Abstract
Background
Ashkenazi Jewish (AJ) LRRK2 carriers are more likely to manifest the postural instability gait difficulty (PIGD) motor phenotype than non-carriers but perform similarly to non-carriers on cognitive screening tests.
Objective
To compare the cognitive profiles of AJ with Parkinson’s disease (PD) with and without LRRK2 G2019S mutations using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery.
Methods
We administered a neuropsychological battery to PD participants in the Michael J. Fox Foundation AJ consortium. Participants (n=236) from Beth Israel Medical Center, NY, Columbia University Medical Center, NY and Tel Aviv Medical Center, Israel included 116 LRRK2 G2019S carriers and 120 non-carriers. Glucocerbrosidase mutation carriers were excluded. We compared performance on each neuropsychological test between carriers and non-carriers. Participants in New York (n=112) were evaluated with the entire battery. Tel Aviv participants (n=124) were evaluated on attention, executive function and psychomotor speed tasks. The association between G2019S mutation status (predictor) and each neuropsychological test (outcome) was assessed using linear regression models adjusted for PIGD motor phenotype, site, sex, age, disease duration, education, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) Part III, levodopa equivalent dose, and Geriatric Depression Score (GDS).
Results
Carriers had longer disease duration (p<0.001) and were more likely to manifest the PIGD phenotype (p=0.024). In adjusted regression models, carriers performed better than non-carriers in Stroop Word Reading (p<0.001), Stroop Interference (p=0.011) and Category Fluency (p=0.026).
Conclusion
In AJ-PD, G2019S mutation status is associated with better attention (Stroop Word Reading), executive function (Stroop Interference) and language (Category Fluency) after adjustment for PIGD motor phenotype.
Keywords: Parkinson, Genetics, LRRK2, neuropsychological tests
Introduction
Mutations in the Leucine-Rich Repeat Kinase 2 (LRRK2) are the most common known cause of autosomal dominant Parkinson Disease (PD), especially in select ethnic populations. 1 For example, the most common mutation, G2019S, is present in up to 18% of Ashkenazi Jews (AJ) with PD.1-3 To date, no other pathogenic mutations in LRRK2 have been identified in the AJ population. The clinical phenotype of LRRK2 mutation carriers is generally considered to be similar to that of idiopathic PD, but we have shown that AJ LRRK2 carriers are more likely to manifest the postural instability gait difficulty (PIGD) phenotype than non-carriers.4 Among idiopathic PD patients, PIGD phenotype is associated with cognitive impairment,5 but it is unknown whether this association applies to genetic cases of PD (e.g. LRRK2). We have previously reported that in our cohort, PIGD phenotype was associated with worse Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) performance than tremor dominant and intermediate phenotype cases among participants with idiopathic PD but not among participants who are LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers.6 In addition, LRRK2 carriers (who are more likely to manifest the PIGD phenotype) performed similarly to non-carriers on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) 4 and on the MoCA.6 In order to further assess cognitive performance among PD patients who are AJ LRRK2 G2019S carriers and non-carriers and explore the association between mutation status, PIGD phenotype and neuropsychological performance, we evaluated 236 participants in the AJ-LRRK2 consortium from Tel Aviv, Israel, and New York with a neuropsychological battery. Given PIGD phenotype, we hypothesized that LRRK2 mutation carriers would perform more poorly than non-carriers on neuropsychological testing.
Methods and Participants
Participants
As previously reported, AJ-PD participants were recruited at three sites, Beth Israel Medical Center (BIMC), Columbia University Medical Center (CUMC), both in New York, and Tel-Aviv Medical Center (TLVMC), in Tel Aviv, Israel.6 Participants were diagnosed with PD by a movement disorder specialist, based on the UK PD brain bank criteria (except that we did not exclude cases with a family history of PD).7 Participants were genotyped for LRRK2 G2019S and glucocerebrosidase (GBA) mutations.6 Carriers of GBA mutations were excluded . A subset of non-carriers (n=120) were selected to enroll in an in-depth clinical evaluation based on willingness and convenience of location. In the current study, G2019S carriers and non-carriers were enrolled in an in-depth clinical evaluation, which included neuropsychological and psychiatric assessments.. Participants (n= 236, including 64 from BIMC, 48 from CUMC and 124 from TLVMC) included 116 LRRK2 G2019S carriers and 120 non-carriers. Cognitive tests administered to 66 participants from TLVMC, including 50 G2019S carriers, were previously described.8 Institutional review boards at all participating sites approved the protocols and all participants signed consent forms prior to participating in any research procedures.
Clinical and Neuropsychological Evaluation
The clinical evaluation included a demographic and family history interview, the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS),9 the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI II), and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Postural Instability Gait Difficulty (PIGD) phenotype was computed as previously reported4, 10 for 229 (of 236) participants with available UPDRS-II and UPDRS-III data. A mean score of 8 tremor items on the UPDRS was computed (self-report of tremor, chin tremor, right and left arm tremor, right and left leg tremor and right and left arm action tremor), as well as a mean score of 5 PIGD items on the UPDRS (self-report of falling, freezing and walking difficulty, and gait and postural instability on exam). A ratio between the two was then computed. PIGD was defined as ratio ≥ 1.5.10-13
The neuropsychological battery included assessments of attention (Stroop Word Reading, Digit Span) , memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test), executive function (Stroop Interference (raw scores)), visuospatial function (Judgment of Line Orientation ), language (Category Fluency, Verbal Fluency (FAS) and psychomotor speed (Finger Tapping, dominant hand Finger Tapping, non-dominant hand ).14-17 Participants in TLVMC were not evaluated for memory or visuospatial function.
Statistical Analysis
Demographic information, clinical characteristics, motor phenotype (PIGD versus not PIGD, including the tremor dominant phenotype and the intermediate phenotype), UPDRS-III, MoCA, BDI-II, GDS score and neuropsychological test performance were compared between carriers and non-carriers using Fisher’s exact test, chi-square tests, and Student T tests as appropriate. Linear regression models were constructed to test the association between the presence or absence of the G2019S mutation (predictor) and the MoCA and each cognitive test score (outcome), separately. Models were adjusted for recruitment-site, age, sex, years of education, duration of PD, UPDRS motor score (UPDRS-III), motor phenotype (PIGD versus not PIGD), levodopa equivalent daily dose and depression (GDS score). Analyses were performed on the entire group and repeated, stratified by recruitment sites (two New York sites versus Israel).
Results
Demographics and disease characteristics of LRRK2 G2019S carriers and non-carriers are presented in Table 1. Carriers had longer disease duration (9.4 years versus 6.3 years, p<0.001), were treated with higher levodopa equivalent daily doses (389mg/day versus 220mg/day, p<0.001), and were more likely to manifest the PIGD phenotype (p=0.024) but had similar UPDRS-III scores.
Table 1.
Demographic and disease characteristics of PD participants with and without LRRK2 G2019S mutation
Non carriers (n=120) |
Carriers (n=116) |
p-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Male sex (%) | 76 (63.3%) | 64 (55.2%) | 0.233 |
Age, yrs (SD) | 64.5 (10.8) | 66.7 (10.0) | 0.109 |
Age-at-onset, yrs (SD) | 58.2 (10.8) | 57.2 (10.8) | 0.506 |
Duration, yrs (SD) | 6.3 (5.4) | 9.5 (6.6) | <0.001 |
Education, yrs (SD) | 16.3 (3.3) | 15.9 (3.3) | 0.313 |
Levodopa equivalent daily dose, mg (SD) |
220 (288) | 389 (402) | <0.001 |
Total UPDRS-III score (SD) | 19.5 (11.1) | 21.4 (12.2) | 0.219 |
PIGD phenotype1 | 60 (50.8%) | 73 (65.8%) | 0.024 |
GDS (SD) | 3.2 (3.2) | 3.7 (3.3) | 0.242 |
BDI II (SD)2 | 8.1 (7.3) | 9.6 (7.0) | 0.190 |
MoCA (SD) | 25.0 (3.4) | 25.4 (2.7) | 0.361 |
UPDRS-III: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, motor score III; PIGD: postural instability motor difficulty; GDS: Geriatric Depression Scale; BDI-II: Beck Depression Inventory-II; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment
Motor phenotype was available for 129 participants; UPDRS-II (which is required for phenotype computation) was not available for five carriers and two non-carriers
BDI-II scores were available from 165 participants including 78 carriers and 87 non-carriers
Performance on the individual neuropsychological tests is presented in Table 2. Carriers performed better than non-carriers on Stroop Word Reading and Category (animal) Fluency. In separate linear regression models adjusted for recruitment site, sex, age, education, PD duration, UPDRS-III, PIGD motor phenotype, levodopa equivalent daily dose and GDS scores, carriers performed better than non-carriers in Stroop Word Reading (p<0.001), Stroop Interference (p=0.011) and Category Fluency (p=0.026). Performance on the MoCA and in memory, visuospatial and psychomotor speed tasks was similar between carriers and non-carriers.
Table 2.
Performance of LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers and non-carriers on neuropsychological testing:
Test | LRRK2 carriers |
LRRK2
Non-carriers |
P
value |
Adjusted
P value1 |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Performance | N | Performance | |||
Attention | ||||||
Stroop Word Reading (raw scores)2
Range |
107 | 92.2 (17.8) 41-135 |
110 | 86.0 (20.5) 26-140 |
0.019 | <0.001 |
Digit Span Range |
97 | 7.5 (1.9) 4-14 |
116 | 7.2 (1.9) 4-14 |
0.262 | 0.225 |
Memory3 | ||||||
Hopkins Verbal Learning-Free Recall (Trials 1-3) Range |
57 | 22.9 (6.0) 12-35 |
54 | 20.7 (5.6) 7-30 |
0.052 | 0.201 |
Hopkins Verbal Learning-Free Recall (Delayed) Range |
57 | 7.3 (3.1) 0-12 |
53 | 6.3 (3.1) 0-12 |
0.071 | 0.237 |
Hopkins Verbal Learning-Recognition Range |
57 | 10.7 (1.5) 6-12 |
53 | 10.4 (1.6) 6-12 |
0.426 | 0.390 |
Visuospatial Function2 | ||||||
Judgment of Line Orientation Total raw score Range |
58 | 12.0 (2.3) 6-16 |
54 | 11.5 (2.9) 3-16 |
0.380 | 0.388 |
Executive Function | ||||||
Stroop Interference raw score2
Range |
100 | 28.1 (18.2) −18 – 65 |
107 | 26.9 (14.8) −7- 64 |
0.590 | 0.011 |
Psychomotor Speed | ||||||
Finger Tapping, dominant hand Range |
110 | 38.3 (12.1) 13.6-60.3 |
108 | 38.5 (13.1) 7.0-67.5 |
0.885 | 0.600 |
Finger Tapping, non-dominant hand Range |
108 | 34.3 (11.8) 6.8-62.9 |
106 | 36.6 (11.7) 8.4-63.6 |
0.164 | 0.764 |
Verbal Fluency | ||||||
Letter (FAS) Fluency Total raw score Range |
116 | 41.9 (17.0) 6-85 |
120 | 39.0 (17.5) 4-82 |
0.197 | 0.213 |
Category (animal) Fluency Total raw score Range |
115 | 19.6 (5.9) 5-34 |
120 | 18.0 (6.5) 0-34 |
0.045 | 0.026 |
P value is obtained from linear regression models where each cognitive test performance is the outcome and G2019S mutation status is the predictor, adjusted for recruitment site, sex, age, education, PD duration, UPDRS-III, motor phenotype, levodopa equivalent daily dose and GDS scores
Stroop scores were missing on 29 participants including 9 color blind individuals
Scores on memory and visuospatial tests were available from 112 New York participants
Discussion
In the current study we found that AJ PD patients who are LRRK2 G2019S carriers perform better than non-carriers on attention and executive tasks in models adjusted for demographic and disease characteristics. As previously reported, carriers were more likely to manifest the PIGD motor phenotype, and had longer disease duration.
Our findings of better cognitive function in LRRK2 carriers than non-carriers are surprising considering that LRRK2 G2019S carriers in our cohort were more likely to manifest the PIGD phenotype. In previous reports on participants from this cohort8 and in an early-onset PD cohort4 we found that LRRK2 G2019S PD cases demonstrate more gait impairment than idiopathic PD, and are more likely to manifest the PIGD phenotype. While gait disturbance and PIGD phenotype are associated with worse cognitive performance in idiopathic PD,5 we and others have not observed differential performance between carriers and non-carriers on basic cognitive screens, including the MMSE,18-20 the MoCA6, 20 and neuropsychological batteries.20-24 Here, we found better attention and executive and language performance in carriers with PD when compared to non-carriers with PD only when all three sites were included, but found no differences in memory, visuospatial performance or processing speed. We cannot conclude whether the findings of differential performance in attention and executive functioning but not in memory and visuospatial processing are a result of a differential cognitive pattern in PD LRRK2 G2019S carriers versus non-carriers, or because of our reduced power due to missing data from the Tel Aviv site. It remains unknown why AJ LRRK2 carriers perform better than non-carriers in the setting of worse gait performance. FMRI studies of LRRK2 G2019S carriers without PD demonstrate altered connectivity during motor and cognitive tasks despite normal performance.25, 26 These findings suggest underlying compensatory mechanisms that precede overt clinical signs. It is possible that these compensatory mechanisms protect mutation carriers from executive and attention dysfunction, which is often present in PD.
Cognitive impairment is among the most debilitating non-motor symptom of PD and cognitive functioning is highly variable among PD patients. Autopsies of PD patients with dementia often demonstrate diffuse Lewy body disease and/or Alzheimer’s changes.27 Established predictors of cognitive impairment in PD include increased severity of motor symptoms,28 PIGD phenotype5 (and probably more specifically, freezing of gait),29 depression,28 older age30 and longer disease duration.30 We and others have shown that a patient’s genetic profile may also be associated with cognitive performance, which tends to be worse in the case of GBA mutations,17 and better in the case of PARKIN mutations.31 The proposed mechanism of differential cognitive performance in these cases is the abundant infiltration of Lewy bodies in the cortex in the case of GBA, and the lack of cortical Lewy body (or other neuropathology) in PARKIN mutation carriers.32 This is the first study that demonstrates better cognitive performance in attention, executive and language tests in LRRK2 G2019S carriers compared to non-carriers. The pathology associated with better cognitive functioning among LRRK2 carriers is unknown. Autopsies of LRRK2 mutation carriers show marked neuropathological heterogeneity, including Lewy body pathology (restricted to brain stem structures or infiltrating the cortex), tau pathology and, rarely, concomitant Alzheimer’s pathology.32, 33 Additional studies are required to test whether there is less cortical Lewy body involvement among LRRK2 G2019S carriers with PD than in non-carriers to account for better cognitive performance.
The major strengths of our study are the homogenous ethnic group, the large sample size and the comprehensive neuropsychological battery employed. We were able to adjust for potential confounders by excluding all GBA mutation carriers (other genes linked to PD, including SNCA, PARK2 and PINK1 are very rarely present in the AJ population)34 and by adjusting for disease duration, motor severity, medications, education and depression (GDS scores) in regression models. Our most significant limitation is the study’s cross-sectional design, which does not allow comparison of cognitive performance longitudinally and disease progression. An additional significant limitation of our study are the missing data on memory and visuospatial performance from the Tel Aviv site, which consists of more than half of the participants in our cohort.
Given the importance of age and disease duration on cognition in PD, longitudinal follow-up is required to compare cognitive outcomes between carriers and non-carriers. The biological mechanisms that underlie the relative protection of LRRK2 G2019S carriers from attention and executive impairment should be explored. Additional LRRK2 G2019S autopsy collection may help clarify the neuropathological basis of cognitive performance in LRRK2 G2019S carriers.
Highlights.
LRRK2-PD patients perform better than iPD on neuropsychological tests, including executive, attention and language tasks.
LRRK2-related PD patient perform similarly to iPD on the MoCA.
Ashkenazi Jewish patients with LRRK2-PD have less tremor and more gait difficulty.
Acknowledgements
LRRK2 Ashkenazi Jewish Consortium:
Columbia University, NY: Roy N. Alcalay MD MS, Ming-X Tang PhD, Helen Mejia Santana MS, Ernest Roos MD, Martha Orbe-Reilly MD, Stanley Fahn MD, Lucien Cote MD, Cheryl Waters MD, Pietro Mazzoni MD PhD, Blair Ford MD,Elan Louis MD MSc, Oren Levy MD PhD, Llency Rosado MD, Diana Ruiz BSc,Tsvyatko Dorovski MS MBA, Paul Greene MD Lorraine Clark PhD, Karen S Marder MD MPH
Tel Aviv Medical Center, Tel Aviv, Israel: Tanya Gurevich MD, Anat Bar Shira PhD, Mali Gana Weisz PhD, Kira Yasinovsky, Maayan Zalis DMD, Avner Thaler MD, Yaacov Balash MD, Shabtai Hertzel MD, Ziv Gan Or PhD, Hila Kobo MSc, Ariella Hillel RN and Anat Shkedy, MD, Avi Orr-Urtreger MD PhD, Nir Giladi MD
Department of Neurology, Beth Israel Medical Center, New York, New York, USA: Andres Deik MD,Matthew James Barrett MD MS, Jose Cabassa MD, Mark Groves MD, Ann L. Hunt DO, Naomi Lubarr MD, Marta San Luciano MD MS, Joan Miravite NP, Christina Palmese PhD, Rivka Sachdev MD, Harini Sarva MD, Lawrence Severt MD PhD, Vicki Shanker MD, Matthew Carrington Swan MD, Jeannie Soto-Valencia BA, Brooke Johannes MS, Robert Ortega MS, Laurie Ozelius PhD, Rachel Saunders-Pullman MD MPH, Deborah Raymond MS, Susan Bressman MD
Funding sources: This study was funded by the Michael J Fox Foundation and the National Institute of Health (through Grant Numbers R56NS036630, K02NS080915, NS050487, NS060113 and UL1 TR000040, formerly the National Center for Research Resources, Grant Number UL1 RR024156 and NINDS 10628097).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Statistical analyses were performed by Roy Alcalay
Conflict of Interest: No conflicts of interests to report with relevance to this publication.
Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Helen Mejia Santana MS, Anat Mirelman PhD, Deborah Raymond MS, Elise Caccappolo PhD, Christina Palmese PhD, Avi Orr-Urtreger MD PhD, Nir Giladi MD report nothing to disclose.
Dr. Alcalay receives research support from the NIH (K02NS080915), the Parkinson’s Disease Foundation, the Smart Foundation and the Michael J. Fox Foundation.
Dr. Saunders-Pullman serves on the Scientific Advisory Board of the Dystonia Medical Research Foundation. She receives research support from the NIH (K02 NS073836), the Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research, the Bachmann-Strauss Dystonia and Parkinson’s Foundation, and the Marcled Foundation.
Dr. Ozelius receives salary support from NIH [NS037409, NS075881, DC011805]. She is a current member of the scientific advisory boards of the National Spasmodic Dysphonia Association, the Benign Essential Blepharospasm Research Foundation and Tourette Syndrome Association, Inc. Dr. Ozelius receives royalty payments from Athena Diagnostics related to patents.
Dr. Clark receives research support from the NIH [NINDS #R01 NS060113 (principal investigator), NINDS #R01 NS073872 (Co-principal investigator), NIA #5P50AG008702 (Project 3, principal investigator), and NINDS #NS36630 (co-investigator) and 2P50NS03837011 (co-investigator)], the Parkinson's Disease Foundation (principal investigator) and the Michael J Fox Foundation (co-investigator).
Dr. Bressman serves on the advisory boards of the Michael J. Fox Foundation, the Dystonia Medical Research Foundation, the Bachmann Strauss Dystonia and Parkinson’s Foundation, and the Board of We Move. She has consulted for Bristol Meyer Squibb. She has received research support from the Michael J. Fox Foundation, National Institutes of Health (NIH), and Dystonia Medical Research Foundation.
Dr. Marder receives research support from the NIH [#NS036630 (PI), 1UL1 RR024156-01 (Director PCIR), PO412196-G (Co-I), and PO412196-G (Co-I)]. She received compensation for participating on the steering committee for U01NS052592 and from the Parkinson Disease Foundation, Huntington's Disease Society of America, the Parkinson Study Group, CHDI, and the Michael J. Fox Foundation.
Reference
- 1.Healy DG, Falchi M, O'Sullivan SS, Bonifati V, Durr A, Bressman S, et al. Phenotype, genotype, and worldwide genetic penetrance of LRRK2-associated Parkinson's disease: a case-control study. Lancet Neurol. 2008;7:583–590. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70117-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ozelius LJ, Senthil G, Saunders-Pullman R, Ohmann E, Deligtisch A, Tagliati M, et al. LRRK2 G2019S as a cause of Parkinson's disease in Ashkenazi Jews. N Engl J Med. 2006;354:424–425. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc055509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Clark LN, Wang Y, Karlins E, Saito L, Mejia-Santana H, Harris J, et al. Frequency of LRRK2 mutations in early- and late-onset Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2006;67:1786–1791. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000244345.49809.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Alcalay RN, Mejia-Santana H, Tang MX, Rosado L, Verbitsky M, Kisselev S, et al. Motor phenotype of LRRK2 G2019S carriers in early-onset Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:1517–1522. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Alves G, Larsen JP, Emre M, Wentzel-Larsen T, Aarsland D. Changes in motor subtype and risk for incident dementia in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2006;21:1123–1130. doi: 10.1002/mds.20897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Alcalay RN, Mirelman A, Saunders-Pullman R, Tang MX, Mejia Santana H, Raymond D, et al. Parkinson disease phenotype in Ashkenazi jews with and without LRRK2 G2019S mutations. Mov Disord. 2013;28(14):1966–71. doi: 10.1002/mds.25647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55:181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mirelman A, Heman T, Yasinovsky K, Thaler A, Gurevich T, Marder K, et al. Fall risk and gait in Parkinson's disease: The role of the LRRK2 G2019S mutation. Mov Disord. 2013;28:1683–1690. doi: 10.1002/mds.25587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Fahn S, Elton R. Members of the UPDRS Development Committee. The Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne DB, Goldstein M, editors. Recent Developments in Parkinson’s Disease. Vol. 2. Macmillan Healthcare Information; Florham Park, NJ: 1987. pp. 153–163. 293-304. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jankovic J, McDermott M, Carter J, Gauthier S, Goetz C, Golbe L, et al. Variable expression of Parkinson's disease: a base-line analysis of the DATATOP cohort. The Parkinson Study Group. Neurology. 1990;40:1529–1534. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.10.1529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mito Y, Yoshida K, Yabe I, Makino K, Tashiro K, Kikuchi S, et al. Brain SPECT analysis by 3D-SSP and phenotype of Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006;241:67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2005.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Burn DJ, Rowan EN, Allan LM, Molloy S, O'Brien JT, McKeith IG. Motor subtype and cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease, Parkinson's disease with dementia, and dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77:585–589. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.081711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Taylor JP, Rowan EN, Lett D, O'Brien JT, McKeith IG, Burn DJ. Poor attentional function predicts cognitive decline in patients with non-demented Parkinson's disease independent of motor phenotype. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79:1318–1323. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.147629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, De Haan RJ, Schmand B. Cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease: a prospective longitudinal study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15:426–437. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B. Cognitive profile of patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;65:1239–1245. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180516.69442.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Caccappolo E, Alcalay RN, Mejia-Santana H, Tang MX, Rakitin B, Rosado L, et al. Neuropsychological Profile of Parkin Mutation Carriers with and without Parkinson Disease: The CORE-PD Study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:91–100. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Alcalay RN, Caccappolo E, Mejia-Santana H, Tang MX, Rosado L, Orbe Reilly M, et al. Cognitive performance of GBA mutation carriers with early-onset PD: the CORE-PD study. Neurology. 2012;78:1434–1440. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318253d54b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Alcalay RN, Mejia-Santana H, Tang MX, Rakitin B, Rosado L, Ross B, et al. Self-report of cognitive impairment and mini-mental state examination performance in PRKN, LRRK2, and GBA carriers with early onset Parkinson's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;32:775–779. doi: 10.1080/13803390903521018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Aasly JO, Toft M, Fernandez-Mata I, Kachergus J, Hulihan M, White L, et al. Clinical features of LRRK2-associated Parkinson's disease in central Norway. Ann Neurol. 2005;57:762–765. doi: 10.1002/ana.20456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ben Sassi S, Nabli F, Hentati E, Nahdi H, Trabelsi M, Ben Ayed H, et al. Cognitive dysfunction in Tunisian LRRK2 associated Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2012;18:243–246. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Belarbi S, Hecham N, Lesage S, Kediha M, Smail N, Benhassine T, et al. LRRK2 G2019S mutation in Parkinson's disease: a neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric study in a large Algerian cohort. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2010;16:676–679. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shanker V, Groves M, Heiman G, Palmese C, Saunders-Pullman R, Ozerlius L, et al. Mood and cognition in leucine-rich repeat kinase 2 G2019S Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2011;26:1875–1880. doi: 10.1002/mds.23746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lohmann E, Leclere L, De Anna F, Lesage S, Dubois B, Agid Y, et al. A clinical, neuropsychological and olfactory evaluation of a large family with LRRK2 mutations. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009;15:273–276. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Goldwurm S, Zini M, Di Fonzo A, De Gaspari D, Siri C, Simons EJ, et al. LRRK2 G2019S mutation and Parkinson's disease: a clinical, neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric study in a large Italian sample. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2006;12:410–419. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Thaler A, Mirelman A, Helmich RC, van Nuenen BF, Rosenberg-Katz K, Gurevich T, et al. Neural correlates of executive functions in healthy G2019S LRRK2 mutation carriers. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior. 2013;49:2501–2511. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.van Nuenen BF, Helmich RC, Ferraye M, Thaler A, Hendler T, Orr-Urtreger A, et al. Cerebral pathological and compensatory mechanisms in the premotor phase of leucine-rich repeat kinase 2 parkinsonism. Brain. 2012;135:3687–3698. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Irwin DJ, White MT, Toledo JB, Xie SX, Robinson JL, Van Deerlin V, et al. Neuropathologic substrates of Parkinson disease dementia. Ann Neurol. 2012;72:587–598. doi: 10.1002/ana.23659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Marder K, Tang MX, Cote L, Stern Y, Mayeux R. The frequency and associated risk factors for dementia in patients with Parkinson's disease. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:695–701. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1995.00540310069018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Factor SA, Steenland NK, Higgins DS, Molho ES, Kay DM, Montimurro J, et al. Postural instability/gait disturbance in Parkinson's disease has distinct subtypes: an exploratory analysis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011;82:564–568. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2010.222042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Aarsland D, Kvaloy JT, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Tang MX, Lolk A, et al. The effect of age of onset of PD on risk of dementia. J Neurol. 2007;254:38–45. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-0234-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Alcalay RN, Caccappolo E, Mejia-Santana H, Tang MX, Rosado L, Orbe Reilly M, et al. Cognitive and Motor Function in Long-Duration PARKIN-Associated Parkinson Disease. JAMA Neurology. 2014;71(1):62–7. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.4498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Poulopoulos M, Levy OA, Alcalay RN. The neuropathology of genetic Parkinson's disease. Movement Disord. 2012;27:831–842. doi: 10.1002/mds.24962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Poulopoulos M, Cortes E, Vonsattel JPG, Fahn S, Waters C, Cote L, et al. Clinical and Pathological Characteristics of LRRK2 G2019S Patients with PD. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience. 2012;47:139–143. doi: 10.1007/s12031-011-9696-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Alcalay RN, Caccappolo E, Mejia-Santana H, Tang MX, Rosado L, Ross BM, et al. Frequency of known mutations in early-onset Parkinson disease: implication for genetic counseling: the consortium on risk for early onset Parkinson disease study. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:1116–1122. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]