Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2014 Dec 29;112(2):578–583. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421930112

Distinct dopamine neurons mediate reward signals for short- and long-term memories

Nobuhiro Yamagata a,b,1, Toshiharu Ichinose a,b, Yoshinori Aso c, Pierre-Yves Plaçais d, Anja B Friedrich b, Richard J Sima b, Thomas Preat d, Gerald M Rubin c,1, Hiromu Tanimoto a,b,1
PMCID: PMC4299218  PMID: 25548178

Significance

A biologically relevant event such as finding food under starvation conditions or being poisoned can drive long-term memory (LTM) in a single training session. Neuronal mechanisms by which such a strong reward or punishment induces stable memory are poorly understood. Here we show that distinct subsets of dopamine neurons signal reward for short- and long-term appetitive memories in Drosophila. The temporal dynamics of memory components triggered by the distinct reward signals are complementary, and together contribute to a temporally stable memory retention. Two subsets of dopamine neurons could signal different reward properties: sweet taste and nutritional value of sugar. Sugar reward is thus intricately encoded in the fly brain, given the importance of long-lasting food-related memory in survival.

Keywords: dopamine, learning and memory, Drosophila, mushroom body

Abstract

Drosophila melanogaster can acquire a stable appetitive olfactory memory when the presentation of a sugar reward and an odor are paired. However, the neuronal mechanisms by which a single training induces long-term memory are poorly understood. Here we show that two distinct subsets of dopamine neurons in the fly brain signal reward for short-term (STM) and long-term memories (LTM). One subset induces memory that decays within several hours, whereas the other induces memory that gradually develops after training. They convey reward signals to spatially segregated synaptic domains of the mushroom body (MB), a potential site for convergence. Furthermore, we identified a single type of dopamine neuron that conveys the reward signal to restricted subdomains of the mushroom body lobes and induces long-term memory. Constant appetitive memory retention after a single training session thus comprises two memory components triggered by distinct dopamine neurons.


Memory of a momentous event persists for a long time. Whereas some forms of long-term memory (LTM) require repetitive training (13), a highly relevant stimulus such as food or poison is sufficient to induce LTM in a single training session (47). Recent studies have revealed aspects of the molecular and cellular mechanisms of LTM formation induced by repetitive training (811), but how a single training induces a stable LTM is poorly understood (12).

Appetitive olfactory learning in fruit flies is suited to address the question, as a presentation of a sugar reward paired with odor induces robust short-term memory (STM) and LTM (6, 7). Odor is represented by a sparse ensemble of the 2,000 intrinsic neurons, the Kenyon cells (13). A current working model suggests that concomitant reward signals from sugar ingestion cause associative plasticity in Kenyon cells that might underlie memory formation (1420). A single activation session of a specific cluster of dopamine neurons (PAM neurons) by sugar ingestion can induce appetitive memory that is stable over 24 h (19), underscoring the importance of sugar reward to the fly.

The mushroom body (MB) is composed of the three different cell types, α/β, α′/β′, and γ, which have distinct roles in different phases of appetitive memories (11, 2125). Similar to midbrain dopamine neurons in mammals (26, 27), the structure and function of PAM cluster neurons are heterogeneous, and distinct dopamine neurons intersect unique segments of the MB lobes (19, 2834). Further circuit dissection is thus crucial to identify candidate synapses that undergo associative modulation.

By activating distinct subsets of PAM neurons for reward signaling, we found that short- and long-term memories are independently formed by two complementary subsets of PAM cluster dopamine neurons. Conditioning flies with nutritious and nonnutritious sugars revealed that the two subsets could represent different reinforcing properties: sweet taste and nutritional value of sugar. Constant appetitive memory retention after a single training session thus comprises two memory components triggered by distinct reward signals.

Results

Distinct Subsets of Dopamine Neurons Drive Appetitive STM and LTM.

We activated different sets of PAM cluster neurons by thermogenetic stimulation with a temperature-sensitive cation channel dTrpA1 (35) in the presence of an odor and evaluated reward signals behaviorally by measuring appetitive odor memory immediately (STM) or 24 h after training (LTM). As we previously showed, R58E02-GAL4 labels the majority of the reward-signaling PAM neurons (Fig. 1A) (19). Thermoactivation paired with an odor presentation in R58E02-GAL4/UAS-dTrpA1 flies after starvation resulted in the formation of robust STM and LTM (Fig. 1 B and C). Our anatomical screen identified two additional drivers, R48B04-GAL4 (Fig. 1D, Fig. S1 C and G, and Movie S1) and R15A04-GAL4 (Fig. 1G, Fig. S1 D and H, and Movie S2), which target transgene expression in distinct sets of PAM cluster neurons with little overlapping expression. Those neurons were mostly tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) immunoreactive (Fig. S2). Activation of a PAM subset with R48B04-GAL4 induced STM but not LTM (Fig. 1 E and F). Strikingly, thermoactivation of the other subset of PAM cluster neurons with R15A04-GAL4 induced robust LTM without forming STM (Fig. 1 H and I), similar to appetitive memory without a short-term component of mutants for TβH (36). The temporal dynamics of these two forms of memory were complementary and sum roughly to the memory induced by activation of the PAM cluster neurons with R58E02-GAL4 (Fig. 1J).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Two dopamine subsets induce appetitive STM and LTM independently. (A) Expression pattern of R58E02-GAL4 shown in a standardized brain. (B and C) Thermoactivation with 58E02-GAL4 tested immediately (B) or 24 h later (C). LI, learning index; STM, short-term memory; LTM, long-term memory; n = 10–12. (D) Expression pattern of R48B04-GAL4 shown in a standardized brain. (E and F) Thermoactivation with 48B04-GAL4 tested immediately (E) or 24 h later (F). n = 12. (G) Expression pattern of R15A04-GAL4 shown in a standardized brain. (H and I) Thermoactivation with 15A04-GAL4 tested immediately (H) or 24 h later (I). n = 12. (J) Retention of induced memory. n = 10–19. (K and L) Blockade of the PAM neurons in R58E02-GAL4, stm-PAM-GAL4 (R48B04-GAL4), and ltm-PAM-GAL4 (R15A04-GAL4) with UAS-shits1 during memory acquisition with sucrose tested immediately (K) or 24 h later (L). n = 11–36. c, conditioning; t, training. Results are means ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001; ns, not significant.

We controlled GAL4 expression outside the PAM neurons in R48B04-GAL4 and R15A04-GAL4 using R58E02-GAL80, which suppresses transgene expression in the PAM cluster neurons (Fig. S1) (19). The addition of R58E02-GAL80 abrogated memory induction in both cases (Fig. S3 A and B) and thus localized the reward signal to the respective PAM neurons. These results suggest that appetitive reinforcement can be functionally broken down into two complementary dopamine signals within the PAM cluster. We here refer to these two neuronal subsets as stm-PAM and ltm-PAM, respectively.

Stm- and ltm-PAM Neurons Are Differentially Required for Sugar-Induced STM and LTM.

To examine whether STM and LTM of the sugar reward respectively require the stm- and ltm-PAM neurons, we targeted the expression of shibirets1 (shits1) (37) to the respective subsets and blocked their outputs during associative training. Inactivation of a majority of the PAM neurons with R58E02-GAL4 impaired both STM and LTM, whereas inactivation with R48B04-GAL4 and R15A04-GAL4 caused selective defects in STM and LTM, respectively (Fig. 1 K and L). The impaired memory caused by inactivating R48B04-GAL4 and R15A04-GAL4 was fully restored by blocking GAL4 activity in the respective PAM neurons using R58E02-GAL80 (Fig. S4). These data demonstrate that a single presentation of sugar triggers parallel reward signals of stm-PAM and ltm-PAM and suggest that constant appetitive memory of sugar is a composite of two separable elements.

Input and Output Sites of stm- and ltm-PAM Neurons Are Spatially Segregated.

The PAM cluster neurons have presynaptic terminals in the MB (18, 19). The R48B04-GAL4 and R15A04-GAL4 target largely distinct dopamine neurons with the exception of overlapping expression in the neurons projecting to the γ5 subdomain (Fig. 2 A and B). To compare the detailed projections of the stm- and ltm-PAM neurons, we applied a nonrigid transformation to individual confocal stacks and computationally registered them onto a standardized average brain. The target regions of the stm-PAM are enriched in the medial region of the MB, whereas the dendrites of these two dopamine subsets in the protocerebrum are also largely segregated (Fig. 2 EG). Interestingly, computational subtraction of one expression pattern from the other revealed that dendritic processes unique to stm- and ltm-PAM neurons are differentially clustered to the medial and lateral protocerebrum, respectively (Fig. 2 H and I). These results suggest that the stm- and ltm-PAM neurons receive distinct synaptic inputs and differentially modulate the local circuits of the MB.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Complementary expression patterns of stm- and ltm-PAM neurons. (A) Schematic drawings of expression patterns of R58E02-GAL4, stm-PAM, and ltm-PAM neurons (Fig. S5) in the MB. (B) The MB subdomains innervated by the three lines. Dark blue in R58E02 and dark magenta in stm-PAM indicates weak GFP expression. (C and D) Expression patterns of R48B04-GAL4 (magenta) and R15A04-GAL4 (green) neurons visualized in different MB subdomains of a standardized brain. Outline, MB. (E and F) Stm-PAM (magenta) and ltm-PAM (green) neurons visualized in a standardized brain at different protocerebrum regions. The R48B04-GAL4 and R15A04-GAL4 expression patterns were refined with R58E02-LexA GRASP (Fig. S5). In the course of this processing, expression pattern of some cell types got weaker (e.g., β′1 neurons in the ltm-PAM). (G) Schematic drawing of innervation areas by the two PAM groups in the protocerebrum. (H and I) Image subtraction of ltm-PAM from stm-PAM (H) and stm-PAM from ltm-PAM (I). To improve the signal-to-noise ratio of the signals, averaged expression patterns (n = 14 for stm-PAM, 9 for ltm-PAM) are used. (Scale bars, 20 μm.) Aimpr, anterior inferiormedial protocerebrum; asmpr, anterior superiormedial protocerebrum; mslpr, middle superiorlateral protocerebrum; and msmpr, middle superiormedial protocerebrum (27).

Stm- and ltm-PAM Neurons Are Differentially Required for Appetitive Memories of Sweetness and Nutritious Sugars.

The nonnutritious sugar arabinose reportedly induces only STM but not LTM when used alone, but induces LTM when combined with sorbitol, a tasteless nutritious sugar (38, 39). This prompted us to examine whether the stm- and ltm-PAM neurons mediate different properties of the sugar reward, such as sweetness and nutrition. We separately blocked the stm- and ltm-PAM neurons while training flies with arabinose with or without additional sorbitol, and measured the flies’ STM and LTM. Blocking the stm- but not ltm-PAM neurons impaired arabinose STM, whereas LTM of the combined sugars selectively required ltm-PAM neurons (Fig. 3 A and B). Consistent with this result, the genomic DNA fragment in R48B04-GAL4 is derived from the Drosophila octopamine receptor gene (40), which mediates the sweet taste and water reward but not nutrition (18, 41).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Differential processing of reinforcing properties of sugar sweetness and nutrition. (A) Blocking stm-PAM but not ltm-PAM neurons during conditioning with arabinose significantly impairs appetitive STM. n = 6–18. (B) Blocking ltm-PAM but not stm-PAM neurons during conditioning with arabinose supplemented with sorbitol significantly impairs appetitive LTM. n = 11–20. c, conditioning; t, training. (C and D) Blockade of Gr43a; Cha7.4kbp-GAL80 neurons during memory acquisition with sucrose tested immediately (C) or 24 h later (D). n = 11–20. (E and F) Overlapped expression patterns of Gr43a; Cha7.4kbp-GAL80 neurons (yellow) with ltm-PAM neurons (green, E) but not with stm-PAM neurons (magenta, F) in the standardized brain. (Scale bars, 20 μm.) Results are means ± SEM. *P < 0.05; ns, not significant.

Sugar ingestion leads to fast elevation of hemolymph fructose level, which may represent nutritional value. Because fructose is sensed by its receptor Gr43a in the brain (42), we blocked Gr43a-expressing neurons (Fig. S6A) during sucrose training and examined STM and LTM. Indeed, this blockade specifically impaired LTM but not STM formation (Fig. 3 C and D and Fig. S6B). Intriguingly, the projections of the Gr43a neurons are confined to the lateral protocerebrum, in close proximity to the dendrites of ltm-PAM (Fig. 3E, Fig. S6 C and D, and Movie S3) but not stm-PAM neurons (Fig. 3F). We imaged calcium responses of these two subsets to sucrose, arabinose, and the mixture of arabinose and sorbitol. Interestingly, the representation of arabinose in the PAM neurons was similar to that of sucrose (Fig. S7 AD), although the intensity was generally lower and off responses were more visible for sucrose. There was no obvious difference in response profiles of two subsets of PAM neurons. As dopaminergic modulation of cAMP drives nonlinear plasticity across the MB lobes (43), it is perhaps not surprising that sugar responses are widely distributed beyond the PAM subsets.

Prediction of Individual Dopamine Neurons for LTM Formation by Combined Behavioral and Anatomical Screens.

We next sought to identify individual reward neurons by examining the correlation between induced memory performances and GAL4 expression in all cell types of PAM cluster neurons. To this end, we used a collection of 20 Split-GAL4 driver lines with specific expression in different PAM cluster neurons; the construction and detailed anatomical analysis of these lines are described in Aso et al. (33, 34). We performed a behavioral screen of thermoactivation in starved flies with this driver collection and measured the formation of STM and LTM. The performances of STM and LTM among these 20 drivers did not show any significant correlation (Fig. 4A), providing further evidence for independent reward neurons for STM and LTM. For each cell type, we performed a linear regression analysis between induced memory performances and GAL4 expression levels, estimated by GFP fluorescence in corresponding MB subdomains, of the 20 drivers (Fig. S8). Although some GAL4 lines induced robust STM, no cell type was significantly correlated with STM performances, implying collective reward signals for STM (Fig. 4 B, D, and E and Fig. S8A). This also implies undefined anatomical connectivity of some PAM neurons and the input neurons, as in the connectivity between the projection neurons and the Kenyon cells (44, 45). In contrast, the regression analysis with LTM performances identified significant positive association with the dopamine neurons projecting to the base of the α-lobe (PAM-α1) (Fig. 4 C, D, and F and Fig. S8B). PAM-α1 was consistently labeled in the top 5 drivers that induced significant LTM (Fig. S8B). The PAM neurons terminating in the tip and shaft of the β-lobe (Fig. 4 D and F) also appeared to be correlated with LTM, suggesting the contributions of other cell types to the reward signal for LTM (18). Thus, PAM-α1 represents an essential component for the reward signal for LTM, whereas different and partially redundant dopamine neurons might collectively signal reward for STM.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

PAM-α1 neurons are necessary and sufficient for appetitive LTM formation. (A) Correlation of STM and LTM scores. For each SplitGAL4 line, LTM score induced by thermoactivation is plotted against STM score. Linear regression is overlaid. Significance of the correlation was tested by bootstrap, revealing no association between STM and LTM (n = 20). (B and C) GAL4 expression levels in each PAM cell type plotted against learning indices of appetitive STM (B) or LTM (C) of all 20 SplitGAL4 lines. Different colors and markers indicate different cell types. Linear regression is shown together for PAM-α1, which marked significant positive association with LTM (C). (D) Correlation coefficient of GAL4 expression level versus STM and LTM in each cell type. (E and F) Color map of logarithm of P values calculated from correlation analyses of GAL4 expression and learning indices of STM (E) or LTM (F). (G) Expression pattern of MB299B-GAL4 shown in a standardized brain. (Scale bar, 20 μm.) (H and I) Blockade of the PAM-α1 neurons by MB299B-GAL4 during memory acquisition with sucrose tested immediately (H) or 24 h later (I). n = 6–12. c, conditioning; t, training. Results with error bars are means ± SEM. **P < 0.01; ns, not significant. (J and K) Blockade of the PAM-α1 neurons has no significant effects during acquisition of arabinose memory tested immediately (J) but impairs formation of 24-h memory induced by arabinose supplemented with sorbitol (K). (L) Model of a reward circuit in the fly brain. Reinforcement properties of sugar sweetness and nutrition are independently conveyed by octopamine or Gr43a neurons to distinct subclusters of the PAM cluster. The stm-PAM neurons collectively mediate reinforcement signal of STM. In contrast, the reinforcement signal of LTM is mainly conveyed by the PAM-α1 neurons.

PAM-α1 Neurons Are Necessary and Sufficient for LTM Formation.

Using one of the Split-GAL4 drivers labeling PAM-α1 (MB299B; Fig. 4G), we analyzed morphology and reward function. This class of dopamine neurons consists of approximately five cells and has the lateral dendrites that are characteristic of the ltm-PAM neurons (Fig. 4G). The blockade of the PAM-α1 neurons during the formation of appetitive memory revealed a specific impairment of LTM but not STM (Fig. 4 H and I). Furthermore, the blockade did not significantly impair STM of arabinose conditioning (Fig. 4J), whereas these dopamine neurons were absolutely necessary for the acquisition of LTM of arabinose supplemented with sorbitol (Fig. 4K). In addition, thermoactivation of MB299B in starved flies revealed sufficiency of these neurons for LTM (Fig. S9). We thus conclude that a specific class of dopamine neurons that project to the base of the α-lobe is necessary and sufficient to signal reward for LTM, potentially upon the input of nutrition (Fig. 4L).

Discussion

Distinct Reward Signals for STM and LTM.

In Drosophila, sugar ingestion in a single training session induces stable appetitive odor memory. Our results showed that observed memory represents the composite of STM and LTM that are induced by the distinct and complementary reward signals of dopamine. These separate reward signals very well corroborate parallel processing of STM and LTM in the MB (22). The stm-PAM neurons induce appetitive memory that decays within hours, whereas memory by ltm-PAM gradually develops after training. These dopamine neurons convey reward signals to spatially segregated synaptic domains of the MB, whereas the other PAM cluster neurons may also contribute to LTM reward. Furthermore, we identified a single type of dopamine neurons (PAM-α1) encoding a reward signal for LTM. The PAM-α1 targets a spatially restricted subdomain in the α-lobe of the MB, suggesting local associative modulation in the α-lobe. These results indicate that sugar ingestion activates multiple reward signals of different qualities to form complementary memories, rather than a single reward system forming STM that later transforms into LTM.

In the defensive siphon and tail withdrawal reflex of Aplysia, different modes of 5-HT application to the sensory neurons in the pedal and pleural ganglion differentially induce short-term and long-term sensitization memories (46, 47). As both ganglia are innervated by a single identified serotonergic neuron (48), it is likely that the tail shock activates the same cell to induce both forms of sensitization independently. This cellular configuration is in strong contrast to the reward system in Drosophila appetitive memory, despite parallel formation of STM and LTM in both systems. The sugar reward may be more intricately encoded in the fly, given the importance of long-lasting food-related memory in survival.

Representations of different reinforcing stimuli by the same transmitter seems to be variable. In Drosophila aversive memory, reinforcing signals of electric shock and heat punishment converge to the same dopamine neurons (49), whereas distinct dopamine neurons are recruited for different aversive stimuli in mammals (50). Appetitive memory of Drosophila is closer to the latter case. However, a critical difference is that stm- and ltm-PAM neurons seem to signal different properties of the same reward, again pointing to more complex representation of the sugar reward. It would be interesting to compare neuronal representations of different rewarding stimuli, such as ethanol (51) and water (41, 52). An important future question would be to understand physiological mechanisms by which the MB computes the distinct dopamine inputs to control approach behavior.

Sequential Regulation by Taste and Nutrition.

Memory induced by ltm-PAM develops gradually after training (Fig. 1). This gradual increase may reflect the time to implement learning-dependent molecular changes. Many molecules that are specifically required for LTM have been identified, and some of these molecules are involved in learning-dependent gene transcription/translation (3, 5357). For instance, fasting-dependent LTM that is formed without training repetition requires CREB-regulated transcription coactivator (CRTC)-mediated cAMP response element binding protein (CREB) transcription in the MB (12). Such a transcription-dependent mechanism takes time and may thus underlie gradual formation of memory induced by the ltm-PAM.

The complementary memory dynamics as a consequence of the distinct reward signals suggest that the “intent” of appetitive memory undergoes a transition from palatability to caloric content. Similar temporal transitions have been found in feeding choice (58), where flies initially choose sugars according to sweet taste, but later prioritize caloric contents. Similarly, ethanol exposure initially acts as an aversive reinforcer, but eventually turns into reward and induces LTM (51). The sequential regulation of appetitive behavior by the same stimulus may be conserved across relevant appetitive stimuli. As palatability is not always a faithful predictor of its nutritional value, it may be a general design of reward systems to balance short-term benefit and long-term fitness.

Materials and Methods

The two GAL4 drivers, R48B04 and R15A04, were identified by screening our confocal image database for PAM cluster neurons (40). Appetitive conditioning and reward substitution with thermoactivation were performed according to previously described protocols of differential conditioning (19, 29, 30). A group of flies received 1 min of odor exposure with sugar reward or temperature-sensitive cation channel (dTrpA1)-mediated thermoactivation of GAL4-expressing neurons, followed by a 1-min presentation of another odor in the absence of reward or temperature elevation. Most of the data did not violate the assumption of the normal distribution and the homogeneity of variance and were therefore subjected to parametric statistics. For data that significantly deviated from the normal distribution (Figs. 3B and 4I), nonparametric statistics were applied. Fluorescent immunohistochemistry was performed as described previously (19, 29, 30). All of the confocal images in the main figures underwent landmark matching-based image registration using BrainAligner (59). For in vivo calcium imaging, female flies expressing GCaMP3 (60) with R48B04-GAL4 and R15A04-GAL4 were prepared essentially as described (19, 61). A droplet of 2 M sucrose, 3 M arabinose, or a mixture of 3 M arabinose and 2 M sorbitol solution in mineral water was delivered on a plastic plate controlled by a micromanipulator (19). See SI Materials and Methods for more detailed experimental procedures.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary File
pnas.201421930SI.pdf (1.7MB, pdf)
Supplementary File
Download video file (9.6MB, avi)
Supplementary File
Download video file (7.8MB, avi)
Supplementary File
Download video file (2.6MB, avi)

Acknowledgments

We thank T. Templier and A. Abe for technical assistance and experiments that inspired this study; P. Garrity, H. Amrein, B. Pfeiffer, the Kyoto Drosophila Genetic Resource Center, and the Bloomington Stock Center for fly stocks; K. Ito and the members of the H.T. laboratory for discussion and/or critical reading of the manuscript. Y.A. and G.M.R. thank members of the Janelia FlyLight Project Team for experimental assistance and the Janelia scientific computing group for help with brain registration and image analysis. N.Y., T.I., P.-Y.P., and R.J.S. were sponsored by the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation, Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Postdoctoral Fellowships for Research Abroad, the Agence Nationale de la Recherche, and the Deutscher Akademischer Austausch Dienst. This work was supported by the Kurata Memorial Hitachi Science and Technology Foundation (N.Y.), the Agence Nationale pour la Recherche (T.P.), the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (G.M.R.), and Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (01GQ0932 to H.T.), Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (TA 552/5-1 to H.T.), Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology–Japan/JSPS KAKENHI (26840110 to N.Y. and 25890003, 26250001, 26120705, and 26119503 to H.T.), the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences “Bioinformatics for Brain Sciences” (H.T.), the Naito Foundation (H.T.), and the Max Planck Gesellschaft (H.T.).

Footnotes

2Present address: The Solomon H. Snyder Department of Neuroscience, Brain Science Institute, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205.

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1421930112/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Carew TJ, Kandel ER. Acquisition and retention of long-term habituation in Aplysia: Correlation of behavioral and cellular processes. Science. 1973;182(4117):1158–1160. doi: 10.1126/science.182.4117.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Carew TJ, Pinsker HM, Kandel ER. Long-term habituation of a defensive withdrawal reflex in aplysia. Science. 1972;175(4020):451–454. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4020.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tully T, Preat T, Boynton SC, Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell. 1994;79(1):35–47. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Garcia J, Kimeldorf DJ, Koelling RA. Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science. 1955;122(3160):157–158. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Alexander J, Jr, Audesirk TE, Audesirk GJ. One-trial reward learning in the snail Lymnea stagnalis. J Neurobiol. 1984;15(1):67–72. doi: 10.1002/neu.480150107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Krashes MJ, Waddell S. Rapid consolidation to a radish and protein synthesis-dependent long-term memory after single-session appetitive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2008;28(12):3103–3113. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5333-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Colomb J, Kaiser L, Chabaud MA, Preat T. Parametric and genetic analysis of Drosophila appetitive long-term memory and sugar motivation. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8(4):407–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00482.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pagani MR, Oishi K, Gelb BD, Zhong Y. The phosphatase SHP2 regulates the spacing effect for long-term memory induction. Cell. 2009;139(1):186–198. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Plaçais PY, et al. Slow oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(4):592–599. doi: 10.1038/nn.3055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zhang Y, et al. Computational design of enhanced learning protocols. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(2):294–297. doi: 10.1038/nn.2990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Huang C, et al. A permissive role of mushroom body α/β core neurons in long-term memory consolidation in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2012;22(21):1981–1989. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hirano Y, et al. Fasting launches CRTC to facilitate long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Science. 2013;339(6118):443–446. doi: 10.1126/science.1227170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Turner GC, Bazhenov M, Laurent G. Olfactory representations by Drosophila mushroom body neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99(2):734–746. doi: 10.1152/jn.01283.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Heisenberg M. Mushroom body memoir: From maps to models. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4(4):266–275. doi: 10.1038/nrn1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gerber B, Tanimoto H, Heisenberg M. An engram found? Evaluating the evidence from fruit flies. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14(6):737–744. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Tomchik SM, Davis RL. Dynamics of learning-related cAMP signaling and stimulus integration in the Drosophila olfactory pathway. Neuron. 2009;64(4):510–521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gervasi N, Tchénio P, Preat T. PKA dynamics in a Drosophila learning center: Coincidence detection by rutabaga adenylyl cyclase and spatial regulation by dunce phosphodiesterase. Neuron. 2010;65(4):516–529. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Burke CJ, et al. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492(7429):433–437. doi: 10.1038/nature11614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Liu C, et al. A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;488(7412):512–516. doi: 10.1038/nature11304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pauls D, Selcho M, Gendre N, Stocker RF, Thum AS. Drosophila larvae establish appetitive olfactory memories via mushroom body neurons of embryonic origin. J Neurosci. 2010;30(32):10655–10666. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1281-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Krashes MJ, Keene AC, Leung B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron. 2007;53(1):103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Trannoy S, Redt-Clouet C, Dura JM, Preat T. Parallel processing of appetitive short- and long-term memories in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2011;21(19):1647–1653. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cervantes-Sandoval I, Martin-Peña A, Berry JA, Davis RL. System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2013;33(23):9846–9854. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Xie Z, Huang C, Ci B, Wang L, Zhong Y. Requirement of the combination of mushroom body γ lobe and α/β lobes for the retrieval of both aversive and appetitive early memories in Drosophila. Learn Mem. 2013;20(9):474–481. doi: 10.1101/lm.031823.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Huang C, Wang P, Xie Z, Wang L, Zhong Y. The differential requirement of mushroom body α/β subdivisions in long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. Protein Cell. 2013;4(7):512–519. doi: 10.1007/s13238-013-3035-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Roeper J. Dissecting the diversity of midbrain dopamine neurons. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36(6):336–342. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lammel S, Lim BK, Malenka RC. Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(Pt B):351–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tanaka NK, Tanimoto H, Ito K. Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom body. J Comp Neurol. 2008;508(5):711–755. doi: 10.1002/cne.21692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Aso Y, et al. Specific dopaminergic neurons for the formation of labile aversive memory. Curr Biol. 2010;20(16):1445–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Aso Y, et al. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genet. 2012;8(7):e1002768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Keleman K, et al. Dopamine neurons modulate pheromone responses in Drosophila courtship learning. Nature. 2012;489(7414):145–149. doi: 10.1038/nature11345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Riemensperger T, et al. A single dopamine pathway underlies progressive locomotor deficits in a Drosophila model of Parkinson disease. Cell Reports. 2013;5(4):952–960. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Aso Y, et al. 2014. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. eLife, 10.7554/eLife.04577.001.
  • 34.Aso Y, et al. 2014. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. eLife, 10.7554/eLife.04580.001.
  • 35.Hamada FN, et al. An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in Drosophila. Nature. 2008;454(7201):217–220. doi: 10.1038/nature07001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Das G, et al. Drosophila learn opposing components of a compound food stimulus. Curr Biol. 2014;24(15):1723–1730. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kitamoto T. Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. J Neurobiol. 2001;47(2):81–92. doi: 10.1002/neu.1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Burke CJ, Waddell S. Remembering nutrient quality of sugar in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2011;21(9):746–750. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cervantes-Sandoval I, Davis RL. Distinct traces for appetitive versus aversive olfactory memories in DPM neurons of Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2012;22(13):1247–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jenett A, et al. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2(4):991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lin S, et al. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(11):1536–1542. doi: 10.1038/nn.3827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Miyamoto T, Slone J, Song X, Amrein H. A fructose receptor functions as a nutrient sensor in the Drosophila brain. Cell. 2012;151(5):1113–1125. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Boto T, Louis T, Jindachomthong K, Jalink K, Tomchik SM. Dopaminergic modulation of cAMP drives nonlinear plasticity across the Drosophila mushroom body lobes. Curr Biol. 2014;24(8):822–831. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Murthy M, Fiete I, Laurent G. Testing odor response stereotypy in the Drosophila mushroom body. Neuron. 2008;59(6):1009–1023. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Caron SJ, Ruta V, Abbott LF, Axel R. Random convergence of olfactory inputs in the Drosophila mushroom body. Nature. 2013;497(7447):113–117. doi: 10.1038/nature12063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Emptage NJ, Carew TJ. Long-term synaptic facilitation in the absence of short-term facilitation in Aplysia neurons. Science. 1993;262(5131):253–256. doi: 10.1126/science.8211146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Martin KC, et al. Synapse-specific, long-term facilitation of aplysia sensory to motor synapses: A function for local protein synthesis in memory storage. Cell. 1997;91(7):927–938. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80484-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wright WG, Jones K, Sharp P, Maynard B. Widespread anatomical projections of the serotonergic modulatory neuron, CB1, in Aplysia. Invert Neurosci. 1995;1(2):173–183. doi: 10.1007/BF02331914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Galili DS, et al. Converging circuits mediate temperature and shock aversive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2014;24(15):1712–1722. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fiorillo CD, Yun SR, Song MR. Diversity and homogeneity in responses of midbrain dopamine neurons. J Neurosci. 2013;33(11):4693–4709. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3886-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kaun KR, Azanchi R, Maung Z, Hirsh J, Heberlein U. A Drosophila model for alcohol reward. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(5):612–619. doi: 10.1038/nn.2805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Unoki S, Matsumoto Y, Mizunami M. Participation of octopaminergic reward system and dopaminergic punishment system in insect olfactory learning revealed by pharmacological study. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22(6):1409–1416. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04318.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Dubnau J, et al. The staufen/pumilio pathway is involved in Drosophila long-term memory. Curr Biol. 2003;13(4):286–296. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00064-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Comas D, Petit F, Preat T. Drosophila long-term memory formation involves regulation of cathepsin activity. Nature. 2004;430(6998):460–463. doi: 10.1038/nature02726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Margulies C, Tully T, Dubnau J. Deconstructing memory in Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2005;15(17):R700–R713. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Bailey CH. Molecular mechanisms of memory storage in Aplysia. Biol Bull. 2006;210(3):174–191. doi: 10.2307/4134556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Keene AC, Waddell S. Drosophila olfactory memory: Single genes to complex neural circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(5):341–354. doi: 10.1038/nrn2098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Stafford JW, Lynd KM, Jung AY, Gordon MD. Integration of taste and calorie sensing in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2012;32(42):14767–14774. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1887-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Peng H, et al. BrainAligner: 3D registration atlases of Drosophila brains. Nat Methods. 2011;8(6):493–500. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Tian L, et al. Imaging neural activity in worms, flies and mice with improved GCaMP calcium indicators. Nat Methods. 2009;6(12):875–881. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Séjourné J, et al. Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(7):903–910. doi: 10.1038/nn.2846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary File
pnas.201421930SI.pdf (1.7MB, pdf)
Supplementary File
Download video file (9.6MB, avi)
Supplementary File
Download video file (7.8MB, avi)
Supplementary File
Download video file (2.6MB, avi)

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES