Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2014 Mar 6;55(3):225–232. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12114

Age and sex related changes in episodic memory function in middle aged and older adults

Astri J Lundervold 1,2,3, Daniel Wollschläger 1,4, Eike Wehling 1,2,5
PMCID: PMC4314696  PMID: 24601911

Abstract

Age-related change in episodic memory function is commonly reported in older adults. When detected on neuropsychological tests, it may still be difficult to distinguish normal from pathological changes. The present study investigates age-and sex-related changes in a group of healthy middle-aged and older adults, participating in a three-wave study on cognitive aging. The California Verbal Learning test (CVLT-II) was used to assess their episodic memory function. A cross-sectional analysis of results from the first wave showed higher performance in females than males, with a steeper age-related decline in males. This was confirmed in a longitudinal analysis using a mixed effects regression model, but with a lower age-related change and smaller difference between the sexes. Information about learning strategies and errors in the third wave turned out to contribute significantly to explain change in episodic memory function across the three waves. We argue that the results from the longitudinal analyses are generalizable to the population of healthy middle-aged and older individuals, and that they could be useful in guiding clinicians when evaluating individuals with respect to cognitive change.

Keywords: CVLT, cognitive aging, longitudinal, mixed effect regression model

Introduction

Older adults frequently report changes in cognitive function, very often with a concern about degenerative disorder. However, the precise distinction between normal and pathological changes in cognitive function may be obscure. Knowledge about characteristics of normal cognitive aging and its influence on test results are therefore essential to a clinical neuropsychologist.

This is clearly exemplified when assessing episodic memory function, where changes are known to represent one of the earliest signs of a neurodegenerative disorder (e.g., Bäckman, Small & Fratiglioni, 2001; Petersen, 2004; Twamley, Ropacki & Bondi, 2006). At the same time, episodic memory function is considered to show age-related decline in healthy individuals, with cross-sectional studies reporting a gradual decline with an onset that may start as early as in the twenties (Salthouse, 2009; Zelinski & Burnight, 1997). These findings are, however, nuanced by results from longitudinal studies. Such studies have both reported a markedly later age of onset of cognitive change and large individual differences in developmental pathways (Nyberg, Lövden, Riklund, Lindenberger & Bäckman, 2012; Rønnlund, Nyberg, Bäckman & Nilsson, 2005; Schaie, 2005), leaving some eighty year olds with a function that is not different from what is shown by much younger individuals.

Tests with multiple learning trials are widely used to assess episodic memory function in elderly, and are among the most sensitive measures when changes in memory function indicate subsequent cognitive decline and dementia (Blacker, Lee, Muzikansky et al., 2007; Kaltreider, Cicerello, Lacritz, Honig, Rosenberg & Cullum, 2000; Rabin, Par, Saykin et al., 2009; Tierney, Yao, Kiss & McDowell, 2005). This is due to characteristics of these tests, involving complex processes of memory function, including working memory, the ability to learn, consolidate and recollect information. Several list-learning tests have therefore been developed, providing information about different aspects of the memory process. Some tests include measures of higher-order cognitive functions, such as the ability to organize information efficiently and correctly, functions that are shown to be important mediators of encoding and recall (Davis, Klebe, Guinther, Schroder, Cornwell & James, 2013). A main strength of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan & Ober, 2000) is its inclusion of measures of learning strategy and errors in addition to more traditional measures of learning, recall and recognition, characteristics that together have contributed to the test's sensitivity to mild cognitive change (Benedict & Zgaljardic, 1998; Greenaway, Lacritz, Binegar, Weiner, Lipton & Munro Cullum, 2006; Lacritz, Cullum, Weiner & Rosenberg, 2001; Rabin et al., 2009) and dementia (Delis et al., 2000).

It has been shown that females tend to outperform males on verbal tests of episodic memory function (Herlitz, Nilsson & Bäckman, 1997). A recent study by Sunderaraman, Blumen, DeMatteo, Apa and Cosentino (2013) indicated that superior recall in females on CVLT is related to their higher semantic clustering score. This sex difference has also been related to a less lateralized brain function in females than males, but results from studies relating verbal function to brain areas have been conflicting (Wallentin, 2009). Gender effects on cognitive function are obviously modulated by several neurobiological factors (see Reinvang, Winjevoll, Rootwelt & Espeseth, 2010b), emphasizing that sex should be taken into account when evaluating age-related changes in cognitive function.

