Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jan 31.
Published in final edited form as: Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014 Nov 29;0:19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.017

Inoculation Stress Hypothesis of Environmental Enrichment

Elizabeth J Crofton 1, Yafang Zhang 1, Thomas A Green 1,
PMCID: PMC4305384  NIHMSID: NIHMS645773  PMID: 25449533

Abstract

One hallmark of psychiatric conditions is the vast continuum of individual differences in susceptibility vs. resilience resulting from the interaction of genetic and environmental factors. The environmental enrichment paradigm is an animal model that is useful for studying a range of psychiatric conditions, including protective phenotypes in addiction and depression models. The major question is how environmental enrichment, a non-drug and non-surgical manipulation, can produce such robust individual differences in such a wide range of behaviors. This paper draws from a variety of published sources to outline a coherent hypothesis of inoculation stress as a factor producing the protective enrichment phenotypes. The basic tenet suggests that chronic mild stress from living in a complex environment and interacting non-aggressively with conspecifics can inoculate enriched rats against subsequent stressors and/or drugs of abuse. This paper reviews the enrichment phenotypes, mulls the fundamental nature of environmental enrichment vs. isolation, discusses the most appropriate control for environmental enrichment, and challenges the idea that cortisol/corticosterone equals stress. The intent of the inoculation stress hypothesis of environmental enrichment is to provide a scaffold with which to build testable hypotheses for the elucidation of the molecular mechanisms underlying these protective phenotypes and thus provide new therapeutic targets to treat psychiatric/neurological conditions.

Keywords: Environmental enrichment, inoculation stress, resilience, drug addiction, corticosterone

1.1 A history of environmental enrichment research

The “nature vs. nurture” debate began in earnest during the Victorian period, championed by Sir Francis Galton, who was inspired by the works of his cousin Charles Darwin. At issue was whether a person’s expressed traits are a product of heritability (i.e. nature) or by his/her own experiences (nurture). Galton, bolstered by Darwin’s theories on heritability came down firmly on the side of “nature”. The opposing “nurture” side of the debate was best defined centuries before by John Locke’s borrowed term “tabula rasa” (i.e. blank slate). The “nurture” side of the argument was further strengthened in the early 1900s by John Watson’s theories on behaviorism.

As science evolved (particularly the advent of genetics), the “nature vs. nurture” debate evolved into a “genes vs. environment” debate, respectively. The battle raged on as scientists on both sides of the argument produced irrefutable evidence for their view. Eventually, scientists realized that both arguments were correct—that a person’s expressed phenotype was due to an interaction of genes with environment. Thus, the Gene/Environment Interaction Theory was born. In a basic sense, the environment controls (to some degree) how genes are expressed. Thus, gene transcription is where the proverbial “rubber hits the road” and seems to play a significant role in the protective phenotypes produced by environmental enrichment (Green et al., 2010; Lobo et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014), which are described below in the beneficial effects of environmental enrichment.

The beginning of modern environmental enrichment research is mostly attributed to Rosenzweig, Renner, Bennett, Diamond and colleagues. This group used the environmental enrichment paradigm to show convincingly that the adult brain still exhibits plasticity and that, just like muscles, brains get stronger with greater use. Rats reared in an enriched condition (EC) have a thicker cortex, more dendritic arborization and greater cognitive abilities than rats reared in an isolated condition (IC) (Diamond et al., 1964; Renner and Rosenzweig, 1987; Rosenzweig and Bennett, 1996). Following these early experiments, many others have used environmental enrichment and found it to be a useful animal model in a variety of fields, particularly because it is a non-drug and non-surgical manipulation.

In parallel with Rosenzweig and colleagues, Harry Harlow was finalizing the ideas for his seminal work on the importance of maternal and social enrichment in rhesus monkeys (Harlow, 1958). Harlow designed inanimate wire and cloth “surrogate” mothers to show that maternal contact is enriching to baby macaques beyond merely providing food. Although Harlow’s early work was oriented to the positive effects of maternal enrichment (i.e. affection), his later work shifted perspective to focus on the isolation aspect (i.e. lack of enrichment) rather than the enrichment itself (Harlow and Suomi, 1971).

2.1 What is environmental enrichment?

Environmental enrichment is complex and there are numerous ways to provide enrichment. There is a lack of consistency in protocols for enrichment between different laboratories, but the most common procedure in rats involves rearing the subjects in a large cage with novel objects and social contact with conspecifics for at least 30 days beginning immediately after weaning. The objects are replaced and rearranged daily to maximize novelty. This arrangement provides three key facets of enrichment: novelty, social contact and exercise. It has been shown in rats that all three aspects are rewarding (Bardo and Bevins, 2000; Belke, 2000; Bevins and Bardo, 1999) and all three release dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (Greenwood et al., 2011; Louilot et al., 1986; Rebec et al., 1997). Thus, it can be said that environmental enrichment is a compound manipulation that provides a daily workout for the dopamine system. Indeed, when the novel objects are replaced each day, the rats display a burst of exploratory activity lasting approximately thirty minutes that is beyond anything seen with locomotor stimulants like cocaine or amphetamine. Additionally, there is a second burst of exploratory/play behavior that occurs at the onset of the dark cycle, the beginning of the rats’ normal period of high activity.

Although environmental conditions have a dramatic impact on the behavior of animals, these differing protocols for enriching rats often produce conflicting results. Parameters such as age of the animal, degree of enrichment, duration of enrichment, species and sex can each affect the results of an experiment. The lack of consistency in protocols likely stems from a lack of consensus regarding the definition of what indeed constitutes “environmental enrichment”. Some might define enrichment based on environmental complexity–that a more complex environment is more enriching; however, environmental complexity alone is not the whole story. Environmental enrichment, by most definitions, should exert a positive influence on the organism, setting enrichment apart from overtly stressful events that have a negative impact on the organism. Thus, enrichment must provide an overall benefit to the organism. Further confusion in the field also arises from the fact that some researchers compare EC rats only to pair-housed social condition (SC) rats or compare only IC with SC rats (see below for discussion of the appropriate control for enrichment). However, without discounting or dismissing the views of others studying environmental enrichment using different protocols, this paper outlines a theory that the mild daily stresses of the enriched lifestyle are adaptive and inoculate rats to produce protective preclinical phenotypes for addiction and depression.

2.2 What are the beneficial effects of environmental enrichment?

As mentioned above, environmental enrichment contains three basic components: novelty, exercise and social contact. Animals are group-housed in a large cage equipped with children’s plastic toys, which are replaced and rearranged every day. In order to study the “preventive” effect of environmental enrichment, rats are usually raised in the enriched condition before exposure to drugs (in the case of addiction research) or stress (in the case of depression research). Environmental enrichment attenuates the reinforcing effects of addictive drugs and produces an antidepressant-like effect (Brenes et al., 2008; Brenes Saenz et al., 2006; Green et al., 2010; Laviola et al., 2008). In addition, environmental enrichment can be studied as a “treatment” model, in which rats are assigned to either an isolated or enriched condition after they are exposed to drugs or stress, which has also been shown to produce adaptive consequences (Grimm et al., 2008; Solinas et al., 2008; Thiel et al., 2010). Below is an overview of the beneficial effects of environmental enrichment. To maintain focus, this hypothesis paper is predominantly centered on rodent research, although environmental enrichment has been studied in other species with success (Harlow, 1958; Harlow and Suomi, 1971; Nader et al., 2012; Solinas et al., 2010).

2.3 The protective addiction phenotype

Rats reared in an enriched condition exhibit lower basal locomotor activity than rats in the isolated condition, making interpretation of drug-stimulated locomotor data challenging. Despite this, the available evidence is fairly clear that EC rats show greater locomotor sensitivity to psychostimulants such as amphetamine and cocaine (Bardo et al., 1999; Bowling and Bardo, 1994a; Bowling et al., 1993; Smith et al., 2009) while at the same time showing reduced sensitization to repeated exposure (Bardo et al., 1995; Green et al., 2003; Smith et al., 1997). In the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm, a paradigm more relevant to addiction, Bowling and Bardo, and then Green and colleagues reported that enrichment produces enhanced CPP to both amphetamine and cocaine in rats (Bowling and Bardo, 1994b; Green et al., 2010). These results lead to the hypothesis that enrichment increases the risk for addiction; however, in the paradigm with the greatest face validity for addiction, the intravenous drug self-administration paradigm, the reinforcing effects of amphetamine, cocaine, ethanol, and methylphenidate are decreased by enrichment and the rats self-administer less of these drugs (Alvers et al., 2012; Bardo et al., 2001b; Deehan et al., 2007; Gill et al., 2013; Green et al., 2010; Green et al., 2002; Stairs et al., 2006).

Enrichment also alters drug taking in treatment models. Exposure to enrichment after exposure to cocaine reduces locomotor activity, eliminates cocaine CPP and reduces cocaine-induced reinstatement of CPP, and decreases cocaine-seeking behavior during extinction and cue-elicited reinstatement (Chauvet et al., 2012; Ranaldi et al., 2011; Solinas et al., 2008; Thiel et al., 2009). Although cue-elicited reinstatement is reduced in the treatment model, enrichment does not alter cocaine-primed reinstatement, suggesting that enrichment reduces the salience of drug-associated environmental cues which could lead to an effective therapy for craving elicited by drug cues in humans (Thiel et al., 2009).

2.4 The protective antidepressant phenotype

Along with the protective addiction phenotype, environmental enrichment also produces a protective antidepressant-like phenotype. In humans, three of the hallmark symptoms of depression are anhedonia, social withdrawal and behavioral despair. Our prior research and others found that, compared to IC rats, EC rats consume more sucrose in a sucrose preference test, indicating decreased anhedonia-like behavior; longer grooming time in the social contact test, suggesting decreased social withdrawal; and greater mobility time in the forced swim test (FST), suggesting reduced “behavioral despair” (Brenes et al., 2008; Brenes Saenz et al., 2006; Green et al., 2010).

2.5 Anxiety

Similar to addiction- and depression-like behavior, multiple labs have demonstrated reduced anxiety-like behavior resulting from environmental enrichment. For example, EC rats display lower basal locomotor activity, yet increased distance traveled in the center of the arena in the open field test, indicating an anxiolytic effect (Urakawa et al., 2013). In addition, enriched rats and mice were found to spend more time in the open arms in the elevated plus maze (EPM), and showed lower amounts of defensive burying and less defensive behavior when in close proximity to a predator, also suggesting reduced anxiety (Friske and Gammie, 2005; Leal-Galicia et al., 2007; Roy et al., 2001). Enriched mice show reduced anxiety in response to social defeat stress, an effect that was abolished by lesioning the infralimbic region of the prefrontal cortex prior to environmental enrichment exposure (Lehmann and Herkenham, 2011), suggesting a role of the prefrontal cortex in anxiety and environmental enrichment.

