Abstract
Background
This study examined gender differences in alcohol involvement and marital interactions among probands with a past 1-year alcohol use disorder (AUD).
Methods
Adults with alcohol dependence (37 males and 17 females) and their spouses were recruited from a local substance abuse treatment center and from the local community. Couples completed a series of self-report measures and a 15-min videotaped marital interaction task that was coded for negative and positive behaviors and sequential interactions. Couples also separately called in to an interactive voice response (IVR) system every night for 14 consecutive nights and reported on their spouse’s negative and positive marital behaviors.
Results
Compared to male probands, female probands reported a) more negative marital interactions in the previous month; b) higher levels of negative reciprocity and a lower positive-to-negative ratio in the marital interaction task; and c) more daily and nightly marital conflict over the 14-day diary period. Negative marital behaviors in the evening by female spouses were associated with higher odds of intoxication among male probands on the following day. By contrast, a) negative marital behaviors by male spouses were cross-sectionally associated with higher odds of intoxication among female probands within the same day; and b) positive marital behaviors by male spouses during the day were associated with lower odds of intoxication among female probands that night.
Conclusions
Marital conflict, assessed via multiple methods over multiple time scales, appears to be more frequent among female compared to male alcoholics. Marital conflict predicts daily intoxication among male and female probands.
1. Introduction
The present study used self-report, behavioral observation, and daily process methods (Mohr et al., 2010) to examine gender differences in the associations between alcohol involvement and marital interactions among married alcoholics. Extensive evidence has documented relationships between alcohol involvement and negative marital interactions, marital dissatisfaction, and marital violence (Leonard & Eiden, 2007; Marshal, 2003), and relationship factors are important predictors of treatment outcomes among adults with alcohol and other substance use disorders (McCrady et al., 2009; for a review, see O’Farrell & Clements, 2012). Yet, consistent with studies showing gender differences in the associations between marital functioning and health (e.g., Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001), there is some evidence that the association between alcohol involvement (including alcohol use disorders, or AUDs) and marital discord may be stronger for women than men (Paolino et al., 1978).
Several theoretical models can illuminate the nature of the association between alcohol involvement and marital interactions. Karney and Bradbury’s (1995) Vulnerability-Stress-Adaptation (VSA) model of marriage hypothesizes that the development of marital quality and stability are influenced by three broad classes of variables: 1) enduring vulnerabilities; 2) stressful events; and 3) adaptive processes (e.g., marital interactions that involve problem-solving). Psychiatric disorders such as AUD are enduring vulnerabilities that may a) confer greater risk of exposure to stressors and b) compromise couples’ coping efforts (Bruce, 1998; Johns et al., 2007). Consistent with this hypothesis, evidence showed that more than 80% of women and men seeking counseling for marital problems reported frequent marital disagreements about alcohol use (Halford & Ogarsby, 1993).
A more general theoretical model of linkages between marriage and physical health was advanced by Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton (2001). They reviewed evidence that positive marital interactions are directly associated with lower probability of risky health behaviors. Evidence also showed that “marital conflict may be both a precursor and consequence of alcohol and drug abuse” (p. 491). Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton summarized several lines of research showing that the physiological effects of negative marital interactions are stronger for women than men (e.g., Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 1993).
Consistent with this model, Kessler et al. (1998) reported that AUDs were significantly associated with divorce for women but not for men, and Ramisetty-Mikler and Caetano (2005) found that female (but not male) alcohol problems predicted higher odds of marital separation over time. Other longitudinal evidence showed that wives’ (but not husbands’) AUDs predicted their own and their husband’s marital dissatisfaction (Cranford et al., 2011), and a study using behavioral observation methods found higher levels of negativity and lower levels of positivity in couples with a female alcoholic proband compared to couples with a male alcoholic proband or no alcoholic proband (Haber & Jacob, 1997). However, a study based on a nationally representative sample found no evidence for gender differences in the relationship between AUD and marital dissolution (Cranford, 2014).
The present study used multiple methods to test hypotheses about gender differences in the relationships between alcohol involvement and marital behaviors in a sample of married alcoholics. A limitation of some previous studies is that they focused on between-persons associations between alcohol involvement and marital interactions. Researchers have increasingly turned to daily process methods (Tennen et al., 2000, 2005) to examine the within- and between-persons associations between alcohol involvement and variables such as coping (Park et al., 2004), daily stress (Armeli et al., 2003), and social interactions (Mohr et al., 2001). Drawing on the VSA model, role incompatibility theory, and previous findings on gender differences in reactivity to marital conflict, we tested the following hypotheses:
-
Hypothesis 1
Negative marital interactions will be more frequent, and positive marital interactions will be less frequent, among females compared to males with AUDs.
-
Hypothesis 2
Daily alcohol involvement will be associated with more negative and fewer positive marital interactions among females compared to males with AUDs.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Alcoholic probands and their spouses were recruited from a local substance abuse treatment center (n=20 couples) and from the local community (n=34 couples). Probands from the clinical sample met DSM-IV clinical diagnosis of past 1-year alcohol abuse or dependence, and probands from the community sample screened positive for a past 1-year AUD based on responses to the Rapid Alcohol Problem Screen 4 (RAPS4; Cherpitel, 2002). A total of 54 couples (37 couples with an alcoholic male proband/female partner and 17 couples with an alcoholic female proband/male partner) comprised the final sample (see Cranford et al., 2010). As shown in Table 1, female probands reported significantly lower personal income than male probands, but no other statistically significant differences were observed.
Table 1.
Male Probands (n = 37) M (SD) / % |
Female Probands (n = 17) M (SD) / % |
t /χ2 | |
---|---|---|---|
Demographicsa | |||
Age | 44.5 (13.4) | 40.9 (12.8) | 0.9 |
Educationa | 4.6 (1.0) | 4.5 (1.0) | 0.5 |
Personal Incomeb | 4.5 (2.3) | 2.6 (2.2) | 2.7* |
Length of Marriage (in years) | 15.4 (14.1) | 9.7 (9.3) | 1.1 |
Race/Ethnicity (White) | 78.4 | 70.6 | 0.4 |
Worked in Past Week | 64.9 | 47.1 | 1.5 |
Any Children (Yes) | 61.1 | 80.0 | 1.7 |
Any Children at Home (Yes) | 51.7 | 41.7 | 0.3 |
| |||
Substance Use | |||
Ever Smoked (Yes) | 64.9 | 70.6 | 0.2 |
Ever Used Marijuana (Yes) | 78.4 | 82.4 | 0.1 |
Ever Used Cocaine (Yes) | 41.2 | 47.1 | 0.2 |
Highest level of education completed ranged from 0 = never attended school to 7 = MD, PhD, JD.
Personal income during the past one year ranged from 0 = less than $1,000 to 9 = $100,000 or more.
p < .05.
2.2. Procedures
2.2.1. Baseline interview, marital interaction task, and interactive voice response (IVR) system training
At baseline, couples completed a series of questionnaires asking about past 1-month moods, marital interactions, and drinking behaviors. Couples then completed a 15-minute marital interaction task. For this task, couples rated the importance of 10 common marital problems (e.g., money; children) using the Marital Interaction Inventory (Knox, 1971), selected the problem that causes the most intense disagreement between them, and discussed it for 15 minutes. Videotapes were coded at the Rapid Marital Interaction Coding System (RMICS) Coding Center under the supervision of Dr. Richard Heyman (Heyman, 2004). Equipment problems led to the loss of data from six couples, leaving us with n=48 codeable interactions. Couples completed an IVR training session immediately following the marital interaction task. Participants called an IVR system separately every day for 14 days, between the hours of 5:00pm and 9:00pm, and reported on their drinking behaviors and marital interactions for two periods: 1) last night after you completed the telephone interview, and 2) since you woke up today.
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Past month and daily negative marital interactions
We assessed past-month negative marital interactions with the 7-item Social Undermining Scale (Vinokur et al., 1996) and three items from the Withdrawn Marital Behavior Scale (Schulz et al., 2004). Participants rated the frequency of their spouse’s negative behaviors (e.g., criticism) during the past one month using a 5-point scale (0 = not at all to 4 = about every day), and scores were calculated as the mean of the item scores (Cronbach’s alpha = .93). Daily negative marital interactions were assessed using 5 of these 10 items, and participants used a yes/no response scale to report on the two time periods. Items were summed to create an index of the total number of negative marital interactions for each period.
2.3.2. Past month and daily positive marital interactions
We assessed past one month positive marital interactions with nine items from Manne et al. (2004) and de Koning and Weiss (2002). Participants rated the frequency of their spouse’s positive behaviors during the past one month, and scores were calculated as the mean of the item scores (Cronbach’s alpha = .89). Daily positive marital interactions were assessed using 5 of these 9 items, using the same format and response options as those for the daily negative marital interaction items. Items were summed to create an index of the total number of positive marital interactions for each period.
2.3.3
Real-time negative and positive marital interactions were coded from the behavioral observation data with the RMICS, which categorizes behavior into a) five negative codes: psychological abuse (PA), distress-maintaining attribution (DA), hostility (HO), dysphoric affect (DY), and withdrawal (WI); b) four positive codes: acceptance (AC), relationship-enhancing attribution (RA), self-disclosure (SD), and humor (HM); c) one neutral code: constructive problem discussion/solution (PD): and d) one other code (Heyman, 2004). In the present study, base rates for PA, DY, and WI were too low to be coded. Kappa coefficients were .92 for DA, .65 for HO, .92 for AC, .85 for RA, .51 for SD, .78 for HM, and .57 for PD.
2.3.4. Past 1 month and daily alcohol involvement
Past 1-month alcohol consumption was assessed with three items: 1) number of days consumed one or more alcoholic beverages (frequency), 2) usual number of drinks per drinking day (quantity), and 3) frequency of binge drinking, defined as consuming 5 drinks for men (4 for women) within a 2-hour period (NIAAA, 2004). Frequency of alcohol-related problems in the past 3 months was measured with the Short Inventory of Problems (SIP; Miller et al., 1995; Cronbach’s alpha = .95). Items asking about daily alcohol involvement were adapted from Helzer et al. (2002) and Kranzler et al. (2004) using the standard definitions of alcoholic beverages (NIAAA, 2007). Variables for number of drinks since waking up today and number of drinks after completing the IVR last night were calculated by summing responses across three beverage types (beer, wine, and hard liquor). For both time periods, participants were asked to indicate their highest level of intoxication (0=perfectly sober and 10=as drunk as you’ve ever been). As this variable was positively skewed, we created a binary version coded 0 = no intoxication, 1 = at least some intoxication (Searles et al., 1995).
2.4. Analytic Approach
Analyses for this report focus on the alcoholic proband in each couple. To test hypothesis 1, male and female probands were compared on daily, past one month, and past one year alcohol involvement. Gender differences in past one month, daily, and real time marital interactions were also tested. All comparisons were between-groups t-tests and Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1992) was used as the measure of effect size.
We used multilevel modeling (MLM; Bolger et al., 2003) to test hypothesis 2. Generalized linear mixed effects models (GLMMs; Fitzmaurice et al., 2004) using the GLIMMIX procedure in SAS (SAS Institute, 2009) were estimated to examine last night’s alcohol use and marital interactions as predictors of 1) today’s intoxication and 2) today’s negative and positive marital interactions. We also tested today’s alcohol involvement and marital interactions as predictors of 1) tonight’s intoxication and 2) tonight’s negative and positive marital interactions. Random intercepts were included to account for individual differences in the outcomes of interest, and within-person residual correlations were modeled with an autoregressive (AR1) structure. All models included age as a level-2 covariate (Birditt et al., 2005). Weekend (operationalized as Friday, Saturday, and Sunday) was also included as a covariate based on evidence that alcohol involvement is higher on weekends compared to weekdays (e.g., Carney et al., 2000). Because random intercepts were included in all models, coefficients are interpretable as unit-specific or conditional multiplicative effects (see Atkins et al., 2013).
3. Results
3.1. Compliance with IVR Protocol
Participants completed a total of 1,418 out of a possible 1,512 (54 × 2 × 14) = daily process reports, for an overall compliance rate of 93.8%. There was no difference in compliance rates between male (94.6%) and female (93.7%) probands. Participants completed an average of 13.1 (SD = 1.1) out of a possible 14 IVR days (range = 9–14 days, median = 13 days, mode = 14 days).
3.2. Gender Differences in Alcohol Involvement and Marital Interactions
Mean levels of alcohol involvement in the past one year, past 30 days, and over the course of the two-week diary period are presented in Table 2. As expected, male probands reported higher levels of alcohol involvement across all measures, but none of the gender differences was statistically significant. Mean levels of marital interactions in the past 30 days, during the 15-min behavioral observation task, and over the course of the ensuing two-week diary period are presented in Table 3. Female probands reported significantly more negative interactions with their husbands a) over the past 1-month and b) during the day and in the evening across the two-week diary period. However, no gender differences in positive marital interactions were observed for either time period. For the behavioral observation measures, three statistically significant differences were observed: female probands showed lower levels of humor, a lower positive-to-negative ratio, and higher levels of negative reciprocity compared to male probands. Most (90%) effect sizes were in the medium-to-large range (Cohen, 1992).
Table 2.
Male Probands (n = 37) M (SD) |
Female Probands (n = 17) M (SD) |
t | d | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Alcohol Involvement: Past 1 Year | ||||
Number of drinking days | 200.7 (120.6) | 130.8 (118.1) | 2.0 | 0.58 |
Drinks per drinking day | 6.2 (5.0) | 5.7 (5.8) | 0.3 | 0.09 |
Binge drinking days | 118.7 (129.8) | 92.1 (115.4) | 0.7 | 0.21 |
| ||||
Alcohol Involvement: Past 1 Month | ||||
Number of drinking days | 11.2 (11.0) | 8.6 (8.8) | 0.8 | 0.25 |
Drinks per drinking day | 4.2 (5.5) | 2.2 (1.8) | 1.4 | 0.43 |
Binge drinking days | 3.8 (5.9) | 2.5 (4.2) | 0.8 | 0.24 |
Alcohol-related problemsa | 13.3 (11.1) | 13.6 (12.7) | −0.10 | −0.03 |
| ||||
Alcohol Involvement: 2 Week IVR Period | ||||
Number of drinking days | 8.1 (4.9) | 6.9 (3.5) | 0.7 | 0.26 |
Drinks per drinking day | 5.9 (5.6) | 2.8 (1.4) | 1.6 | 0.66 |
Binge drinking days | 1.9 (3.4) | 1.1 (1.6) | 0.7 | 0.27 |
Note. Degrees of freedom ranged from 48 to 52.
The time frame for the SIP was the past 3 months.
d = Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1992).
Table 3.
Male Probandsa M (SD) |
Female Probandsa M (SD) |
t | d | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Marital Interactions: Past 1 Monthb | ||||
Spouse undermining | 0.9 (0.6) | 1.3 (1.1) | −2.1* | −0.45 |
Spouse withdrawal | 0.8 (0.8) | 1.7 (1.3) | −3.1* | −0.84 |
Spouse acceptance | 3.0 (1.2) | 3.3 (0.8) | −0.9 | −0.29 |
Spouse disclosure | 2.8 (1.1) | 2.8 (0.5) | −0.1 | −0.05 |
Spouse humor | 2.6 (1.0) | 3.1 (1.1) | −1.5 | −0.45 |
| ||||
Marital Interactions: 15-Minute Behavioral Observation Task | ||||
Distress-maintaining attributions | 2.0 (4.3) | 3.1 (3.0) | −0.9 | −0.30 |
Hostility | 4.3 (10.5) | 9.5 (11.8) | −1.5 | −0.47 |
Acceptance | 1.2 (1.6) | 2.5 (5.2) | −1.3 | −0.34 |
Relationship-enhancing attributions | 10.0 (5.5) | 12.1 (6.3) | −0.7 | −0.35 |
Self-disclosure | 3.0 (4.2) | 2.5 (2.0) | 0.5 | 0.24 |
Humor | 5.5 (6.4) | 1.8 (3.1) | 2.2* | 0.74 |
Constructive problem discussion | 71.3 (13.1) | 68.0 (12.7) | 0.8 | 0.26 |
Ratio of positive to negative behavior | 0.82 (0.23) | 0.66 (0.28) | 2.2* | 0.62 |
Proband HO →Spouse HO | 0.19 (0.47) | 0.88 (0.16) | −2.3* | 0.62 |
Proband RA →Spouse RA | 0.75 (0.88) | 0.44 (0.73) | 1.2 | −1.95 |
Proband PD →Spouse PD | 29.4 (14.7) | 28.4 (14.0) | 0.2 | 0.07 |
| ||||
Marital Interactions: 2 Week IVR Periodc | ||||
Negative interactions last night | 0.4 (0.4) | 1.2 (1.1) | −3.6* | −0.90 |
Negative interactions today | 0.5 (0.5) | 1.3 (1.1) | −3.6* | −0.91 |
Positive interactions last night | 3.9 (0.9) | 3.4 (1.3) | 1.5 | 0.41 |
Positive interactions today | 3.8 (0.9) | 3.5 (1.2) | 1.0 | 0.28 |
Note. Degrees of freedom ranged from 46 to 52. HM = Humor, HO = Hostility, DA = Distress-Maintaining Attributions, RA = Relationship-Enhancing Attributions, PD = Constructive Problem-Solving Discussions.
For the marital interaction results, n = 32 male probands and n = 16 female probands. All other results are based on n = 37 male probands and n = 17 female probands.
Scaled so that 0 = “not at all,” 1 = “less than once a week,” 2 = “once a week,” 3 = “more than once a week,” and 4 = “about every day.”
Average of the total number of spouse’s behaviors for a given time period (possible range 0 to 5 for last night and today).
d = Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1992).
p < .05.
3.3. Predictors of Today’s and Tonight’s Intoxication
3.3.1. Today’s intoxication
Results from GLMMs predicting today’s intoxication are presented in the top panel of Table 4. Within-person correlations between last night’s and today’s negative and positive marital interactions were very high (rs = .88 and .91, respectively). Because of this, we estimated separate models for each set of predictors, i.e., model 1 is a prospective model that includes last night’s negative and positive marital interactions as predictors of today’s intoxication, and model 2 is a cross-sectional model that includes today’s negative and positive marital interactions as predictors of today’s intoxication. As seen in the top panel of Table 4, among male probands, last night’s intoxication and last night’s negative marital interactions predicted higher odds of today’s intoxication. Today’s marital interactions were not associated with today’s intoxication. By contrast, among female probands, today’s negative marital interactions predicted higher odds of today’s intoxication. Last night’s marital interactions were not associated with today’s intoxication.
Table 4.
Male Probands (n = 37) | Female Probands (n = 17) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
OR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | AOR (95% CI) | |
1. Predictors of Today’s Intoxication | ||||
Last night’s intoxication | 6.3* (2.7, 14.9) | 6.7* (1.6, 15.9) | 1.2 (0.6, 2.4) | 1.2 (0.7, 2.2) |
Weekend | 1.2 (0.7, 2.0) | 1.4 (0.7, 3.5) | 0.8 (0.3, 2.5) | 0.8 (0.2, 3.4) |
Last night’s negative interactions | 1.8* (1.2, 2.7) | 1.9* (1.2, 3.1) | 1.2 (0.9, 1.7) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) |
Last night’s positive interactions | 0.9 (0.7, 1.3) | 1.2 (0.8, 1.9) | 0.8 (0.6, 1.1) | 0.9 (0.6, 1.2) |
2. Predictors of Today’s Intoxication | ||||
Last night’s intoxication | 6.1* (2.7, 14.9) | 6.1*(2.5, 14.7) | 1.2 (0.6, 2.3) | 1.3 (0.4, 4.5) |
Weekend | 1.4 (0.7, 1.9) | 1.3 (0.7, 2.3) | 0.8 (0.2, 3.4) | 0.8 (0.3, 2.3) |
Today’s negative interactions | 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) | 1.0 (0.6, 1.8) | 1.4* (1.1, 2.0) | 1.5* (1.1, 2.2) |
Today’s positive interactions | 0.9 (0.7, 1.1) | 0.9 (0.7, 1.4) | 0.8 (0.6, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.7, 1.6) |
| ||||
1. Predictors of Tonight’s Intoxication | ||||
Today’s intoxication | 3.2* (1.5, 6.6) | 3.6* (1.6, 7.7) | 0.4 (0.1, 2.0) | 0.5 (0.1, 2.9) |
Weekend | 1.6 (0.9, 2.8) | 1.9* (1.1, 3.4) | 2.2 (0.9, 5.3) | 2.0 (0.7, 5.5) |
Today’s negative interactions | 1.0 (0.7, 1.4) | 0.9 (0.6, 1.3) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) | 0.9 (0.6, 1.3) |
Today’s positive interactions | 1.0 (0.7, 1.3) | 0.9 (0.7, 1.3) | 0.6* (0.4, 0.8) | 0.5* (0.4, 0.8) |
2. Predictors of Tonight’s Intoxication | ||||
Today’s intoxication | 3.2* (1.5, 6.6) | 3.4* (1.6, 7.4) | 0.4 (0.1, 2.0) | 0.5 (0.1, 2.2) |
Weekend | 1.6 (0.9, 2.8) | 1.8* (1.1, 3.1) | 2.2 (0.9, 5.3) | 2.0 (0.8, 5.4) |
Tonight’s negative interactions | 1.4* (1.0, 2.0) | 1.3 (0.9, 1.9) | 0.9 (0.7, 1.2) | 0.9 (0.6, 1.4) |
Tonight’s positive interactions | 0.7* (0.6, 0.9) | 0.8 (0.6, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.7, 1.3) | 1.0 (0.6, 1.4) |
Note. OR = odds ratio, AOR = adjusted odds ratio from generalized linear mixed model analyses.
p < .05.
3.3.2. Tonight’s intoxication
Results from GLMMs predicting tonight’s intoxication are presented in the bottom panel of Table 4. Here, model 1 is a prospective model that includes today’s negative and positive marital interactions as predictors of tonight’s intoxication, and model 2 is a cross-sectional model that includes tonight’s negative and positive marital interactions as predictors of tonight’s intoxication. As seen in the bottom panel of Table 4, among male probands, weekend and today’s intoxication predicted higher odds of tonight’s intoxication. Neither today’s nor tonight’s marital interactions were associated with tonight’s intoxication. By contrast, among female probands, today’s positive marital interactions predicted lower odds of tonight’s intoxication. Tonight’s marital interactions were not associated with tonight’s intoxication.
3.4. Predictors of Today’s and Tonight’s Negative and Positive Marital Interactions
3.4.1. Today’s negative marital interactions
Results from GLMMs predicting today’s negative marital interactions are presented in the top panel of Table 5. Among male probands, last night’s and today’s positive interactions and last night’s negative marital interactions predicted lower and higher rates, respectively, of today’s negative marital interactions. Neither last night’s nor today’s intoxication were associated with negative marital interactions. Similarly, among female probands, last night’s negative interactions and today’s positive interactions predicted the rate of today’s negative marital interactions. Neither last night’s nor today’s intoxication predicted today’s negative marital interactions.
Table 5.
Male Probands (n = 37) | Female Probands (n = 17) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
RR (95% CI) | ARR (95% CI) | RR (95% CI) | ARR (95% CI) | |
1. Predictors of Today’s Negative Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.0 (0.7, 1.5) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.4) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.4) |
Last night’s intoxication | 1.2 (0.8, 1.9) | 1.0 (0.7, 1.5) | 0.8 (0.6, 1.3) | 0.9 (0.6, 1.3) |
Last night’s positive interactions | 0.7* (0.6, 0.8) | 0.8* (0.7, 0.9) | 0.8* (0.7, 0.9) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) |
Last night’s negative interactions | 1.4* (1.2, 1.6) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.4) | 1.3* (1.2, 1.4) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.4) |
2. Predictors of Today’s Negative Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.0 (0.7, 1.5) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.6) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) | 1.1 (0.9, 1.5) |
Today’s intoxication | 1.4 (0.9, 2.3) | 1.2 (0.8, 1.9) | 1.7* (1.2, 2.5) | 1.4† (1.0, 2.0) |
Today’s positive interactions | 0.65* (0.6, 0.7) | 0.7*(0.6, 0.8) | 0.8* (0.7, 0.9) | 0.86* (0.8, 0.9) |
Last night’s negative interactions | 1.4* (1.2, 1.6) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.4) | 1.3* (1.2, 1.4) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.3) |
| ||||
1. Predictors of Today’s Positive Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.1*(1.01, 1.13) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) |
Last night’s intoxication | 0.9 (0.8, 1.03) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.05) | 1.1 (0.9, 1.3) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.2) |
Last night’s negative interactions | 0.9* (.87, .95) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.01) | .89* (.85, .93) | .92* (.87, .98) |
Last night’s positive interactions | 1.24*(1.2, 1.3) | 1.23* (1.2, 1.30) | 1.15*(1.1, 1.2) | 1.10*(1.03, 1.2) |
2. Predictors of Today’s Positive Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.07*(1.02, 1.13) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) |
Today’s intoxication | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 0.97 (0.9, 1.0) | 0.9 (0.8, 1.2) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) |
Today’s negative interactions | 0.88* (0.8, 0.9) | 0.94* (0.91, 0.98) | 0.9* (0.86, 0.94) | .91* (.87, .96) |
Last night’s positive interactions | 1.24*(1.2, 1.3) | 1.22*(1.2, 1.3 | 1.15*(1.1, 1.2) | 1.11*(1.06, 1.2) |
Note. RR = rate ratio, ARR = adjusted rate ratio from generalized linear mixed model analyses.
p < .05.
p = .06.
3.4.2. Today’s positive marital interactions
Results from GLMMs predicting the number of today’s positive marital interactions are presented in the bottom panel of Table 5. Neither last night’s nor today’s intoxication were associated with the rate of today’s positive marital interactions among male or female probands. Last night’s positive and today’s negative marital interactions emerged as common predictors of today’s positive marital interactions among male and female probands. Weekend was associated with a higher rate of today’s positive interactions among male (but not female) probands.
3.4.3. Tonight’s negative and positive marital interactions
Results from GLMMs predicting the number of tonight’s negative and positive marital interactions are presented Table 6. Neither today’s nor tonight’s intoxication was associated with tonight’s negative or positive marital interactions for male or female probands. However, for male and female probands, tonight’s negative marital interactions were predicted by a) today’s and tonight’s negative interactions and b) tonight’s positive interactions.
Table 6.
Male Probands (n = 37) | Female Probands (n = 17) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
RR (95% CI) | ARR (95% CI) | RR (95% CI) | ARR (95% CI) | |
1. Predictors of Tonight’s Negative Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.3 (0.8, 1.9) | 1.2 (0.9, 1.7) | 1.0 (0.7, 1.5) | 0.9 (0.7, 1.3) |
Today’s intoxication | 0.9 (0.5, 1.5) | 0.8 (0.5, 1.5) | 1.1 (0.6, 2.1) | 0.9 (0.5, 1.7) |
Today’s positive interactions | 0.82* (0.70, 0.96) | 0.9 (0.7, 1.01) | 0.82* (0.69, 0.97) | 0.9 (0.8, 1.04) |
Today’s negative interactions | 1.4* (1.2, 1.6) | 1.3* (1.1, 1.5) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.4) | 1.2* (1.0, 1.3) |
2. Predictors of Tonight’s Negative Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.3 (0.8, 1.9) | 1.1 (0.8, 1.5) | 1.0 (0.7, 1.5) | 1.0 (0.7, 1.3) |
Tonight’s intoxication | 1.5 (0.9, 2.5) | 1.2 (0.8, 1.9) | 0.8 (0.5, 1.4) | 0.9 (0.5, 1.4) |
Tonight’s positive interactions | 0.62* (0.5, 0.7) | 0.61* (0.5, 0.7) | 0.67* (0.6, 0.7) | 0.66* (0.6, 0.7) |
Today’s negative interactions | 1.4* (1.2, 1.6) | 1.3* (1.1, 1.4) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.4) | 1.2* (1.1, 1.3) |
| ||||
1. Predictors of Tonight’s Positive Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.02) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.1) |
Today’s intoxication | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.3) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.3) |
Today’s negative interactions | 0.96* (0.93, 0.99) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.01) | 0.98 (0.93, 1.03) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) |
Today’s positive interactions | 1.0 (0.9, 1.05) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.06) | 1.1* (1.02, 1.2) | 1.1* (1.01, 1.2) |
2. Predictors of Tonight’s Positive Interactions | ||||
Weekend | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.02) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.2) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) |
Tonight’s intoxication | 0.9 (0.8, 1.01) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.1) | 1.1 (0.9, 1.3) | 1.0 (0.8, 1.2) |
Tonight’s negative interactions | 0.85* (0.82, 0.89) | 0.85* (0.81, 0.88) | 0.80* (0.76, 0.84) | 0.85* (0.81, 0.90) |
Today’s positive interactions | 1.0 (0.9, 1.05) | 1.0 (0.9, 1.04) | 1.1* (1.02, 1.2) | 1.07* (1.01, 1.15) |
Note. RR = rate ratio, ARR = adjusted rate ratio from generalized linear mixed model analyses.
p < .05.
4. Discussion
Although several studies used daily process methods to collect data from married alcoholics (Dunn et al., 1987), to our knowledge this is the first study to use IVR technology and behavioral observation methods in the same study to examine marital processes in this population.
4.1. Gender Differences in Marital Interaction Among Married Alcoholics
In partial support of hypothesis 1, female probands generally reported higher levels of negative marital interaction over the past 1 month and over the 14-day diary period compared to male probands. Contrary to hypothesis, this difference did not extend to the realm of positive behaviors. Results also showed that, during the 15-minute behavioral observation task, female probands displayed less humor, a lower positive-to-negative ratio of positive to negative behaviors, and more negative reciprocity. Taken together, results showed that marital conflict, assessed via multiple methods over multiple time spans, was higher in female proband/male spouse couples.
According to role incompatibility theory (Fu & Goldman, 2000), some behavior patterns (e.g., substance use) are incompatible with adult social roles (e.g., marriage). This role conflict may be exacerbated among alcoholic women, leading to greater strain on the relationship and increases in the frequency and severity of marital conflicts. Interestingly, with the exception of humor, the differences between male and female probands were limited to negative marital interactions. Indeed, all probands were characterized by relatively high levels of positive behaviors across time frames and assessment methods. With respect to the relatively high levels of both positive and negative marital interactions among female probands, Fincham and Linfield (1997) advanced a two-dimensional model of marital quality and found evidence for what they termed “ambivalent” spouses, i.e., those who report high levels of positive and negative marital quality. Similarly, Huston and Melz (2004) described a two-dimensional model of the “emotional climates” of marital relationships that included “tempestuous” couples that are characterized as “both highly affectionate and antagonistic” (p. 952). The female proband/male spouse couples in the current study appear to fit this category.
4.2. Gender Differences in Associations between Marital Interaction and Alcohol Involvement
Neither last night’s nor today’s positive interactions were associated with today’s intoxication for female or male probands. Among female probands, the only predictor of today’s intoxication was today’s negative marital interactions. In addition, today’s positive interactions predicted lower odds of tonight’s intoxication among female but not male probands. By contrast, among male probands, the only predictor of today’s intoxication was last night’s negative marital interactions. These findings suggest a closer temporal linkage between intoxication and negative marital interactions among female alcoholic/male spouse couples, and point to the possibly unique role of positive marital interactions as a buffer against heavy drinking among female probands.
To the extent that these results suggest a closer temporal linkage between drinking and marital conflict for female probands, they are consistent with previous work showing that wives’ (but not husbands’) AUDs predicted their own and their husband’s marital dissatisfaction (Cranford et al., 2011). Results are also conceptually similar to those reported by Homish and Leonard (2007), who found a significant association between wives’ heavy drinking and subsequent marital satisfaction, but found no association between husbands’ heavy drinking and subsequent marital satisfaction.
Our findings are also similar to results reported by Olenick and Chalmers (1991). They showed that alcoholic and nonalcoholic men were more likely than women to report that their alcohol involvement was a source of marital conflict (drinking → marital conflict). By contrast, alcoholic women were more likely than alcoholic men to report that marital conflict led to their drinking (marital conflict → drinking). Thus, results for male probands are amenable to a stress-generation explanation (where intoxication → marital conflict; cf. Dunn et al., 1987) whereas results for female probands are consistent with a reactivity explanation (where marital conflict → intoxication).
4.3. Limitations and Future Directions
This study has several limitations. We were unable to determine the AUD status of the proband’s spouse, making it impossible to test hypotheses about couple concordance on AUDs (e.g., Mudar et al., 2002). Results from previous work showed that marital outcomes varied as a function of couple concordance on alcohol involvement (for reviews, see Leonard & Eiden, 2007; Marshal, 2003). In addition, it has been suggested that couples recruited from clinical settings are less likely to be concordant for AUDs and may have higher levels of marital commitment (Leonard and Eiden, 2007). Thus, while our discussion has focused on gender differences, we cannot rule out the hypothesis that our findings are driven by dyad-level concordance on AUD.
Another limitation is that the relatively small sample size – particularly for female probands – imposes restrictions on the generalizability of our findings. Also, kappa coefficients for some of the behavioral observation codes were low. In addition, although we used a daily process design, participants still relied on retrospection to report on today’s and last night’s drinking behaviors and marital interactions. While memory biases may be minimized in end-of-day reports of salient and discrete events (Stone and Shiffman, 2002), the accuracy of retrospective daily reports of drinking and marital interactions is unknown.
To summarize, compared to male probands, female probands a) reported more negative marital interactions in the previous month; b) displayed higher levels of negative reciprocity in a marital interaction task; and c) reported more daily and nightly marital conflict over a 14-day diary period. In addition, among female probands, today’s negative marital interactions were associated with higher odds of today’s intoxication, and today’s positive interactions predicted lower odds of tonight’s intoxication among female but not male probands. By contrast, among male probands, the only predictor of today’s intoxication was last night’s negative marital interactions. Although clinical implications are limited due to the small sample size, results suggest that negative marital interactions might be a particularly important intervention target for female probands. Future research using more intensive data collection protocols (i.e., ecological momentary assessment; Shiffman et al., 2008) and assessing drinking contexts (Levitt & Cooper, 2010) will further our understanding of the causal direction of these associations.
HIGHLIGHTS.
Past 1-month marital conflict was more frequent among female than male alcoholics.
Marital conflict over 14 days was more frequent among female than male alcoholics.
Negative and positive marital behaviors were associated with daily intoxication.
Acknowledgments
Role of Funding Sources
This research was support by Grant R21 AA015105 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, National Institutes of Health. The NIAAA had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
The authors thank Janie Slayden, Jenna Johnson, Ann Mooney, Ashley Dominique, and the staff, clinicians, and director of the University of Michigan Addiction Treatment Services (UMATS) for their assistance with this research.
Footnotes
Contributors
Authors J. A. Cranford, H. Tennen, and R. A. Zucker designed the study. Author J. A. Cranford managed the literature searches and summaries of previous related work, and undertook the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
James A. Cranford, Addiction Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Rachel Upjohn Building, 4250 Plymouth Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2700, Phone: (734) 232-0215, Fax: (734) 998-7992
Howard Tennen, University of Connecticut, Department of Community Medicine, and Health Care, University of Connecticut Health Center, 263 Farmington Avenue, MC 6325, Farmington, CT 06030-6325.
Robert A. Zucker, University of Michigan, Addiction Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, Rachel Upjohn Building, 4250 Plymouth Road, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-2700
References
- Armeli S, Tennen H, Todd M, Carney MA, Mohr CD, Affleck G, Hromi A. A daily process examination of the stress-response dampening effects of alcohol consumption. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2003;17:266–276. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.4.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atkins DC, Baldwin SA, Zheng C, Gallop RJ, Neighbors C. A tutorial on count regression and zero-altered count models for longitudinal substance use data. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2013;27(1):166–177. doi: 10.1037/a0029508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birditt KS, Fingerman KL, Almeida DM. Age differences in exposure and reactions to interpersonal tensions: A daily diary study. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(2):330–340. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolger N, Davis A, Rafaeli E. Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54:579–616. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce ML. Divorce and psychopathology. In: Dohrenwend BP, editor. Adversity, stress, and psychopathology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998. pp. 219–232. [Google Scholar]
- Carney MA, Armeli S, Tennen H, Affleck G, O’Neil TP. Positive and negative daily events, perceived stress, and alcohol use: A diary study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68(5):788–798. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cherpitel CJ. Screening for alcohol problems in the U.S. general population: Comparison of the CAGE, RAPS4, and RAPS4-QF by gender, ethnicity, and service utilization. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2002;26(11):1686–1691. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000036300.26619.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cranford JA. DSM-IV alcohol dependence and marital dissolution: Evidence from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2014;75(3):520–529. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cranford JA, Floyd FJ, Schulenberg JE, Zucker RA. Husbands’ and wives’ alcohol use disorders and marital interactions as longitudinal predictors of marital adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120(1):210–222. doi: 10.1037/a0021349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cranford JA, Tennen H, Zucker RA. Feasibility of using interactive voice response to monitor daily drinking, moods, and relationship processes on a daily basis in alcoholic couples. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2010;34:499–508. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01115.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Koning E, Weiss RL. The Relational Humor Inventory: Functions of humor in close relationships. American Journal of Family Therapy. 2002;30:1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Dunn NJ, Jacob T, Hummon N, Seilhamer RA. Marital instability in alcoholic-spouse relationships as a function of drinking pattern and location. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1987;96:99–107. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.96.2.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fincham FD, Linfield KJ. A new look at marital quality: Can spouses feel positive and negative about their marriage? Journal of Family Psychology. 1997;11(4):489–502. [Google Scholar]
- Fitzmaurice GM, Laird NM, Ware JH. Applied longitudinal analysis. Hoboken, N.J: Wiley-Interscience; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Fu H, Goldman N. The association between health-related behaviours and the risk of divorce in the USA. Journal of Biosocial Science. 2000;32(1):63–88. doi: 10.1017/s0021932000000638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber JR, Jacob T. Marital interactions of male versus female alcoholics. Family Process. 1997;36(4):385–402. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1997.00385.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halford WK, Osgarby SM. Alcohol abuse in clients presenting with marital problems. Journal of Family Psychology. 1993;6:245–254. [Google Scholar]
- Helzer JE, Badger GJ, Rose GL, Mongeon JA, Searles JS. Decline in alcohol consumption during two years of daily reporting. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;63(5):551–558. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heyman RE. Rapid Marial Interaction Coding System (RMICS) In: Kerig PK, Baucom DH, editors. Couple observation coding systems. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2004. pp. 67–93. [Google Scholar]
- Huston TL, Melz H. The case for (promoting) marriage: The devil is in the details. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2004;66(4):943–958. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00064.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Homish GG, Leonard KE. The drinking partnership and marital satisfaction: The longitudinal influence of discrepant drinking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75(1):43–51. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johns AL, Newcomb MD, Johnson MD, Bradbury TN. Alcohol-related problems, anger, and marital satisfaction in monoethnic Latino, biethnic Latino, and European American newlywed couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2007;24:255–275. [Google Scholar]
- Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:3–34. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Walters EE, Forthofer MS. The social consequences of psychiatric disorders, III: Probability of marital stability. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;155(8):1092–1096. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.8.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Malarkey WB, Chee M, Newton TL, Cacioppo JT, Mao HY, Glaser R. Negative behavior during marital conflict is associated with immunological down-regulation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1993;55(5):395–409. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199309000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:472–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knox D. Marriage happiness: A behavioral approach to counseling. Champaign, Ill: Research Press; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Kranzler HR, Abu-Hasaballah K, Tennen H, Feinn R, Young K. Using daily Interactive Voice Response technology to measure drinking and related behaviors in a pharmacotherapy study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2004;28:1060–1064. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000130806.12066.9c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard KE, Eiden RD. Marital and family processes in the context of alcohol use and alcohol disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2007;3(1):285–310. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt A, Cooper ML. Daily alcohol use and romantic relationship functioning: Evidence of bidirectional, gender-, and context-specific effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2010;36(12):1706–1722. doi: 10.1177/0146167210388420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manne S, Ostroff J, Rini C, Fox K, Goldstein L, Grana G. The interpersonal process model of intimacy: The role of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and partner responsiveness in interactions between breast cancer patients and their partners. Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18:589–599. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.4.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshal MP. For better or for worse? The effects of alcohol use on marital functioning. Clinical Psychology Review. 2003;23:959–997. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2003.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCrady BS, Epstein EE, Cook S, Jensen N, Hildebrandt T. A randomized trial of individual and couple behavioral alcohol treatment for women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77(2):243–256. doi: 10.1037/a0014686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Tonigan JS, Longabaugh R. The Drinker Inventory of Consequences (DrInC): An instrument for assessing adverse consequences of alcohol abuse. Vol. 4. Rockville, MD: NIAAA; 1995. Vol. Project MATCH Monograph Series. [Google Scholar]
- Mohr CD, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G, Hromi A. Daily interpersonal experiences, context, and alcohol consumption: Crying in your beer and toasting good times? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:489–500. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohr CD, Armeli S, Tennen H, Todd M. The complexities of modeling mood–drinking relationships: Lessons learned from daily process research. In: Kassel JD, editor. Substance abuse and emotion. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 189–216. [Google Scholar]
- Mudar P, Kearns JN, Leonard KE. The transition to marriage and changes in alcohol involvement among Black couples and White couples. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;63(5):568–576. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. NIH Publication No 04-5346. Bethesda, MD: NIAAA Office of Research Translation and Communications; Winter. 2004. NIAAA Council approves definition of binge drinking. [Google Scholar]
- National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. NIH Publication No 07-3769. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health; 2007. Helping patients who drink too much: A clinician’s guide. [Google Scholar]
- O’Farrell TJ, Clements K. Review of outcome research on marital and family therapy in treatment for alcoholism. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 2012;38(1):122–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2011.00242.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olenick NL, Chalmers DK. Gender-specific drinking styles in alcoholics and nonalcoholics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1991;52(4):325–330. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1991.52.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paolino TJ, McCrady BS, Diamond S. Statistics on alcoholic marriages: An overview. International Journal of the Addictions. 1978;13(8):1285–1293. doi: 10.3109/10826087809039342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park CL, Armeli S, Tennen H. The daily stress and coping process and alcohol use among college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2004;65:126–135. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2004.65.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramisetty-Mikler S, Caetano R. Alcohol use and intimate partner violence as predictors of separation among U.S. couples: A longitudinal model. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2005;66(2):205–212. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SAS Institute. SAS/STAT 9.2 user’s guide. 2. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Schulz MS, Cowan PA, Pape Cowan C, Brennan RT. Coming home upset: Gender, marital satisfaction, and the daily spillover of workday experience into couple interactions. Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18:250–263. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Searles JS, Perrine MW, Mundt JC, Helzer JE. Self-report of drinking using touch-tone telephone: Extending the limits of reliable daily contact. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1995;56(4):375–382. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1995.56.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone AA, Shiffman S. Capturing momentary, self-report data: A proposal for reporting guidelines. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2002;24(3):236–243. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2403_09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennen H, Affleck G, Armeli S. Personality and daily experience revisited. Journal of Personality. 2005;73(6):1465–1484. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00355.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennen H, Affleck G, Armeli S, Carney MA. A daily process approach to coping: Linking theory, research, and practice. American Psychologist. 2000;55(6):626–636. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.55.6.626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vinokur AD, Price RH, Caplan RD. Hard times and hurtful partners: How financial strain affects depression and relationship satisfaction of unemployed persons and their spouses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71(1):166–179. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.1.166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]