The present study investigates changes in episodic memory function associated with normal aging. One obvious aim of studies on age-related changes is to obtain predictions that can be generalized to the population under study. As stated previously, predictions based on cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have come to different conclusions, with the former underestimating cognitive abilities in older age (Nyberg et al., 2012). However, longitudinal studies are also challenged by several factors. In addition to measurement errors and learning effects (Levine, Svoboda, Hay, Winocur & Moscovitch, 2002; Salthouse, 2012), longitudinal analyses commonly must take into account varying time-spans between assessments of individual participants, incomplete data sets and different correlation structures over time. This makes it crucial to use a statistical procedure that can handle these factors appropriately. Traditional linear regression models fall short in this regard (Long, 2012), while they are much more appropriately handled by mixed effects regression models (Galecki & Burzykowski, 2013). Another important strength of using mixed effects models is their ability to explicitly incorporate the effect of individual differences, and by this get more reliable estimates of the variability in a population characterized by cognitive heterogeneity.

The present study, including a sample of healthy middle-aged and older adults, investigates age-related changes in episodic memory function both cross-sectionally by using a standard regression model and longitudinally by using a mixed effects regression model. Episodic memory function was assessed by the standard version of the second edition of CVLT (CVLT-II) in three study waves with about three years apart. In the cross-sectional analysis, including measures of learning, recall, recognition, the use of an active learning strategy and accuracy in responses, we expected females to perform better than males, with a steeper age-related decline in males. To minimize measurement errors in the longitudinal analysis, we used a composite memory score calculated from the measures of learning, recall and recognition as a dependent variable. To control for the effect of repeated testing, the effect of a second assessment was included as an independent variable. From earlier studies we expected that the longitudinal analysis would demonstrate a lower age-related change than revealed in the cross-sectional analysis, and that this lower change would be found in both sexes. When a neuropsychologist uses CVLT-II to evaluate if an individual has shown a decline in episodic memory function, performance reflecting learning strategy and errors will commonly contribute to the conclusion. We therefore extended the longitudinal analysis by investigating how much information about these variables from the third wave adds to the longitudinal estimate of change in episodic memory function explained by age and sex.

Methods

Sample

Healthy individuals were invited through advertisement to take part in the first wave of a longitudinal study on cognitive aging (N = 163). All participants were examined according to an extensive neuropsychological test protocol, including the Norwegian translation of CVLT-II (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan & Ober, 2004) and a test of intellectual function. Subjects with a history of substance abuse, present neurologic or psychiatric disorder, or other significant medical conditions, were excluded from the study. Based on neuropsychological test results, none of the participants showed a mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as defined by Petersen (2004). Five participants were excluded from the present study, three due to pathology detected at the following waves, one due to an IQ < 80 and one due to missing data on the CVLT-II, leaving 158 participants included in the cross-sectional analyses of the data from the first wave. All individuals from the first wave were invited to a follow-up study about three years after the first examination and to a third wave about three years after the second one. In the longitudinal analyses we included all individuals participating in at least two waves (n = 126), of whom 103 participated in all three waves. The analyses of the contribution of performance characteristics in explaining change across the three waves were restricted to these 103 participants. Information about age and intellectual function is presented separately for males and females in Table 1.

Table 1.

Means and standard deviations of demographic and CVLT-II raw-scores for females and males (Wave 1)

Females n = 106 Males n = 52
Age 61.1 (7.6) 61.7 (8.3)
Education 13.8 (3.2) 13.4 (3.6)
IQ 114.0 (12.5) 115.5 (12.8)
Matrix Reasoning 58.1 (8.6) 59.9 (8.6)
Vocabulary 57.3 (8.2) 57.3 (8.8)
Learning 54.6 (9.5) 45.2 (10.5)
Recall 24.5 (5.3) 19.5 (6.1)
Recognition 15.0 (1.3) 14.7 (1.4)
cvltSumZ –0.71 (2.4) –1.7 (2.7)
Semantic clustering 1.76 (2.1) 0.94 (1.8)
Consistency 85.6 (8.3) 78.6 (9.9)
Primacy effect 29.4 (5.5) 31.1 (7.0)
Intrusions 5.1 (5.7) 5.7 (6.3)
False Positives 1.6 (2.0) 3.6 (3.6)

Instruments

Intellectual function

First wave performances on two subtests from the Norwegian translation of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, 2007), the Matrix Reasoning and Vocabulary subtests, were used to estimate intellectual function (IQ). Performance was scored and the full-scale IQ (FSIQ) estimated according to norms presented in the test-manual (Wechsler, 1999). The Matrix Reasoning subtest, included in all waves, showed stable age corrected T-scores over time, (r = 0.65, p < 0.001), and a significant correlation with the FSIQ measure in the second (r = 0.85, p < 0.001) and third wave (r = 0.66, p < 0.001). This stability indicates that the IQ measure from the first wave could be used as an estimate across all study waves.

California Verbal Learning Test

In CVLT-II (Delis et al., 2000) 16 words (List A), drawn from four semantic categories, are presented five times. After each presentation the participant is asked to repeat as many words as possible. Immediately after the fifth trial, a new list (List B) is read and the participant is asked to repeat as many words as possible. A short delayed test (CVLT-SD) is presented immediately after recall of List B, where the participant is asked to recall the words of List A, first without cues (free) and then with cues. This last procedure is repeated after a delay of 20 minutes (CVLT-LD). Finally, a yes-no recognition test is presented, including the 16 items from List A, the 16 from List B and 16 random distractor items.

In the present study we included three main measures of episodic memory function: the total number of correct words reported across the five learning trials (Learning), a sum of words recalled on the free versions of CVLT-SD and CVLT-LD (Recall), and the number of hits on the recognition trial (Recognition). Three scores were included to represent the learning strategy: Semantic clustering, defined as the number of correct list A items consecutively recalled from the same category relative to the expected number based on chance; Consistency, defined as the percentage of items from Trial 1 to 4 that were recalled on the next trial; and the Primacy effect, defined as the percentage of items recalled from the primacy region for Trials 1-5. Higher scores on each of these variables indicate an active learning strategy. Two scores were used to represent accuracy: number of Intrusions across all recall trials and the number of False positive errors on the Recognition trial.

Statistics

All analyses were performed using R (version 3.0.1; R Development Core Team, 2013). The impact of age, sex and the interaction between the two (age: sex) were analyzed in a series of separate regression analyses with scores on the selected CVLT-II measures as dependent variables. Significant models were followed by hierarchical analyses to investigate the contribution from the independent variables. The longitudinal analyses were performed using a linear mixed effects regression model (Galecki & Burzykowski, 2013), using the lme4 package (Bates, Maechler & Bolker, 2013) to model the relationship between CVLT-II performance and age-related change for females and males.

The linear mixed effects regression model included a composite score representing episodic memory function as the dependent variable, with retest effect (cvltRe), age, sex and the interaction between age and sex as the sequence of independent variables. In addition to these fixed-effects terms, the model included a random intercept term. The composite memory score cvltSumZ was defined as the sum of z-transformation of all values of the raw scores from the total learning trials and the raw scores from the short and long free recall trials over all waves from all subjects. The retest effect was accounted for in the longitudinal model by including the time-varying factor cvltRe with two levels as a predictor. Following the recommendation from Ferrer, Salthouse, Stewart and Schwartz (2004), cvltRe was set to ‘no’ for measurements from the first wave, and to ‘yes’ for measurements from the second and third wave. Thus, parameter estimates for other covariates were adjusted for an additive practice effect, which we assumed to occur mainly at the second testing, with the third testing adding no additional learning benefit. This strategy was successfully employed in a recent longitudinal study on cognitive aging (Finkel, Reynolds, Larsson, Gatz & Pedersen, 2011).

The Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) were used to evaluate the most plausible model in the set of models being tested (Long, 2012). In all subsequent analyses the significance of fixed effect parameters were tested using step-down model comparisons that started with the full model, then sequentially dropped predictor terms. We report two-tailed p-values resulting from partial F-tests with degrees of freedom estimated according to the Kenward-Roger procedure as implemented in the R package pbkrtest (Halekoh & Højsgaard, 2013). A similar procedure was used to analyze the contribution of learning strategy and error measures in explaining the change in episodic memory function (i.e., cvltSumZ). The measures were included in separate analyses in a model controlling for cvltRe, age and sex.

Results

Cross-sectional analyses -wave 1

Demographics, IQ and CVLT-II results

Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for age, years of education, intellectual function and CVLT-II raw-scores in males (N = 52) and females (N = 106). All sex-differences on demographic variables were non-significant in a series of t-tests comparing males and females. The age-corrected IQ score and its subscores showed means in the upper level of normal function, with no significant sex difference. At wave 1, the females obtained statistically significant higher scores on Learning (t = 5.6, p < 0.001), Recall (t = 5.4, p < 0.001), but not Recognition (t = 1.5, ns). On the measures of learning strategy, the females obtained a significantly higher (better) score on Semantic clustering (t = 2.4, p = 0.019) and Consistency (t = 4.7, p < 0.001), but not on the Primacy effect (t = 1.6, ns). The sex-difference was also statistically significant for the False positive errors, t = 4.3, p > 0.001, but not for Intrusions, t = 0.61, ns.

Primary memory measures

Regression models including age, sex and the interaction between age and sex as dependent variables were statistically significant for Learning, Recall, but not for Recognition (Table 2). For Learning the model showed a significant effect of age (t = 4.15, p < 0.001), sex (t = 2.21, p < 0.05), and the age:sex interaction (t = 2.98, p < 0.01). For Recall there was a significant effect of age (t = 3.3, p < 0.001). Females showed a lower intercept and less age-related decline than males: with Learning as a dependent variable, the average loss was 0.07 units per year for females and 0.7 for males, with Recall the average loss per year was 0.1 units for females and 0.3 for males (Table 2). The hierarchical model showed a significant effect of sex when controlling for age both for Learning, F(1,155) = 33.2, p < 0.001, and Recall, F(1,155) = 33.3, p < 0.001.

Table 2.

Effects of age, sex and the age:sex interaction on CVLT-II scores in wave 1

F-test p-value Adj.R2 I:Fa Agea Age:Fa
Learning 17.4 <0.001 0.25 –27.2 –0.66 0.59
Recall 14.8 <0.001 0.21 –7.0 –0.30 0.20
Recognition 1.7 0.18 0.01 –1.1 –0.03 0.02
CvltSumZ 16.7 <0.001 0.23 –4.5 –0.15 0.11
Semantic clustering 0.1 0.02 0.05 –2.1 −0.07 0.05
Consistency 8.1 <0.001 0.12 –6.3 −0.24 0.22
Primacy effect 1.0 0.41 0.00 –4.9 –0.01 –0.05
Intrusions 1.2 0.32 0.00 6.9 0.17 –0.12
False positives 8.6 <0.001 0.13 4.2 0.11 –0.10

Note:

a

Estimated unstandardized regression coefficients: I:F = intercept differences females versus males; Age = age-related change with males as a reference; Age:F = the age-related effect for females added to the age-related change with males as a reference.

The results for the composite score of episodic memory function (cvltSumZ) confirmed the lower intercept for females than males (p < 0.05) at reference age (65), and a steeper age-related change for males (–0.15) than for females (–0.04). The regression line is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The composite score cvltSumZ in males and females in the cross-sectional analysis of wave 1 data with fixed effects uncertainty.

Among the learning strategy measures, the regression model was statistically significant for Consistency and Semantic clustering (Table 2), with females showing a lower intercept and a lower age-related decline than males. The hierarchical models showed statistically significant effects of sex for both Semantic clustering, F(1,155) = 5.3, p = 0.02 and Consistency, F(1,155) = 21.3, p > 0.001. For the error measures, the regression model was only statistically significant for False positive errors (Table 2). The intercept was higher in females than males, with a very small age-related change in females (0.01) compared to the change in males (0.11).

Longitudinal analyses

Demographics and CVLT-II variables

There was a mean follow-up time of 6.5 years (range = 5.2 –7.7 years) for participants included in the longitudinal analyzes, with a mean of 3.5 years (range = 2.8 – 4.7 years) between the first two waves, and 3 years (range 2 – 4 years) between Wave 2 and 3. The 23 participants who were lost to follow up for wave 3 were not significantly different on any of the demographic and CVLT-II variables selected for the study. The changes in mean scores on the composite measure of episodic memory function (cvltSumZ) and all CVLT-II scores are shown in Table 3. The largest change occurred between Wave 2 and Wave 3, where all except the Recognition score were significantly lower in the latter. From the first to the second wave, there was a significant increase on the Learning, the composite score cvltSumZ and the Semantic Clustering scores.

Table 3.

Means and standard deviations for CVLT-II raw-scores across the three waves, mean changes across waves and associated p-values from paired t-tests Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3

CVLT-II variables n = 126 n = 126 n = 103 t 1–2a p 1–2a t 2–3b p 2–3 b
Learning 52.9 (9.9) 55.6 (11.4) 52.9 (10.9) –3.5 < 0.001 5.5 < 0.001
Recall 23.8 (5.6) 24.4 (6.0) 21.7 (6.6) –1.4 0.1 7.8 < 0.001
Recognition 15.0 (1.2) 15.2 (1.1) 15.8 (1.2) –1.4 0.2 0.9 0.40
cvltSumZ 0.3 (2.5) 0.8 (2.8) –0.6 (2.9) –2.5 0.01 9.2 < 0.001
Semantic clustering 1.5 (2.0) 2.4 (2.7) 1.5 (5.7) –4.2 < 0.001 4.8 < 0.001
Consistency 85.0 (8.3) 85.0 (10.0) 83.0 (5.9) 1.5 0.14 –2.1 0.04
Primacy 29.6 (5.9) 28.7 (2.2) 29.4 (6.3) –0.2 0.80 2.9 0.01
Intrusions 4.6 (5.2) 4.1 (4.4) 6.3 (5.7) 0.9 0.40 –4.2 < 0.001
False Positives 2.0 (2.7) 1.7 (2.8) 3.3 (4.1) 1.5 0.10 -6.4 < 0.001

Note:

a

t 1-2/p 1-2: t-test/p-value between score in Wave 1 and 2.

b

t 2-3/p 2-3: t-test/p-value between score in Wave 2 and 3.

The retest factor cvltRe showed a Kenward-Roger partial F-value of 1.78 ( p = 0.18) for cvltSumZ, with a regression weight of 0.37. This means that on average, the retest was 0.37 cvltSumZ units higher than the score in Wave 1.

The effect of age and sex on change in episodic memory function

A linear mixed effects model (Table 4) including cvltRe, age, sex and age:sex interaction as independent variables and the composite score cvltSumZ as dependent variable, showed a considerable variation in estimated individual intercepts. The variance was 3.86 units change in cvltSumZ score, highlighting the need to take individual differences into account in the model. The age-related change in males was –0.14 units on the cvltSumZ score, with females showing a somewhat lower change (–0.12). The AIC and BIC showed the best model fit when cvltRe, age and sex were included, with no improvement of fit by the age:sex interaction. The Kenward-Roger partial F-test showed a significant contribution from both age and sex, but the difference between age-related change in males and females was far lower than in the cross-sectional analyses (Table 4).

Table 4.

Results from mixed effects regression analysis with Kenward-Roger partial F-tests and estimated regression coefficients for the composite episodic memory score (cvltSumZ), and the Learning and Recall scores

Dependent variables the regression model AICa BICa F-test df1 df2 P-value I: Fb Ageb Age:Fb
cvltSumZ 1.28 –0.14 0.02
 cvltRe 1582 1598 1.8 1 229.2 0.18
 Age 1554 1574 31.4 1 173.2 < 0.001
 Sex 1528 1551 31.8 1 123.7 < 0.001
 Age:Sex 1530 1537 0.1 1 275.8 0.71
Learning 4.57 –0.52 0.06
 cvltRe 2149 2164 0.1 1 229.5 0.76
 Age 2603 2619 27.0 1 162.2 < 0.001
 Sex 2556 2579 27.7 1 123.8 < 0.001
 Age:sex 2558 2585 0.1 1 251.3 0.73
Recall 2.26 –0.30 0.04
 CvltRe 2149 2164 3.1 1 229.2 0.08
 Age 2124 2143 27.2 1 171.1 < 0.001
 Sex 2098 2121 31.6 1 123.7 < 0.001
 Age:sex 2099 2127 0.2 1 272.0 0.64

Note:

a

AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion.

b

Estimated regression coefficients: I:F = intercept difference females vs. males; Age = age-related change with males as a reference; Age:F = the added age-related effect for females to the age-related change with males as a reference.

The linear mixed effects model computed separately for Learning and Recall confirmed the high variation in individual intercept, with a variance of 54.4 and 18.4 units change, respectively. Their model fit was not increased by including the age:sex interaction in the model, while the contribution from age and sex was confirmed by the Kenward-Roger partial F-test on both variables. Although the age-related decline was steeper for males than females (–0.52 and –0.30), it was not markedly different from the decline in females (–0.48 and –0.26, see Table 4). The regression line is illustrated in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The composite score cvltSumZ in males and females in the longitudinal analysis with fixed effects uncertainty.

Change in episodic memory function explained by learning strategy and errors

In the next series of analyses we investigated the add-on effect of information about learning strategy and errors in the third wave in explaining the change in cvltSumZ score. Each measure of learning strategy or errors from the third wave was included as predictor in separate analyses after inclusion of cvltRe, age and sex. The improved model fit was shown by a lower AIC or BIC score than when only the three basic variables were controlled for. The effects were, however, marginal for the Primacy effect and Intrusions (Table 5). The Kenward-Roger partial F-tests and the estimated regression weights showed strongest effect for Semantic clustering, Consistency and False positive errors. In the model adding Semantic clustering as a predictor, the estimated age-related decline was 0.10 units of the cvltSumZ score per year, and cvltSumZ was estimated to increase by 0.51 units for each additional unit of the Semantic clustering score. The add-on effect of the Consistency score was 0.10 units per year, and for each additional False Positive error, the cvltSumZ was estimated to drop by 0.35 units. (Table 5).

Table 5.

Contributions from learning strategy and errors to explain prior change in episodic memory function with Kenward-Roger partial F-test and regression weights

Predictor variables the regression model AICa BICa F-test df2 p-value Ageb Addb t-testb
Learning strategy and errors
cvltRe 1377 1392 2.2 205.0 00.14
Age 1346 1365 34.6 144.8 < 0.001
Sex 1327 1349 23.5 100.1 < 0.001
Semantic clustering 1297 1323 38.5 100.0 < 0.001 –0.10 0.51 6.2
Consistency 1291 1318 46.3 99.8 < 0.001 –0.10 0.13 6.8
Primacy effect 1324 1351 4.4 99.7 0.04 –0.14 –0.07 –2.1
Intrusions 1322 1348 7.2 99.5 0.009 –0.14 –0.09 –2.7
False positive errors 1279 1305 68.1 100.3 < 0.001 –0.08 –0.35 –8.3

Note:

a

AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion. **Regression weights.

b

Age = the regression weight estimate of Age; **Add = the added regression weight estimate when cvltRe, age and sex were controlled for; **t-test: t-test for the estimate of the learning strategy and error measures.

Discussion

Cross-sectional analyses of CVLT-II results in the current sample showed a higher function in females than males across all ages, with a steeper age-related decline in males. This was found on all primary measures of episodic memory function, the learning strategy measure of Consistency and the error measure from the recognition trial (False positive errors). The main pattern of age-related change was confirmed by the longitudinal analyses, but with a somewhat smaller age-related change and a much lower difference between the two sexes. Finally, information about learning strategy and accuracy in the third wave was shown to add to the age-related change across the three study waves. By using a statistical analysis taking both group mean, individual differences and learning effect into account, the findings of the present study should give a valid estimate of change in episodic memory function in a population of healthy middle-aged and older adults. The impact of accuracy and learning strategies on change from previous study waves should be considered by clinical neuropsychologist evaluating individuals reporting that they already have experienced change in episodic memory function.

Overall, the pattern of age-related change was confirmed both by the cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses, with a somewhat lower change in the latter. A similar pattern of change was shown for some of the measures of learning strategy and accuracy in the cross-sectional analyses. However, the main contribution of the present study was the results from the longitudinal analyses using a mixed effects regression model, letting group effects be moderated by individual differences and vice versa. By this, it should give a reasonable estimate of age- and sex-related changes that can be generalized to other groups of middle-aged and older participants, although the relatively few males included in the study may limit the power of the sex-related results. The importance of using a mixed model was emphasized by detecting a substantial variance in individual intercepts, meaning that a strong effect of individual differences may well have influenced results from the cross-sectional analyses. The inclusion of information about the learning-effect in the statistical model was another important aspect of the study, and may explain some differences between our results and results in previous studies. The age-related change in the present main longitudinal analysis was somewhat different from the results in studies documenting a rather stable cognitive function until the individual is more than 60 years old (e.g., (Nilsson, Sternang, Rønnlund & Nyberg, 2009; Rønnlund et al., 2005; Schaie, 2005; Zelinski & Stewart, 1998). Interestingly, some longitudinal studies have shown improvement in cognitive function over time in middle-aged and older adults (e.g., Flicker, Ferris & Reisberg, 1993). This was also found in the present study, but was interpreted as an improvement due to repeated assessment. We argue that the statistical procedure presented by Ferrer et al. (2004) gave us a feasible alternative to more resource intensive designs, such as inclusion of new samples when the original longitudinal participants are tested at the second time (e.g., Rønnlund et al., 2005) or by using long intervals between test sessions (e.g., Rabbitt, Diggle, Smith, Holland & Mc Innes, 2001).

Finally, the present study showed the importance of information about learning strategy and accuracy in explaining change across the three study waves. The results were convincing by showing a high contribution from almost all included variables. The strongest effect was found for number of false positive errors on the recognition trial. This may be related to what Schacter Koutstaal & Norman (1997) described as a decline in how distinctly older people encode information. It may also explain why errors on the recognition task are more prominent than on the recall measures: answers on the recognition trial rely highly on familiarity (see Martin-Ordas & Call, 2013), giving the participants cues that are on the top of what Stretch and Wixted (1998) referred to as a ‘strength-of-evidence axis’. When participants are uncertain about an answer, but find the word familiar, it may be tempting to give a positive rather than a negative answer. Although age-related changes in learning strategy and accuracy may be part of normal cognitive aging, the present study showed an add-on effect: a less active learning strategy and a high number of errors were related to a change in episodic memory function that was more substantial than the change expected from age and sex alone. In the present study we used information about learning strategy and accuracy to explain change from previous study waves, assuming that this may correspond to the information obtained by a neuropsychologist evaluating an individual who reports change in memory function. Future studies of their value in predicting future decline are necessary before concluding about its significance as a symptom of a neurodegenerative disorder.

There are several limitations to the present study. First of all, the study has not included information about biological factors of importance to cognitive change in samples of presumably healthy individuals, for example, hormonal sex-differences (Sherwin, 1998; Shaywitz, Shaywitz, Pugh et al., 1999), small age-dependent vascular lesions (Papp, Kaplan, Springate et al., 2013), genetic factors (Bender & Raz, 2012, Reinvang, Espeseth & Westlye, 2013) and sensory problems (Rønnberg et al., 2011). In addition, there are several other essential factors, which are well described in cognitive reserve- (Stern, 2002, 2009), scaffolding (Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009) and brain maintenance (Nyberg et al., 2012; Pudas, Persson, Josefsson, de Luna, Nilsson & Nyberg, 2013) models of cognitive aging. The composite score of episodic memory function was calculated from subtests from CVLT-II. Although this may restrict our conclusions to results on CVLT-II, we will argue that it may also represent a strength: the scores were expected to tap the same cognitive domain and to have the same sensitivity and level of difficulty, which may be difficult to obtain when using tests from different test-batteries and traditions. Although we showed that CVLT-II was sensitive to age-related cognitive changes, information about other aspects of memory function (see Wang, Li, Li & Zhang, 2013), attention and executive function (Reinvang, Deary, Fjell, Steen, Espeseth & Parasuraman, 2010a, Reinvang et al. 2010b) are of importance before concluding about cognitive change in an individual. Finally, we used a linear regression model, which gave us a good approximation of cognitive change. More complicated regression models could be employed, for example by using quadratic terms and splines. This would, however, require a larger sample and should also include more waves.

Conclusions

The CVLT-II identified cognitive change in a sample of healthy middle-aged and older individuals. The lower age-related change and sex difference in the longitudinal than cross-sectional analyses were probably related to using a linear mixed effects model taking both individual and group factors into account. The add-on value of information about learning strategy and accuracy in explaining age-related change in episodic memory function emphasizes the importance of taking such qualitative characteristics into account when evaluating patients reporting cognitive change. Assuming that these measures reflect abilities to associate and organize the words in the learning list, it is obvious that aspects of attention and executive function are involved in the observed cognitive change. Further longitudinal studies of larger cohorts including a wide range of individual factors in a proper statistical framework are thus warranted.

Acknowledgments

We thank Professor Ivar Reinvang for initiating the first wave of the study on cognitive aging and for providing fruitful collaboration with the Department of Psychology, University of Oslo. The study was financially supported by Western Norway Health Authority (grant 911397 and 911687 to AJL, and grant 911461 to EW).

References

  1. Bäckman L, Small BJ, Fratiglioni L. Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2001;124:96–102. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.1.96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B. 2013. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes [Computer software]. Retrieved 1 September 2013 from https://github.com/lme4/lme4/ (R package version 0.999999-2)
  3. Bender AR, Raz N. Age-related differences in episodic memory: A synergistic contribution of genetic and physiological vascular risk factors. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:442–450. doi: 10.1037/a0028669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benedict RH, Zgaljardic DJ. Practice effects during repeated administrations of memory tests with and without alternate forms. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1998;20:339–352. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.3.339.822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blacker D, Lee H, Muzikansky A, Martin EC, Tanzi R, McArdle JJ, et al. Neuropsychological measures in normal individuals that predict subsequent cognitive decline. Archives of Neurology. 2007;64:862–871. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.6.862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Davis HP, Klebe KJ, Guinther PM, Schroder KB, Cornwell RE, James LE. Subjective organization, verbal learning, and forgetting across the life span: From 5 to 89. Experimental Aging Research. 2013;39:1–26. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2013.741956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan D, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test. 2nd edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  8. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan D, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) Pearson Assessment: Norwegian manual supplement. Stockholm; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  9. Ferrer E, Salthouse TA, Stewart WF, Schwartz BS. Modeling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. Psychology and Aging. 2004;19:243–259. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Finkel D, Reynolds CA, Larsson M, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. Both odor identification and ApoE-ε4 contribute to normative cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging. 2011;26:872–883. doi: 10.1037/a0023371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Flicker C, Ferris SH, Reisberg B. A longitudinal study of cognitive function in elderly persons with subjective memory complaints. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1993;41:1029–1032. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1993.tb06448.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Galecki A, Burzykowski N. Linear mixed-effects models using R: A step-by-step approach. New York: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  13. Greenaway MC, Lacritz LH, Binegar D, Weiner MF, Lipton A, Munro Cullum C. Patterns of verbal memory performance in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease, and normal aging. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2006;19:79–84. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000208290.57370.a3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Halekoh U, Højsgaard S. 2013. pbkrtest: Parametric bootstrap and Kenward Roger based methods for mixed model comparison [Computer software]. Retrieved 1 September 2013 from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package = pbkrtest (R package version 0.3-5)
  15. Herlitz A, Nilsson LG, Bäckman L. Gender differences in episodic memory. Memory and Cognition. 1997;25:801–811. doi: 10.3758/bf03211324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kaltreider LB, Cicerello AR, Lacritz LH, Honig LS, Rosenberg RN, Cullum MC. Comparison of the Cerad and CVLT list-learning tasks in Alzheimer's disease. Clinical Neuropsychology. 2000;14:269–274. doi: 10.1076/1385-4046(200008)14:3;1-P;FT269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lacritz LH, Cullum CM, Weiner MF, Rosenberg RN. Comparison of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-revised to the California Verbal Learning Test in Alzheimer's disease. Applied Neuropsychology. 2001;8:180–184. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0803_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17:677–689. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Long J. Longitudinal data analysis for the behavioral sciences using R. Iowa City, IO: Sage; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  20. Martin-Ordas G, Call J. 2013. Episodic memory: A comparative approach. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 63. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00063.
  21. Nilsson L-G, Sternang O, Rønnlund M, Nyberg L. Challenging the notion of an early-onset of cognitive decline. Neurobiology of Aging, 30. 2009:521–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.11.013. discussion 530–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Nyberg L, Lövden M, Riklund K, Lindenberger U, Bäckman L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2012;16:292–305. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Papp KV, Kaplan RF, Springate B, Moscufo N, Wakefield DB, Guttmann CRG, et al. Processing speed in normal aging: Effects of white matter hyperintensities and hippocampal volume loss. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 2013;21:197–213. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2013.795513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Petersen RC. Mild Cognitive Impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pudas S, Persson J, Josefsson M, de Luna X, Nilsson L-G, Nyberg L. Brain characteristics of individuals resisting age-related cognitive decline over two decades. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:8668–8677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2900-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. R Development Core Team. 2013. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved xxxxx form http:// http://www.r-project.org/
  28. Rabbitt P, Diggle P, Smith D, Holland F, Mc Innes L. Identifying and separating the effects of practice and of cognitive aging during a large longitudinal study of elderly community residents. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:532–543. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00099-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rabin LA, Par N, Saykin AJ, Brown MJ, Wishart HA, Flashman LA, et al. Differential memory test sensitivity for diagnosing amnestic mild cognitive impairment and predicting conversion to Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition. 2009;16:357–376. doi: 10.1080/13825580902825220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Reinvang I, Deary IJ, Fjell AM, Steen VM, Espeseth T, Parasuraman R. Neurogenetic effects on cognition in aging brains: A window of opportunity for intervention? Frontiers Aging Neuroscience. 2010a;2 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00143. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Reinvang I, Espeseth T, Westlye LT. APOE-related biomarker profiles in non-pathological aging and early phases of Alzheimer's disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:1322–1335. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Reinvang I, Winjevoll IL, Rootwelt H, Espeseth T. Working memory deficits in healthy APOE epsilon 4 carriers. Neuropsychologia. 2010b;48:566–573. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rønnberg J, Danielsson H, Rudner M, Arlinger S, Sternang O, Wahlin A, et al. Hearing loss is negatively related to episodic and semantic long-term memory but not to short-term memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2011;54:705–726. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0088). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rønnlund M, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Nilsson L-G. Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:3–18. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging. 2009;30:507–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Salthouse TA. Robust cognitive change. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2012;18:749–756. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Schacter DL, Koutstaal W, Norman KA. False memories and aging. Trends in Cognitive Science. 1997;1:229–236. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01068-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Schaie KW. What can we learn from longitudinal studies of adult development? Research in human development. 2005;2:133–158. doi: 10.1207/s15427617rhd0203_4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, Mencl WE, et al. Effect of estrogen on brain activation patterns in postmenopausal women during working memory tasks. JAMA. 1999;281:1197–1202. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.13.1197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Sherwin BB. Cognitive assessment for postmenopausal women and general assessment of their mental health. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1998;34:323–326. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:448–460. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stretch V, Wixted JT. Decision rules for recognition memory confidence judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1998;24:1397–1410. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.24.6.1397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sunderaraman P, Blumen HM, DeMatteo D, Apa ZL, Cosentino S. Task demand influences relationships among sex, clustering strategy, and recall: 16-word versus 9-word list learning tests. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2013;26:78–84. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e31829de450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Tierney MC, Yao C, Kiss A, McDowell I. Neuropsychological tests accurately predict incident Alzheimer's disease after 5 and 10 years. Neurology. 2005;64:1853–1859. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000163773.21794.0B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Twamley EW, Ropacki SAL, Bondi MW. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging changes in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006;12:707–735. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wallentin M. Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain and Language. 2009;108:175–183. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wang P, Li J, Li H, Zhang S. Differences in learning rates for item and associative memories between amnestic mild cognitive impairment and healthy controls. Behavior and Brain Function, 9. 2013:29. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-9-29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-9-29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. WASI manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  50. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) Pearson Assessment: Norwegian Manual Supplement. Stockholm; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  51. Zelinski EM, Burnight KP. Sixteen-year longitudinal and time lag changes in memory and cognition in older adults. Psychology and Aging. 1997;12:503–513. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.3.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Zelinski EM, Stewart ST. Individual differences in 16-year memory changes. Psychology and Aging. 1998;13:622–630. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.13.4.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scandinavian Journal of Psychology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES