Abstract
Repeated exposure to stressful events across the lifespan, referred to as cumulative adversity, is a potent risk factor for depression. Research indicates that cumulative adversity detrimentally affects emotion regulation processes, which may represent a pathway linking cumulative adversity to vulnerability to depression. However, empirical evidence that emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between cumulative adversity and depression is limited, particularly in adult populations. We examined the direct and indirect effects of cumulative adversity on depressive symptomatology in a large community sample of adults (n = 745) who were further characterized by risk status: never-depressed (n = 638) and “at-risk” remitted mood-disordered (n = 107). All participants completed the Cumulative Adversity Inventory (CAI), the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS), and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). Bootstrapped confidence intervals were computed to estimate the indirect effect of emotion dysregulation on the relationship between cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology and to test whether this indirect effect was moderated by risk status. Emotion dysregulation partially and significantly mediated the relationship between cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology independent of risk status. Overall, cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation accounted for 50% of the variance in depressive symptomatology. These findings support the hypothesis that disruption of adaptive emotion regulation processes associated with repeated exposure to stressful life events represents an intrapersonal mechanism linking the experience of adverse events to depression. Our results support the utility of interventions that simultaneously emphasize stress reduction and emotion regulation to treat and prevent depressive vulnerability and pathology.
Keywords: Depression, emotion regulation, cumulative stress, adversity, mediation
Lifetime cumulative adversity, defined as the joint effect of repeated exposure to stressful events across the lifespan, has consistently been associated with incrementally worse health outcomes, including increased prevalence of psychiatric symptoms and increased vulnerability to psychiatric disorders (e.g. Benjet et al., 2010; Mabunda and Idemudia, 2012; Turner and Lloyd, 1995, 2004; Shrira, 2012). These findings are consistent with longitudinal investigations that have documented an association between exposure to stressful life events and depressive symptomatology (Caspi et al., 2003; Hammen, 2005; Kessler, 1997; Turner and Lloyd, 1995). Notably, however, lifetime cumulative adversity has been shown to provide a more robust account of variability in depressive symptomatology than more constrained measures of stress exposure, such as those that limit the exposure timeframe (typically to the past year or the past six months) or that are restricted to a single event or event category, such as parental neglect (Turner and Lloyd, 1995; Turner et al., 1995). The identification of mechanisms linking adversity to poor health outcomes is essential for the development of preventive interventions; however, these mechanisms remain inadequately understood.
Emotion regulation has been conceptualized as a multimodal process through which individuals consciously and nonconsciously modulate their emotions (Bargh and Williams, 2007; Rottenberg and Gross, 2003) to respond to environmental demands, including stressful events (Campbell-Sills and Barlow, 2007; Gratz and Roemer, 2004; Gross, 1998; Thompson, 1994). Emotion regulation has been identified as a central component in a wide range of psychiatric symptoms and disorders (Aldao et al., 2010; Bradley et al., 2011) and is increasingly regarded as a key mechanism of change in therapeutic contexts (Fairholme et al., 2009; Gratz and Tull, 2010; Mennin and Fresco, 2009). Depression, which has been conceptualized as a “distress disorder,” (Watson, 2005) is widely regarded as a consequence of affective dysregulation (Campbell-Sills and Barlow, 2007; Gross and Munoz, 1995; Mennin et al., 2007). The multiple linkages between stress exposure, emotion dysregulation, and depression suggest that alterations to emotion regulation processes may represent a pathway linking cumulative adversity to vulnerability to depression (Repetti et al., 2002).
There is increasing evidence that exposure to acute and chronic stress reduces emotion regulation capacity (Dvir et al., 2014; Kim, 2013; McEwen, 2004, Sinha, 2001). Multiple studies have documented the profound neurobiological consequences of repeated exposure to stress on prefrontal and limbic-striatal functioning involved in the processing and regulation of emotions (Ansell et al., 2012; Davidson et al., 2002; Seo et al., 2014). There is evidence that efficient use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies may protect against the adverse consequences of stress (Hopp et al., 2011; Ochsner and Gross, 2005; Shallcross et al., 2010; van der Veek et al., 2009); however, a recent study demonstrating that acute stress provocation markedly impairs the efficacy of cognitive emotion regulation (Raio et al., 2013) suggests that emotion regulation capacity may be actively impaired during stressful experiences, which may, in turn, enhance psychiatric vulnerability.
To date, only a relative handful of studies have specifically examined emotion dysregulation as a potential mediator of the stress-psychopathology association, but these studies have consistently supported the hypothesis that emotion dysregulation mediates the stress-psychopathology relationship (e.g. Coates and Moore, 2014; Crow et al., 2014; Goldsmith et al., 2013; McLaughlin and Hatzenbuehler, 2009; Moriya and Takahashi, 2013; Stevens et al., 2013). For example, McLaughlin and Hatzenbuehler (2009) reported that emotion dysregulation mediated the relationship between recent stressful life events and changes in internalizing symptomatology over time in a short-term longitudinal study of adolescents. However, the studies that have been conducted to date have been consistently limited by the use of questionnaire-based assessments to measure participants’ exposure to and appraisal of stressful events. Research indicates that questionnaire-based assessments are less accurate and more subject to bias than interview-based measures and that individuals’ subjective perceptions of distress are likely to be confounded with the symptomatology of depression (Hammen, 2005). In addition, virtually all of the studies that have been conducted to date have been restricted to adolescent populations or have focused exclusively on early life adversity (Grant et al., 2006).
This overriding emphasis in the extant literature on early life adversity may be shortsighted in light of the durable and cumulative effects of traumatic stress (Turner and Lloyd, 1995), stress-related neural plasticity in the mature brain (McEwen, 2012), and evidence that stress accumulation may accelerate biological aging processes leading to increased stress-related vulnerability in geriatric populations (Shrira, 2012). Shrira (2012) reported that lifetime cumulative adversity was related to continuous vulnerability to depressive symptoms, as well as increased risk of mental health deterioration over time, in a longitudinal sample of elderly adults. To our knowledge, no prior studies have tested the hypothesis that emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between lifetime cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology in early and middle adulthood.
The present analysis deployed cross-sectional data from a large community sample of never-depressed and “at-risk” remitted mood disordered adults who completed the Cumulative Adversity Inventory (CAI; Turner et al., 1995), the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz and Roemer, 2004), and the Center for Epidemologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). Although the sample did not include currently depressed individuals, thereby limiting the generalizability of the proposed mediation model to clinical populations, prior research indicates substantial continuity between subthreshold depressive symptomatology and syndromal depression (Enns et al., 2001; Flett et al., 1997; Solomon et al., 2001). Furthermore, subthreshold depression constitutes a major risk factor for the development of syndromal depression and is associated with significant functional impairment (Cuijpers and Smit, 2004; Cuijpers et al., 2007; Kessler et al., 1997; Lewinsohn et al., 2000; Rodriguez et al., 2012).
We hypothesized first (1) that cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation would be independently associated with depressive symptomatology in both the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts; second (2) that the indirect effect of emotion dysregulation would significantly mediate the association between cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology; and third (3) that the strength of the risk pathways would be significantly stronger in the at-risk cohort than in the never-depressed cohort. The latter two hypotheses were tested simultaneously in a moderated mediation model that incorporated both the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts. In addition, secondary mediation analyses and pairwise contrasts were conducted to determine the independent contributions of the six emotion dysregulation subscales.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Six hundred and thirty-eight never-depressed and one hundred and seven “at-risk” remitted mood disordered individuals were recruited from a community sample in and around the New Haven, CT area. Demographics for these samples are reported in Table 1. No significant differences between the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts were detected in racial-ethnic composition, χ2(2, n = 745) = 5.85, p = .054 or marital status, χ2(2, n = 745) = 2.45 p = .29. However, statistically significant differences were detected in gender, χ2(1, n = 745) = 8.14, p = .01; age t(743) = −2.43, p = .02; and years of education t(743) = −1.96, p = .050. On average, the at-risk cohort scored significantly higher than the never-depressed cohort on measures of cumulative adversity, t(743) = −4.689, p < .001; emotion dysregulation, t(743) = −5.89, p < .001; and depressive symptomatology, t(135.32) = −5.90, p < .001. Notably, the mean CES-D score in the at-risk cohort was approximately 16, which is the clinical cut-off for depression risk (Radloff, 1977; Weissmann et al., 1977), whereas the mean CES-D score in the never-depressed cohort fell below this threshold.
Table 1.
Never-Depressed (n = 638) | At-Risk (n = 107) | |
---|---|---|
Age | ||
Mean (SD) | 29.5 (9.24) | 31.3 (9.49) |
Median | 26 | 29 |
Range | 18–50 | 18–50 |
Years of Education (SD) | 15.18 (2.29) | 15.64 (2.30) |
Gender, n (%) | ||
Male | 280 (43.9) | 34 (31.8) |
Female | 358 (56.1) | 73 (68.2) |
Race-Ethnicity, n (%) | ||
Non-Hispanic White | 455 (71.3) | 86 (80.3) |
Non-Hispanic Black | 135 (21.2) | 16 (15.0) |
Other | 48 (7.5) | 5 (4.7) |
Marital Status, n (%) | ||
Never Married | 452 (70.8) | 70 (65.4) |
Married | 109 (17.1) | 22 (20.6) |
Divorced, Separated, or Widowed | 77 (12.1) | 15 (14.0) |
Adverse Life Events (SD) | 10.34 (6.23) | 12.73 (6.36) |
Total Emotion Dysregulation (SD) | 68.69 (19.85) | 78.88 (21.42) |
Depression Symptoms (SD) | 10.81 (8.69) | 16.04 (10.2) |
All participants were between the ages of 18 and 50 years, able to read and write in English at least at a sixth grade level as assessed by self-report, and met stringent health requirements as assessed by a specialist research nurse. Exclusion criteria included DSM-IV criteria for current mood or anxiety disorders, dependence on any drug other than nicotine, head injury, and current use of prescription medications for any psychiatric or medical disorders. All participants gave both written and verbal informed consent, and the study was approved by the Human Investigation Committee of the Yale University School of Medicine, which is compliant with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association and consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki. In total, eleven hundred individuals were consented into the study, of whom eight hundred and eight-one met the initial eligibility screening and signed informed consent, and, of those, seven hundred and sixty-one completed the study.
Procedures
Potential subjects completed a telephone screening to determine eligibility based on inclusion and exclusion criteria. Following initial eligibility screening, participants were scheduled for assessment sessions as part of a larger protocol on stress and emotion regulation, during which time they met with research assistants to complete informed consent; medical, substance abuse, and psychiatric health assessments, including the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (First et al., 1996); the CAI (Turner et al., 1995); the DERS (Gratz and Roemer, 2004); the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977); and a baseline demographic questionnaire. Participants also received a physical examination with a research nurse assessing cardiovascular, renal, hepatic, pancreatic, hematopoietic, and thyroid function to ensure that participants were not acutely ill or receiving active treatment for a medical illness. Breathalyzer and urine toxicology screens were conducted at each appointment to ensure drug-free status among participants. In total, participants completed three to four appointments over the course of four to six weeks, and all participants received monetary compensation for their research participation.
Measures
The Cumulative Adversity Index (CAI; Turner et al., 1995) is a 140-item interview that measures lifetime exposure to an array of traumatic and nontraumatic stressful life events, thereby facilitating analysis of the joint effect of repeated exposure to stressful life events, referred to as cumulative adversity. The CAI encompasses four stress domains comprising four subscales: major life events, life trauma, recent life events, and chronic stress. Trained interviewers asked participants about the occurrence, timing, and frequency of specific stressful events during their lifetime (Supplemental Table 1 presents the frequency with which specific life events were endorsed on the CAI in the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts). Prior research supports the use of interview-based assessments to efficiently improve the reliability and validity of retrospective reports of stressful life events (Dohrenwend, 2006; Hammen, 2005). In order to further eliminate potential sources of bias, the present study excluded the chronic stress subscale of the CAI, which elicits individuals’ subjective perception of distress. A composite cumulative adversity score was computed as the sum of the three adverse life event subscales: major life events, life trauma, and recent life events. The CAI has previously demonstrated validity in both cross-sectional and prospective investigations of the onset of psychiatric and physical health conditions (e.g. Ansell et al., 2012; Gayman et al., 2008; Turner and Lloyd, 2004; Scott et al., 2008), including good internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and demonstrated good reliability in the current sample (α = .83).
The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz and Roemer, 2004) was used to assess emotion dysregulation. The DERS is a multidimensional instrument that incorporates six separate, but related subscales: (a) lack of emotional awareness (Awareness); (b) lack of emotional clarity (Clarity); (c) non-acceptance of emotional responses (Nonacceptance); (d) limited access to emotion regulation strategies (Strategies); (e) impulse control difficulties (Impulse); and (f) difficulties engaging in goal-oriented behavior (Goals). Respondents indicate the frequency with which each item applies to them using a scale ranging from 1 (almost never, 0%–10%) to 5 (almost always, 91%–100%). Items are summed to calculate a total score, and higher total scores indicate increased emotion dysregulation. Previous research indicates that the DERS has sound psychometric properties including good internal consistency, acceptable test-retest reliability, and discriminant validity in nonclinical samples (Gratz and Roemer, 2004), and the DERS demonstrated good reliability in this sample (α = .84). Additionally, all six of the DERS subscales have previously been found to be positively correlated with depression in adults (Ortega, 2009).
Depressive symptomatology was assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). The CES-D is a brief 20-item self-report scale designed to measure depressive symptomatology in the general population. The CES-D incorporates affective (thirteen items) and somatic (seven items) symptoms of depression. The CES-D has been validated for use with a variety of populations, including both adolescents and adults and healthy and nonhealthy individuals (Devins et al., 1988; Garrison et al., 1991). Participants rate each item for the past week from 0 (rarely) to 3 (most or all of the time). The CES-D has sound psychometric properties, including excellent internal consistency, adequate test-retest reliability, and discriminant validity (Radloff, 1977), and demonstrated excellent reliability in this sample (α = .90).
Data Analysis
Pearson correlations were computed to assess the zero-order relationships among cumulative adversity, emotion dysregulation, and depressive symptomatology in the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts. Next, we collapsed the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts in order to test a moderated mediation model in ordinary least squares regression, following procedures described by Preacher and Hayes (2008). This model tested emotion dysregulation total score as a mediator of the relationship between cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology. The model simultaneously tested risk status (never-depressed vs. at-risk) as a moderator of each of the three direct and indirect pathways in order to determine whether the strength of the hypothesized pathways differed significantly between the two cohorts.
The sampling distribution of indirect effects was bootstrapped 10,000 times to provide nonparametric estimates of sampling distributions of the indirect effects. This approach has generally outperformed the more traditional model described by Baron and Kenny (1986) and has been shown to maximize statistical power (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). Effects were considered significant if zero was not included in the 95% confidence interval. Kappa-squared mediation effect size was computed using procedures outlined by Preacher and Kelley (2011). Kappa-squared is the ratio of the obtained indirect effect to the maximum possible size the indirect effect could have been, given the variances. The overall effect of the moderator, risk status, on the indirect effect of emotion dysregulation was considered significant if the 95% confidence interval for the index of moderated mediation (Hayes, 2013, in press) did not include zero.
Finally, secondary mediation analyses were conducted to assess the parallel contributions of the six DERS subscales, and pairwise contrasts were used to test whether these indirect paths were significantly different from one another (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). All of the regression and mediation analyses were controlled for age, gender, and marital status, which are the most consistent sociodemographic correlates of depression (Bromet et al., 2011). All analyses were conducted using SPSS version 20.0 and the PROCESS macro developed by Andrew Hayes (2013).
Results
Descriptive statistics
Table 2 displays the zero-order correlations between key study variables. In both the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts, cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation were independently associated with depression symptoms, and cumulative adversity was significantly associated with emotion dysregulation.
Table 2.
Never-Depressed (n = 638)
|
At-Risk (n = 107)
|
Total (n = 745)
|
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | (1) | (2) | (3) | (1) | (2) | (3) | |
(1) Cumulative Adversity | — | .13** | .30*** | — | .20** | .28** | — | .17*** | .32*** |
(2) Emotion Dysregulation | — | .62*** | — | .69*** | — | .66*** | |||
(3) Depression Symptoms | — | — | — |
Note.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001
Primary mediation analyses
Figure 1 displays the path coefficients from the primary bootstrapped regression and moderated mediation analyses, which treated emotion dysregulation as a mediator of the association between cumulative adversity and depression symptoms and risk status as a moderator of each of the three direct and indirect pathways. The regression coefficients are unstandardized and are adjusted for age, gender, and marital status.
The overall regression model was significant, F(8, 736) = 75.05, p < .001, R2 = .50, although no statistically significant effects of age (B = −.02, SE = .03), gender (B = −.52, SE = .52), or marital status (B = −.71, SE = .43) were detected. Cumulative adversity was significantly predictive of the hypothesized mediating variable, emotion dysregulation (B = .49, SE = .14). The conditional total direct effect of cumulative adversity on depressive symptomatology was significant across both the never-depressed (B = .45, SE = .05) and at-risk (B = 49, SE = .10) cohorts. After accounting for the indirect effect of emotion dysregulation, the relationship between cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology decreased slightly, but remained significant, indicating partial mediation. The 95% confidence interval for the conditional indirect effect of emotion dysregulation did not include zero in either the never-depressed [.05, .20] or at-risk [.05, .20] cohorts, indicating that the observed partial mediation effects were significant. The kappa-squared effect size indicated the presence of a medium mediation effect in the full sample, κ2 = .10, 95% CI [.04, .17].
No significant interactions were detected between cumulative adversity and risk status on emotion dysregulation, B = .24, SE = .33, 95% CI [−.41, .88]; between emotion dysregulation and risk status on depressive symptomatology, B = .06, SE = .03, 95% CI [−.002, .13]; or between cumulative adversity and risk status on depressive symptomatology, B = −.06, SE = .11, 95% CI [−.28, .15]. Furthermore, the 95% confidence interval for the index of moderated mediation [−.12, .37] included zero. Collectively, these results indicate that neither the direct effect of cumulative adversity nor the indirect effect of emotion dysregulation was significantly conditional on the effect of risk status. A three-dimensional graphical representation of the linear relationships between cumulative adversity, emotion dysregulation, and depression symptoms in the full sample is presented in Figure 2.
Secondary mediation analyses
In order to determine which of the emotion dysregulation dimensions were responsible for the observed indirect effect, we conducted a multiple mediator analysis, with the six emotion dysregulation subscales treated as parallel mediators. Because the results of the primary moderated mediation model did not indicate the presence of significant group differences, this multiple mediator analysis was tested in the full sample. Consistent with the primary mediation analyses, the results indicated that the emotion dysregulation subscales partially mediated the effect of cumulative adversity on depression symptoms. Table 4 summarizes the indirect effects of the emotion dysregulation subscales on the relationship between cumulative adversity and depression symptoms. Specifically, the indirect effects of Goals and Strategy on the relationship between cumulative adversity and depression were significant; however, the indirect effects of the other four subscales were not significant.
Table 4.
B | SE | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
Strategy | |||
versus Awareness | .12* | .03 | .07, .20 |
versus Clarity | .12* | .03 | .06, .20 |
versus Goals | .11* | .03 | .06, .19 |
versus Impulse | .10* | .04 | .03, .20 |
versus Nonacceptance | .13* | .03 | .07, .21 |
Goals | |||
versus Awareness | .01 | .01 | −.01, .04 |
versus Clarity | .00 | .01 | −.02, .03 |
versus Impulse | −.01 | .02 | −.05, .04 |
versus Nonacceptance | .02* | .01 | .01, .05 |
Note.
Significantly greater indirect effect as determined by boostrapped 95% confidence intervals.
Finally, we conducted pairwise contrasts of the indirect effects using bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). Based on this analysis, confidence intervals that do not include zero indicate the presence of a significantly stronger indirect effect. Table 5 summarizes these results. Pairwise contrasts revealed that the indirect effect of Strategy was significantly stronger than that of any of the other subscales, including the indirect effects of Goals. The indirect effect of Goals, although significant, was not significantly stronger than that of the other subscales, with the exception of Nonacceptance.
Discussion
Prior research indicates that repeated exposure to stressful life events across the lifespan, referred to as lifetime cumulative adversity, is a potent determinant of poor physical and mental well-being and a major risk factor for depression. The conceptualization of emotion dysregulation as a potential pathway linking cumulative adversity to psychiatric vulnerability is consistent with research indicating that emotion dysregulation is itself a major risk factor for the development of diverse psychopathologies, including major depressive disorder, and that exposure to acute and chronic stress adversely affects adaptive emotion regulation processes.
The extant literature contains a number of investigations that have reported a significant mediating effect of emotion dysregulation on the stress-psychopathology association; however, the research that has been conducted to date has tended to focus exclusively on early life adversity and/or adolescent psychopathology. Indeed, to our knowledge, the results of the current study are the first empirical evidence that emotion dysregulation mediates the association between lifetime cumulative adversity and depressive symptoms throughout early and middle adulthood. That the observed mediation effect was partial emphasizes the multiple mechanisms by which stress “gets under the skin,” as well as the multiple linkages between the pathophysiological sequelae of adversity and allostatic load and the syndrome of depression (McEwen, 2004, 2012; Tafet and Bernardini, 2003).
Crucially, the regression models explained fully half of the variance in depressive symptomatology, suggesting that cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation are jointly associated with tremendous vulnerability. Moreover, contrary to our expectations, the proposed mediation model was equally successful in describing the relationships between lifetime cumulative adversity, emotion dysregulation, and depressive symptomatology in both the never-depressed and at-risk cohorts. This finding suggests that vulnerability to depression associated with cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation is not contingent on a unique pathophysiology of depression; instead, cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation are incrementally associated with increased depressive symptomatology across the spectrum of vulnerability.
There are several plausible interpretations for the observed indirect effect of the Strategies subscale. According to Gratz and Roemer (2004), the Strategies subscale represents “the belief that there is little that can be done to regulate emotions effectively once an individual is upset.” The indirect effect of Strategies may reflect a structural knowledge deficit, suggesting that the individuals who are most likely to be exposed to high lifetime adversity are also the least likely to possess the requisite knowledge and skills to effectively regulate their emotions, thereby producing enhanced vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. Alternatively, the indirect effect of Strategies may reflect a kind of learned helplessness (Seligman and Cook, 1978), whereby individuals who are repeatedly exposed to stressful life events develop a generalized sense of powerlessness in the face of uncontrollable adversity. The indirect effect of Strategy also emphasizes the importance of self-efficacy within the domain of emotion regulation, consistent with prior research on the etiology of depression (Bandura et al., 2003; Caprara et al., 2010; Maddux and Meier, 1995).
These results have important implications for the development of interventions that seek to treat stress-related pathology and vulnerability to depression. In particular, our results underscore the importance of emotion regulation as a specific target for intervention, consistent with recent perspectives in psychotherapy (Berking et al., 2008; Fairholme et al., 2009; Mennin and Fresco, 2009). Given the apparent primacy of the domain of emotion regulation captured by the Strategies subscale, skills-based emotion regulation training, such as that included in Dialectical Behavior Therapy, may be particularly effective. Our results highlight the utility of such interventions across the adult lifespan and suggest that individuals who have been exposed to high lifetime adversity might particularly benefit. At the same time, the fact that the observed mediation effect was partial suggests that interventions that target emotion regulation may be most successful when they are paired with stress reduction interventions.
Several existing evidence-based interventions have been shown to facilitate adaptive emotion regulation, including behavioral activation therapy (Syzdek et al., 2009) and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Corcoran et al., 2009), both of which were developed specifically for the treatment of depression symptoms. In addition, several novel interventions that target emotion regulation capacity are in the process of being developed, including emotion regulation therapy (Mennin and Fresco, 2009), affect regulation training (Berking et al., 2013), and contextual emotion regulation therapy (Kovacs and Lopez-Duran, 2012), the latter of which was designed for the treatment of pediatric depression. In a randomized clinical trial, Berking et al. (2013) found that an integrated therapeutic approach that combined cognitive behavioral therapy and an abbreviated variation of affect regulation training significantly outperformed cognitive behavioral therapy.
There are several limitations to the current findings. First, because the data were cross-sectional, it is impossible to determine the precise nature of the relationship between cumulative adversity, emotion dysregulation, and depression. However, our data were consistent with longitudinal investigations of the stress-depression and stress-emotion dysregulation associations. Second, our results were based on self-reports, including retrospective reports of stressful life events, which may have been susceptible to reporting bias and over-reliance on memory; however, all of the measurement instruments, including the CAI, are well established and well validated. Third, the sample was predominantly composed of non-Hispanic White/Caucasian and Black/African-American participants, meaning that cross-cultural generalizability may be limited. A final limitation in considering the generalizability of these findings is that the sample was comprised of individuals who did not meet criteria for current mood or anxiety disorders. Although prior research suggests that there is substantial continuity between subthreshold and clinical depression, the proposed models may not adequately represent the effects in clinically depressed or anxious samples. These limitations notwithstanding, the current study was unique in that it deployed data from a large community sample of adults, including a medium-sized cohort of “at-risk” remitted mood disordered adults, and utilized an interview-based measure of lifetime adversity, paired with established measures of emotion dysregulation and depressive symptomatology, to demonstrate that emotion dysregulation partially mediates the relationship between the lifetime cumulative adversity and depressive symptomatology.
Conclusion
Overall, the current findings suggest that future longitudinal studies to assess the direct and indirect effects of cumulative adversity and emotion dysregulation on psychiatric vulnerability and pathology are warranted. In order to extend the model across the full spectrum of vulnerability, future studies ought to incorporate clinical populations. Crucially, our results emphasize the importance of stress reduction and emotion regulation skills as simultaneous targets for interventions that seek to treat or reduce the prevalence of depressive vulnerability and pathology.
Supplementary Material
Table 3.
B | SE | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
Awareness | .01 | .01 | −.00, .03 |
Clarity | .01 | .01 | .00, .04 |
Goals | .02* | .01 | .01, .04 |
Impulse | .02 | .02 | −.01, .07 |
Nonacceptance | .00 | .01 | −.01, .02 |
Strategy | .13* | .03 | .07, .21 |
Total | .20 | .04 | .11, .29 |
Note.
Significant indirect effect as determined by bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals.
Highlights.
We examined direct and indirect effects of life event stress on depression
We modeled emotion dysregulation as a mediator of the stress-depression association
Both stress and emotion dyregulation were significantly associated with depressive symptoms
Emotion dysregulation significantly mediated the stress-depression association
The strength of the proposed mediation pathways was not conditional on risk status
Acknowledgments
Role of funding source
The current study was supported in part by National Institutes of Health grant DK099039 (Rajita Sinha PI). Study sponsors had no role in study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation; the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
We would like to thank all of the members of research staff who assisted with data collection and entry. Additionally, we would like to express our gratitude to Adam Hong for providing conceptual guidance pertaining to the initial design of data analyses and to Dr. Emily Ansell for offering her expertise on moderated mediation. This study was supported by National Institutes of Health grant DK099039.
Footnotes
Conflicts of interest
We wish to confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest associated with this Publication.
Contributors
Protocol design and implementation was performed by Rajita Sinha. Data analysis was performed by Benjamin Abravanel. Benjamin Abravanel prepared the first draft of the manuscript. All authors have contributed to and approved the submission of the manuscript. We affirm that the manuscript has been read and approved by all named authors and that there are no other persons who satisfied the criteria for authorship but are not listed. We further affirm that the order of authors listed in the manuscript has been approved by all of us.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Ansell EB, Rando K, Tuit K, Guarnaccia J, Sinha R. Cumulative adversity and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insula regions. Biol Psychiat. 2012;72:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;10:217–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bandura A, Caprara GV, Barbaranelli C, Gerbino M, Pastorelli C. Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse spheres of psychosocial functioning. Child Dev. 2003;74:769–82. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bargh JA, Williams LE. On the nonconscious of emotion regulation. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 429–45. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Baron RM, Kenney DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173–82. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Benjet C, Borges G, Medina-Mora ME. Chronic childhood adversity and onset of psychopathology during three life stages: Childhood, adolescence and adulthood. J Psychiat Res. 2010;44:732–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Berking M, Ebert D, Cuijpers P, Hofmann SG. Emotion regulation skills training enhances the efficacy of inpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for major depressive disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom. 2013;82:234–45. doi: 10.1159/000348448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Berking M, Wupperman P, Reichhardt A, Pejic T, Dippel A, Znoj H. Emotion-regulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46:1230–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bradley B, DeFife JA, Guarnaccia C, Phifer J, Fani N, Ressler KJ, et al. Emotion dysregulation and negative affect: Association with psychiatric symptoms. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:685–91. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06409blu. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bromet E, Andrade LH, Hwang I, Sampson NA, Alonso J, de Girolamo G, et al. Cross-national epidemiology of DSM-IV major depressive episode. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;9:90. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH. Incorporating emotion regulation intro conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 542–59. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Caprara GV, Gerbino M, Paciello M, Di Giunta L, Pastorelli C. Counteracting depression and delinquency in late adolescence: The role of regulatory emotional and interpersonal self-efficacy beliefs. Eur Psychol. 2010;15:34–48. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Coates AA, Messman-Moore TL. A structural model of mechanisms predicting depressive symptoms in women following childhood psychological maltreatment. Child Abuse Negl. 2014;38:103–13. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Corcoran KM, Farb N, Anderson A, Segal ZV. Mindfulness and emotion regulation: Outcomes and possible mediating mechanisms. In: Kring AM, Sloan DM, editors. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 339–55. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Crow T, Cross D, Powers A, Bradley B. Emotion dysregulation as a mediator between childhood emotional abuse and current depression in a low-income African American sample. Child Abuse Negl. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cuijpers P, Smit F. Subthreshold depression as a risk indicator for major depressive disorder: A systematic review of prospective studies. Acta Psychiat Scand. 2004;109:325–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00301.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cuijpers P, Smit F, Van Straten A. Psychological treatments for subthreshold depression: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychiat Scand. 2007;115:434–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00998.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Davidson RJ, Pizzagalli D, Nitschke JB, Putnam K. Depression: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Ann Rev of Psychol. 2002;53:545–74. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Devins GM, Orme CM, Costello CG, Binik YM, Frizzell B, Stam HJ, et al. Measuring depressive symptoms in illness populations: Psychometric properties of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale. Psychol Health. 1988;2:139–56. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dohrenwend BP. Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychol Bull. 2006;132:477–95. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dvir Y, Ford JD, Hill M, Frazier JA. Childhood maltreatment, emotional dysregulation, and psychiatric comorbidities. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2014;22:149–61. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Fairholme CP, Boisseau CL, Ellard KK, Ehrenreich JT, Barlow DH. Emotions, emotion regulation, and psychological treatment: A unified perspective. In: Kring AM, Sloan DM, editors. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 283–309. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Enns MW, Cox BJ, Borger SC. Correlates of analogue and clinical depression: A further test of the phenomenological continuity hypothesis. J Affect Disord. 2001;66:175–183. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00305-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbons M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders - Patient Version (SCID-I/P) New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fladung AK, Baron U, Gunst I, Kiefer M. Cognitive reappraisal modulates performance following negative feedback in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychol Med. 2010;40:1703–10. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Flett GL, Vredenburg K, Krames L. The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:395–416. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Galea S, Ahern J, Tracy M, Hubbard A, Cerda M, Goldmann E, et al. Longitudinal determinants of posttraumatic stress in a population-based cohort study. Epidemiology. 2008;19:47–54. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31815c1dbf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Garrison CZ, Addy CL, Jackson KL, McKeown RE, Waller JL. The CES-D as a screen for depression and other psychiatric disorders in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Psy. 1991;30:636–41. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199107000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gayman MD, Turner RJ, Cui M. Physical limitations and depressive symptoms: Exploring the nature of the association. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2008;63:S219–28. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.4.s219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Goldsmith RE, Chesney SA, Heath NM, Barlow MR. Emotion regulation difficulties mediate associations between betrayal trauma and symptoms of posttraumatic stress, depression, and anxiety. J Trauma Stress. 2013;26:376–84. doi: 10.1002/jts.21819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Grant KE, Compas BE, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Gipson PY, Campbell AJ, et al. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: evidence of moderating and mediating effects. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:257–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav. 2004;26:41–54. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gratz KL, Tull MT. Emotion regulation as a mechanism of change in acceptance- and mindfulness-based treatments. In: Baer RA, editor. Assessing mindfulness and acceptance processes in clients: Illuminating the theory and practice of change. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications; 2010. pp. 107–34. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. 1998;2:271–99. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Gross JJ, Munoz RF. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clin Psychol-Sci Pr. 1995;2:151–64. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hammen C. Stress and depression. Annu Rev of Clin Psychol. 2005;1:293–319. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hayes AF. An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivar Behav Res. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hopp H, Troy AS, Mauss IB. The unconscious pursuit of emotion regulation: Implications for psychological health. Cognition Emotion. 2011;25:532–45. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.532606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.-Kessler RC. The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annu Rev Psychol. 1997;48:191–214. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kessler RC, Zhao S, Blazer DG, Swartz M. Prevalence, correlates, and course of minor depression and major depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. J Affect Disord. 1997;45:19–30. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00056-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kim P, Evans GW, Angstadt M, Ho SS, Sripada CS, Swain JE, et al. Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:18442–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308240110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kovacs M, Lopez-Duran NL. Contextual emotion regulation therapy: A developmentally-based intervention for pediatric depression. Child Adul Psych Cl. 2012;21:327–42. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2012.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lewinsohn PM, Solomon A, Seeley JR, Zeiss A. Clinical implications of “subthreshold” depressive symptoms. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109:345–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.-Lloyd DA, Turner RJ. Cumulative adversity and posttraumatic stress disorder: Evidence from a diverse community sample of young adults. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2003;73:281–91. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.73.4.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mabunda M, Idemudia ES. Cumulative adversities and mental health of employees in workplace settings in Gauteng Province, South Africa. J Soc Sci. 2012;33:189–99. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Maddux JE, Meier LJ. Self efficacy and depression. In: Maddux JE, editor. Self-efficacy, adjustment, and adaptation: theory, research, and application. New York: Plenum Press; 1995. pp. 143–69. [Google Scholar]
- 48.-McEwen BS. Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: Allostatis and allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1032:1–7. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:17180–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121254109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.McLaughlin KA, Hatzenbuehler ML. Stressful life events, anxiety sensitivity, and internalizing symptoms in adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118:659–69. doi: 10.1037/a0016499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Emotion regulation as a framework for understanding and treating psychopathology. In: Kring AM, Sloan DM, editors. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 256–379. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mennin DS, Holoway RM, Fresco DM, Moore MT, Heimberg RG. Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety and mood psychopathology. Behav Ther. 2007;38:284–302. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Moriya J, Takahashi Y. Depression and interpersonal stress: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Motiv Emotion. 2013;37:600–8. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9:242–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ortega V. Specificity of age differences in emotion regulation. Aging Ment Health. 2009;13:818–26. doi: 10.1080/13607860902989661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Paykel ES, Myers JK, Dienelt MN, Klerman GL, Lindenthal JJ, Pepper MP. Life events and depression: A controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1969;21:753–60. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1969.01740240113014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40:879–91. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Preacher KJ, Kelley K. Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychol Methods. 2011;16:93–115. doi: 10.1037/a0022658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psych Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Raio CM, Orederu TA, Palazzolo L, Shurick AA, Phelps EA. Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:15139–44. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305706110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rodriguez MR, Nuevo R, Chatterji S, Ayuso Mateos JL. Definitions and factors associated with subthreshold depressive conditions: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12:181. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychol Bull. 2002;128:330. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rottenberg J, Gross JJ. When emotion goes wrong: Realizing the promise of affective science. Clin Psychol-Sci Pr. 2003;10:227–32. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Scott KM, Von Korff M, Alonso J, Angermeyer MC, Benjet C, Bruffaerts R, et al. Childhood adversity, early-onset depressive/anxiety disorders, and adult-onset asthma. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:1035–43. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318187a2fb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Seligman MEP, Cook L. Learned helplessness and depression. In: Finley GE, Marin G, editors. Readings in contemporary psychology. Mexico City: Trillas; 1978. pp. 270–82. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Seo D, Tsou KA, Ansell EB, Potenza MN, Sinha R. Cumulative adversity sensitizes neural response to acute stress: Association with health symptoms. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;39:670–80. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Shallcross AJ, Troy AS, Boland M, Mauss IB. Let it be: Accepting negative emotional experiences predicts decreased negative affect and depressive symptoms. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48:921–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Shrira A. The effect of lifetime cumulative adversity on change and chronicity in depressive symptoms and quality of life in older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24:1988–97. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212001123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Silver RC, Holman EA, McIntosh DN, Poulin M, Gil-Rivas V. Nationwide longitudinal study of psychological responses to September 11. JAMA. 2002;288:1235–44. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.10.1235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology. 2001;158:343–59. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Solomon A, Haaga DA, Arnow BA. Is clinical depression distinct from subthreshold depressive symptoms? A review of the continuity issue in depression research. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189:498–506. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200108000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Stevens NR, Gerhart J, Goldsmith RE, Heath NM, Chesney SA, Hobfoll SE. Emotion regulation difficulties, low social support, and interpersonal violence mediate the link between childhood abuse and posttraumatic stress symptoms. Behav Ther. 2013;44:152–61. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2012.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Syzdek MR, Addis ME, Martell CR. Working with emotion and emotion regulation in behavioral activation therapy for depressed mood. In: Kring AM, Sloan DM, editors. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 405–26. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tafet GE, Bernardini R. Psychoneuroendocrinological links between chronic stress and depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2003;27:893–903. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(03)00162-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child. 1994;59:25–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Turner RJ, Lloyd DA. Lifetime traumas and mental health: The significance of cumulative adversity. J Health Soc Behave. 1995;36:360–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Turner RJ, Lloyd DA. Stress burden and the lifetime incidence of psychiatric disorder in young adults: racial and ethnic contrasts. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:481–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.5.481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Turner RJ, Wheaton B, Lloyd DA. The epidemiology of social stress. Am Sociol Rev. 1995;60:104–25. [Google Scholar]
- 79.van der Veek SMC, Kraaij V, Garnefski N. Cognitive coping strategies and stress in parents of children with Down syndrome: A prospective study. Intellect Dev Disabil. 2009;47:295–306. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-47.4.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Watson D. Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: A quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-V. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:522–36. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Weissman MM, Sholomskas D, Pottenger M, Prusoff BA, Locke BZ. Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: A validation study. Am J Epidemiol. 1977;106:203–14. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.