However, not all are convinced that enrichment is anxiolytic. Our own research demonstrated greater sucrose neophobia in EC rats and more fecal boli in the cold-stress defecation test; both suggesting increased anxiety (Green et al., 2010). In addition, latency to ejaculation has been used as a measure of anxiety (Wallace et al., 2009), and EC rats exhibit increased latency to ejaculate, again suggesting increased anxiety (Urakawa et al., 2014). It is noteworthy that the last three tests (sucrose neophobia, cold-stress defecation, and latency to ejaculation) are not a function of exploratory activity in a novel environment. Because environmental enrichment involves extended exposure to novel environments whereas IC rats have very little to no experience in novel environments, this confounds the results of behavioral tests involving exploration of a novel environment. In addition, environmentally enriched rats show less social withdrawal than isolated rats, which may confound the results of social defeat tests on enriched animals (Green et al., 2010). Taken as a whole, the benefits of enrichment on anxiety-like behavior are not as clear as other areas, but further analysis of anxiety-like behavior in enriched animals is an interesting avenue for future experimentation.

2.6 Other disorders

Environmental enrichment has also been shown to have beneficial effects in rodent models of neurodegenerative diseases. For example, in a mouse model of Huntington’s disease, enrichment delays the onset and slows disease progression by minimizing the loss of cerebral volume and by rescuing protein deficits (Hockly et al., 2002; Spires et al., 2004; van Dellen et al., 2000). Enrichment can also prevent neurodegeneration in C57BL/6 mice caused by a neurotoxin that causes Parkinson’s disease-like symptoms in humans by regulating expression of the dopamine transporter (DAT) (Bezard et al., 2003). In addition, an increasing number of studies reported the beneficial effect of environmental enrichment in improving learning and memory in behavioral and molecular aspects of Alzheimer’s disease (Bouet et al., 2011; Jankowsky et al., 2005; Jankowsky et al., 2003; Levi et al., 2003; Wolf et al., 2006). A study on Tg2576 mice, a model of Alzheimer’s, found that environmental enrichment counteracts the deleterious effects of chronic unpredictable stress in Alzheimer’s disease progression (Jeong et al., 2011). Further, a recent proteomic study found that the microtubule-associated protein tau was upregulated in EC rats compared to IC rats (Fan et al., 2013a). That paper and two other proteomics papers also identified huntingtin, presenilin 1, tau and amyloid precursor protein as major upstream regulators for environmental enrichment (Fan et al., 2013b; Lichti et al., 2014). These results warrant further investigation of neurodegenerative disorders using the environmental enrichment paradigm.

2.7 Species differences

Although environmental enrichment has been studied extensively in rats, other species have garnered considerably less attention, save for the work of Harlow in monkeys described above (Harlow, 1958; Harlow and Suomi, 1971). However, the available evidence from the primate literature suggests that enrichment is a protective factor for stress, as well as for addiction-related behavior (Harlow, 1958; Harlow and Suomi, 1971; Kozorovitskiy et al., 2005; Nader et al., 2012). There are, however, some important rat/mouse differences in the effects of environmental enrichment. For example, rats show the unusual phenotype where environmental enrichment increases responsiveness to stimulants such as cocaine or amphetamine in locomotor, CPP and neurochemical studies, while these same rats show decreased drug self-administration (Bardo et al., 2001a; Bardo et al., 1999; Bowling and Bardo, 1994a; Bowling et al., 1993; Green et al., 2010; Green et al., 2002); mice, on the other hand, show decreased sensitivity to stimulants after environmental enrichment (Solinas et al., 2009). Regardless, environmental enrichment produces a net benefit in addiction related behavior in both species (Solinas et al., 2010).

3.1 What is inoculation stress?

As described above, the environmental enrichment paradigm is a non-drug, non-surgical preclinical animal model useful for studying various psychiatric and neurological conditions. Environmental enrichment produces protective phenotypes in addiction and depression models, which are robust and replicable. The major question is how environmental enrichment can produce such robust individual differences in a wide range of behaviors associated with addiction, depression and anxiety and beneficial effects even for animal models of neurodegenerative diseases. Our hypothesis is that enriched rats undergo inoculation stress. In short, chronic very mild stress from living in a complex environment and interacting non-aggressively with conspecifics inoculates enriched rats against subsequent stressors and/or drugs of abuse.

Inoculation stress, described previously in human studies, is a process of developing resilience to future stressful events by first being exposed to mildly stressful experiences early in life (Dienstbier, 1989; Fox et al., 2006; Khoshaba and Maddi, 1999; Lyons et al., 2009; Meichenbaum, 2007; Parker et al., 2004; Rutter, 2006). Exposure to stress or adversity that toughens an individual is protective, much like a vaccination that exposes an individual to a non-harmful version of a disease in order to develop immunity to that illness for the future (Lyons et al., 2009; Rutter, 2006). For example, adults who are exposed to work stress as adolescents have fewer negative health effects from work-related stress as adults (Mortimer and Staff, 2004). Unlike a vaccine, however, inoculation stress does not protect only against a single disease, but toughens the individual in general and prepares them to cope with a variety of stressors later in life. For example, individuals that have successfully coped with some adverse events in adolescence have overall better mental health and are better able to cope with serious illness, spousal loss, or a major accident as adults (Khoshaba and Maddi, 1999; Lyons et al., 2009; Seery et al., 2013).

An important distinction to make is that inoculation stress is not simply exposure to any stress early in life, but rather having positive and adaptive responses to mild stressors is critical. Severe stress early in life often causes the individual to be more vulnerable to stress later in life whereas mild stress exposure with an adaptive response can protect the individual (Khoshaba and Maddi, 1999; Parker and Maestripieri, 2011). Stress that is inoculating conditions the individual, and provides specific coping strategies for exposure to future stressors, ultimately leading to resilience.

Stress inoculation has also been described in non-human primates and rodents. Squirrel monkeys and rodents exposed to a manageable stress early in life are less stress-reactive in the future (Lyons et al., 2009; Lyons et al., 2010). In particular, environmental enrichment in adolescence is able to provide protection from the deleterious effects of subsequent stressors (Fox et al., 2006; Larsson et al., 2002; Lyons et al., 2009; Parker et al., 2004), providing the impetus for the hypothesis that environmental enrichment is a chronic mild stress environment leading to stress inoculation.

4.1 Allostasis and allostatic load

In trying to make sense of how mild/moderate stressors are adaptive yet severe stressors are maladaptive, it helps to frame the picture with respect to allostasis and allostatic load. For the purposes of this paper, the term “allostasis” will be used in its broadest sense, disregarding the narrow application of this term only to energy regulation (McEwen and Wingfield, 2010). Thus, allostasis is defined as the process of returning a dynamic system to its stable set point after a challenge to that system. In a conceptual sense, homeostasis is maintaining stability in a non-dynamic system while allostasis is regulating stability of a dynamic system.

The genome is chock full of allostatic mechanisms for maintaining stability in hundreds of dynamic systems. However, maintaining stability of a dynamic system is not without cost to the organism. This cost to the organism is termed “allostatic load” and can be paid in a currency as diverse as energy usage, ion concentrations (e.g. neuronal activity), or protein turnover. Regulatory systems have evolved to deal with allostatic load to a certain extent, but most systems are susceptible to “allostatic overload” if the demands on the system outpace the allostatic capacity. At that point, the system fails to maintain stability and pathology develops. In the arena of psychiatric conditions, allostatic overload is evident when severe stressors surpass the allostatic capacity of the person and induce pathological conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder, major depression, addiction, and anxiety disorders (among others).

One thing that differentiates allostasis from homeostasis is that allostasis, by virtue of regulating a dynamic system, can predict future allostatic load and adjust its capacity accordingly in anticipation of that future load. For example, forcing the average person to run 12 miles will result in allostatic overload of many systems (energy, oxygen, joints, muscles) whereas the seasoned runner’s body has predicted the possibility of a 12 mile run and adjusted the allostatic capacity of these systems to match the high load. As a result, instead of being pathogenic, subsequent stressors can even be motivating to the individual, allowing them to thrive in adverse conditions. Thus, individual responses to the same stressful event can be highly variable, and the resulting effect on the individual depends on that individual’s allostatic capacity to handle stress.

Allostatic overload of stress response systems can lead to psychiatric disorders, but how can inoculation stress or exposure to mild stress become protective against developing psychiatric conditions? Unfortunately stress has garnered a negative connotation, making experiments on stress with humans and animal models sometimes ambiguous. In 1976, Hans Selye defined stress as “the nonspecific response of the body to any demand made upon it” (Selye, 1976). The term “stress” does not inherently specify whether the stimulus is adaptive (i.e. inoculation stress) or maladaptive (i.e. allostatic overload), but even in Selye’s own work it was often assumed that “stress” meant maladaptive stress. However, whether a stressor is positive or negative chiefly depends on the allostatic capacity of the individual. Most individuals exposed to stress are resilient to psychiatric conditions but those that are susceptible have a lower allostatic capacity for stress. Inoculation stress causes the organism to predict future stress and increase the allostatic capacity of the individual accordingly, providing resilience to subsequent stressors that might otherwise produce psychiatric conditions. Thus, environmental enrichment is hypothesized to increase allostatic load capacity from the repeated exposure to very mild stresses, inoculating against subsequent stressors.

Since allostatic capacity for stress is associated with psychiatric disorders, it is useful to assess allostatic load capacity for stress in rodents. There is a wide range of stressors from which to choose to preclinically examine allostatic load capacity. Mildly stressful stimuli include brief periods of restraint stress, mild footshock, brief exposure to temperature changes as in the cold stress-induced defecation paradigm, and handling by an experimenter (Gärtner et al., 1980; Green et al., 2010; Gregus et al., 2005; Rabasa et al., 2011b). Some severe stressors are long periods of immersion in cold water, long periods of food or water deprivation, and intense footshock (Rabasa et al., 2011b; Willner, 1997). One very severe stressor is social defeat by an aggressive conspecific (Covington and Miczek, 2005; Koolhaas et al., 1996; Müller et al., 2000). It is important to note that individual stressors can be considered mild or severe depending on duration and intensity. For example, brief maternal separation in rats is adaptive whereas lengthy maternal separation leads to allostatic overload and susceptibility in models of psychiatric conditions (Lyons et al., 2009; Sih, 2011). Thus, footshock, maternal separation, physical restraint, and temperature changes can be considered mild stressors or severe stressors. Predictability also seems important; the so-called chronic unpredictable mild stress paradigm (CMS), despite the label of “mild”, can be a strong enough insult to induce allostatic overload and prevent inoculation (see below). Therefore, evaluating the response of the rodent to various stressors can aid in gauging the allostatic capacity of the rodent (i.e. whether they are inoculated against future stressors).

5.1 Stress, addiction and depression

As mentioned in the previous section, severe stress can lead to various psychiatric disorders; therefore, understanding the consequences of adaptive and maladaptive stress is translationally relevant. Stress is not only implicated in conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder, major depression, and anxiety disorders, but also drug use disorders. Severe stress in adolescence (such as being abused as a child) is associated with higher risk for alcoholism, substance abuse, depression, suicide attempts, obesity and poorer general health (Felitti et al., 1998; McEwen, 2000). Evidence suggests there is overlap in the underlying mechanisms for mental health disorders and drug use disorders (Levin et al., 2008; Worley et al., 2012), and psychological stress may be the common link. The environmental enrichment paradigm produces robust protective phenotypes for depression-like and drug abuse behaviors. Therefore, our hypothesis is not only does maladaptive stress increase the likelihood of psychological disorders and drug abuse, but also the protective effects of environmental enrichment may be due to adaptive responses to stress.

Regarding stress and depression, stress is the leading factor for both the development of and relapse to major depression (Hardeveld et al., 2013; Kendler et al., 1998; Kendler et al., 1999; Morris et al., 2010). Stress in the workplace and major life events, such as the death of a spouse, can trigger depressive episodes and increase the risk of major depression (Heim et al., 1997; Tennant, 2001). Evidence suggests that alterations in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis, which is involved in coordinating the body’s response to various stressors (i.e. managing allostatic load), are involved in depression. Cortisol is a glucocorticoid released by the adrenal cortex in response to environmental stimuli and individuals with higher cortisol reactivity to low stress conditions had more depressive symptoms over time than those with low cortisol reactivity (Morris et al., 2012). In order to study this phenomenon preclinically alterations in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis should be evaluated.

Regarding addiction, stress is a leading factor contributing to relapse to drug use in humans (Pohorecky, 1991; Sinha, 2001; Sinha et al., 1999; Sinha et al., 2006), an effect modeled by self-administration studies in rats (Ahmed and Koob, 1997; Erb et al., 1996). Stress exposure in humans, particularly to a severe stressor, significantly increases cocaine craving during abstinence (Pohorecky, 1991). In addition to contributing to relapse, there is increasing preclinical evidence that stress contributes to the initial development of addiction (Burke and Miczek, 2014; Covington and Miczek, 2005; Goeders, 2002; Piazza and Le Moal, 1998). For humans, stress increases the likelihood that someone will start smoking along with increasing the risk of relapse to cigarette smoking (Bruijnzeel, 2012). Additionally, exposure to stress increases cocaine craving (Sinha et al., 1999) and more stress-induced cocaine craving increases the likelihood of relapse in cocaine-dependent individuals (Sinha et al., 2006).

Beyond stress/addiction and stress/depression interactions, there is evidence linking addiction directly to depression (Levin et al., 2008; Worley et al., 2012). For example, individuals with major depression are more likely to smoke than the average person (Bruijnzeel, 2012). Major depression and substance use disorders are often comorbid in humans, and the symptoms are often more severe together than with only one disorder (Kessler et al., 2005; Pettinati et al., 2013). The rate of comorbidity of major depression with alcohol use disorders is 40.3%, and major depression with a drug use disorder is 17.2% (Pettinati et al., 2013). The high rate of comorbidity makes developing an effective treatment very complicated. If the mood disorder is solely substance-induced, controlling the drug use would solve both disorders, but if the mood disorder has some other etiology, then antidepressants may be required (Pettinati et al., 2013). Often treatment for individuals with these comorbid disorders is focused on one disorder or the other and disregards the fact that stress may underlie both disorders.

Depression and addiction phenotypes often go hand in hand not only in humans but also in rodents (Green et al., 2006; Green et al., 2010; Green et al., 2008; Pettinati et al., 2013). For example, overexpression of a dominant negative inhibitor of CREB (inducible cAMP early repressor/ICER or a mutant CREB/mCREB) or knockdown of CREB in the NAc produces an antidepressant-like phenotype and also decreases cocaine self-administration in a similar manner to environmental enrichment (Green et al., 2006; Green et al., 2010). Similarly, the transcription factor ΔFosB in the NAc is associated with stress and also cocaine-taking behavior (Vialou et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2014). Thus, it is very difficult to tease apart addiction and depression phenotypes in humans and in rodent models. Therefore analysis of an animal model that addresses both disorders simultaneously, such as environmental enrichment, is very valuable. Further, the close ties between depression and addiction are hypothesized to explain how inoculation to stress can affect drug-taking behavior.

As a whole, the evidence linking stress, depression and addiction provides a plausible rationale for how the repeated mild stress of environmental enrichment can protect against addiction-related behavior.

6.1 Does cortisol/corticosterone equal stress?

Because stress is linked with psychological disorders and drug dependence disorders, assessing stress in preclinical models is translationally relevant but can prove to be difficult without a good objective measure of stress in animals. Often, measurements of plasma cortisol, or the rodent equivalent corticosterone (CORT), have been used as a measure of stress in a variety of experiments. CORT is the primary glucocorticoid released by the adrenal cortex as a final product of the HPA axis. The HPA axis is activated in response to environmental stressors and activation of this system releases CORT. As a result, many scientists use CORT as a de facto indication of a subject’s stress level. However, it is important to note that CORT does not equal stress. The following paragraphs will argue: (1) CORT levels fluctuate throughout the day independent of stress, (2) rewarding stimuli induce CORT release, (3) CORT induction is attenuated with chronic stress, (4) CORT itself is reinforcing and (5) behavioral responses to CORT administration alone do not mimic responses to stress. In addition, there are several caveats when attempting to extrapolate emotional and behavioral state from plasma CORT levels, not the least of which is that the mere act of acquiring a blood sample to measure CORT can be stressful itself, especially with high frequency sampling (Abelson 2005). Therefore, evaluating stress in environmentally enriched animals based on CORT levels has contributed to differing hypotheses on whether enrichment or isolation is inherently stressful because of inconsistent findings of corticosterone levels between enriched and isolated animals (Konkle et al., 2010). However, this is not to say that CORT is not important, or that corticosterone is not involved in the beneficial effects of environmental enrichment. In fact, CORT and the HPA axis may be involved in the inoculation stress of environmental enrichment (see Section 8.1 below for elaboration). However, measured CORT levels do not provide a complete picture of the adaptive or maladaptive nature of the stress responses of an animal.

6.2 CORT levels fluctuate throughout the day

CORT measurements taken at different times during the circadian cycle will vary because CORT has a characteristic circadian rhythm. Regardless of stress level, CORT generally peaks as rats awaken just prior to the dark cycle and is lowest at the beginning of the light cycle (Allen and Kendall, 1967; Butte et al., 1976). The dark cycle is the period where the animals are awake and highly active versus the light cycle when the animals are mostly sleeping. Thus, if one were to use CORT as a de facto measure of stress, the circadian rhythm of CORT will likely produce confounding results depending on when blood is collected.

6.3 Rewarding stimuli induce CORT

Phasic CORT release subsequent to an environmental stimulus is generally assumed to be an indication that the animal is in a negative emotional state and that the stimulus had a negative impact. For example, social defeat, where a test subject is physically dominated by a more aggressive conspecific, is a noxious stressor and causes release of CORT in the defeated male (Buwalda et al., 2012). However, assumptions of a negative state are not always true. For example, sexual activity, a positive and rewarding stimulus, releases similar amounts of CORT as social defeat (Buwalda et al., 2012). Another stimulus, exercise, which is regarded as positive and rewarding, can also cause an increase in cortisol in humans (Buono et al., 1986; Deinzer et al., 1997; Rojas Vega et al., 2006). Voluntary exercise can also increase circulating CORT in Sprague-Dawley rats (Fediuc et al., 2006). In addition, rewarding drugs, including cocaine, cause CORT release (Moldow and Fischman, 1987; Torres and Rivier, 1992). Both rewarding and noxious stimuli cause alterations in CORT; therefore, CORT levels alone cannot differentiate between negative and positive stimuli.

6.4 CORT induction is attenuated with chronic stress

If CORT is a de facto measure of stress, one would expect repeated stress to increase CORT. Multiple studies have shown, however, that induction of CORT after a mild/moderate stressor attenuates with repeated exposure to the stressor in rats (Barnum et al., 2007; Carter et al., 2004; De Boer et al., 1990; Magarinos and McEwen, 1995; Mizoguchi et al., 2001; Natelson et al., 1988; Rabasa et al., 2011a). In humans, cortisol release also habituates with repeated exposures to the same stressor although there are individual differences in cortisol responses to stress (Deinzer et al., 1997; Gerra et al., 2001). This evidence suggests that repeated stress increases the allostatic capacity to future stressors, which is a possible mechanism for the inoculation stress underlying environmental enrichment effects.

6.5 CORT is reinforcing

Additional evidence CORT is not the same as maladaptive stress is demonstrated by the fact that CORT itself has reinforcing properties. Rats will intravenously (Piazza et al., 1991) and orally self-administer CORT (Deroche et al., 1993), causing release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) (Graf et al., 2013). CORT injections also potentiate amphetamine self-administration at medium and high doses of amphetamine (10 and 30 μg/infusion) (Piazza et al., 1991). These experiments and others show that CORT is reinforcing at circulating levels similar to that released by mild stress (Piazza et al., 1991).

6.6 Responses to CORT differ from responses to stress

If CORT equals stress, then CORT administration should produce the same responses as stress. MacDougall and Howland found that rats injected with CORT versus rats exposed to 30 minutes of restraint stress (a mild stressor) had the same amount of circulating CORT, but only restrained rats showed changes in short- and long-term synaptic plasticity in the subiculum (MacDougall and Howland, 2013). Retana-Marquez and colleagues (1998) found that CORT injections were not able to mimic the behavioral effects of social defeat stress even at very high circulating plasma levels. Social defeat causes decreases in male sexual behavior and decreases in testosterone, whereas CORT injections do not (Retana-Marquez et al., 1998). Conversely, rats restrained for 6 hours a day for 21 days did not show an increase in depression-like behavior in the forced swim test but rats injected with CORT did show an increase in depression-like behavior, suggesting that in some cases, elevated CORT can cause more maladaptive changes than mild restraint stress (Gregus et al., 2005). Thus, CORT and stress sometimes produce different behavioral effects, and CORT administration alone cannot reproduce the behavioral effects of stress; therefore CORT does not equal stress.

All told, it is clear that although stress usually releases CORT, the circulating level of CORT is not a direct measurement of stress level. Further, it is important to remember that animals in a chronically mild-stress environment show attenuated rather than potentiated CORT induction to stress or drugs. If CORT is not an adequate measure of stress, how can we determine if enriched rats are actually more stressed than isolated rats?

7.1 Are enriched rats really stressed?

The inoculation stress hypothesis of environmental enrichment proposes that enriched rats are repeatedly stressed. However, at first sight, it is exceedingly difficult to make this case. Young male Sprague-Dawley rats (unlike mice) typically establish dominance hierarchies through play behavior and, as long as there are no female rats in the vicinity, typically do not feel the need to challenge these hierarchies over time. Thus, in this rat enrichment utopia, fighting is rare, food is plentiful, space is expansive, and rats get all of the novelty, social contact (rats are social creatures), and exercise they desire. Additionally, it has repeatedly been proposed that enrichment is the “functional opposite of stress” (Fox et al., 2006; Solinas et al., 2010; Wright and Conrad, 2008). If true, how can one make the case that enriched rats are chronically stressed?

Although there are multiple good lines of evidence suggesting enrichment produces the functional opposite of stress (Fox et al., 2006; Solinas et al., 2010; Wright and Conrad, 2008), none of the reports gives a possible explanation for how enrichment produces this effect. The inoculation stress hypothesis of environmental enrichment outlined here posits that enrichment is a chronic mildly stressful condition that induces neuronal and neuroendocrine plasticity leaving enriched rats more resistant (i.e. greater allostatic capacity) to overtly stressful stimuli. Environmental enrichment exposes animals to novelty, social contact, and exercise and multiple studies have found that these variables cause stress-like responses. Acute voluntary exercise induces the secretion of CORT (Fediuc et al., 2006) and exposure to novelty will induce secretion of both CORT and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in rats (Hennessy 1979, Ostrander 2006, Piazza 1991, Larsson 2002). Finally, rats housed together with conspecifics had higher circulating CORT levels than isolated rats suggesting that social contact also causes a stress-like response (Raz, 2013). However, in the absence of a truly reliable and objective measure of stress, one must rely on circumstantial evidence comparing the effects of enrichment with the effects of repeated mild stress (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Parallels between repeated stress and enrichment

Effect Repeated mild stress Environmental enrichment
Endocrine Blunted CORT induction Natelson et al., 1988, De Boer et al., 1990, Magarinos and McEwen 1995, Barnum et al., 2007, Rabasa et al., 2011b, Carter et al., 2004 Stairs et al., 2011, Stairs and Bardo 2009, Skwara et al., 2012
Blunted ACTH induction Gadek-Michalska and Bugajski, 2003 Skwara et al., 2012, Belz et al., 2003
Enlarged adrenals Swanson and van de Poll, 1983, Marti et al., 1993 Mlynarik et al., 2004, Moncek et al., 2004
Blunted adrenaline release Dobrakovova et al. 1990 Moncek et al., 2004
Neurobiology ΔFosB accumulation in NAcc Perrotti et al., 2004, Lobo et al., 2013 Zhang et al., 2014, Lobo et al., 2013, Solinas et al., 2009
Attenuated immediate-early gene induction in NAcc Shoji and Mizoguchi, 2010 (cFos), Green et al. 2008 (ATF3), Zhang et al. 2014, manuscript in preparation
Behavior higher sensitivity to locomotor activating effects of amphetamine and cocaine Deroche et al., 1992, Lepsch et al., 2005 Bowling et al., 1993, Bowling and Bardo 1994, Smith et al., 2009, Green et al., 2010
decreased stimulant self-administration Matthews et al., 1996, Moffett et al., 2006, however, see: Carroll and Meisch 1984, Piazza et al., 1990a, Goeders and Guerin 1994, Shaham and Stewart 1994, and Miczek and Mutschler 1996, Kosten et al., 2000 Bardo et al., 2001, Green et al., 2002, Stairs et al., 2006, Thiel et al., 2009, Green et al., 2010, Alvers et al., 2012, Puhl et al., 2012
increased defecation under stress conditions Jorge et al. 2010 Green et al., 2010
decreased locomotor activity to novelty Cruz et al., 2012 Bowling et al, 1993, Green et al., 2003, Green et al. 2010
Physiology heart rate returns to baseline more quickly after stress Carter et al., 2004, Chen and Herbert, 1995 Sharp et al., 2002
lower body weight Harris et al., 2004 Pena et al., 2009

As summarized in Table 1, there are numerous parallels between the effects of environmental enrichment and repeated mild stress, adding strength to the idea that enriched rats are chronically stressed. Due to the importance of stress as a contributing factor to depression and addiction (see above), the effects of chronic stress on the body have been studied in depth. Results of these studies have produced a clear picture of the endocrine, neurobiological and behavioral sequelae of chronic stress in humans and in rat models. Environmentally enriched animals have also been assessed for the same endocrine, neurobiological and behavioral effects. There is much evidence that repeated mild stress blunts CORT induction in response to subsequent stressors (Barnum et al., 2007; Carter et al., 2004; De Boer et al., 1990; Magarinos and McEwen, 1995; Natelson et al., 1988; Rabasa et al., 2011b) and environmental enrichment also results in blunted CORT induction to stress (Skwara et al., 2012; Stairs and Bardo, 2009; Stairs et al., 2011). Repeated exposure to severe stressors (i.e. those producing allostatic overload), such as social defeat stress, do not show reductions in CORT induction to subsequent stress (Barnum et al., 2007). Repeated stress also blunts stress-induced adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) induction (Gadek-Michalska and Bugajski, 2003) and stress-induced release of adrenaline (Dobrakovova et al., 1990). Environmental enrichment also blunts ACTH induction (Belz et al., 2003; Skwara et al., 2012) and reduces stress-induced release of adrenaline (Moncek et al., 2004). In addition, enlarged adrenal glands have been found in environmentally enriched animals (Mlynarik et al., 2004; Moncek et al., 2004) and in animals exposed to repeated mild stress (Marti et al., 1993; Swanson and van de Poll, 1983). Environmental enrichment and repeated mild stress also both produce lower body weights (Harris et al., 2004; Pena et al., 2009) and following stress, the animal’s heart rate returns to baseline more quickly (Carter et al., 2004; Chen and Herbert, 1995; Sharp et al., 2002).

Not only do environmental enrichment and repeated mild stress show the same endocrine consequences, they also show concordant neurobiological consequences in the nucleus accumbens. Environmental enrichment reduces induction of immediate early genes (IEGs) (Zhang et al., 2014), as does repeated stress (Alibhai et al., 2007; Green et al., 2008; Shoji and Mizoguchi, 2010). In contrast to other IEGs, environmental enrichment causes accumulation of basal ΔFosB protein in the nucleus accumbens (Lobo et al., 2013; Solinas et al., 2009), which also occurs in repeatedly stressed animals (Lobo et al., 2013; Perrotti et al., 2004).

Environmental enrichment also produces robust effects on behavior that show similarities to the behavior of repeatedly stressed animals. For example, enriched animals show increased defecation to a mild stressor (novel cage under cold conditions) (Green et al., 2010), and rats that were alternately restrained for 1 hour or placed on a platform surrounded by water for 1 hour a day for 5 days also show increased defecation (Jorge et al., 2010). Environmentally enriched animals are more sensitive to the locomotor activating effects of amphetamine (Bowling and Bardo, 1994a; Bowling et al., 1993) and cocaine (Smith et al., 2009). Repeatedly stressed animals are also more sensitive to the locomotor activating effects of amphetamine and cocaine (Deroche et al., 1992; Lepsch et al., 2005). Despite being more sensitive to locomotor stimulants, environmental enrichment decreases spontaneous locomotor activity in response to a novel environment (Bowling et al., 1993; Green et al., 2010; Green et al., 2003). After mild stress, animals also show a similar decrease in locomotor activity when placed in a novel environment (Cruz et al., 2012).

As described above and illustrated in Table 1, environmental enrichment and repeated mild stress have matching endocrine, physiological, neurobiological, and behavioral effects. Although this is circumstantial evidence, it supports the argument that environmentally enriched animals are in a state of chronic mild stress and this mild stress in adolescence inoculates against future stressors.

The mild stress of environmental enrichment, however, is distinct from the stress of paradigms such as the chronic mild stress (CMS) paradigm or the chronic unpredictable stress paradigm in that environmental enrichment constitutes predictable stress that produces adaptive responses. The chronic mild stress paradigm typically involves stressors such as food deprivation, water deprivation, brief exposure to another subject, lights on during the dark cycle, periods of titling the cage by 30 degrees, and long periods of wet bedding material which occur randomly throughout the week for several weeks (Murison and Hansen, 2001; Willner et al., 1992; Willner et al., 1987). The CMS procedure can reduce sucrose and saccharine preference after several weeks of this unpredictable stress exposure (Hatcher et al., 1997; Willner et al., 1987), an anhedonic-like effect that can be reversed by several weeks of treatment with a tricyclic antidepressant (Willner et al., 1987). However, the CMS paradigm does not typically have other depression-like effects, and under some conditions this paradigm can actually increase sucrose consumption, suggesting inconsistencies in the paradigm (Murison and Hansen, 2001). An inoculation stress interpretation of these inconsistent data would posit that the unpredictable nature of the CMS can elevate the “mild” stress to a level that induces allostatic overload and that the inconsistencies in anhedonic-like behavior are a function of degree. The CMS by Murison and Hansen (2001) may not have induced allostatic overload, thus producing an inoculating effect whereas the other two reports induced a more severe stress (Hatcher et al., 1997; Murison and Hansen, 2001; Willner et al., 1987). Indeed, Hatcher and Hansen reported only finding an anhedonic saccharin response when the CMS paradigm included food deprivation.

8.1 CORT as a possible mediator of the protective EC phenotype

The sections above argue that CORT is not the same as stress; however, that is not to say that CORT is irrelevant. In fact, CORT responses may contribute substantially to the environmental enrichment protective phenotypes. For example, the environmental enrichment protective addiction phenotype fits well with what is known of the influences of CORT on stimulant self-administration. As mentioned above, CORT itself can be self-administered by rats (Deroche et al., 1993; Piazza et al., 1991), but there also exists evidence that CORT plays a significant role in stimulant self-administration. For example, higher CORT induction was associated with greater self-administration of low unit doses of cocaine regardless of whether the rats were stressed with contingent footshock, noncontingent footshock or no footshock (Goeders and Guerin, 1996b). Next, acquisition of cocaine self-administration can be completely blocked by bilateral adrenalectomy, partially reduced by pharmacological inhibition of corticosterone release by metyrapone in rats, and self-administration can be partially recovered by adding CORT to the drinking water (Goeders and Guerin, 1996a). Given that stimulants induce CORT release and blocking that release blocks self-administration, it is likely that the amount of CORT release from a stimulant determines the ability of low doses of that stimulant to engender or maintain self-administration. Blunted CORT release from stimulants in EC rats (Stairs and Bardo, 2009) could be the underlying molecular mechanism whereby inoculation stress produces the protective EC addiction phenotype. Further investigations are warranted to test this hypothesis.

9.1 Stress influences on self-administration

As discussed above, acute stress in humans is a major factor in relapse to addiction, so it is not surprising that acute stress in rodents produces reinstatement of cocaine seeking (Erb et al., 1996). However, the question at hand is how prior stress (i.e., not during or immediately before the session) affects subsequent stimulant self-administration. The logic in the above sections suggests that inoculation stress blunts CORT induction and that a blunted CORT response leads to less stimulant self-administration. Hence, one would predict that prior repeated mild stress (i.e. inoculation stress) would decrease drug self-administration. Two reports show that rats exposed to short-term maternal separation stress as pups later show reduced acquisition of cocaine self-administration at low unit doses (Matthews et al., 1996; Moffett et al., 2006). Despite these reports however, there are several reports showing that repeated stress increases stimulant self-administration (Carroll and Meisch, 1984; Goeders and Guerin, 1994; Kosten et al., 2000; Miczek and Mutschler, 1996; Piazza et al., 1990; Shaham and Stewart, 1994). Multiple factors could account for this discrepancy. First, the stressors in some of these papers are severe stressors that would exceed the allostatic capacity of the rats. Second, it is possible (even likely) that inoculation stress is most pronounced in very young animals (as with maternal separation and environmental enrichment). Third, it is possible that many other factors affect self-administration and that one or more of these factors are at play in some of these experiments. Regardless, if the inoculation stress hypothesis of environmental enrichment is correct, one would predict that environmental enrichment would decrease stimulant self-administration, which is undoubtedly the case (Alvers et al., 2012; Bardo et al., 2001b; Green et al., 2010; Green et al., 2002; Puhl et al., 2012; Stairs et al., 2006; Thiel et al., 2009). In any case, the fact remains that EC rats have blunted CORT responses, blunted CORT responses are associated with less self-administration, and EC rats self-administer stimulants less readily than IC rats which supports the inoculation stress hypothesis.

10.1 What is the best control for enrichment?

An important problem in the enrichment field is the difficulty of being able to compare results between labs because of inconsistencies in enrichment protocols and the use of different control groups. Environmental enrichment is a compound manipulation whereby rats are chronically exposed to novelty, social contact, and exercise. The most rigorous scientific approach would be to study each aspect individually and then in combination. For example, it would be nice to know the relative contributions of social contact vs. object novelty vs. exercise, and if the combination of these factors is redundant, additive or synergistic. However, it is not possible to fully separate these aspects because social interaction is a form of novelty and greatly increases activity (i.e. exercise). In addition, this approach would entail studying eight different conditions, and we as scientists have an ethical obligation to reduce as much as possible the number of animals used in biomedical research (Council, 2011). Additionally, as the number of conditions increases, there rapidly comes a point of diminishing returns where the cost (in money and time) of running an increasing number of conditions exceeds the small incremental benefit of the knowledge gained. Thus, the number of conditions must be limited. The fact that novelty, social contact and exercise all fall under the umbrella term “environmental enrichment” presupposes some commonality among the three constituents of enrichment. Indeed, each of these factors is rewarding to rats, and each releases dopamine in the nucleus accumbens, a critical brain region involved in stress, addiction, and depression (Greenwood et al., 2011; Louilot et al., 1986; Rebec et al., 1997). Accordingly, the many different conditions can now be reduced to as few as two: environmental enrichment and the appropriate control group.

Identifying the appropriate control group for cocaine administration is easy—an injection with no cocaine. Logic would dictate that the appropriate control for environmental enrichment (composed of novelty, social contact and exercise) would be a group with the absence of novelty, social contact, and exercise (i.e. isolation). Because pair housing is a form of enrichment (Council, 2011), comparing EC to pair-housed “control” rats would be akin to comparing a 20 mg/kg cocaine group to a 10 mg/kg “control”. The problem is that “standard” laboratory housing for rats is two per cage in most research laboratories, and as such, these pair-housed rats are viewed by many scientists to constitute the “normal”, or control condition. From this viewpoint, instead of seeing a continuum of enrichment ranging from isolation to pair-housed to full enrichment, environmental enrichment is seen as one manipulation (compared to pair-housed rats) and environmental isolation is seen as a different kind of manipulation.

At first sight, the case for isolation being a different kind of manipulation might seem to have merit. The case that many researchers make is that isolation itself is a stressor, and as such should not be used as a control. Indeed, many studies have shown that maternal separation (isolating pups from dam) and neonatal isolation (isolating pups from dam and other pups) are significant stressors, evoking ultrasonic vocalizations and inducing CORT release in pups (Hennessy and Weinberg, 1990; Kehne et al., 1991; Kuhn et al., 1990; Levine et al., 1991; McCormick et al., 1998; Viau et al., 1996). In addition, acute isolation of group-housed rats also induces CORT, which is taken as a clear indication of stress (Takatsu-Coleman et al., 2013). Many researchers thus make the leap of considering isolation rearing a condition of chronic stress, citing large CORT induction from acute stress as evidence. However, the inoculation stress hypothesis states that enriched animals are in a state of chronic very mild stress. As described above, blunted CORT induction is a sign of chronic stress, which is seen in EC rats and chronic mildly stressed animals alike. Table 1 further illustrates the many other signs of chronic stress that EC rats show that IC rats do not show. It is important to make the distinction that exhibiting a greater response to a stressor is not the same as being chronically stressed—quite the opposite. The isolated animals essentially have a lack of daily stimulation (i.e. stress) and therefore show a greater response to stress than EC rats, which are constantly stimulated (i.e. stressed) and therefore show a lesser response to subsequent stress.

Arguments frequently used in favor of pair-housed controls over isolated controls is that the IC group is not a “natural” condition, nor a “normal” condition, and has less “translational relevance” than pair-housed rats. Pair-housing rats in a laboratory is certainly not more translationally relevant than any other housing condition. The only place it is normal for two humans to be confined in the same small space is in prison. As for “normal”, even wild rats are social animals and are found in groups rather than pairs that stick together constantly. Regardless, is a control group itself supposed to be “normal”? In the case of a pharmacological control group, would it be “normal” for a human to take an injection of saline rather than a drug? A control group should be the lack of a manipulation, but the pair-housed or social condition contains some of the variables of enrichment, namely social interaction and elements of exercise or play, and therefore is an intermediate level of enrichment capable of producing some behavioral effects of full enrichment, but not all (Bardo et al., 2001b; Green et al., 2010; Rosenzweig, 2003; Zakharova et al., 2012). For as described above in the inoculation stress hypothesis, environmental enrichment is a combination of exercise, novelty, and social contact with conspecifics, all of which are mild stressors resulting in inoculation against future stressors. Neither social interaction, novelty, nor any other single variable can account for all of the effects of enrichment (van Praag et al., 2000). However, the goal in using the environmental enrichment paradigm is not to tease apart the different aspects of enrichment or different gradations of enrichment but rather to determine differences between enrichment and the lack of enrichment to find the molecular determinants of the resilience to addiction and depression. Therefore, in our opinion in light of the inoculation stress hypothesis, the isolated condition is the correct control for enrichment and the inclusion of an intermediate group such as the social condition is unnecessary.

11.1 Conclusions

The inoculation stress hypothesis is an under-appreciated framework for understanding many of the complexities of stress and an organism’s response to that stress. This hypothesis also has utility as a scaffold for which to build other novel hypotheses concerning susceptibility and resistance to psychiatric conditions such as addiction and major depression. Accordingly, application of the inoculation stress hypothesis to the environmental enrichment paradigm helps clarify the nature of nurture (i.e. role of environment) and its contribution to the resilience to addiction- and depression-related behavior. Future research designed from a better understanding of stress and environment will help to identify novel targets for the treatment of addiction and depression. Current research is already making headway (Fan et al., 2013a; Fan et al., 2013b; Pavlovsky et al., 2013).

Highlights.

  • Enrichment produces protective phenotypes for addiction and depression

  • Enriched rats have a chronic mild stress-like phenotype

  • This chronic mild stress inoculates enriched rats against subsequent stressors or drugs of abuse

  • CORT is not the same as stress

  • Isolated rats are the best control for environmental enrichment

Acknowledgments

This research is funded by NIDA DA029091 and NS081121.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Cocaine- but not food-seeking behavior is reinstated by stress after extinction. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;132:289–295. doi: 10.1007/s002130050347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alibhai IN, Green TA, Potashkin JA, Nestler EJ. Regulation of fosB and DeltafosB mRNA expression: in vivo and in vitro studies. Brain research. 2007;1143:22–33. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allen C, Kendall JW. Maturation of the circadian rhythm of plasma corticosterone in the rat. Endocrinology. 1967;80:926–930. doi: 10.1210/endo-80-5-926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alvers KM, Marusich JA, Gipson CD, Beckmann JS, Bardo MT. Environmental enrichment during development decreases intravenous self-administration of methylphenidate at low unit doses in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2012;23:650–657. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283584765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bardo M, Klebaur J, Valone J, Deaton C. Environmental enrichment decreases intravenous self-administration of amphetamine in female and male rats. Psychopharmacology. 2001a;155:278–284. doi: 10.1007/s002130100720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bardo M, Valone J, Robinet P, Shaw W, Dwoskin L. Environmental enrichment enhances the stimulant effect of intravenous amphetamine: search for a cellular mechanism in the nucleus accumbens. Psychobiology. 1999;27:292–299. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bardo MT, Bevins RA. Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2000;153:31–43. doi: 10.1007/s002130000569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bardo MT, Bowling SL, Rowlett JK, Manderscheid P, Buxton ST, Dwoskin LP. Environmental enrichment attenuates locomotor sensitization, but not in vitro dopamine release, induced by amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1995;51:397–405. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)00413-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bardo MT, Klebaur JE, Valone JM, Deaton C. Environmental enrichment decreases intravenous self-administration of amphetamine in female and male rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001b;155:278–284. doi: 10.1007/s002130100720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barnum CJ, Blandino P, Jr, Deak T. Adaptation in the corticosterone and hyperthermic responses to stress following repeated stressor exposure. J Neuroendocrinol. 2007;19:632–642. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2007.01571.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Belke TW. Studies of wheel-running reinforcement: Parameters of Herrnstein’s (1970) response-strength equation vary with schedule order. Journal of the experimental analysis of Behavior. 2000;73:319–331. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2000.73-319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Belz EE, Kennell JS, Czambel RK, Rubin RT, Rhodes ME. Environmental enrichment lowers stress-responsive hormones in singly housed male and female rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2003;76:481–486. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2003.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bevins RA, Bardo MT. Conditioned increase in place preference by access to novel objects: antagonism by MK-801. Behavioural brain research. 1999;99:53–60. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00069-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bezard E, Dovero S, Belin D, Duconger S, Jackson-Lewis V, Przedborski S, Piazza PV, Gross CE, Jaber M. Enriched environment confers resistance to 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine and cocaine: involvement of dopamine transporter and trophic factors. J Neurosci. 2003;23:10999–11007. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-35-10999.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bjørnebekk A, Mathé AA, Brené S. The antidepressant effect of running is associated with increased hippocampal cell proliferation. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8:357–368. doi: 10.1017/S1461145705005122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Moore KA, Craighead WE, Herman S, Khatri P, Waugh R, Napolitano MA, Forman LM, Appelbaum M. Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depression. Archives of internal medicine. 1999;159:2349–2356. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.19.2349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bouet V, Freret T, Dutar P, Billard JM, Boulouard M. Continuous enriched environment improves learning and memory in adult NMRI mice through theta burst-related-LTP independent mechanisms but is not efficient in advanced aged animals. Mech Ageing Dev. 2011;132:240–248. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bowling S, Bardo M. Locomotor and rewarding effects of amphetamine in enriched, social, and isolate reared rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1994a;48:459–464. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90553-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bowling SL, Bardo MT. Locomotor and rewarding effects of amphetamine in enriched, social, and isolate reared rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994b;48:459–464. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90553-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bowling SL, Rowlett JK, Bardo MT. The effect of environmental enrichment on amphetamine-stimulated locomotor activity, dopamine synthesis and dopamine release. Neuropharmacology. 1993;32:885–893. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(93)90144-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Brenes JC, Rodriguez O, Fornaguera J. Differential effect of environment enrichment and social isolation on depressive-like behavior, spontaneous activity and serotonin and norepinephrine concentration in prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 2008;89:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Brenes Saenz JC, Villagra OR, Fornaguera Trias J. Factor analysis of Forced Swimming test, Sucrose Preference test and Open Field test on enriched, social and isolated reared rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;169:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bruijnzeel AW. Tobacco addiction and the dysregulation of brain stress systems. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2012;36:1418–1441. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Buono MJ, Yeager JE, Hodgdon JA. Plasma adrenocorticotropin and cortisol responses to brief high-intensity exercise in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1986;61:1337–1339. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1986.61.4.1337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Burke AR, Miczek KA. Stress in adolescence and drugs of abuse in rodent models: Role of dopamine, CRF, and HPA axis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2014;231:1557–1580. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3369-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Butte JC, Kakihana R, Noble EP. Circadian rhythm of corticosterone levels in rat brain. J Endocrinol. 1976;68:235–239. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.0680235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Buwalda B, Scholte J, de Boer SF, Coppens CM, Koolhaas JM. The acute glucocorticoid stress response does not differentiate between rewarding and aversive social stimuli in rats. Hormones and behavior. 2012;61:218–226. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Carroll ME, Meisch RA. Increased drug-reinforced behavior due to food deprivation. Advances in behavioral pharmacology. 1984;4:47–88. [Google Scholar]
  29. Carter RN, Pinnock SB, Herbert J. Does the amygdala modulate adaptation to repeated stress? Neuroscience. 2004;126:9–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Chauvet C, Goldberg SR, Jaber M, Solinas M. Effects of environmental enrichment on the incubation of cocaine craving. Neuropharmacology. 2012;63:635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chen X, Herbert J. Regional changes in c-fos expression in the basal forebrain and brainstem during adaptation to repeated stress: correlations with cardiovascular, hypothermic and endocrine responses. Neuroscience. 1995;64:675–685. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)00532-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Council NR. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals: Eighth Edition. The National Academies Press; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Covington HE, 3rd, Miczek KA. Intense cocaine self-administration after episodic social defeat stress, but not after aggressive behavior: dissociation from corticosterone activation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:331–340. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0190-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cruz FC, Marin MT, Leao RM, Planeta CS. Behavioral and neuroendocrine effects of the exposure to chronic restraint or variable stress in early adolescent rats. International journal of developmental neuroscience: the official journal of the International Society for Developmental Neuroscience. 2012;30:19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. De Boer SF, Koopmans SJ, Slangen JL, Van der Gugten J. Plasma catecholamine, corticosterone and glucose responses to repeated stress in rats: effect of interstressor interval length. Physiology & behavior. 1990;47:1117–1124. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90361-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Deehan GA, Jr, Cain ME, Kiefer SW. Differential rearing conditions alter operant responding for ethanol in outbred rats. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2007;31:1692–1698. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00466.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Deinzer R, Kirschbaum C, Gresele C, Hellhammer DH. Adrenocortical responses to repeated parachute jumping and subsequent h-CRH challenge in inexperienced healthy subjects. Physiology & behavior. 1997;61:507–511. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00465-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Deroche V, Piazza PV, Casolini P, Maccari S, Le Moal M, Simon H. Stress-induced sensitization to amphetamine and morphine psychomotor effects depend on stress-induced corticosterone secretion. Brain research. 1992;598:343–348. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90205-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Deroche V, Piazza PV, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Simon H. Rats orally self-administer corticosterone. Brain research. 1993;622:315–320. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90837-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Diamond MC, Krech D, Rosenzweig MR. The effects of an enriched environment on the histology of the rat cerebral cortex. The Journal of comparative neurology. 1964;123:111–120. doi: 10.1002/cne.901230110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dienstbier RA. Arousal and physiological toughness: implications for mental and physical health. Psychol Rev. 1989;96:84–100. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.96.1.84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dobrakovova M, Kvetnansky R, Oprsalova Z, Macho L. [The effect of chronic stress on the activity of the sympathetic-adrenomedullary system] Bratislavske lekarske listy. 1990;91:587–592. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Duman CH, Schlesinger L, Russell DS, Duman RS. Voluntary exercise produces antidepressant and anxiolytic behavioral effects in mice. Brain Res. 2008;1199:148–158. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dunn AL, Trivedi MH, O’Neal HA. Physical activity dose-response effects on outcomes of depression and anxiety. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2001;33:S587–597. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200106001-00027. discussion 609–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Erb S, Shaham Y, Stewart J. Stress reinstates cocaine-seeking behavior after prolonged extinction and a drug-free period. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;128:408–412. doi: 10.1007/s002130050150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fan X, Li D, Lichti CF, Green TA. Dynamic proteomics of nucleus accumbens in response to acute psychological stress in environmentally enriched and isolated rats. PloS one. 2013a;8:e73689. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fan X, Li D, Zhang Y, Green TA. Differential phosphoproteome regulation of nucleus accumbens in environmentally enriched and isolated rats in response to acute stress. PloS one. 2013b;8:e79893. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fediuc S, Campbell JE, Riddell MC. Effect of voluntary wheel running on circadian corticosterone release and on HPA axis responsiveness to restraint stress in Sprague-Dawley rats. Journal of applied physiology (Bethesda, Md.: 1985) 2006;100:1867–1875. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01416.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American journal of preventive medicine. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fox C, Merali Z, Harrison C. Therapeutic and protective effect of environmental enrichment against psychogenic and neurogenic stress. Behav Brain Res. 2006;175:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Friske JE, Gammie SC. Environmental enrichment alters plus maze, but not maternal defense performance in mice. Physiology & behavior. 2005;85:187–194. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gadek-Michalska A, Bugajski J. Repeated handling, restraint, or chronic crowding impair the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical response to acute restraint stress. Journal of physiology and pharmacology: an official journal of the Polish Physiological Society. 2003;54:449–459. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gärtner K, Büttner D, Döhler K, Friedel R, Lindena J, Trautschold I. Stress response of rats to handling and experimental procedures. Laboratory Animals. 1980;14:267–274. doi: 10.1258/002367780780937454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gerra G, Zaimovic A, Mascetti GG, Gardini S, Zambelli U, Timpano M, Raggi MA, Brambilla F. Neuroendocrine responses to experimentally-induced psychological stress in healthy humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:91–107. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00046-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gill KE, Beveridge TJ, Smith HR, Porrino LJ. The effects of rearing environment and chronic methylphenidate administration on behavior and dopamine receptors in adolescent rats. Brain Res. 2013;1527:67–78. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Goeders NE. Stress and cocaine addiction. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics. 2002;301:785–789. doi: 10.1124/jpet.301.3.785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Goeders NE, Guerin GF. Non-contingent electric footshock facilitates the acquisition of intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994;114:63–70. doi: 10.1007/BF02245445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Goeders NE, Guerin GF. Effects of surgical and pharmacological adrenalectomy on the initiation and maintenance of intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Brain research. 1996a;722:145–152. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00206-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Goeders NE, Guerin GF. Role of corticosterone in intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Neuroendocrinology. 1996b;64:337–348. doi: 10.1159/000127137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Graf EN, Wheeler RA, Baker DA, Ebben AL, Hill JE, McReynolds JR, Robble MA, Vranjkovic O, Wheeler DS, Mantsch JR, Gasser PJ. Corticosterone acts in the nucleus accumbens to enhance dopamine signaling and potentiate reinstatement of cocaine seeking. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2013;33:11800–11810. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1969-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Green TA, Alibhai IN, Hommel JD, DiLeone RJ, Kumar A, Theobald DE, Neve RL, Nestler EJ. Induction of inducible cAMP early repressor expression in nucleus accumbens by stress or amphetamine increases behavioral responses to emotional stimuli. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2006;26:8235–8242. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0880-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Green TA, Alibhai IN, Roybal CN, Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Birnbaum SG, Graham AR, Unterberg S, Graham DL, Vialou V, Bass CE, Terwilliger EF, Bardo MT, Nestler EJ. Environmental enrichment produces a behavioral phenotype mediated by low cyclic adenosine monophosphate response element binding (CREB) activity in the nucleus accumbens. Biological psychiatry. 2010;67:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Green TA, Alibhai IN, Unterberg S, Neve RL, Ghose S, Tamminga CA, Nestler EJ. Induction of activating transcription factors (ATFs) ATF2, ATF3, and ATF4 in the nucleus accumbens and their regulation of emotional behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:2025–2032. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5273-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Green TA, Cain ME, Thompson M, Bardo MT. Environmental enrichment decreases nicotine-induced hyperactivity in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;170:235–241. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1538-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Green TA, Gehrke BJ, Bardo MT. Environmental enrichment decreases intravenous amphetamine self-administration in rats: dose-response functions for fixed- and progressive-ratio schedules. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;162:373–378. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1134-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Greenwood BN, Foley TE, Le TV, Strong PV, Loughridge AB, Day HE, Fleshner M. Long-term voluntary wheel running is rewarding and produces plasticity in the mesolimbic reward pathway. Behavioural brain research. 2011;217:354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gregus A, Wintink AJ, Davis AC, Kalynchuk LE. Effect of repeated corticosterone injections and restraint stress on anxiety and depression-like behavior in male rats. Behav Brain Res. 2005;156:105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Grimm JW, Osincup D, Wells B, Manaois M, Fyall A, Buse C, Harkness JH. Environmental enrichment attenuates cue-induced reinstatement of sucrose seeking in rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2008;19:777–785. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32831c3b18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hardeveld F, Spijker J, De Graaf R, Nolen WA, Beekman AT. Recurrence of major depressive disorder and its predictors in the general population: results from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS) Psychological medicine. 2013;43:39–48. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Harlow HF. The nature of love. American psychologist. 1958;13:673. doi: 10.1037/h0029383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Harlow HF, Suomi SJ. Social recovery by isolation-reared monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1971;68:1534–1538. doi: 10.1073/pnas.68.7.1534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Harris RB, Gu H, Mitchell TD, Endale L, Russo M, Ryan DH. Increased glucocorticoid response to a novel stress in rats that have been restrained. Physiology & behavior. 2004;81:557–568. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Hatcher JP, Bell DJ, Reed TJ, Hagan JJ. Chronic mild stress-induced reductions in saccharin intake depend upon feeding status. Journal of psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 1997;11:331–338. doi: 10.1177/026988119701100408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Hattori S, Hashimoto R, Miyakawa T, Yamanaka H, Maeno H, Wada K, Kunugi H. Enriched environments influence depression-related behavior in adult mice and the survival of newborn cells in their hippocampi. Behav Brain Res. 2007;180:69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Heim C, Owens MJ, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. The role of early adverse life events in the etiology of depression and posttraumatic stress disorder. Focus on corticotropin-releasing factor. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1997;821:194–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48279.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Hennessy MM, Weinberg J. Adrenocortical activity during conditions of brief social separation in preweaning rats. Behavioral and neural biology. 1990;54:42–55. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(90)91231-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Hockly E, Cordery PM, Woodman B, Mahal A, van Dellen A, Blakemore C, Lewis CM, Hannan AJ, Bates GP. Environmental enrichment slows disease progression in R6/2 Huntington’s disease mice. Annals of neurology. 2002;51:235–242. doi: 10.1002/ana.10094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Jankowsky JL, Melnikova T, Fadale DJ, Xu GM, Slunt HH, Gonzales V, Younkin LH, Younkin SG, Borchelt DR, Savonenko AV. Environmental enrichment mitigates cognitive deficits in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5217–5224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5080-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Jankowsky JL, Xu G, Fromholt D, Gonzales V, Borchelt DR. Environmental enrichment exacerbates amyloid plaque formation in a transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2003;62:1220–1227. doi: 10.1093/jnen/62.12.1220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Jeong YH, Kim JM, Yoo J, Lee SH, Kim HS, Suh YH. Environmental enrichment compensates for the effects of stress on disease progression in Tg2576 mice, an Alzheimer’s disease model. J Neurochem. 2011;119:1282–1293. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2011.07514.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Jorge E, Fernandez JA, Torres R, Vergara P, Martin MT. Functional changes induced by psychological stress are not enough to cause intestinal inflammation in Sprague-Dawley rats. Neurogastroenterology and motility: the official journal of the European Gastrointestinal Motility Society. 2010;22:e241–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2010.01507.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kehne JH, McCloskey TC, Baron BM, Chi EM, Harrison BL, Whitten JP, Palfreyman MG. NMDA receptor complex antagonists have potential anxiolytic effects as measured with separation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations. European journal of pharmacology. 1991;193:283–292. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(91)90141-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA. Stressful life events and major depression: risk period, long-term contextual threat, and diagnostic specificity. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1998;186:661–669. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199811000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA. Causal Relationship Between Stressful Life Events and the Onset of Major Depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:837. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.6.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of general psychiatry. 2005;62:617–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Khoshaba DM, Maddi SR. Early Experiences in Hardiness Development. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research. 1999;51:106–116. [Google Scholar]
  87. Konkle AT, Kentner AC, Baker SL, Stewart A, Bielajew C. Environmental-Enrichment–Related Variations in Behavioral, Biochemical, and Physiologic Responses of Sprague–Dawley and Long Evans Rats. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science: JAALAS. 2010;49:427. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Koolhaas J, De Boer S, De Rutter A, Meerlo P, Sgoifo A. Social stress in rats and mice. Acta physiologica scandinavica. Supplementum. 1996;640:69–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Kosten TA, Miserendino MJ, Kehoe P. Enhanced acquisition of cocaine self-administration in adult rats with neonatal isolation stress experience. Brain research. 2000;875:44–50. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02595-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Kozorovitskiy Y, Gross CG, Kopil C, Battaglia L, McBreen M, Stranahan AM, Gould E. Experience induces structural and biochemical changes in the adult primate brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:17478–17482. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508817102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Kuhn CM, Pauk J, Schanberg SM. Endocrine responses to mother-infant separation in developing rats. Developmental psychobiology. 1990;23:395–410. doi: 10.1002/dev.420230503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Larsson F, Winblad B, Mohammed AH. Psychological stress and environmental adaptation in enriched vs. impoverished housed rats. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 2002;73:193–207. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00782-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Laviola G, Hannan AJ, Macri S, Solinas M, Jaber M. Effects of enriched environment on animal models of neurodegenerative diseases and psychiatric disorders. Neurobiology of disease. 2008;31:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Leal-Galicia P, Saldívar-González A, Morimoto S, Arias C. Exposure to environmental enrichment elicits differential hippocampal cell proliferation: role of individual responsiveness to anxiety. Dev Neurobiol. 2007;67:395–405. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Lehmann ML, Herkenham M. Environmental enrichment confers stress resiliency to social defeat through an infralimbic cortex-dependent neuroanatomical pathway. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:6159–6173. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0577-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Lepsch L, Gonzalo L, Magro F, Delucia R, Scavone C, Planeta C. Exposure to chronic stress increases the locomotor response to cocaine and the basal levels of corticosterone in adolescent rats. Addiction biology. 2005;10:251–256. doi: 10.1080/13556210500269366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Levi O, Jongen-Relo AL, Feldon J, Roses AD, Michaelson DM. ApoE4 impairs hippocampal plasticity isoform-specifically and blocks the environmental stimulation of synaptogenesis and memory. Neurobiol Dis. 2003;13:273–282. doi: 10.1016/s0969-9961(03)00045-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Levin FR, Bisaga A, Raby W, Aharonovich E, Rubin E, Mariani J, Brooks DJ, Garawi F, Nunes EV. Effects of major depressive disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on the outcome of treatment for cocaine dependence. Journal of substance abuse treatment. 2008;34:80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2006.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Levine S, Huchton DM, Wiener SG, Rosenfeld P. Time course of the effect of maternal deprivation on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the infant rat. Developmental psychobiology. 1991;24:547–558. doi: 10.1002/dev.420240803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Lichti CF, Fan F, English RD, Zhang Y, Li D, Kong F, Sinha M, Andersen CR, Spratt H, Luxon BA, Green TA. Environmental enrichment alters protein expression as well as the proteomic response to cocaine in rat nucleus accumbens. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Lobo MK, Zaman S, Damez-Werno DM, Koo JW, Bagot RC, DiNieri JA, Nugent A, Finkel E, Chaudhury D, Chandra R, Riberio E, Rabkin J, Mouzon E, Cachope R, Cheer JF, Han MH, Dietz DM, Self DW, Hurd YL, Vialou V, Nestler EJ. DeltaFosB induction in striatal medium spiny neuron subtypes in response to chronic pharmacological, emotional, and optogenetic stimuli. J Neurosci. 2013;33:18381–18395. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1875-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Louilot A, Le Moal M, Simon H. Differential reactivity of dopaminergic neurons in the nucleus accumbens in response to different behavioral situations. An in vivo voltammetric study in free moving rats. Brain research. 1986;397:395–400. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90646-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Lyons DM, Parker KJ, Katz M, Schatzberg AF. Developmental cascades linking stress inoculation, arousal regulation, and resilience. Front Behav Neurosci. 2009;3:32. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.032.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Lyons DM, Parker KJ, Schatzberg AF. Animal models of early life stress: Implications for understanding resilience. Developmental psychobiology. 2010;52:402–410. doi: 10.1002/dev.20429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. MacDougall MJ, Howland JG. Acute stress, but not corticosterone, disrupts short- and long-term synaptic plasticity in rat dorsal subiculum via glucocorticoid receptor activation. Cerebral cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991) 2013;23:2611–2619. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Magarinos AM, McEwen BS. Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: comparison of stressors. Neuroscience. 1995;69:83–88. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00256-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Marti O, Gavalda A, Jolin T, Armario A. Effect of regularity of exposure to chronic immobilization stress on the circadian pattern of pituitary adrenal hormones, growth hormone, and thyroid stimulating hormone in the adult male rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1993;18:67–77. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(93)90056-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Matthews K, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW. Repeated maternal separation of preweanling rats attenuates behavioral responses to primary and conditioned incentives in adulthood. Physiology & behavior. 1996;59:99–107. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02069-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. McCormick CM, Kehoe P, Kovacs S. Corticosterone release in response to repeated, short episodes of neonatal isolation: evidence of sensitization. International journal of developmental neuroscience: the official journal of the International Society for Developmental Neuroscience. 1998;16:175–185. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(98)00026-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. McEwen BS. Allostasis and allostatic load: implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:108–124. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00129-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. What is in a name? Integrating homeostasis, allostasis and stress. Hormones and behavior. 2010;57:105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Meichenbaum D. Stress Inoculation Training: A Preventative and Treatment Approach. In: Lehrer PM, Woolfolk RL, Sime WS, editors. Principles and Practice of Stress Management. 3. Guilford Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  113. Miczek KA, Mutschler NH. Activational effects of social stress on IV cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;128:256–264. doi: 10.1007/s002130050133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Mizoguchi K, Yuzurihara M, Ishige A, Sasaki H, Chui DH, Tabira T. Chronic stress differentially regulates glucocorticoid negative feedback response in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:443–459. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00004-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Mlynarik M, Johansson BB, Jezova D. Enriched environment influences adrenocortical response to immune challenge and glutamate receptor gene expression in rat hippocampus. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1018:273–280. doi: 10.1196/annals.1296.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Mobily KE, Rubenstein LM, Lemke JH, Wallace RB. Walking and depression in a cohort of older adults: The Iowa 65+ rural health study. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity. 1996;4:119–135. [Google Scholar]
  117. Moffett MC, Harley J, Francis D, Sanghani SP, Davis WI, Kuhar MJ. Maternal separation and handling affects cocaine self-administration in both the treated pups as adults and the dams. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics. 2006;317:1210–1218. doi: 10.1124/jpet.106.101139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Moldow RL, Fischman AJ. Cocaine induced secretion of ACTH, beta-endorphin, and corticosterone. Peptides. 1987;8:819–822. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(87)90065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Moncek F, Duncko R, Johansson B, Jezova D. Effect of environmental enrichment on stress related systems in rats. Journal of neuroendocrinology. 2004;16:423–431. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2004.01173.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Morris MC, Ciesla JA, Garber J. A prospective study of stress autonomy versus stress sensitization in adolescents at varied risk for depression. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2010;119:341–354. doi: 10.1037/a0019036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Morris MC, Rao U, Garber J. Cortisol responses to psychosocial stress predict depression trajectories: Social-evaluative threat and prior depressive episodes as moderators. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;143:223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Mortimer JT, Staff J. Early work as a source of developmental discontinuity during the transition to adulthood. Development and psychopathology. 2004;16:1047–1070. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Müller MB, Landgraf R, Preil J, Sillaber I, Kresse AE, Keck ME, Zimmermann S, Holsboer F, Wurst W. Selective Activation of the Hypothalamic Vasopressinergic System in Mice Deficient for the Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone Receptor 1 Is Dependent on Glucocorticoids 1. Endocrinology. 2000;141:4262–4269. doi: 10.1210/endo.141.11.7767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Murison R, Hansen AL. Reliability of the chronic mild stress paradigm: implications for research and animal welfare. Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science. 2001;36:266–274. [Google Scholar]
  125. Nader J, Claudia C, El Rawas R, Favot L, Jaber M, Thiriet N, Solinas M. Loss of environmental enrichment increases vulnerability to cocaine addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1579–1587. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Natelson BH, Ottenweller JE, Cook JA, Pitman D, McCarty R, Tapp WN. Effect of stressor intensity on habituation of the adrenocortical stress response. Physiology & behavior. 1988;43:41–46. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90096-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Parker KJ, Buckmaster CL, Schatzberg AF, Lyons DM. Prospective investigation of stress inoculation in young monkeys. Archives of general psychiatry. 2004;61:933–941. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Parker KJ, Maestripieri D. Identifying key features of early stressful experiences that produce stress vulnerability and resilience in primates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:1466–1483. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Pavlovsky AA, Boehning D, Li D, Zhang Y, Fan X, Green TA. Psychological stress, cocaine and natural reward each induce endoplasmic reticulum stress genes in rat brain. Neuroscience. 2013;246:160–169. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.04.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Pena Y, Prunell M, Rotllant D, Armario A, Escorihuela RM. Enduring effects of environmental enrichment from weaning to adulthood on pituitary-adrenal function, pre-pulse inhibition and learning in male and female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1390–1404. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Perrotti LI, Hadeishi Y, Ulery PG, Barrot M, Monteggia L, Duman RS, Nestler EJ. Induction of deltaFosB in reward-related brain structures after chronic stress. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2004;24:10594–10602. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2542-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Pettinati HM, O’Brien CP, Dundon WD. Current status of co-occurring mood and substance use disorders: a new therapeutic target. The American journal of psychiatry. 2013;170:23–30. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Piazza PV, Deminiere JM, le Moal M, Simon H. Stress-and pharmacologically-induced behavioral sensitization increases vulnerability to acquisition of amphetamine self-administration. Brain research. 1990;514:22–26. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90431-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Piazza PV, Le Moal M. The role of stress in drug self-administration. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1998;19:67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0165-6147(97)01115-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Piazza PV, Maccari S, Deminière JM, Le Moal M, Mormède P, Simon H. Corticosterone levels determine individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88:2088–2092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.6.2088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Pohorecky LA. Stress and alcohol interaction: an update of human research. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1991;15:438–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1991.tb00543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Puhl MD, Blum JS, Acosta-Torres S, Grigson PS. Environmental enrichment protects against the acquisition of cocaine self-administration in adult male rats, but does not eliminate avoidance of a drug-associated saccharin cue. Behav Pharmacol. 2012;23:43–53. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32834eb060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Rabasa C, Delgado-Morales R, Munoz-Abellan C, Nadal R, Armario A. Adaptation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and glucose to repeated immobilization or restraint stress is not influenced by associative signals. Behav Brain Res. 2011a;217:232–239. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Rabasa C, Munoz-Abellan C, Daviu N, Nadal R, Armario A. Repeated exposure to immobilization or two different footshock intensities reveals differential adaptation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Physiology & behavior. 2011b;103:125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Ranaldi R, Kest K, Zellner M, Hachimine-Semprebom P. Environmental enrichment, administered after establishment of cocaine self-administration, reduces lever pressing in extinction and during a cocaine context renewal test. Behav Pharmacol. 2011;22:347–353. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283487365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Raz S. Ameliorative effects of brief daily periods of social interaction on isolation-induced behavioral and hormonal alterations. Physiology & behavior. 2013:116–117. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Rebec GV, Grabner CP, Johnson M, Pierce RC, Bardo MT. Transient increases in catecholaminergic activity in medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens shell during novelty. Neuroscience. 1997;76:707–714. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(96)00382-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Renner MJ, Rosenzweig MR. Enriched and Impoverished Environments: Effects on Brain and Behavior. Springer; New York: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  144. Retana-Marquez S, Bonilla-Jaime H, Velazquez-Moctezuma J. Lack of effect of corticosterone administration on male sexual behavior of rats. Physiology & behavior. 1998;63:367–370. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00437-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Rojas Vega S, Strüder HK, Vera Wahrmann B, Schmidt A, Bloch W, Hollmann W. Acute BDNF and cortisol response to low intensity exercise and following ramp incremental exercise to exhaustion in humans. Brain research. 2006;1121:59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Rosenzweig MR. Effects of differential experience on the brain and behavior. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;24:523–540. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2003.9651909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behav Brain Res. 1996;78:57–65. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00216-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Roy V, Belzung C, Delarue C, Chapillon P. Environmental enrichment in BALB/c mice: effects in classical tests of anxiety and exposure to a predatory odor. Physiology & behavior. 2001;74:313–320. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00561-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Rutter M. Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006;1094:1–12. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Seery MD, Leo RJ, Lupien SP, Kondrak CL, Almonte JL. An upside to adversity?: moderate cumulative lifetime adversity is associated with resilient responses in the face of controlled stressors. Psychological science. 2013;24:1181–1189. doi: 10.1177/0956797612469210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Selye H. Stress without distress. In: Serban G, editor. Psychopathology of Human Adaptation. Springer; New York: 1976. pp. 137–146. [Google Scholar]
  152. Shaham Y, Stewart J. Exposure to mild stress enhances the reinforcing efficacy of intravenous heroin self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994;114:523–527. doi: 10.1007/BF02249346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Sharp JL, Zammit TG, Azar TA, Lawson DM. Stress-like responses to common procedures in male rats housed alone or with other rats. Contemporary topics in laboratory animal science/American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2002;41:8–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Shoji H, Mizoguchi K. Acute and repeated stress differentially regulates behavioral, endocrine, neural parameters relevant to emotional and stress response in young and aged rats. Behavioural brain research. 2010;211:169–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Sih A. Effects of early stress on behavioral syndromes: an integrated adaptive perspective. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2011;35:1452–1465. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;158:343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Sinha R, Catapano D, O’Malley S. Stress-induced craving and stress response in cocaine dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999;142:343–351. doi: 10.1007/s002130050898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Sinha R, Garcia M, Paliwal P, Kreek MJ, Rounsaville BJ. Stress-induced cocaine craving and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine relapse outcomes. Archives of general psychiatry. 2006;63:324–331. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Skwara AJ, Karwoski TE, Czambel RK, Rubin RT, Rhodes ME. Influence of environmental enrichment on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) responses to single-dose nicotine, continuous nicotine by osmotic mini-pumps, and nicotine withdrawal by mecamylamine in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;234:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Smith JK, Neill JC, Costall B. Post-weaning housing conditions influence the behavioural effects of cocaine and d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;131:23–33. doi: 10.1007/s002130050261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Smith MA, Iordanou JC, Cohen MB, Cole KT, Gergans SR, Lyle MA, Schmidt KT. Effects of environmental enrichment on sensitivity to cocaine in female rats: importance of control rates of behavior. Behav Pharmacol. 2009;20:312–321. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32832ec568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Solinas M, Chauvet C, Thiriet N, El Rawas R, Jaber M. Reversal of cocaine addiction by environmental enrichment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105:17145–17150. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806889105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Solinas M, Thiriet N, Chauvet C, Jaber M. Prevention and treatment of drug addiction by environmental enrichment. Progress in neurobiology. 2010;92:572–592. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Solinas M, Thiriet N, El Rawas R, Lardeux V, Jaber M. Environmental enrichment during early stages of life reduces the behavioral, neurochemical, and molecular effects of cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1102–1111. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Spires TL, Grote HE, Varshney NK, Cordery PM, van Dellen A, Blakemore C, Hannan AJ. Environmental enrichment rescues protein deficits in a mouse model of Huntington’s disease, indicating a possible disease mechanism. J Neurosci. 2004;24:2270–2276. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1658-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Stairs DJ, Bardo MT. Neurobehavioral effects of environmental enrichment and drug abuse vulnerability. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2009;92:377–382. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Stairs DJ, Klein ED, Bardo MT. Effects of environmental enrichment on extinction and reinstatement of amphetamine self-administration and sucrose-maintained responding. Behavioural pharmacology. 2006;17:597–604. doi: 10.1097/01.fbp.0000236271.72300.0e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Stairs DJ, Prendergast MA, Bardo MT. Environmental-induced differences in corticosterone and glucocorticoid receptor blockade of amphetamine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;218:293–301. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2448-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Swanson HH, van de Poll NE. Effects of an isolated or enriched environment after handling on sexual maturation and behaviour in male and female rats. Journal of reproduction and fertility. 1983;69:165–171. doi: 10.1530/jrf.0.0690165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Takatsu-Coleman AL, Patti CL, Zanin KA, Zager A, Carvalho RC, Borcoi AR, Ceccon LM, Berro LF, Tufik S, Andersen ML, Frussa-Filho R. Short-term social isolation induces depressive-like behaviour and reinstates the retrieval of an aversive task: mood-congruent memory in male mice? Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience: JPN. 2013;38:259–268. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Tennant C. Work-related stress and depressive disorders. Journal of psychosomatic research. 2001;51:697–704. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00255-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Thiel KJ, Pentkowski NS, Peartree NA, Painter MR, Neisewander JL. Environmental living conditions introduced during forced abstinence alter cocaine-seeking behavior and Fos protein expression. Neuroscience. 2010;171:1187–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Thiel KJ, Sanabria F, Pentkowski NS, Neisewander JL. Anti-craving effects of environmental enrichment. The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology/official scientific journal of the Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum (CINP) 2009;12:1151–1156. doi: 10.1017/S1461145709990472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Torres G, Rivier C. Cocaine-induced stimulation of the rat hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis is progressively attenuated following hourly-interval regimens of the drug. Life sciences. 1992;51:1041–1048. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(92)90503-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Urakawa S, Mitsushima D, Shimozuru M, Sakuma Y, Kondo Y. An enriched rearing environment calms adult male rat sexual activity: implication for distinct serotonergic and hormonal responses to females. PloS one. 2014;9:e87911. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Urakawa S, Takamoto K, Hori E, Sakai N, Ono T, Nishijo H. Rearing in enriched environment increases parvalbumin-positive small neurons in the amygdala and decreases anxiety-like behavior of male rats. BMC Neurosci. 2013;14:13. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-14-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. van Dellen A, Blakemore C, Deacon R, York D, Hannan AJ. Delaying the onset of Huntington’s in mice. Nature. 2000;404:721–722. doi: 10.1038/35008142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2000;1:191–198. doi: 10.1038/35044558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Vialou V, Maze I, Renthal W, LaPlant QC, Watts EL, Mouzon E, Ghose S, Tamminga CA, Nestler EJ. Serum response factor promotes resilience to chronic social stress through the induction of DeltaFosB. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2010;30:14585–14592. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2496-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Viau V, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Changes in plasma adrenocorticotropin, corticosterone, corticosteroid-binding globulin, and hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor occupancy/translocation in rat pups in response to stress. Journal of neuroendocrinology. 1996;8:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1996.tb00680.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Wallace DL, Han MH, Graham DL, Green TA, Vialou V, Iniguez SD, Cao JL, Kirk A, Chakravarty S, Kumar A, Krishnan V, Neve RL, Cooper DC, Bolanos CA, Barrot M, McClung CA, Nestler EJ. CREB regulation of nucleus accumbens excitability mediates social isolation-induced behavioral deficits. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12:200–209. doi: 10.1038/nn.2257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Willner P. Validity, reliability and utility of the chronic mild stress model of depression: a 10-year review and evaluation. Psychopharmacology. 1997;134:319–329. doi: 10.1007/s002130050456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Willner P, Muscat R, Papp M. Chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia: a realistic animal model of depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1992;16:525–534. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80194-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Willner P, Towell A, Sampson D, Sophokleous S, Muscat R. Reduction of sucrose preference by chronic unpredictable mild stress, and its restoration by a tricyclic antidepressant. Psychopharmacology. 1987;93:358–364. doi: 10.1007/BF00187257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Wolf SA, Kronenberg G, Lehmann K, Blankenship A, Overall R, Staufenbiel M, Kempermann G. Cognitive and physical activity differently modulate disease progression in the amyloid precursor protein (APP)-23 model of Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1314–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Worley MJ, Trim RS, Roesch SC, Mrnak-Meyer J, Tate SR, Brown SA. Comorbid depression and substance use disorder: longitudinal associations between symptoms in a controlled trial. Journal of substance abuse treatment. 2012;43:291–302. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Wright RL, Conrad CD. Enriched environment prevents chronic stress-induced spatial learning and memory deficits. Behav Brain Res. 2008;187:41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Zakharova E, Starosciak A, Wade D, Izenwasser S. Sex differences in the effects of social and physical environment on novelty-induced exploratory behavior and cocaine-stimulated locomotor activity in adolescent rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;230:92–99. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Zhang Y, Crofton EJ, Li D, Lobo MK, Fan X, Nestler EJ, Green TA. Overexpression of DeltaFosB in nucleus accumbens mimics the protective addiction phenotype, but not the protective depression phenotype of environmental enrichment. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8 doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES