradually decreasing food availability to reach the final food availability window of 4 hr (from ZT/CT6–10) for 2 weeks of FR. Tissues were harvested at 2 hr after removal of the food. (B, C) Representative double-plotted actograms of 2 Fx/Fx control (B) and 2 BKO mice (C) during 2 weeks of ad lib feeding followed by the 5-day FR ramp and subsequent 2 weeks of FR in DD. The boxed area toward the left side of each actogram indicates the daily interval of food availability under FR (ZT/CT6–10). (D) Phase map of circadian rhythms of various peripheral tissues from Fx/Fx controls in DD + FR (n = 11). The peak phases (or averaged peak phases) were plotted against the feeding time (green bar). Tissues from the same animal are connected by colored lines with matched symbols. (E) Phase map of (D) was converted to CT for the harvest time. (F) Phase map of BKOs in DD + FR (n = 11). The peak phases (or averaged peak phases) were plotted against the feeding time (green bars). (G) Circular plots of peak bioluminescence rhythms in the SCN and peripheral tissues presented in (DF). The mapping strategy is the same as in Figure 5E. The sample size, mean phase angle, and circular variance for each dataset are summarized in Figure 8—source data 1A. (H) Scattered plot of circular variance in each individual mouse. Degree of variance among the peak phase values of pituitary, liver, kidney, heart, lung, and spleen in an individual mouse was calculated and expressed as circular variance (see Figure 8—source data 1B) and compared between Fx/Fx controls and BKOs in DD + FR. The bar is ±SD. Note that the degree of variance is not changed by phase data conversion from ZT to CT. A significant effect was found between Fx/Fx control and BKO mice (p = 0.0002 by Mann–Whitney test). (I) Normalized amplitude of bioluminescence rhythms from Fx/Fx control (open bar) vs BKO (dark bar) in DD + FR. Mean relative amplitude ±SD are shown. The sample size is the same as in (D, F). ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001 by t-test.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.016

Figure 8—source data 1. (A) Summary of statistical analysis of circular plots presented in Figure 8. (B) Statistical analysis of circular variance for peak bioluminescence in individual mice under restriction feeding.
elife04617s002.docx (38.6KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.04617.017

Consistent with a previous report (Storch and Weitz, 2009), Fx/Fx control mice exhibited both free-running activity and FAA in constant darkness (Figure 8B). The peripheral tissues in these mice displayed variable phase relationships with respect to the feeding time (Figure 8D). However, when the peak phases were converted to the circadian time of the animal at harvest, the phases in a majority of tissues examined (except for pituitary and liver) became markedly clustered (Figure 8E,G; Figure 8—source data 1A), suggesting that the circadian rhythms in many peripheral tissues in wild-type mice are still dictated by the signals from the intact central clock. On the other hand, the peripheral tissues of BKOs showed distinct changes in phase expression patterns in response to restricted feeding cues (Figure 8F). Although BKOs exhibited prolonged pre-feeding activity in DD (Figure 7F; Figure 8C), only the liver and kidney showed tightly clustered phases (Figure 8G; Figure 8—source data 1A). Other tissues (heart, lung, spleen) were variable in phase, though their phase distribution was different either from that found in an ad libitum condition (see also Figure 5D) or from those in Fx/Fx (Figure 8G, Figure 8—source data 1A). These results suggest that feeding cues differentially entrain peripheral clocks; FR strongly synchronizes circadian clocks in the liver and kidney but has weaker effects on other peripheral tissues such as the pituitary, heart, lung, and spleen.

To our surprise, with the exception of the liver, oscillatory amplitude of peripheral rhythms of BKOs remained lower than that of Fx/Fx control mice even after 2 weeks of FR (Figure 8I). Lower amplitude is not related to the state of phase synchrony, because relative amplitude of kidney still remained low even though FR synchronized its phase. This indicates that the damped rhythms were not completely restored by food signals.

To compare ad libitum and FR conditions directly, we performed a ‘meta-analysis’ on phase and amplitude within Fx/Fx and BKOs, respectively (Figure 9A–D). Statistical comparison between ad libitum in DD (BKO from DD) and FR in DD (BKO from DD + FR) confirmed tissue specificity of phase entrainment by feeding (Figure 9; Figure 9—source data 1A,B). This result is in striking contrast to the case in which BKOs were exposed to LD cycles, which synchronized all peripheral rhythms examined even in the absence of a functioning SCN (Figure 9B,D; Figure 9—source data 1B). Nevertheless, both internal synchrony and oscillatory amplitude in peripheral tissues were maintained most optimally when an intact SCN was present (Figure 5; Figure 6A; Figure 8H). Taken together, these results indicate that, while individual tissues can be entrained by light/dark cycles and feeding schedules independently, the central clock plays an important role in sustaining coherence among peripheral tissues and robust circadian programs at the organismal level.

Figure 9. Analysis of circadian rhythms of peripheral tissues under different conditions.

Figure 9.

(A, B) Comparison of peak bioluminescence rhythms in the peripheral tissues in Fx/Fx (A) and BKO (B) mice under LD, DD, and FR conditions. Circular plots are as presented in Figure 5E and Figure 8G. Watson-Williams F-test and bootstrap analysis were performed to compare the mean phase angle (μ) and the variance (V, distribution of peak phase values) between two circular data, respectively. Statistical comparison was summarized in Figure 9—source data 1A,B). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. N.S. = not significant. (C, D) Comparison of normalized amplitude of bioluminescence rhythms from Fx/Fx (C) and BKO (D) mice under LD, DD, and FR conditions. Each bar graph is as presented in Figure 6A,B and Figure 8I. Mean relative amplitude ±SD are shown. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 by ANOVA.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.018

Figure 9—source data 1. (A) Summary of statistical comparison of peak phases from Fx/Fx mice under different conditions. (B) Summary of statistical comparison of peak phases from BKO mice under different conditions.
elife04617s003.docx (35.4KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.04617.019

Discussion

Circadian behavioral rhythmicity is abolished by deleting Bmal1 from the forebrain

Bmal1 is the only component of the mammalian circadian clock whose sole deletion generates a complete loss of circadian rhythms in mice (Bunger et al., 2000). Therefore, elimination of Bmal1 from a target tissue provides an invaluable model to study the behavioral and physiological consequences of abolishing circadian rhythms in a tissue-specific manner. In this study, we succeeded in removing Bmal1 specifically from the forebrain using a Camk2a::iCreBAC line (Casanova et al., 2001). Notably, this CamiCre driver was able to efficiently recombine and delete our target floxed gene in the entire SCN. These forebrain/SCN-specific Bmal1 knockouts exhibited a total loss of circadian behavioral rhythmicity, and their circadian phenotype in DD and LL is similar to that of global Bmal1 knockouts (Bunger et al., 2000). Thus, Bmal1 in the forebrain is essential for normal circadian activity rhythms. However, a Cre driver that can express more specifically in the SCN would be necessary to confirm the dominance of the SCN over other sites of the brain.

BKOs displayed apparent entrainment to LD cycles; however, additional experiments revealed two deficits in these mice. First, entrainment in a skeleton photoperiod was abnormal with a bimodal pattern. A similar pattern is found in pinealectomized sparrows in which a population of oscillators entrains with two different and opposite phase angles (Takahashi and Menaker, 1982). Second, there is a delay in masking behavior to light under LD 3.5:3.5 which is similar to that seen in Clock mutants (Redlin et al., 2005). Thus, the variable phase angle of entrainment in BKOs on LD 12:12 is consistent with weaker synchronization of residual oscillators as well as a small deficit in masking.

Impact of a loss of a functional forebrain clock on circadian organization of peripheral tissues

Despite a complete loss of circadian behavioral rhythmicity, Bmal1 forebrain-specific knockout mice are as healthy as wild-type mice, consistent with our earlier results using tissue-specific rescue of Bmal1−/− mice (McDearmon et al., 2006). These results suggest that Bmal1's contribution to the circadian phenotype seen in Bmal1−/− mice is distinct from its role in the maintenance of normal activity levels, body weight, reproduction, and longevity. For instance, the skeletal muscle defects seen in Bmal1−/− mice (Andrews et al., 2010) are likely a causative factor in their low activity. Muscle-rescued Bmal1−/− mice exhibit improvement of body weight, activity, and longevity but not of arthropathy (McDearmon et al., 2006). Our findings as well as others' suggest that the effects of BMAL1 can be dissociated between brain and other tissues including the reproductive system.

While the effect of Bmal1 deletion is local, the impact of disabling the brain's clock on peripheral circadian organization is significant. Previous studies have investigated the effect of SCN ablation on peripheral rhythms by making SCN lesions and recording circadian gene expression of peripheral tissues either ex vivo (Yoo et al., 2004) or in vivo (Tahara et al., 2012; Saini et al., 2013). Here, we used a Cre-mediated genetic excision approach to remove a critical component of the molecular oscillator from the intact SCN network. This strategy demonstrates that internal synchrony and high amplitude rhythms are lost in the absence of a functional master oscillator in the forebrain/SCN and supports previous reports (Yoo et al., 2004; Tahara et al., 2012; Saini et al., 2013). Nevertheless, further studies are needed to elucidate the functional consequences of damped circadian rhythms in each peripheral organ.

On the other hand, the dispersed phases in BKOs were not observed when mice were placed in an LD condition (Figure 5C,E). Given that the BKO's circadian behavior is driven by light from retinal pathways presumably acting via the SCN, this demonstrates that cyclic photic input (Figure 3) is sufficient to sustain coherent temporal synchrony of peripheral oscillators. Alternatively, the effects of light could also be mediated by activity (wheel-running)-associated stimuli driven by LD as another source of systemic signals impinging on peripheral clocks analogous to that found previously (Kornmann et al., 2007; Hughes et al., 2012).

Forebrain Bmal1 expression is not necessary for food entrainment behavior

BKOs offer an advantage for investigating the effects of a disabled master clock in the brain because they do not suffer from the morbid phenotypes observed with a global loss of Bmal1. Using the forebrain Bmal1-deficient animals, we were able to conduct food restriction experiments under completely standard conditions. In contrast to global Bmal1 knockouts, altered experimental conditions such as enriched liquid food (Storch and Weitz, 2009), LD 18:6 (Pendergast et al., 2009), or food placed on the cage bottom, and gentle handling to prompt feeding (Fuller et al., 2008) were not required for these mice. Our results show that BKOs indeed displayed normal FAA as other studies using global Bmal1 knockouts have reported (Mistlberger et al., 2008; Pendergast et al., 2009; Storch and Weitz, 2009), demonstrating that BMAL1 in the forebrain is not essential for food entrainment behavior. Specifically, our Bmal1 in situ results in the DMH indicate that the DMH is not necessary for FAA as reported previously (Landry et al., 2006, 2007; Moriya et al., 2009; Acosta-Galvan et al., 2011; Landry et al., 2011). In our experiments, BKOs tend to exhibit intact food-anticipatory behavior under constant darkness conditions. However, overall activity of BKO's circadian behavior was also significantly elevated in DD, which makes it difficult to distinguish whether the elevated pre-feeding activity is due to enhanced FAA or to the effect of overall activity increase. Nevertheless, these results suggest that an inhibitory system (Davis and Menaker, 1980) suppresses the total activity of both circadian behavior and FAA. It remains to be determined what factors underlie suppression of the activity and how their regulation in the SCN and other brain regions is altered by the deletion of BMAL1.

Differential effects of light/dark cycles and feeding cues

Previous work has shown that restricted feeding can alter the phase of peripheral rhythms without affecting the phase of the SCN (Damiola et al., 2000; Stokkan et al., 2001). While peripheral oscillators can be uncoupled from the central clock in the SCN, recent studies have suggested that SCN-derived signals counteract the feeding cues for rapid phase-shifting of peripheral clocks (Saini et al., 2013) and that phase-shifting rates are variable among different tissues (Damiola et al., 2000; Yamazaki et al., 2000). Consistent with these observations, peripheral tissues in our control mice displayed variable phases relative to the FR schedule when conducted in constant darkness (Figure 8D). In these free-running conditions, the circadian activity rhythms of the mice were not affected by FR; and as a consequence, the phase relationships between the FR schedule and circadian phase could be dissociated. When control mice in DD under ad libitum conditions were compared to control mice in DD under FR using circadian phase as a marker, the liver, kidney and heart significantly shifted their mean circular phase in response to feeding, showing an effect of FR on these tissues [Figure 9A, compare Fx/Fx from DD with Fx/Fx from DD + FR (in CT); Figure 9—source data 1A]. However, the phases of the peripheral rhythms were significantly clustered with the phase of the activity rhythm (Figure 8E), strongly suggesting that signals from the SCN continue to dictate the expression of peripheral phases in wild-type mice despite the expression of FAA behavior. Indeed, only the liver was strongly clustered in phase with FR in DD (Figure 8G, Fx/Fx from DD + FR) compared to the circadian phase of activity [Figure 8G, Fx/Fx from DD + FR (in CT)] in which all peripheral tissues were clustered in phase. Thus we see a rather complex picture in control mice in which FR exerts tissue-specific effects on peripheral organ circadian phase and coherence.

In contrast, in the absence of a functional pacemaker in the forebrain, liver and kidney clocks shifted to a distinct phase in response to food restriction and became synchronized (Figure 8F,G; Figure 9B), which supports the results of an in vivo study using SCN-lesioned animals (Saini et al., 2013). We found, however, that other tissues such as heart, lung, and spleen did not entrain to food restriction even after 2 weeks of treatment (Figure 9B; Figure 9—source data 1A). Although the precise mechanism is not known, the lower degree of synchrony of these tissues could be due to the longer duration of pre-feeding behavioral activity which could mobilize additional (conflicting) signals. At the same time, these tissues showed distinct phase shifts in response to feeding (Figure 9B; Figure 9—source data 1B). Circular phase variance analysis indicates that internal coherence among different organs within an individual mouse still remained disrupted (Figure 8H). Therefore, these results suggest that, while synchrony of individual tissues such as liver and kidney can be maintained independently by a feeding schedule, synchrony among peripheral tissues requires central clocks in the forebrain/SCN.

The effects of FR were different from the effects of LD cycles (Figure 5; Figure 9B), which sustained both phase synchrony and oscillatory amplitude in peripheral clocks in BKOs (Figure 9). Notably, both phase angle (Figure 5E; Figure 5—source data 1A) and phase coherence among peripheral tissues (Figure 5F) were similar to those in Fx/Fx control mice which possess an intact central clock. We speculate that, while feeding cues can antagonize SCN-derived signals, LD cycles appear to act in concert with the SCN or signals arising from the SCN. Although detailed pathways through which the SCN controls the peripheral clocks remain unknown, recently, SRF was identified as a transcription factor that regulates gene expression in peripheral tissues in response to systemically oscillating signals (Gerber et al., 2013). It will be interesting to explore how these signals affect circadian gene expression in vivo to determine the mechanisms by which the SCN communicates with peripheral oscillators. Our experiments reveal the diverse behavioral and peripheral physiological consequences of inactivating a forebrain clock. They pave the way for further exploring the complex relationship between central and peripheral clocks.

Materials and methods

Animals

All mice were housed under LD 12:12 unless otherwise noted. Camk2a::iCreBAC mice (CamiCre) (MGI:2181426) were kindly provided by Dr G. Schutz (Casanova et al., 2001). For a Cre reporter mouse, Rosa26-CAG-LSL-tdTomato (Ai14, kindly provided by Dr Hongkui Zeng) was used (Madisen et al., 2010). Bmal1fx/fx mice (Johnson et al., 2014) are homozygous for a Bmal1 allele which has loxP sites inserted into the introns surrounding exon 4. Bmal1fx/fx mice (129SvJ × C57BL/6J N3 backcross) were crossed to CamiCre to produce Bmal1fx/fx (Fx/Fx), CamiCre+;Bmal1fx/+ (Het) , and CamiCre+; Bmal1fx/fx (BKO) mice. CamiCre mice were mated to their siblings (FVB/N × C57BL/6J N3 backcross) to generate CamiCre hemizygous controls. Bmal1−/− global knockout mice (Bunger et al., 2000) were 129SvJ × C57BL/6J N14 backcross congenic animals. For the real-time reporting assay, PER2::LUC mice (Yoo et al., 2004) were crossed to Bmal1fx/fx to produce CamiCre+; Bmal1fx/fx; Per2Luc/+ and its control littermate, Bmal1fx/fx; Per2Luc/+. For the bioluminescence imaging experiments, Per2Luc mice were homozygous. For all experiments, male and female mice were used in balanced ratios; mice were 2–7 months old for behavior experiments and 3–11 months old for bioluminescence recordings and age-matched across groups, except for FR (males only were used for behavior experiments), where mice were 1.9–3.6 months old at the start of the experiment. All animal studies and Materials and methods were in accordance with Northwestern University and UT Southwestern Medical Center guidelines for animal care and use.

PCR genotyping

The following sets of genotyping primers were used: CamiCre: iCre-PCR-F 5′-TCTGATGAAGTCAGGAAGAACC-3′ and iCre-PCR-R 5′-GAGATGTCCTTCACTCTGATTC-3′ (amplified a 400 bp product). PCR reactions were carried out in a final volume of 30 µl buffer consisting of 5 µl Promega 5× Colored buffer, 2.7 µl 25 mM MgCl2, 0.38 µl 20 µM each primer, 3 µl 2 mM dNTPs, 3 µl NaOH-extracted tail genomic DNA, and 0.3 µl Promega Go Taq (1 U/rx; Promega, Madison, WI). PCR conditions were: 5 min at 95°C, followed by 30 cycles of 1 min at 94°C, 2 min at 58°C, and 2 min at 72°C, followed by 8 min at 72°C. The reaction products were analyzed on a 2% agarose gel. Bmal1fx/fx: OL5436 5′- CCC TGA ACA TGG GAA AGA GA -3′ and OL6013 5′- ATT CAC CTT TTG GGG AGG AC -3′ (floxed band: 360 bp, WT band: 310 bp). PCR reactions were carried out in a final volume of 25 µl buffer consisting of 5 µl Promega 5× Colored buffer, 3 µl 25 mM MgCl2, 1 µl 20 µM each primer, 2.5 µl 2 mM dNTPs, 3 µl NaOH-extracted tail genomic DNA, and 0.3 µl Promega Go Taq (1.25 U/rx). PCR conditions were: 5 min at 95°C; followed by 37 cycles of 30 s at 95°C, 30 s at 60°C, and 30 s at 72°C, followed by 5 min at 72°C. Ai14: Ai14F 5′- TAC GGC ATG GAC GAG CTG TAC AAG TAA -3′and Ai14R 5′- CAG GCG AGC AGC CAA GGA AA -3′ (amplified a 517 bp product). Bmal1−/− and PER2::LUC mice were genotyped as previously described (Bunger et al., 2000; Yoo et al., 2004).

Behavioral experiments and activity monitoring

To record the rhythm of locomotor activity, adult mice (at least 8-week old) were individually housed in activity wheel-equipped cages under LD 12:12 for at least 10 days. Locomotor activity was recorded and analyzed using ClockLab software (Actimetrics, Wilmette, IL) as previously described (Vitaterna et al., 1999; Siepka and Takahashi, 2005; McDearmon et al., 2006; Vitaterna et al., 2006). Fluorescent lights (300–600 lux inside the cage) were used for behavior experiments. Mice were transferred into DD for 4 weeks, returned to LD for 2 weeks, released into LL for 4 weeks, and then returned to LD for at least 2 weeks. For the entrainment experiment, mice were placed in LD 12:12 for at least 10 days, released into DD for 4 weeks, followed by LD for 2 weeks, before they were introduced to a skeleton photoperiod (LDLD 1:10:1:12) for 4 weeks. The mice were returned to LD for 2 weeks and then switched to an ultradian cycle (LD 3.5:3.5) for 3 weeks.

Food restriction experiments

For the food restriction experiment, mice were placed in wheel-equipped cages under LD 12:12 for 2 weeks, then underwent a 5-day FR ramp of gradually decreasing food availability (Figure 7A), followed by 14 days of FR in LD. They were returned to ad lib feeding under LD for 2 weeks, DD for 2 weeks, and LD for 2 weeks. Then the mice underwent a 5-day FR ramp after which they were released into DD for 14 days of FR in DD. On the first day of the FR ramp, food was removed at ZT/CT18 and 12 hr later food was returned. On each successive day, food was removed 2 hr earlier until food was available for 4 hr, from ZT/CT6–10 (Figure 7A). This was the final food availability window for 14 days of FR. A regular mouse diet (5K52; Lab Diet, Richmond, IN; contains 19.3% protein and 6.2% fat, or 2918; Irradiated Global Diet, Teklad Diets, Madison, WI; contains 18.6% protein and 6.2% fat) was used during FR experiments. Food pellets were weighed daily for each mouse before and after food availability to monitor food consumption. Individual mean daily activity profiles were computed by ClockLab; group average profiles were made using Microsoft Excel.

For the bioluminescence recording experiment, mice were initially entrained in LD 12:12 for a minimum of 6 days (6–12 days), as the light-on time was delayed for 6 hr, before they were released into DD for 2 weeks. Then the mice underwent a 5-day FR ramp as above, after which 4-hr FR was conducted from ZT/CT6–10 for 2 weeks (Figure 8A). On the final day of 2 weeks of FR, tissues were harvested for bioluminescence recordings at 2 hr after the food was withdrawn (ZT/CT12).

Behavioral data analysis

Wheel-running activity was recorded and analyzed essentially as previously described (Vitaterna et al., 2006). The free-running periods in DD and LL were calculated from the entire 28-day interval by using χ2 periodogram analysis (Clocklab software, Actimetrics). The amplitude of circadian rhythm was analyzed using the fast Fourier transform (FFT), which estimates the relative power of approximately 24 hr period rhythm in comparison with all other periodicities in the time-series (Shimomura et al., 2001). The power spectral densities for frequencies ranging from 0 to 1 cycle/hr were determined and normalized to a total power (area under the curve) of 1.0. The peak in the circadian range (18- to 30-hr period or 0.033–0.055 cycles/hr) of the relative power was determined for each animal for comparison. If no significant periodicity in the 18–30 hr range was detected by FFT, the free-running period was not scored. For analysis of total daily activity, the total number of wheel revolutions per day was averaged in LD (last 10 days of initial LD interval), DD, and LL (28 days) using the same days described above for free-running period and amplitude analysis. Effects of genotype were analyzed by a Generalized Model (GLM) ANOVA by using NCSS (Kayesville, UT), with Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests for pairwise comparisons.

Two-sample t-tests were used to analyze subjective day vs night activity during skeleton photoperiod and amplitudes during LD 3.5:3.5, and Repeated Measures GLM ANOVA with Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests was used to compare the time course of masking between genotypes during days 3–15 of the 21-day LD 3.5:3.5 cycle. FFT was used to analyze amplitudes in the circadian as well as 7-hr range during LD 3.5:3.5. To analyze FR data, Repeated Measures GLM ANOVA with Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests was used to compare the effect of genotype, time, and interaction between the two on FAA (absolute counts), FAA (% of total activity), weights, total daily activity, and food intake. Two-sample t-test and paired t-test, respectively, were used to calculate FAA onset and activity fold-change from ad lib to FR.

In situ hybridization

For the time course collection of brain tissue for in situ hybridization, after 2 weeks of entrainment in LD, animals were released into DD for 2 days and tissues were harvested on the third day of DD. Brains were rapidly dissected and frozen on dry ice and stored at −80°C until further processing. Standard in situ hybridization procedures were used as previously described (Sangoram et al., 1998). Alternate 20-µm thick coronal sections were collected from each brain from the rostral to the caudal end of the SCN or DMH (24–32 total, 12–16 for each probe) by using a Leica CM3050S cryostat. Sections were thaw-mounted onto SuperFrost plus microscope slides (VWR, Radnor, PA), then stored at −80°C until all sectioning was completed. Alternate sections were hybridized to a combination oligoprobe against mBmal1 exon 4 and to a riboprobe against Per2. For the Bmal1 combination oligoprobe, three 37 to 50-mer oligonucleotide probes (IDT, Coralville, IA) were radiolabeled at the 3′ ends with 33P via terminal I deoxynucleotidyl transferase (Gibco/Invitrogen, Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY) and used in equal proportions: Mop3Ex4Probe1 5′-AACTGTTCATTTTGTCCCGACGCCTCTTTTCAATCTGACTGTGGGCCTCC-3′, Mop3Ex4Probe2 5′-CCTGGACATTGCATTGCATGTTGGTACCAAAGAAGCCAATTCATC-3′, Mop3Ex4Probe3 5′-CTGAACAGCCATCCTTAGCACGGTGAGTTTATCTAAC-3′. Templates for the anti-sense and sense Per2 probes were PCR-generated by using the following primers: Per2-insitu-f 5′-ACG AGA ACT GCT CCA CGG GAC-3′, Per2-insitu-r 5′-ACA GCC ACA GCA AAC ATA TCC GC-3′. PCR products were cloned into a TA cloning vector (Invitrogen). The T7 promoter from the TA cloning vector was used to generate both anti-sense and sense probes. All probes were labeled with 33P. Quantitation of the autoradiogram signal was performed by using NIH ImageJ 1.34s software (NIH, Bethesda, MD) and normalized to radioactive standards as described previously (Vitaterna et al., 1999). The optical density (OD) of individual SCN or DMC was normalized by subtracting the OD of an area of identical size in the lateral hypothalamus or the magnocellular nucleus of lateral hypothalamus, respectively, from the same side (left or right) and section. Normalized values from three sections near the middle (anterior–posterior) SCN or caudal DMH (at the level of DMC) were used to calculate an average for each brain. Effects of genotype, time, or their interactions were analyzed by GLM ANOVA, with Tukey–Kramer multiple comparison post-tests for pairwise comparisons.

Western blotting

For Westerns blots, tissues were harvested at ZT16 from mice in LD 12:12. Tissues were homogenized by a Polytron homogenizer in a buffer containing 44.6 mM Tris–HCl, 5.5 mM Tris–Base, 154 mM NaCl, 29.8 mM sodium pyrophosphate, 20 mM glycerol 2-phosphate, 50 mM NaF, 1 µg/ml aprotinin, 1 µg/ml leupeptin, 1 mM PMSF, 1 mM sodium orthovanadate, 1 mM EDTA, 1 mM p-nitrophosphate, 0.1% SDS, 0.5% sodium deoxycholate, and 1% NP-40. Homogenates were centrifuged at 15,000×g for 20 min at 4°C. Supernatants were collected and protein concentration was estimated using Bio-Rad DC Protein Assay according to the manufacturer's instructions. Total protein (10 µg) was diluted 1:1 with Laemmli sample buffer and resolved on a 10% SDS-polyacrylamide gel by electrophoresis. Thereafter, proteins were electrotransferred onto a GE Healthcare PVDF transfer membrane. The membranes were blocked with PBST (PBS + 0.1% Tween-20) containing 5% non-fat powdered milk for 1 hr and then incubated with the rabbit polyclonal anti-MOP3 antibody (1:6,250, generated in Dr Bradfield's lab and is available at NB100-2288, Novus Biologicals LLC, Littleton, CO), followed by anti-rabbit IgG secondary antisera horseradish peroxidase (1:1000; PI-1000, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). Proteins were visualized with a chemiluminescence detection system (ECL Western blotting detection analysis system; Amersham Pharmacia, GE Healthcare Bio-Sciences, Pittsburgh, PA) and with subsequent exposure to autoradiographic films.

Immunohistochemistry and microscopy

Brain tissues for immunohistochemistry were harvested at ZT16 under LD 12:12. Animals were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine/saline cocktail (10 mg/ml ketamine, 10 mg/ml xylazine) at 0.01 ml/g body weight or with sodium pentobarbital at 120 mg/kg body weight and then perfused intracardially with 25 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde (pH 7.4, Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO) in 63.4 mM phosphate buffer (pH 7.5). The brains were removed and post-fixed for 2 hr at 4°C in 4% paraformaldehyde in 63.4 mM phosphate buffer and transferred to 20% sucrose/phosphate buffer overnight. For immunohistochemistry, 50-µm coronal sections were collected through the entire SCN using a Leica cryostat and processed free-floating. Sections were incubated with the rabbit polyclonal anti-MOP3 (BMAL1) antibody (1:1000) followed by anti-rabbit IgG secondary biotinylated antisera (1:200; BA-1000, Vector Laboratories) and visualized using Vectastain ABC Kit and 0.5 mg/ml diaminobenzidine, 0.01% hydrogen peroxide, and 0.03% NiCl2 in 0.05 M Tris (pH 7.2). Sections were mounted onto gelatin-coated microscope slides using aqueous mounting medium (3:1 glycerol/phosphate buffer), cover-slipped, and imaged with a Leica DM-RB upright microscope using Openlab software in the Biological Imaging Facility at Northwestern University. For the whole brain imaging, a Zeiss Stereo Discovery Microscope V12 was used under a bright field. Fluorescence images were taken using an Olympus MVX10 (1×) and a Zeiss LSM 510 confocal microscope (20× objective).

Real-time bioluminescence recording and data analysis

To monitor real-time reporting of PER2::LUC oscillations, tissues were harvested after cervical dislocation between ZT/CT10.5–13. Coronal sections of the brain were sliced at 300-μm thickness by a vibratome in ice-cold Hank's balanced salt solution (HBSS, Invitrogen). Each individual SCN was dissected out and cultured on a Millicell organotypic membrane (PICM ORG50, Millipore, Billerica, MA) in a 35 mm tissue culture dish containing 1.2 ml DMEM (90-013-PB, Mediatech, Manassas, VA), supplemented with 2% B27 (Invitrogen), 10 mM Hepes (pH 7.2), 4 mM L-glutamine, 0.035% sodium bicarbonate, 25 units/ml penicillin, 25 μg/ml streptomycin, and 0.1 mM luciferin (L-8240, Biosynth AG, Staad, Switzerland). Other tissues were processed as previously described (Yamazaki and Takahashi, 2005) and cultured as above. The bioluminescence was monitored by a light-tight 32-channel LumiCycle luminometry (Actimetrics, Wilmette, IL) maintained at 36°C and was recorded at an interval of 10-min continuously for a minimum of 8 days followed by medium change to confirm the viability of the samples.

All data were analyzed essentially as described previously (Izumo et al., 2006). Briefly, raw data of bioluminescence records were corrected for background counts and PMT gain. For animals harvested from a DD condition (30–44 days in DD), activity onset of each Fx/Fx control mouse was set as CT12. The time-series data were detrended by a 24-hr running average and then subjected to FFT-NLLS analysis to calculate periods and phases. The initial 20 hr of data were trimmed because they contain an acute effect of explant preparation. Data beyond 192 hr before medium change were also trimmed except for one group (1 Fx/Fx control and 1 BKO), which was affected by an electrical shutdown on day 6. The amplitude was computed at a 36–60 hr range and normalized as previously described (Izumo et al., 2006). For all data, the calculations were averaged where duplicated samples were prepared from the same tissue within an individual mouse, and the averaged value was used to map or calculate for all mice collected. Samples with RelAmp Error ≥0.84 (background counts) or out of a circadian (17–31 hr) range (24 hr ± 30% of 24 hr) in the periodicity were excluded from the analysis. A total of 1106 LumiCycle files were processed and used in this study. To construct a phase map, smoothed time-series were used to determine the peak on the second day. Phase angles and circular variances of circular plots were computed using Oriana (Kovach Computing Services, Wales, UK). The variance of the peak phases between two groups was compared using bootstrap analysis (Sato et al., 2007). For each iteration of the bootstrap, the difference of the variance between the two groups was calculated by random sampling with replacement from the data within the each of the two groups. Following 20,000 iterations, ninety-five percent confidence intervals were used to determine whether the differences were significantly different from zero.

Bioluminescence imaging and data analysis

For real-time bioluminescence imaging analysis, age-matched pairs of mice (Fx/Fx control and BKO) were placed in DD for >30 days before harvesting the tissues. The tissue collection was repeated for a total of three pairs of mice. The tissues were processed and prepared in the same manner as in the luminometry recording, except that a glass bottom culture dish (MatTek, Ashland, MA) was used. The sealed culture was placed onto the stage chamber maintained at 36°C inside the LV200 Bioluminescence Imaging System (Olympus America, Irving, TX). The sample was imaged using a 10× objective lens with 10-min exposure time at 25-min interval for 7 days. The imaging files were converted to linear time-series by quantifying the signals on grid matrices. The heat map was generated using the top 200 time-series data starting with the strongest signals. The top 50 of these time-series data were analyzed for period, phase, and relative amplitude by FFT-NLLS as described above. The phases calculated by FFT-NLLS were further converted to the actual time and divided by each circadian period to normalize period differences. Statistical analysis was conducted between paired mice as described above.

Acknowledgements

We thank Ms Amanda Falk for help with mouse production, Dr Tereza Smejkalova for help with Westerns, Dr William Russin at the Biologial Imaging Facility and Dr Xinran Liu for help with imaging, Dr David Ferster for help with data analysis, Mr Giuseppe Fruci for assistance with figure production, Drs Martha Vitaterna, Hee-Kyung Hong, Marleen de Groot, Kazuhiro Shimomura, and Ethan Buhr for helpful discussions. We also thank Dr Hongkui Zeng at Allen Institute for Brain Science for Ai14 mice. JST is an investigator in the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute H0690101 to Joseph S Takahashi.

  • National Institute of Mental Health U01 MH61915, P50 MH074924 to Joseph S Takahashi.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke 1F30NS056551-01 to Martina Pejchal.

  • Brain and Behavior Research Foundation to Mariko Izumo.

  • National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences R01 ES005703 to Christopher A Bradfield.

  • Japan Science and Technology Agency to Takashi R Sato.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

MI, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

MP, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

ACS, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

RPL, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

JAW, Analysis and interpretation of data, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

TRS, Analysis and interpretation of data, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

XW, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

CAB, Drafting or revising the article, Contributed unpublished essential data or reagents.

JST, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animal care and use procedures were in accordance with guidelines of the Northwestern University (Protocol 2006-0035) and UT Southwestern Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (Protocols 2009-0054 and 2012-0090).

References

  1. Abe M, Herzog ED, Yamazaki S, Straume M, Tei H, Sakaki Y, Menaker M, Block GD. Circadian rhythms in isolated brain regions. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:350–356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-01-00350.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abraham U, Prior JL, Granados-Fuentes D, Piwnica-Worms DR, Herzog ED. Independent circadian oscillations of Period1 in specific brain areas in vivo and in vitro. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:8620–8626. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2225-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Acosta-Galvan G, Yi CX, van der Vliet J, Jhamandas JH, Panula P, Angeles-Castellanos M, Del Carmen Basualdo M, Escobar C, Buijs RM. Interaction between hypothalamic dorsomedial nucleus and the suprachiasmatic nucleus determines intensity of food anticipatory behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2011;108:5813–5818. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015551108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alvarez JD, Hansen A, Ord T, Bebas P, Chappell PE, Giebultowicz JM, Williams C, Moss S, Sehgal A. The circadian clock protein BMAL1 is necessary for fertility and proper testosterone production in mice. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 2008;23:26–36. doi: 10.1177/0748730407311254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andrews JL, Zhang X, McCarthy JJ, McDearmon EL, Hornberger TA, Russell B, Campbell KS, Arbogast S, Reid MB, Walker JR, Hogenesch JB, Takahashi JS, Esser KA. CLOCK and BMAL1 regulate MyoD and are necessary for maintenance of skeletal muscle phenotype and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2010;107:19090–19095. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014523107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bass J, Takahashi JS. Circadian integration of metabolism and energetics. Science. 2010;330:1349–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.1195027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boden MJ, Varcoe TJ, Voultsios A, Kennaway DJ. Reproductive biology of female Bmal1 null mice. Reproduction. 2010;139:1077–1090. doi: 10.1530/REP-09-0523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Buhr ED, Yoo SH, Takahashi JS. Temperature as a universal resetting cue for mammalian circadian oscillators. Science. 2010;330:379–385. doi: 10.1126/science.1195262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bunger MK, Walisser JA, Sullivan R, Manley PA, Moran SM, Kalscheur VL, Colman RJ, Bradfield CA. Progressive arthropathy in mice with a targeted disruption of the Mop3/Bmal-1 locus. Genesis. 2005;41:122–132. doi: 10.1002/gene.20102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bunger MK, Wilsbacher LD, Moran SM, Clendenin C, Radcliffe LA, Hogenesch JB, Simon MC, Takahashi JS, Bradfield CA. Mop3 is an essential component of the master circadian pacemaker in mammals. Cell. 2000;103:1009–1017. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)00205-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Casanova E, Fehsenfeld S, Mantamadiotis T, Lemberger T, Greiner E, Stewart AF, Schütz G. A CamKIIalpha iCre BAC allows brain-specific gene inactivation. Genesis. 2001;31:37–42. doi: 10.1002/gene.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Damiola F, Le Minh N, Preitner N, Kornmann B, Fleury-Olela F, Schibler U. Restricted feeding uncouples circadian oscillators in peripheral tissues from the central pacemaker in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Genes & Development. 2000;14:2950–2961. doi: 10.1101/gad.183500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davis FC, Menaker M. Hamsters through time's window: temporal structure of hamster locomotor rhythmicity. The American Journal of Physiology. 1980;239:R149–R155. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1980.239.1.R149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fuller PM, Lu J, Saper CB. Differential rescue of light- and food-entrainable circadian rhythms. Science. 2008;320:1074–1077. doi: 10.1126/science.1153277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fuller PM, Lu J, Saper CB. Standards of evidence in chronobiology: a response. Journal of Circadian Rhythms. 2009;7:9. doi: 10.1186/1740-3391-7-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gavériaux-Ruff C, Kieffer BL. Conditional gene targeting in the mouse nervous system: Insights into brain function and diseases. Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 2007;113:619–634. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2006.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gekakis N, Staknis D, Nguyen HB, Davis FC, Wilsbacher LD, King DP, Takahashi JS, Weitz CJ. Role of the CLOCK protein in the mammalian circadian mechanism. Science. 1998;280:1564–1569. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5369.1564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gerber A, Esnault C, Aubert G, Treisman R, Pralong F, Schibler U. Blood-borne circadian signal stimulates daily oscillations in actin dynamics and SRF activity. Cell. 2013;152:492–503. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gooley JJ, Schomer A, Saper CB. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is critical for the expression of food-entrainable circadian rhythms. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:398–407. doi: 10.1038/nn1651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Green CB, Takahashi JS, Bass J. The meter of metabolism. Cell. 2008;134:728–742. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.08.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Guo H, Brewer JM, Champhekar A, Harris RB, Bittman EL. Differential control of peripheral circadian rhythms by suprachiasmatic-dependent neural signals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2005;102:3111–3116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409734102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Guo H, Brewer JM, Lehman MN, Bittman EL. Suprachiasmatic regulation of circadian rhythms of gene expression in hamster peripheral organs: effects of transplanting the pacemaker. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6406–6412. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4676-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hogenesch JB, Gu YZ, Jain S, Bradfield CA. The basic-helix-loop-helix-PAS orphan MOP3 forms transcriptionally active complexes with circadian and hypoxia factors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 1998;95:5474–5479. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.10.5474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Huang N, Chelliah Y, Shan Y, Taylor CA, Yoo SH, Partch C, Green CB, Zhang H, Takahashi JS. Crystal structure of the heterodimeric CLOCK: BMAL1 transcriptional activator complex. Science. 2012;337:189–194. doi: 10.1126/science.1222804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hughes ME, Hong HK, Chong JL, Indacochea AA, Lee SS, Han M, Takahashi JS, Hogenesch JB. Brain-specific rescue of Clock reveals system-driven transcriptional rhythms in peripheral tissue. PLOS Genetics. 2012;8:e1002835. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Husse J, Zhou X, Shostak A, Oster H, Eichele G. Synaptotagmin10-Cre, a driver to disrupt clock genes in the SCN. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 2011;26:379–389. doi: 10.1177/0748730411415363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Izumo M, Sato TR, Straume M, Johnson CH. Quantitative analyses of circadian gene expression in mammalian cell cultures. PLOS Computational Biology. 2006;2:e136. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Johnson BP, Walisser JA, Liu Y, Shen AL, McDearmon EL, Moran SM, McIntosh BE, Vollrath AL, Schook AC, Takahashi JS, Bradfield CA. Hepatocyte circadian clock controls acetaminophen bioactivation through NADPH-cytochrome P450 oxidoreductase. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2014;111:18757–18762. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421708111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. King DP, Zhao Y, Sangoram AM, Wilsbacher LD, Tanaka M, Antoch MP, Steeves TD, Vitaterna MH, Kornhauser JM, Lowrey PL, Turek FW, Takahashi JS. Positional cloning of the mouse circadian clock gene. Cell. 1997;89:641–653. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80245-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ko CH, Yamada YR, Welsh DK, Buhr ED, Liu AC, Zhang EE, Steeves TD, Vitaterna MH, Kornhauser JM, Lowrey PL, Turek FW, Takahashi JS. Emergence of noise-induced oscillations in the central circadian pacemaker. PLOS Biology. 2010;8:e1000513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kondratov RV, Kondratova AA, Gorbacheva VY, Vykhovanets OV, Antoch MP. Early aging and age-related pathologies in mice deficient in BMAL1, the core componentof the circadian clock. Genes & Development. 2006;20:1868–1873. doi: 10.1101/gad.1432206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kornmann B, Schaad O, Bujard H, Takahashi JS, Schibler U. System-driven and oscillator-dependent circadian transcription in mice with a conditionally active liver clock. PLOS Biology. 2007;5:e34. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kume K, Zylka MJ, Sriram S, Shearman LP, Weaver DR, Jin X, Maywood ES, Hastings MH, Reppert SM. mCRY1 and mCRY2 are essential components of the negative limb of the circadian clock feedback loop. Cell. 1999;98:193–205. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81014-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lamia KA, Storch KF, Weitz CJ. Physiological significance of a peripheral tissue circadian clock. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2008;105:15172–15177. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806717105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Landry GJ, Kent BA, Patton DF, Jaholkowski M, Marchant EG, Mistlberger RE. Evidence for time-of-day dependent effect of neurotoxic dorsomedial hypothalamic lesions on food anticipatory circadian rhythms in rats. Plos One. 2011;6:e24187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Landry GJ, Simon MM, Webb IC, Mistlberger RE. Persistence of a behavioral food-anticipatory circadian rhythm following dorsomedial hypothalamic ablation in rats. American Journal of Physiology Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2006;290:R1527–R1534. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00874.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Landry GJ, Yamakawa GR, Webb IC, Mear RJ, Mistlberger RE. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is not necessary for the expression of circadian food-anticipatory activity in rats. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 2007;22:467–478. doi: 10.1177/0748730407307804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lee C, Etchegaray JP, Cagampang FR, Loudon AS, Reppert SM. Posttranslational mechanisms regulate the mammalian circadian clock. Cell. 2001;107:855–867. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00610-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lin CR, Kapiloff MS, Durgerian S, Tatemoto K, Russo AF, Hanson P, Schulman H, Rosenfeld MG. Molecular cloning of a brain-specific calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 1987;84:5962–5966. doi: 10.1073/pnas.84.16.5962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lowrey PL, Takahashi JS. Mammalian circadian biology: elucidating genome-wide levels of temporal organization. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics. 2004;5:407–441. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genom.5.061903.175925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lowrey PL, Takahashi JS. Genetics of circadian rhythms in Mammalian model organisms. Advances in Genetics. 2011;74:175–230. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-387690-4.00006-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Madisen L, Zwingman TA, Sunkin SM, Oh SW, Zariwala HA, Gu H, Ng LL, Palmiter RD, Hawrylycz MJ, Jones AR, Lein ES, Zeng H. A robust and high-throughput Cre reporting and characterization system for the whole mouse brain. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:133–140. doi: 10.1038/nn.2467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Marcheva B, Ramsey KM, Buhr ED, Kobayashi Y, Su H, Ko CH, Ivanova G, Omura C, Mo S, Vitaterna MH, Lopez JP, Philipson LH, Bradfield CA, Crosby SD, JeBailey L, Wang X, Takahashi JS, Bass J. Disruption of the clock components CLOCK and BMAL1 leads to hypoinsulinaemia and diabetes. Nature. 2010;466:627–631. doi: 10.1038/nature09253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McDearmon EL, Patel KN, Ko CH, Walisser JA, Schook AC, Chong JL, Wilsbacher LD, Song EJ, Hong HK, Bradfield CA, Takahashi JS. Dissecting the functions of the mammalian clock protein BMAL1 by tissue-specific rescue in mice. Science. 2006;314:1304–1308. doi: 10.1126/science.1132430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Meijer J, Rietveld W. Neurophysiology of the suprachiasmatic circadian pacemaker in rodents. Physiological Reviews. 1989;69:671–707. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1989.69.3.671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mieda M, Sakurai T. Bmal1 in the nervous system is essential for normal adaptation of circadian locomotor activity and food intake to periodic feeding. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:15391–15396. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2801-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mieda M, Williams SC, Richardson JA, Tanaka K, Yanagisawa M. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus as a putative food-entrainable circadian pacemaker. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2006;103:12150–12155. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604189103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mistlberger RE, Buijs RM, Challet E, Escobar C, Landry GJ, Kalsbeek A, Pevet P, Shibata S. Food anticipation in Bmal1-/- and AAV-Bmal1 rescued mice: a reply to Fuller, et al. Journal of Circadian Rhythms. 2009a;7:11. doi: 10.1186/1740-3391-7-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mistlberger RE, Buijs RM, Challet E, Escobar C, Landry GJ, Kalsbeek A, Pevet P, Shibata S. Standards of evidence in chronobiology: critical review of a report that restoration of Bmal1 expression in the dorsomedial hypothalamus is sufficient to restore circadian food anticipatory rhythms in Bmal1-/- mice. Journal of Circadian Rhythms. 2009b;7:3. doi: 10.1186/1740-3391-7-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mistlberger RE, Yamazaki S, Pendergast JS, Landry GJ, Takumi T, Nakamura W. Comment on 'Differential rescue of light- and food-entrainable circadian rhythms'. Science. 2008;322:675. doi: 10.1126/science.1161284. author reply 675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mohawk JA, Green CB, Takahashi JS. Central and peripheral circadian clocks in mammals. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2012;35:445–462. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Moriya T, Aida R, Kudo T, Akiyama M, Doi M, Hayasaka N, Nakahata N, Mistlberger R, Okamura H, Shibata S. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is not necessary for food-anticipatory circadian rhythms of behavior, temperature or clock gene expression in mice. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:1447–1460. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06697.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Musiek ES, Lim MM, Yang G, Bauer AQ, Qi L, Lee Y, Roh JH, Ortiz-Gonzalez X, Dearborn JT, Culver JP, Herzog ED, Hogenesch JB, Wozniak DF, Dikranian K, Giasson BI, Weaver DR, Holtzman DM, Fitzgerald GA. Circadian clock proteins regulate neuronal redox homeostasis and neurodegeneration. The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2013;123:5389–5400. doi: 10.1172/JCI70317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Nagoshi E, Saini C, Bauer C, Laroche T, Naef F, Schibler U. Circadian gene expression in individual fibroblasts: cell-autonomous and self-sustained oscillators pass time to daughter cells. Cell. 2004;119:693–705. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2004.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pendergast JS, Nakamura W, Friday RC, Hatanaka F, Takumi T, Yamazaki S. Robust food anticipatory activity in BMAL1-deficient mice. PLOS ONE. 2009;4:e4860. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ralph MR, Foster RG, Davis FC, Menaker M. Transplanted suprachiasmatic nucleus determines circadian period. Science. 1990;247:975–978. doi: 10.1126/science.2305266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Redlin U, Hattar S, Mrosovsky N. The circadian Clock mutant mouse: impaired masking response to light. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 2005;191:51–59. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0570-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rudic RD, McNamara P, Curtis AM, Boston RC, Panda S, Hogenesch JB, Fitzgerald GA. BMAL1 and CLOCK, two essential components of the circadian clock, are involved in glucose homeostasis. PLOS Biology. 2004;2:e377. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Saini C, Liani A, Curie T, Gos P, Kreppel F, Emmenegger Y, Bonacina L, Wolf JP, Poget YA, Franken P, Schibler U. Real-time recording of circadian liver gene expression in freely moving mice reveals the phase-setting behavior of hepatocyte clocks. Genes & Development. 2013;27:1526–1536. doi: 10.1101/gad.221374.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Saini C, Morf J, Stratmann M, Gos P, Schibler U. Simulated body temperature rhythms reveal the phase-shifting behavior and plasticity of mammalian circadian oscillators. Genes & Development. 2012;26:567–580. doi: 10.1101/gad.183251.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sangoram AM, Saez L, Antoch MP, Gekakis N, Staknis D, Whiteley A, Fruechte EM, Vitaterna MH, Shimomura K, King DP, Young MW, Weitz CJ, Takahashi JS. Mammalian circadian autoregulatory loop: a timeless ortholog and mPer1 interact and negatively regulate CLOCK-BMAL1-induced transcription. Neuron. 1998;21:1101–1113. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80627-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sato TR, Gray NW, Mainen ZF, Svoboda K. The functional microarchitecture of the mouse barrel cortex. PLOS Biology. 2007;5:e189. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shimomura K, Low-Zeddies SS, King DP, Steeves TD, Whiteley A, Kushla J, Zemenides PD, Lin A, Vitaterna MH, Churchill GA, Takahashi JS. Genome-wide epistatic interaction analysis reveals complex genetic determinants of circadian behavior in mice. Genome Research. 2001;11:959–980. doi: 10.1101/gr.171601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Siepka SM, Takahashi JS. Methods to record circadian rhythm wheel running activity in mice. Methods Enzymol. 2005;393:230–239. doi: 10.1016/S0076-6879(05)93008-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stokkan KA, Yamazaki S, Tei H, Sakaki Y, Menaker M. Entrainment of the circadian clock in the liver by feeding. Science. 2001;291:490–493. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5503.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Storch KF, Weitz CJ. Daily rhythms of food-anticipatory behavioral activity do not require the known circadian clock. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2009;106:6808–6813. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902063106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sujino M, Masumoto KH, Yamaguchi S, van der Horst GT, Okamura H, Inouye ST. Suprachiasmatic nucleus grafts restore circadian behavioral rhythms of genetically arrhythmic mice. Current Biology. 2003;13:664–668. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00222-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sun Y, Yang Z, Niu Z, Wang W, Peng J, Li Q, Ma MY, Zhao Y. The mortality of MOP3 deficient mice with a systemic functional failure. Journal of Biomedical Science. 2006;13:845–851. doi: 10.1007/s11373-006-9108-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Tahara Y, Kuroda H, Saito K, Nakajima Y, Kubo Y, Ohnishi N, Seo Y, Otsuka M, Fuse Y, Ohura Y, Komatsu T, Moriya Y, Okada S, Furutani N, Hirao A, Horikawa K, Kudo T, Shibata S. In vivo monitoring of peripheral circadian clocks in the mouse. Current Biology. 2012;22:1029–1034. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Takahashi JS, Menaker M. Entrainment of the circadian system of the house sparrow: a population of oscillators in pinealectomized birds. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology. 1982;146:245–253. doi: 10.1007/BF00610244. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Takasu NN, Kurosawa G, Tokuda IT, Mochizuki A, Todo T, Nakamura W. Circadian regulation of food-anticipatory activity in molecular clock-deficient mice. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e48892. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Taniguchi H, He M, Wu P, Kim S, Paik R, Sugino K, Kvitsiani D, Fu Y, Lu J, Lin Y, Miyoshi G, Shima Y, Fishell G, Nelson SB, Huang ZJ. A resource of cre driver lines for genetic targeting of GABAergic neurons in cerebral cortex. Neuron. 2011;71:995–1013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Travnickova-Bendova Z, Cermakian N, Reppert SM, Sassone-Corsi P. Bimodal regulation of mPeriod promoters by CREB-dependent signaling and CLOCK/BMAL1 activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2002;99:7728–7733. doi: 10.1073/pnas.102075599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Vitaterna MH, Ko CH, Chang AM, Buhr ED, Fruechte EM, Schook A, Antoch MP, Turek FW, Takahashi JS. The mouse Clock mutation reduces circadian pacemaker amplitude and enhances efficacy of resetting stimuli and phase-response curve amplitude. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2006;103:9327–9332. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0603601103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vitaterna MH, Selby CP, Todo T, Niwa H, Thompson C, Fruechte EM, Hitomi K, Thresher RJ, Ishikawa T, Miyazaki J, Takahashi JS, Sancar A. Differential regulation of mammalian period genes and circadian rhythmicity by cryptochromes 1 and 2. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 1999;96:12114–12119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.21.12114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Welsh DK, Takahashi JS, Kay SA. Suprachiasmatic nucleus: cell autonomy and network properties. Annual Review of Physiology. 2010;72:551–577. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Welsh DK, Yoo SH, Liu AC, Takahashi JS, Kay SA. Bioluminescence imaging of individual fibroblasts reveals persistent, independently phased circadian rhythms of clock gene expression. Current Biology. 2004;14:2289–2295. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yamaguchi Y, Suzuki T, Mizoro Y, Kori H, Okada K, Chen Y, Fustin JM, Yamazaki F, Mizuguchi N, Zhang J, Dong X, Tsujimoto G, Okuno Y, Doi M, Okamura H. Mice genetically deficient in vasopressin V1a and V1b receptors are resistant to jet lag. Science. 2013;342:85–90. doi: 10.1126/science.1238599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yamazaki S, Numano R, Abe M, Hida A, Takahashi R, Ueda M, Block GD, Sakaki Y, Menaker M, Tei H. Resetting central and peripheral circadian oscillators in transgenic rats. Science. 2000;288:682–685. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5466.682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Yamazaki S, Takahashi JS. Real-time luminescence reporting of circadian gene expression in mammals. Methods in Enzymology. 2005;393:288–301. doi: 10.1016/S0076-6879(05)93012-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Yoo SH, Yamazaki S, Lowrey PL, Shimomura K, Ko CH, Buhr ED, Siepka SM, Hong HK, Oh WJ, Yoo OJ, Menaker M, Takahashi JS. PERIOD2::LUCIFERASE real-time reporting of circadian dynamics reveals persistent circadian oscillations in mouse peripheral tissues. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2004;101:5339–5346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308709101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2014 Dec 19;3:e04617. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04617.020

Decision letter

Editor: Louis Ptáček1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

Thank you for sending your work entitled “Light But Not Feeding Rescues Temporal Organization of Peripheral Circadian Clocks in a Brain Bmal1 Conditional Mutant” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by a Senior editor, a Reviewing editor, and 3 reviewers.

The following individuals responsible for the peer review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Louis Ptacek (Reviewing editor); Ralph Mistlberger (peer reviewer). Two other reviewers remain anonymous.

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewers discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

The primary hypothesis being addressed in this paper is that temporal organization of peripheral clocks may be maintained in vivo by light and/or feeding cues, when the master clock in the suprachiasmatic nuclei is genetically altered. For that purpose, the authors generated forebrain Bmal1 conditional mutant mice (noted KO thereafter). In constant darkness, these mice are behaviorally arrhythmic, while their peripheral clocks were still oscillating, albeit in a desynchronized way as compared to control mice.

1) The authors consider that light, but not feeding, rescues temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice. The data provided suggest alternative interpretations. Because the temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice exposed to a light-dark cycle (LD) matches remarkably that in SCN-functional control mice (Figure 5), this indicates that the direct effects of light cues given by LD are sufficient to maintain temporal organization in peripheral clocks (i.e., light cues do not rescue any defect in KO mice). Based on the immediate behavioral arrhythmicity after transfer to DD, while the rhythmicity is apparent under regular LD, there is no obvious reason to speak of weak entrainment to light in KO mice. The apparent rhythmicity under LD is likely due to direct effects of daily exposure to 12 hours of light per cycle. A recent study using Syt10(Cre) driver to alter the clock in the SCN has shown that timing in a peripheral clock (i.e., adrenal gland) can be maintained by exposure to LD (Husse et al. 2014 FASEB J). If really the authors consider that light rescues temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice without involving masking effects of light, they should prove it by investigating such temporal organization in KO mice exposed to lighting conditions that maintain entrainment of the suprachiasmatic nuclei, while limiting the masking effects of light, such as a skeletal photoperiod (as used here for behavioral measurements shown in Figure 3) or daily 1 h light pulses followed by 23 h of darkness.

2) Feeding cues in constant darkness are said not to rescue temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice. However, visual inspection of Figure 8 suggests that phase dispersion between peripheral clocks is not different, if not improved, in KO mice (Figure 8, panel F) as compared to control fx/fx mice (Figure 8, panel D). The authors' conclusion is based on data converted in circadian times for control mice, and compared to data expressed according to Zeitgeber (feeding) time in KO mice. Even with data conversion, phase dispersion in KO mice is not different for the pituitary, liver and kidney. The question here is to investigate the synchronizing effects of feeding in DD and because it is not possible to convert data of KO mice in circadian times, data from both fx/fx and KO mice should be compared according to feeding time. The results will probably lead to the conclusion that feeding cues have synchronizing effects in KO mice, at least not worst, and maybe better than in control mice with functional SCN. Together, these points suggest that both light and feeding maintain temporal organization of peripheral circadian clocks in forebrain Bmal1 conditional mutant mice.

Some conclusions were felt to be too strong and restatement of these is necessary, as follows.

3) A major result and substantive conclusion of this study, as stated in the abstract, is that “Light cycles, but not restricted feeding treatment, rescued this defect [loss of synchrony of peripheral clocks, between and within tissues, in brain bmal1 deficient mice, BKOs], revealing the dominance of photic entrainment pathways acting via the SCN over nutrient signals”. This conclusion is summarized in the Discussion section, by statements that light appears to be a stronger entraining signal than feeding cues for most peripheral clocks, and that the SCN plays a 'critical' role in actively sustaining internal synchronization. I would argue that this representation of the results is not quite accurate, and that the conclusion is overstated, for the following reasons:

a) The midday restricted feeding schedule did fully rescue the clock in the liver (both phase and amplitude), and rescued phase in the kidney. Figure 8G also shows that phase in the heart, lung, pituitary and spleen are at least partly rescued, compared to the adlib DD condition. The statement of the results in the Abstract should be revised to acknowledge that loss of synchrony was rescued by restricted feeding in the liver and kidney, but only partially in the other tissues. Synchrony among peripheral tissues requires central bmal1 dependent clocks, while synchrony of individual tissues to a feeding schedule may not require central clocks.

b) The statement that light appears to be stronger than feeding for 'most' peripheral clocks might give the impression that most peripheral tissues were examined. If the stomach, duodenum, colon and pancreas had been included, perhaps 'most' of the clocks would have been found to be rescued, i.e., the conclusion reversed. Selection of the tissues is no doubt important, and worth emphasizing.

c) The statement that light appears to be 'stronger' than feeding in phase control of peripheral clocks is not supported by the data, because the study wasn't designed to compare relative strengths. Relative strength has already been established by studies of intact animals; when food is restricted to the daytime, peripheral clocks uncouple from LD and SCN and shift to align with mealtime. There may be conditions under which light is more effective than feeding, for some tissues (possibly the minority, pending further study), but references to relative strength of food and light should be avoided, or stated in a more nuanced fashion.

4) The statement that the SCN plays a 'critical' role in actively sustaining internal synchronization is also debatable, because Bmal1 was absent from most of the rest of the brain, and some of the loss of amplitude or phase control in peripheral tissues, particularly in the restricted feeding condition, could be due to loss of inputs from Bmal1-dependent FEOs in the brain outside of the SCN. Presumably, hypothetical bmal1-dependent FEOs (in PVN, DMH, arcuate, brainstem?) communicating with peripheral organs would have to be distinct from FEOs responsible for food anticipatory behavioral rhythms, given that the behavioral rhythms were quite robust in the BKOs.

5) The conclusion that cyclic photic input is sufficient to sustain coherent temporal synchrony of peripheral oscillators is supported by the data, but the mechanism by which light does this remains an open question. The LD cycle in BKO mice drives a daily rhythm of activity and presumably feeding, and the effects of light may be mediated by stimuli associated with these behaviors. Indeed, it would not be surprising if the heart and lung circadian clocks are regulated by various systemic effects of wheel running (exercise); the phase of those clocks may be regulated by the timing of behavior. FAA in DD was much longer in duration in the BKO mice, and that might explain the lower degree of synchrony of heart and lung clocks during restricted feeding in those mice. I would recommend briefly noting this as a potential mechanism.

eLife. 2014 Dec 19;3:e04617. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04617.021

Author response


1) The authors consider that light, but not feeding, rescues temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice. The data provided suggest alternative interpretations. Because the temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice exposed to a light-dark cycle (LD) matches remarkably that in SCN-functional control mice (Figure 5), this indicates that the direct effects of light cues given by LD are sufficient to maintain temporal organization in peripheral clocks (i.e., light cues do not rescue any defect in KO mice). Based on the immediate behavioral arrhythmicity after transfer to DD, while the rhythmicity is apparent under regular LD, there is no obvious reason to speak of weak entrainment to light in KO mice. The apparent rhythmicity under LD is likely due to direct effects of daily exposure to 12 hours of light per cycle. A recent study using Syt10(Cre) driver to alter the clock in the SCN has shown that timing in a peripheral clock (i.e., adrenal gland) can be maintained by exposure to LD (Husse et al. 2014 FASEB J). If really the authors consider that light rescues temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice without involving masking effects of light, they should prove it by investigating such temporal organization in KO mice exposed to lighting conditions that maintain entrainment of the suprachiasmatic nuclei, while limiting the masking effects of light, such as a skeletal photoperiod (as used here for behavioral measurements shown in Figure 3) or daily 1 h light pulses followed by 23 h of darkness.

We agree with the reviewers’ comment that LD cycles are likely the primary stimulus maintaining temporal organization of peripheral clocks in BKO mice. It is not our intention to say that masking effects of light are not involved. To avoid confusion and also to incorporate the comments below, we have modified the title to, “Differential effects of light and feeding on circadian organization of peripheral clocks in a forebrain Bmal1 mutant” and emphasized “light/dark cycles” in the revised manuscript.

2) Feeding cues in constant darkness are said not to rescue temporal organization of peripheral clocks in KO mice. However, visual inspection of Figure 8 suggests that phase dispersion between peripheral clocks is not different, if not improved, in KO mice (Figure 8, panel F) as compared to control fx/fx mice (Figure 8, panel D). The authors' conclusion is based on data converted in circadian times for control mice, and compared to data expressed according to Zeitgeber (feeding) time in KO mice. Even with data conversion, phase dispersion in KO mice is not different for the pituitary, liver and kidney. The question here is to investigate the synchronizing effects of feeding in DD and because it is not possible to convert data of KO mice in circadian times, data from both fx/fx and KO mice should be compared according to feeding time. The results will probably lead to the conclusion that feeding cues have synchronizing effects in KO mice, at least not worst, and maybe better than in control mice with functional SCN. Together, these points suggest that both light and feeding maintain temporal organization of peripheral circadian clocks in forebrain Bmal1 conditional mutant mice.

We agree with the reviewers and thank them for their careful consideration of our description. In an attempt to oversimplify our description of the results, we overgeneralized our conclusions. We have now described the results in more detail to emphasize the differential effects of FR on peripheral rhythms. We have performed additional analysis of the phase data of Fx/Fx and BKO tissues by comparing the peak phases plotted against feeding time (Figure 8–source data 1a, Fx/Fx ZT vs BKO). The statistical analysis indeed showed a significance shift in the mean phase angle in kidney, heart, lung, and spleen (but not in pituitary and liver) in BKOs. Comparison of the circular variance also showed that the phases of BKO’s were more clustered in liver, kidney, lung, and spleen (but not in pituitary and heart) as compared to Fx/Fx, suggesting that feeding cues have synchronizing effects on peripheral clocks in the absence of functional SCN.

This analysis as well as additional analyses that we present in a new Figure 9 (Figure 8–source data 1a, Figure 9, and Figure 9–source data 1a,b as described below), together suggest that both light (in the form of LD cycles) and feeding have an effect on the temporal organization of peripheral oscillators in the forebrain Bmal1 conditional knockout mice. However, we also noted that their effects are not equal. While the synchronizing effect of light is similar to the effect from the SCN (as manifested in the DD condition), the restricted feeding shifts the phases of peripheral rhythms differently and clusters the phase variance only in liver and kidney in BKO’s. To aid in the visualization of all results in one page, we performed a “meta-analysis” of all phase and amplitude data taken under different conditions and summarized the analysis in Figure 9 and Figure 9–source data 1a,b. This analysis illustrates the differential effects of light and feeding more clearly.

Some conclusions were felt to be too strong and restatement of these is necessary, as follows.

3) A major result and substantive conclusion of this study, as stated in the abstract, is that “Light cycles, but not restricted feeding treatment, rescued this defect [loss of synchrony of peripheral clocks, between and within tissues, in brain bmal1 deficient mice, BKOs], revealing the dominance of photic entrainment pathways acting via the SCN over nutrient signals”. This conclusion is summarized in the Discussion section, by statements that light appears to be a stronger entraining signal than feeding cues for most peripheral clocks, and that the SCN plays a 'critical' role in actively sustaining internal synchronization. I would argue that this representation of the results is not quite accurate, and that the conclusion is overstated, for the following reasons:

a) The midday restricted feeding schedule did fully rescue the clock in the liver (both phase and amplitude), and rescued phase in the kidney. Figure 8G also shows that phase in the heart, lung, pituitary and spleen are at least partly rescued, compared to the adlib DD condition. The statement of the results in the Abstract should be revised to acknowledge that loss of synchrony was rescued by restricted feeding in the liver and kidney, but only partially in the other tissues. Synchrony among peripheral tissues requires central bmal1 dependent clocks, while synchrony of individual tissues to a feeding schedule may not require central clocks.

We have revised the title, Abstract, and other text in the manuscript by specifying which tissues were synchronized by restricted feeding. Please note, however, that the statistical analysis for Figure 9B (which includes Figure 8G) did not reveal a difference in the phase dispersion in the heart, lung, spleen, and pituitary between the adlib DD and FR condition (Figure 9–source data 1b, which was previously numbered as Figure 8–source data 1b): i.e., the phases of these tissues remained dispersed.

Also, we performed analysis on the circular variance among different tissues within individual mice that underwent restricted feeding (current Figure 8–source data 1b). BKO’s still show a significant phase variation between organs (new Figure 8H), supporting the hypothesis that “synchrony among peripheral tissues requires central Bmal1 dependent clocks, while synchrony of individual tissues to a feeding schedule may not require central clocks”. Based on this additional analysis, we have modified our Results and Discussion to state this point.

b) The statement that light appears to be stronger than feeding for 'most' peripheral clocks might give the impression that most peripheral tissues were examined. If the stomach, duodenum, colon and pancreas had been included, perhaps 'most' of the clocks would have been found to be rescued, i.e., the conclusion reversed. Selection of the tissues is no doubt important, and worth emphasizing.

We agree and have revised relevant sentences by emphasizing the selection of the tissues used in the study instead of making a sweeping statement.

c) The statement that light appears to be 'stronger' than feeding in phase control of peripheral clocks is not supported by the data, because the study wasn't designed to compare relative strengths. Relative strength has already been established by studies of intact animals; when food is restricted to the daytime, peripheral clocks uncouple from LD and SCN and shift to align with mealtime. There may be conditions under which light is more effective than feeding, for some tissues (possibly the minority, pending further study), but references to relative strength of food and light should be avoided, or stated in a more nuanced fashion.

Based also on the comments and our responses in item #2, we altered the phrases from “relative strengths” to “differential effects” of light and feeding where applicable in the revised manuscript.

4) The statement that the SCN plays a 'critical' role in actively sustaining internal synchronization is also debatable, because Bmal1 was absent from most of the rest of the brain, and some of the loss of amplitude or phase control in peripheral tissues, particularly in the restricted feeding condition, could be due to loss of inputs from Bmal1-dependent FEOs in the brain outside of the SCN. Presumably, hypothetical bmal1-dependent FEOs (in PVN, DMH, arcuate, brainstem?) communicating with peripheral organs would have to be distinct from FEOs responsible for food anticipatory behavioral rhythms, given that the behavioral rhythms were quite robust in the BKOs.

We have deleted this statement in the Discussion. We agree that it is not possible to speculate about the roles of other brain regions at this time, given that our conditional Bmal1 knockout is not specific to the SCN. Therefore, we state that “a Cre driver that can express more specifically in the SCN would be necessary to confirm the dominance of the SCN over other sites of the brain.”

5) The conclusion that cyclic photic input is sufficient to sustain coherent temporal synchrony of peripheral oscillators is supported by the data, but the mechanism by which light does this remains an open question. The LD cycle in BKO mice drives a daily rhythm of activity and presumably feeding, and the effects of light may be mediated by stimuli associated with these behaviors. Indeed, it would not be surprising if the heart and lung circadian clocks are regulated by various systemic effects of wheel running (exercise); the phase of those clocks may be regulated by the timing of behavior. FAA in DD was much longer in duration in the BKO mice, and that might explain the lower degree of synchrony of heart and lung clocks during restricted feeding in those mice. I would recommend briefly noting this as a potential mechanism.

We have incorporated this point as a potential mechanism in Discussion in the revised manuscript.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 5—source data 1. (A) Summary of statistical analysis of circular plots presented in Figure 5. (B) Statistical analysis of circular variance for peak bioluminescence in individual mice in LD. (C) Statistical analysis of circular variance for peak bioluminescence in individual mice in DD.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.011

    elife04617s001.docx (45.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.04617.011
    Figure 8—source data 1. (A) Summary of statistical analysis of circular plots presented in Figure 8. (B) Statistical analysis of circular variance for peak bioluminescence in individual mice under restriction feeding.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.017

    elife04617s002.docx (38.6KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.04617.017
    Figure 9—source data 1. (A) Summary of statistical comparison of peak phases from Fx/Fx mice under different conditions. (B) Summary of statistical comparison of peak phases from BKO mice under different conditions.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04617.019

    elife04617s003.docx (35.4KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.04617.019

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES