Abstract
Foragers must often travel from a central place to exploit aggregations of prey. These patches can be identified behaviorally when a forager shifts from travel to area restricted search, identified by a decrease in speed and an increase in sinuosity of movement. Faster, more directed movement is associated with travel. Differentiating foraging behavior at patches from travel to patches is important for a variety of research questions and has now been made easier by the advent of small, GPS devices that can track forager movement with high resolution. In the summer and fall of 2012, movement data were collected from GPS devices placed on foraging trips originating in the artisanal fishing village of Desa Ikan (pseudonym), on the east coast of the Caribbean island nation of the Commonwealth Dominica. Moored FADs are human-made structures anchored to the ocean floor with fish attraction material on or near the surface designed to effectively create a resource patch. The ultimate goal of the research is to understand how property rights are emerging after the introduction of fish aggregating device (FAD) technology at the site in 1999. This paper reports on research to identify area-restricted search foraging behavior at FAD patches. For 22 foraging trips simultaneous behavioral observations were made to ground-truth the GPS movement data. Using a cumulative sum method, area restricted search was identified as negative deviations from the mean travel speed and the method was able to correctly identify FAD patches in every case.
Introduction
FADs (Fish Aggregating Devices) are manmade structures designed to float on or near the surface of a body of water, attract fish and facilitate their capture [1–3]. Moored FADs create resource patches at known locations significantly reducing search time, effort, and fuel costs for fishers. Globally, industrial drifting FADs account for an increasing proportion of many economically important fisheries [4]. In 2011, FADs accounted for over 40% of the world’s tropical tuna catches [5]. Globally widespread FAD use is a recent development. Since the 1970s the technology in various forms has spread from SE Asia where traditional FAD technology, like the palou of the Philippines, has been deployed by local fishers for centuries [6]. In many parts of the world, including the Caribbean, FADs are increasingly being used by small scale, artisanal fishers to access fish species otherwise difficult to harvest in large numbers [7,8]. In the Commonwealth of Dominica, a Caribbean island nation, where they have been used only since the late 1990s, FADs are now anchored in the deep water off of the insular shelf by artisanal fishers who use them seasonally to target pelagic fish, primarily tuna (Thunnus spp.) and marlin (Makaira spp.) [9].
FADs are interesting in a variety of ways. How and why they attract fish is an open question [3] and their impact on fish stocks raises concerns [10,11]. In many places, including Dominica, where legislation to regulate and management FAD fishing is still in development, FADs create property rights conflicts over who owns the aggregated fish [12,13], and the technology is so new that use-right norms are still evolving. FADs are an excellent example of niche construction [14,15] as fishers alter the environment to create patchiness in an otherwise relatively homogenous foraging seascape.
The analysis below is framed by foraging theory [16]. In the most general sense, foragers search for prey; this involves movement in space over time to facilitate encounters with patches. Patches are aggregations of prey that foragers often travel to from a central place [17]. At patch sites, like those where fish aggregate around FADs, prey encounters are expected to be spatiotemporally auto correlated [18,19]; once a forager encounters a prey item there is an increased probability of finding additional prey nearby. In this case, foraging theory predicts that foragers should conduct area-restricted search (ARS), defined by a decrease in speed and an increase in sinuosity of movement [20,21]. ARS has been identified in a wide variety of foragers, including humans [22–26]. Searching for patches differs in its nature from localized searching within a patch, sometimes referred to as extensive verses intensive search, respectively [20,27]. When traveling between patches or back to a central place the forager is expected to travel more quickly and more linearly [28].
The work that we report here is part of a larger project with the goal of understanding how Dominican artisanal FAD fishermen develop solutions (or not) to property rights conflicts. In this paper we report on research that will help us identify the resource patches created by the FADs so that we can accurately measure how different fishermen use those patches. Following the arguments above, patches will be identified behaviorally when the Dominican fishers shift from travel to ARS. Using GPS data to track the movement of the boats, we use the partial sum method developed by Knell and Codling [29,30] to identify ARS at the FADs, and test the model using simultaneous behavioral observations made to ground-truth the GPS movement data.
Ethnographic context: Foraging trips by artisanal FAD fishermen
Data were collected between August 29 and December 15, 2012 from foraging trips originating at the medium-sized fish landing site located in the community of Desa Ikan (pseudonym) on the rural east coast of the Commonwealth of Dominica. The site features a large bay with a harbor protected behind a stone breakwater where boats are secured along the shore. The landing site is host to fishers and boat owners who live in Desa Ikan and nearby communities. Sixteen boats made 95% of all recorded trips (N = 505) out of a total of 22 boats that fished during the period reported here. Thirty-two fishers made 95% of all trips out of a total of 53 individuals who fished. Fishing at Desa Ikan is classically artisanal and small-scale [31]. Boats are small (4m–8m in length), constructed with wood or wood reinforced with fiberglass, and powered with two-stroke outboard motors that range from 15hp to 85hp motors (mode = 48hp). Modal crew size is two, and ranges from one to three persons. Trips are diurnal, rarely last more than 12 hours or travel more than 50km from shore. There is no export market in Dominica. Fish are sold locally, in nearby communities, or occasionally transported 45km by road to Roseau, the capital.
As a group, the fishers target a variety of both demersal and pelagic fish. Strategies at Desa Ikan vary with season across three general strategies. Near-shore, shallow-water, bank fishing with hook and line and traps for demersals such as snapper (Lutjanus spp.) is practiced year-around. Most fishers take part in the late winter-spring ‘channel’ fishing season which involves hunting for schools of dolphinfish (Coryphaena hippurus) and other pelagics and capturing them with hook and line in the deep water off the banks. Finally, with the introduction of FADs around 1999, a number of the fishermen have been able to target large coastal and migratory pelagic fish, particularly tuna (Thunnus spp.), marlin (Makaira spp.) and dolphinfish [32].
The deep water FADs deployed from Desa Ikan are anchored to the ocean floor with halved 55 gallon drums filled with concrete (Fig. 1; see also [33]). A length of polypropylene rope joins the anchor to the FAD’s head at the surface which usually consists of a number of round floats in addition to the FAD’s attraction. To prevent twisting, metal swivels are placed at key connection points. Attraction usually consists of tarps and nets attached to both the mainline near the surface and the head of the FAD. The FADs that are the focus of this research are placed in water between 1000m–2500m deep at distances of 15–50km from shore and are referred here to as deep water FADs to contrast them with the shallow water FADs that are placed close to shore and primarily target barracudas (Sphyraena spp).
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement
This study was approved by Texas A&M University’s Office of Research Compliance Internal Review Board (Study Number: IRB2009–0209) and the Commonwealth of Dominica Fisheries Division (Research License RP-05/129 FIS-3). Texas A&M Institutional Review Board approved the use of oral consent obtained from fishers and boat owner under the criteria listed by the U. S. Code of Federal Regulations section 45 CFR 46.117. Our experience suggested that asking for signed consent in the context of ethnographic research, like that done in Dominica, puts significant strain on the research—subject relationship. It is likely that the research could not have been carried out without the waiver, as suspicion of our motives would have increased significantly. Subjects were completely informed concerning the nature of the research with the clear option of declining.
Sample
During the data collection period, 505 fishing trips were observed leaving and returning to the landing site at Desa Ikan. We placed GPS devices aboard the boats and collected movement data from a sample of 351 of these trips [33]. For 25 of these trips, along with the GPS data, ground truth data were simultaneously collected during time-synchronized focal follows where one of us joined the crew and recorded behavioral data during the course of the trip. Twenty-three trips involved trips to FADs. One set of notes was lost overboard leaving a sample of N = 22 FAD focal follows. A variety of data were collected during these focal follows, but germane to this analysis, the observer noted when the boat was at a FAD. The analysis here is limited primarily to the 22 focal follows trips that included trips to FADs.
GPS
The GPS loggers that were placed on the boats were model GT-31 manufactured by LOCOSYS Technology (http://www.locosystech.com/). Light and small (90mm x 58mm x 24.5mm; 98g), we housed them in hard plastic water proof cases (Pelican 1010; 196gms). The loggers use SiRF Star 3 high sensitivity, low power GPS chips energized with a built-in lithium-ion polymer rechargeable battery. Output data format is NMEA 0183, an industry standard. The manufacturer reports a 95% probability that the reported position will be within the area of a circle with a radius of 10m.around the actual location and the unobstructed conditions of the open sea were ideal for satellite reception.
The secure housing provided by the case and the compact nature of the devices cases made placement easy in the waterproof tubs that most fishers keep aboard their boats in order to keep dry a variety of their own items including their own GPS devices. Before the boats left in the morning we activated the devices and placed them in a tub or handed them to the fishers. The devices did not experience any problems receiving a signal though the two layers of plastic. We retrieved the devices when fishers returned from the sea in the afternoon. Boat owners were given a fee of 10EC ($3.7US) for each trip their boat carried a GPS device.
The GPS devices were programed to collect data at one second intervals generating a time-series with associated x, y spatial coordinates. The data have fine spatiotemporal scale and describe the movement of the boats during each fishing trip in detail. The data files can be accessed here: http://repository.tamu.edu/handle/1969.1/152005. We edited the track data in GPS Track Editor, a free software product from MapSphere (http://www.gpstrackeditor.com). Pre and post trip data points were removed from each trip’s track and the file was imported into Google Earth, a free software product from Google (http://www.google.com/earth) where they were overlaid on top of satellite imagery of the east coast of Dominica. Tracks were easily viewed across the seascape at a variety of spatial scales. Using Google Earth’s Time Slider tool the tracks could also be animated and viewed at different scales of temporal resolution.
Ground truth via focal follows
We supplemented the GPS data collection with simultaneous behavioral observations during focal follows. For this method, a focal subject is accompanied for a period of time and data are recorded [34,35]. In this case, the subject can be considered the captain because he is responsible for directing the movement of the boat. The sample of observed trips included three captains and two boats and was not randomly selected but reflected our closer relationship with particular fishers. One boat accounted for 21 of the 22 trips; 16 of the 21 trips on that boat were with the same captain. A second individual captained for the balance of five trips on that boat. We observed one trip on a second boat that was captained by a third individual. We address issues related to the homogeneity of this sample in the discussion section below. We used event sampling to record behavioral changes or events germane to the research question and the times they occurred [36]. Salient events included departure, travel, fishing and navigation behavior, and encounters with floating debris, birds, FADs, and other boats. During the trips, our watches and the GPS devices were synchronized to allow correspondence between the subsequent GPS track and what we observed during the trips. This technique can be considered a type of ground truth common to analyses that use remotely sensed data [37]. Ground truth refers to data collected at an actual location that can be related to data collected at a distance, via satellites for example [38]. Germane to this analysis, the observer identified the time when the foragers entered and departed from foraging patches, i.e., the transition from extensive to intensive search.
Domain knowledge
Domain knowledge is a term from information science that refers to expert knowledge [39]. The fishers with whom we interacted are experts in the domain of FAD fishing. Discussion with fishers concerning the nature of FAD foraging before, during, and after their trips informed our conclusions based on observations; during focal follows captain and crew would confirm periods of FAD fishing. Direct observation by a research observer is also a kind of domain knowledge. Based on domain knowledge, we developed expectations regarding what tracks should look like for behavior of interest.
Results
Observations at the FAD
The fishermen are spatially informed foragers and enlighten analysis of the otherwise mechanistic ARS models [40]. Captains have a cognitive map generated from their memory of past foraging trips [41] as well as culturally transmitted information provided by other fishers about their experiences with FADs [42]. Captains use this information to direct their outward bound travel in the general direction of specific deep water FADs. When deployed, a FAD’s mainline is given slack (scope) to reduce strain in heavy current and rough seas [43] and captains know that FAD heads will drift with the ocean currents. Given the known location of the anchor drop, fishers report that the head will be located within a drift zone with a radius defined by the scope; the actual location of the head on any particular day is determined by the ocean current. Fishers who frequent FADs usually bring GPS aboard to help locate FADs. For the FADs that they regularly visit, fishers maintain coordinates of locations that lie on the circumference of drift zones and captains will search for a particular location based on their recent experience, information from others who fished the FAD recently, and conditions of wind and current. Fishers without GPS devices sometimes take advantage of those who do and follow them to FAD patches, a kind of social foraging sometimes seen in nonhuman foragers [44]. If a FAD has not been visited in some time, or if few marks are known, more scouting may be required. If the current is strong, a FAD head can be pulled under the surface and cannot be found.
Based on experience, captains have an approximate idea how much time it will take them to travel to the area where the target FAD is located. If there is a GPS device aboard, the bow man will take a fix as they approach within a few kilometers of where they think the FAD is located and will point or shout navigation directions to the captain. Captains will reduce speed as they search. Some FADs have flags to aid the search. Once they have located the head, they will often not travel right to it, rather first trolling the area in order to catch live bait to set on the first traps of the day. Once they have their first bait, the captain will approach the head to check the direction of the attraction to gauge a more precise direction of the current.
The primary technique used in Dominica to catch tuna and marlin around a FAD involves setting what the fishers refer to as traps and results in boat movements consistent with ARS. Similar to the technique involved in float or jug-fishing [45], traps are crafted from meter-long floats (referred to as fenders) that are secured to a length of fishing line (~100m) and a baited hook. The fishers travel up-current from the FAD and throw the free floating trap out of the boat with the goal that it drifts down-current past the FAD head. If a fish does not take the bait, the trap is collected and the process is repeated. Boats usually deploy multiple traps simultaneously and since trap-setting and collecting occur at opposite ends of the patch space, the resulting tracks (discussed below) are characterized by back and forth movements around the FAD head as traps are set and collected. In addition to trap setting, drift fishing is also common around FADs and is characterized by slow speeds where the engine is off or idling and the boat is moving slowly with the current as the fishers use hook and line to catch bait or other smaller fish.
The section above describes search for deep water FADs; other kinds of patches are found other ways. The shallow water FADs referred to locally as the Twins are close to shore and easily found using landmark navigation. Deep water FADs, on contrast, are placed at a distance from shore so that such landmark navigation is usually difficult. A tash is a school of fish, most commonly dolphinfish. Fishers are often alerted to tash patches by the presence of birds, often frigatebirds (Fregata spp.) that are feeding on the same prey, often flying fish, as the target prey fish. During the channel season, in the winter and spring, search focuses on these birds. In the vernacular, wood generically refers to floating patches of debris where fish are commonly encountered. Drifting logs, timbers, marine litter such as plastic objects and polystyrene foam, lost fishing gear, and Sargassum sp. (a type of seaweed) will also attract fish into patches [46]. Birds are also present at these wood patches.
Visual analysis
We first did a visual analysis of the trips by examining plots of their trajectories mapped onto the seascape. Results show that the captains were practicing classic central place foraging [47]. After departure from Desa Ikan, each boat traveled with ballistic (i.e., straight line) movement directed toward locations where intensive search was subsequently conducted at the FAD patches. The 22 trips analyzed here were sampled for focal follow observation because the captains stated targets were deep water FADs. After foraging at various patches, boats returned to their starting point at Desa Ikan via ballistic travel. Four example tracks plotted in Fig. 2 highlight some of the variability in the nature of the trips.
Table 1 provides summary data on the N = 22 trips. Crew size was 2 men for each trip (not counting observer). Trip length varied from 6.9–10.9hr with a mean of 8.87hr. We calculated instantaneous travel speeds from the distance traveled between each pair of consecutive one second locations. The mean speed for the trips ranged from 2.97m/s to 4.17 m/s (SD = 2.53–3.57). As mentioned above, one boat and its two captains accounted for 21 of the observed trips. Mean travel speed did not differ between the two captains on the same boat [3.30m/s [n = 5] versus 3.44m/s [n = 16]; t(6.5) = -1.1, p = 0.30]. The one observed trip in the second boat captained by a third man traveled at a mean speed of 4.17 m/s. The small sample precluded testing for differences between the boats. Time series plots of speed for the four example trips shows heterogeneity along the series with periods of consistently higher speeds that observers associated as travel, and periods of slower speeds that are associated with drifting and slow powered movement at patches (Fig. 3). The frequency distributions of travel speeds show a consistent tri-modal distribution. Fig. 4 shows four representative histograms and scree plots. We used k-means cluster analysis to bin and calculate the means for each mode for each trip. The results are presented in Table 2 and show a mean range of 0.37–1.06 m/s for the first mode, 2.84–5.23 m/s for the second, and 6.17–8.54 m/s for the last. Multimodal speed distributions have been identified for nonhuman foragers [48,49], as well as fishers [50–52] and have been used to identify different states of behavior. For example, Walker et al. [52] use Vessel Monitoring System (VMS) GPS data from the French purse-seiner fleet in the Indian Ocean to identify three modes of travel speed that they identified as stop, track, and cruise behavior.
Table 1. Parameters for individual foraging trips by Dominican FAD fishers.
TripID | Date | Trip Duration (h) | Distance traveled (km) | Average speed (m/s) | Standard deviation speed | N |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
474 | 8/31/2012 | 8.06 | 103.71 | 3.66 | 2.78 | 28,586 |
482 | 9/1/2012 | 8.24 | 101.69 | 3.48 | 2.95 | 29,378 |
512 | 9/5/2012 | 8.80 | 110.30 | 3.48 | 2.78 | 31,696 |
519 | 9/6/2012 | 9.52 | 102.11 | 2.98 | 2.97 | 34,271 |
526 | 9/8/2012 | 10.95 | 124.74 | 3.16 | 2.86 | 39,420 |
567 | 9/17/2012 | 8.60 | 98.07 | 3.18 | 2.89 | 30,895 |
571 | 9/18/2012 | 9.44 | 109.78 | 3.23 | 2.74 | 33,968 |
579 | 9/19/2012 | 10.12 | 115.27 | 3.16 | 2.78 | 36,421 |
593 | 9/21/2012 | 8.67 | 114.88 | 3.68 | 2.70 | 31,211 |
596 | 9/24/2012 | 6.86 | 90.53 | 3.66 | 2.69 | 24,722 |
606 | 9/26/2012 | 9.81 | 110.18 | 3.12 | 2.93 | 35,320 |
640 | 9/28/2012 | 10.27 | 115.42 | 3.12 | 2.77 | 36,989 |
649 | 10/1/2012 | 7.83 | 102.94 | 3.65 | 2.74 | 28,192 |
672 | 10/5/2012 | 9.31 | 111.26 | 3.32 | 2.76 | 33,525 |
682 | 10/8/2012 | 8.99 | 113.56 | 3.51 | 2.78 | 32,344 |
689 | 10/9/2012 | 8.53 | 109.80 | 3.59 | 2.61 | 30,647 |
713 | 10/16/2012 | 9.09 | 112.53 | 3.44 | 2.81 | 32,716 |
731 | 10/18/2012 | 8.06 | 102.69 | 3.54 | 2.59 | 29,010 |
757 | 10/22/2012 | 8.18 | 122.98 | 4.17 | 3.57 | 29,472 |
777 | 10/24/2012 | 8.35 | 111.96 | 3.72 | 2.59 | 30,058 |
789 | 10/26/2012 | 8.95 | 103.32 | 3.20 | 2.71 | 32,234 |
795 | 10/27/2012 | 8.56 | 114.70 | 3.72 | 2.53 | 30,812 |
Mean | 8.87 | 109.20 | 3.44 | 2.80 |
Table 2. Results of k-means analysis of travel speed.
Mode | Range of means (m/s) | Mean of means (m/s) |
---|---|---|
Mode1 | 0.37–1.06 | 0.63 |
Mode2 | 2.84–5.23 | 3.36 |
Mode3 | 6.17–8.54 | 6.82 |
Mode 1 is associated with drifting with current. Mode 2 is slow powered movement; Mode 3 is full powered travel.
For our data, the three modal speeds are temporally distributed over the course of the trip in a way that is consistent with gross behavioral differences associated with different speeds noted by the observers. Persistent high speed (mode 3) was associated primarily with travel to patches at the start of a trip, travel between patches during the course of the trip, and return travel to Desa Ikan at the end of the trip. Slower speeds were associated with drifting (mode 1) and slower powered movement (mode 2) in patches; we did experience occasional high speed in patches but it was for short duration. This was confirmed when we examined the tracks visually with the points binned and colored-coded for speed according to the three k-means modes specific to each trip. At a gross visual spatial scale, travel seems to dominate the tracks in Fig. 2, but at higher resolution we see periods of speed heterogeneity, chiefly at the patches. Fig. 5 illustrates, for the example tracks, the segments that we identified as patches based on our observations and subsequently identified as ARS (below). Variance in speeds is clearly apparent with the points color-coded and showing that activities at the patches involve all three modes of speed identified from the k-means analysis. While difficult to discern from a spatial representation, over the course of a trip fishers often allocated more time to mode 1 (i.e., drifting) than to mode 3 (traveling). Pooling all the points across all the trips (N = 701,887, 195.17 hours), find 42.6% are mode 1, 21.4% mode 2 and 36.0% mode 3.
Segmenting
One of the key methods of movement analysis involves parsing tracks into segments [53] based on spatiotemporal criteria such as location, speed, heading, and sinuosity [54]. A segment is defined as a temporally contiguous portion of the trajectory that is homogenous with respect to criteria chosen to correspond to behaviors of interest. For our purposes, the challenge is to segment the track into two types of behavior—travel and ARS. There are a variety of methods used to identify ARS in foraging animals using GPS track data, including first passage time [55], fractal landscape method [56], multi-scale straightness index [57], net squared displacement [58], behavioral change point analysis [59], and state space models [60].
For our analysis we follow Knell and Codling [29] who developed a method they refer to as the partial sum method. It is based on the cumulative sums of deviations from the mean values of three different movement path properties: speed, absolute turning angle (direction), and the sum of the normalized absolute turning angle and inverse of the normalized speed. We chose a cumulative sum method for its analytic simplicity and demonstrated efficiency correctly categorizing time steps in simulated movement trajectories. Cumulative sum methods are commonly used in quality control applications and are designed to identify prolonged periods of consistent directional deviations from normal [61,62]. We follow their methods below, but limit our analysis to speed for two reasons. First, as we will demonstrate below, speed works very well for our purposes. Using speed alone we are able to identity each ARS segment during all 22 trips without error. Second, results obtained using turning angle produce inconsistent and erratic results. The measure described as “turning angle” by Knell and Codling actually represents the direction of travel during one time period. As discussed in more detail in S1 Appendix, consistent deviations from mean direction do not necessarily indicate ARS.
For each foraging trip in the sample, we calculated the mean speed of all steps and then, for each step (S t) in the time series, we calculated the deviation from the mean. Deviates greater than the mean speed have positive values and those less than the mean have negative values. We then calculated the cumulative sum of deviations across all steps using the following cumulative sum equation [29]:
(1) |
The cumulative sum plot that results from Equation 1 shows consistent positive slope when the forager consistently travels at speeds greater than the mean; the slope is negative when the forager moves persistently at speeds less than the mean. Fig. 6 shows how the cumulative sum plots for the four trips in Fig. 2 describe behavior during the trips. The time series for trip TID482 (Fig. 6, panel A) shows three major segments (TID refers to the trip identification number). The first segment is a period of consistent positive deviations from the mean trip speed corresponding to observed high speed travel from Desa Ikan; the segment starts at approximately t = 0 and ends at approximately t = 5,000. Next is a period of negative deviations from the mean trip speed that continues until approximately t = 24,000. This segment indicates ARS and will subsequently be associated with a FAD visit (FVID73) to FAD 22 (see below). The third and final segment is a section with positive slope associated with travel back to Desa Ikan that continues until approximately the end of the trip. The cumulative sum plot for TID579 (Fig. 6, panel B) shows two major segments of ARS (negative slope) corresponding to two FADs (FVID26 and FVID27), between which is a period of travel between the patches (negative slope). Panel C is the plot for TID689 and includes two FAD visits (FVID267 and FVID395) and a third negatively sloped segment corresponding to an ARS at a shallow water FAD. Finally, the plot for TID795 shows two FAD visits (FVID327 and FVID328), and an ARS at a wood (Fig. 6, panel D).
The analysis above identifies major ARS and travel segments, but a closer examination of the cumulative sum plots identifies other possible candidates for segments that suggest ARS. Within travel segments there are sometimes short periods of slower movement that we do not want to identify as ARS. A captain might slow the boat in order to urinate, to evaluate passing weather, to change the gas line from one tank to the other, to recover from a very large wave, or for a variety of other reasons unrelated to ARS. For example, there is a short period of what appears to be ARS on the second travel segment of TID482 (Fig. 6, panel A). In this case, the boat briefly stopped in response to the presence of another boat whose captain asked for bait. Conversely, movement within a patch might include brief periods of rapid movement from one point within a patch to another during ARS. For example, for TID795 there appears to be a short period of travel within the third period of ARS at the wood (Fig. 6, panel D). What counts as a separate bout of ARS? A problem with methods used to examine movement (as well as other ecological data) is that the results are often dependent on the spatial and temporal scale of the analysis [23,63,64]. If the resolution is too fine we run the risk of making a false positive error; that is, we might identify noise as ARS within a period of travel. If the resolution is too course, we risk missing an ARS patch, identifying it instead as travel.
Knell and Codling [29] develop a procedure called the Max—Min Algorithm (MMA) that they designed to identify the cut points along the cumulative sum time series plot where speed changes from segments of above average speed (rising curve) to below average speed (falling curve) and vice versa. The cut points are identified as local minima and maxima and identify the start and endpoints that define segments associated with different behavior. In order to set the appropriate scale, the Max—Min algorithm uses a window or threshold (w) which is the timeframe over which a particular maximum or minimum remains so. Once an extremum is tentatively identified, the algorithm continues to look past that point along the cumulative time series to see if the point remains extreme. If another point is identified within the window that is higher than the peak or lower than the valley being considered, a new tentative extremum is identified and the previous discarded. If no new tentative point is found and the length of the window has been exhausted, the original point is identified as a permanent maximum or minimum and the algorithm moves on to search for the next extremum. We coded the algorithm in R [65] (S1 R Script).
Knell and Codling [29] offer a method to determine the optimal size of w by first noting that there is an inverse relationship between window size and number of segments produced. They suggest that the optimal window size lies within the range of values that provide the first relatively long consecutive period without a change in the number of segments. We determined the size of the window inductively as the smallest window that identified each patch, including FAD visits, as determined by the focal follow data. The smallest window that identified all the FAD ARS was w = 998 and we chose w = 1000 for the following analysis.
For the 22 trips, with the window set at w = 1,000 seconds, the algorithm produced a total of 178 segments and we assigned each to a behavioral category based on the focal follow observations (Table 3). Travel is defined as sustained, above average speed. Eighty-two of the segments were identified as travel with a mean speed of 6.5m/s; this closely matches the mode 3 speed identified from the k-means analysis (mean mode 3 across all trips is 6.82m/s; Table 2). Based on the criteria of sustained periods of consistent directional negative deviations from normal speed, we initially classified ninety-six of the segments as ARS. These segments had mean speeds ranging from 0.75m/s to 1.76m/s with a mean of 1.32m/s. These are speeds corresponding with mode 1 and 2 identified from the k-means analysis (mean mode 1 and 2 speeds across all trips are 0.63m/s and 3.36 respectively; Table 2).
Table 3. Patches and behaviors associated with segments.
Behavior | Number of segments | Total time (hr) | Average speed (m/s) |
---|---|---|---|
FAD | 42 | 104.76 | 1.56 |
Travel | 82 | 73.83 | 6.48 |
Wood | 9 | 8.14 | 0.97 |
Twin | 6 | 5.78 | 0.75 |
Trip start | 20 | 1.02 | 1.12 |
Tash | 1 | 0.77 | 1.76 |
Trip end | 16 | 0.38 | 1.30 |
Social | 1 | 0.30 | 1.28 |
Search | 1 | 0.19 | 1.54 |
Data from the focal follow observations indicate that not all of the segments initially identified as ARS involved intensive search at resource patches. Thirty-six of the segments were short segments at the very beginning (N = 20) or end (N = 16) of a trip with mean lengths of 184 seconds and 85 seconds respectively. Most of these cases involve slower movement at the landing site in the harbor as a trip was getting underway or coming to an end (Fig. 7). Eighteen of the remaining segments identified as ARS were patches that did not involve FAD fishing. Nine of the patches were encounters with floating debris (wood). One was an encounter with a school or tash of dolphinfish. One segment identified as ARS was a period of socializing with other boats that were met at sea. Six segments were ARS at one of the shallow water FADs (Twins). Finally, one segment of ARS involved an intensive search for the FAD itself that was ultimately not found. FADs are not always found with ease. The remaining 42 segments are associated with the 42 FAD visits identified during observations.
The cumulative sum method combined with the MMA predicts precise times when ARS starts and stops at each FAD providing an objective and reproducible method for delineating patch arrival, leaving, and residence time—measures important for a wide variety of foraging problems [28,66,67]. In order to determine the accuracy of cut points produced by the MMA we compared them to the observations of arrivals and departures from FAD patches we observed during the trips. MMA provides resolution to the second, but the observations were recorded with one minute resolution so we rounded observations by adding 30 seconds. The mean difference between the MMA estimate of the arrival times on the FADs and the observed is about-36 seconds. In other words, on average the method predicts the start of FAD ARS about a half a minute earlier than the focal follow reported. Some of the difference is due to cases where the FAD head was difficult to find after the boat had arrived at its expected vicinity. In this case, the algorithm correctly identifies the beginning of ARS, but it is ARS for the FAD head instead of ARS for fish. A paired-samples t-test shows the difference between observed and expected is not significantly different from zero [t(41) = -0.9337, p = 0.3559]. The mean difference between the observed and MMA estimates of FAD departure is 54 seconds; this means that the algorithm estimates departure from the FAD on average about a minute after the observer indicates departure. This difference is small but statistically significant and probably represents the time it takes to accelerate the boat to traveling speed after the captain indicated to the observer that they were leaving [t(41) = -4.119, p = 0.0001]. Another way to examine the error is with respect to total time in the patch. The average observed patch residence time for the FAD visits is 2.46hrs (8,874 seconds). Errors of 54 and 36 seconds represent 0.6% and 0.4% of this number respectively, a small and probably acceptable degree of error for many applications. The average predicted patch residence time for the FAD visits is 2.49hrs; this differs from observed by 1.3%.
Discussion and Conclusions
This analysis of GPS tracking data was designed to discern ARS from travel in order to identify when Dominican fishers were exploiting FAD patches. Following the work of Knell and Codling [29] our analysis focused on changes in boat travel speed to identify ARS at FAD patches as periods of consistent negative deviations from the mean trip speed. The cumulative sum method was able to correctly identify all the patches exploited by fishers, including all the visits to deep water FAD for the sample of 22 observed foraging trips. The cut points identifying the beginning and ending of FAD patch residency predicted by the model closely matched the times indicated by direct observation during focal follows.
Foraging theory provided theoretical expectations concerning the nature of fishers’ movements around FAD patches [20,22]. Data from observations of the fishers at FADs used in conjunction with simultaneously collected GPS tracking data were used to validate our understanding of ARS behavior at FAD patches in terms of movement and we confirmed the theoretical expectations. At FAD patches, boats slow to a quarter of travel speed (Table 3). Speed is also more variable within patches (Fig. 5) with ranges across all three modes identified earlier (Fig. 4). Movement is also clearly more sinuous at FAD patches but that was not tested in this application (see more on sinuosity below).
The methods we use here are similar to Vessel Monitoring Systems (VMS) used by some nations to monitor fishing effort in managed areas while avoiding the cost of placing monitors on all the vessels [51,68]. These methods use location data, often collected via GPS, ARGOS, and Inmarsat systems to identity periods of fishing and non-fishing from the spatial data. Validation with onboard observers for a sample of trips is key. Chang and Yuan [69] report that vessel speed is the most widely used criteria to characterize fishing activities. See also Table 1 in Lee et al. [51]. The data used in these systems often have coarse resolution and simply use a speed category to classify a particular fishing activity.
Characterizing fishing effort across space is both challenging and crucial for both evaluating the sustainability and environmental impacts of both industrial fishing and small scale artisanal fishing [70]. Spatial analysis to understand effort by artisanal fishing is limited (see however [71–74]) but takes on new urgency as fishers adopt more effective fishing gear such as FADs. High quality information concerning how fishers use fish aggregating devices is important for setting policy and management goals with regard to fish aggregating devices and their impact on fisheries in the Caribbean and other regions [10,75,76].
Confidence in results from GPS data is important because while direct observation of foraging is ideal, it is difficult—often impossible, time consuming, and costly. In our case, having observers aboard a boat is inconvenient for fishers and involves risk to the observers. A large sample requires many observers. In contrast, for the application here, the advantages of the validated GPS method include a larger sample, reduced risk and cost, computational and analytic simplicity, and very good results. Our results provides a level of confidence and caution as we plan to apply the method to the larger sample of foraging tracks for which we have not collected focal follow data.
The small sample of observed boats and captains does raise the issue of whether the cumulative sum method we describe in this paper is applicable to other captains and boats in our larger sample of trips at Desa Ikan. There is good reason to believe that the relatively small sample of observations provides a sufficiently accurate description of FAD trips to generalize the method to other boats and captains. Visual analysis of tracks from the larger sample of FAD trips indicates a general pattern of directed, higher speed travel between patches and slower and sinuous movement at FAD locations and other presumed patches [33]. Discussions with fishers confirm that the pattern of movement at FADs identified during the observed trips as ARS is a general feature of FAD fishing at Desa Ikan. Foraging theory predicts foragers should adopt ARS at patches [20,27] and the evidence supports the conclusion that FAD fishermen at Desa Ikan do. At Desa Ikan, different boats and captains may travel at slightly different speeds during trips, but it should be kept in the mind that the cumulative sum method scales to the mean speed of each particular trip and not the mean speed of all trips.
Prudence should also be exercised moving forward with respect to generalizing the model across different fishing or foraging contexts. The trips we included in this analysis were chosen for focal follows because the captains indicated that FADs were the trip’s intended goal. This created a homogenous sample that well served the goals of the research, but does not represent the breadth of foraging trip types that originate at Desa Ikan. As mentioned above, FAD fishing is one foraging tactic of a suite utilized by the fishers. Characterizing patches encountered during channel fishing and bank fishing trips might be challenging for the methods used here. While our application used only speed, other foraging situations can be imagined where sinuosity would be more helpful; for example in a context where there is less variation in speed between travel and speed while foraging in patch. Similarly, Knell and Coding [29] point out that in situations where travel is a smaller portion of the foraging trip the method becomes less useful. An example of this type of case would be bank fishing for demersals at Desa Ikan. Bank patches are found on the insular shelf close to shore (<10km). Compared to FAD trips, travel is a much smaller portion of the foraging day for these sorts of trips and the mean overall trip speed is lower. This could make it more difficult to distinguish travel from ARS. The temporal length of ARS bouts with respect to the duration of travel segments should also be considered when choosing the threshold used by the Max—Min algorithm to determine what scale of deviation can be ignored as noise and what can be understood to be actual behavioral change.
As we apply this method to the larger sample, one challenge will be to distinguish ARS at FADs from ARS at other patches. Since moored FADs are anchored to the ocean floor we expect their associated patches to be found at known, fixed locations to which fishers regularly return over the course of multiple foraging trips. This is not expected for wood and tash patches. The extent to which FADs are found at fixed locations over time can be seen by examining the location of FAD patches over the course of the sample period. Three different FADs were visited (Table 4). Two FADs received all but one of the visits; the third FAD was newly deployed during the sample period (there were also other additional FADs visited by boats from Desa Ikan during this period). The mean centroid distance to the FAD patches from the central place ranged from 35km-38km. In order to learn if this changed over time, we plotted the mean locations of each FAD patch as determined by ARS to show that the patches were moving in wide arcs over time presumably with the currents (Fig. 8). Recall that during placement, the fishers add scope to the mainline to avoid excess strain and reduce failure. As a result, the patches created by the moored FADs move over the course of weeks and months. To estimate the size of this larger area, we used the maximum distance between two patch locations for each FAD to determine that the area of the drift zones range between approximately 4km2 and 5km2. For our analysis of the sample of trips without observers, we can confidently identify ARS within these areas as FAD fishing. In addition to movement of the FAD head, fishers report that current can also drag FAD anchors along the ocean floor, sometimes for kilometers. There was no evidence that this occurred during the sample period.
Table 4. FADs visited.
FAD | Number of visits | Mean distance from Central place (km) | Mean patch residence time (hr) |
---|---|---|---|
19 | 18 | 37.9 | 1.72 |
22 | 23 | 35.2 | 3.18 |
22S | 1 | 36.5 | 0.41 |
The segments at the start and end trips can be rejected as deep water FAD patches as can the ARS at the sites identified as the Twins which are located approximately 2km from shore. Both tash and wood patches are also straightforwardly distinguished from FADs. While FADs are essentially stationary over the course of one visit, and are repeatedly found in the same general location over many trips, each wood and tash patch is independent of others and are encountered at different locations throughout the seascape on different days. In addition, wood are free to drift with the current while tashes move as a school of fish moves. Visual examination of GPS tracks collected in 2013 during the channel season when fishers were focused on hunting schools of dolphinfish, show tash encounters playing out across a swath of seascape larger than FAD patches as fishers search for, follow, and engage schools.
FADs regularly fail in the harsh conditions of the Atlantic and new ones are deployed [77]. For example, in Desa Ikan fishers report that the three FADs identified in this analysis (as well as others) were destroyed in April 2013 by a particular strong tropical easterly wave. In the absence of additional direct observations, we can determine the location of new FAD placements using only GPS data by identifying ARS consistently in the same 4km2–5km2 area over the course of a number of foraging trips. Our conclusions can be confirmed by fishers’ reports. It is clear that distinguishing FAD ARS from among the other ARS segments benefits from domain knowledge of the system; that is, the analysis can be enhanced by the expert knowledge of the fishers [78]. These methods presented here could potentially be adapted to examine within patch behavior. Fig. 5 shows considerable variation in speed and sinuosity around FADs and presumably reflect the range of different behaviors occurring when a FAD patch is exploited. Some of these behaviors can be discerned visually. For example, drift behaviors can be identified in the tracks as repeated, straight, often parallel, and slow segments across the proscribed area (these segments are colored blue in the Fig. 5). These segments are parallel because they are following the surface current and wind which often tends to remain constant over any particular FAD visit. It might also be possible to distinguish individual trap deployments which would be useful as a measure of how different fishers exploit different FADs.
The capacity to identify patches and infer foraging behavior using high resolution tracking data like those described here is being used to characterize foraging behavior among a wide variety of organisms, especially birds and marine mammals. Those studies usually do not have the advantage of a sample of observed foraging trips and must infer behavior from theory and the tracks alone [25,49,79]. Using direct observation, we were able to associate foraging with ARS at patches; this result can provide researchers who lack observational data with more confidence as they infer foraging behavior based on GPS tracks alone.
The ability to identify patches is important for testing a wide variety of basic foraging models [80–83]. Research with terrestrial human hunters have been successful testing prey choice models [84,85] but have had less success with patch models since patches are difficult to characterize [86]. High resolution spatial data in combination with more sophisticated models to identify resources patches and foraging behavior promise new insight into how foragers move across the landscape (and seascape) and use resources [58,87]. In spite of challenges, the method described here is simple, cost-effective and has good potential for future research.
Accession Number
The GPS data files are available at the following site: http://repository.tamu.edu/handle/1969.1/152005.
Supporting Information
Data Availability
GPS data files are available from the Texas A&M University Digital Repository (http://hdl.handle.net/1969.1/152005).
Funding Statement
Funding to MA was provided from the Program to Enhance Scholarly and Creative Activities, Texas A&M, Office of the Vice President for Research (http://vpr.tamu.edu/researchdevelopment/funding/internal/ScholarlyCreative2013), the Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research (http://www.wennergren.org), and the National Science Foundation under Grant No. BCS-126020 (http://www.nsf.gov). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Castro J, Santiago J, Santana-Ortega A (2001) A general theory on fish aggregation to floating objects: An alternative to the meeting point hypothesis. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 11: 255–277. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Gooding R, Magnuson J (1967) Ecological significance of a drifting object to pelagic fishes. Pacific Science 21: 486–497. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Dempster T, Taquet M (2004) Fish aggregation device (FAD) research: gaps in current knowledge and future directions for ecological studies. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 14: 21–42. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Fonteneau A, Chassot E, Bodin N (2013) Global spatio-temporal patterns in tropical tuna purse seine fisheries on drifting fish aggregating devices (DFADs): Taking a historical perspective to inform current challenges. Aquatic Living Resources 26: 37–48. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Taquet M (2013) Fish aggregating devices (FADs): good or bad fishing tools? A question of scale and knowledge. Aquatic Living Resources 26: 25–35. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Dickson J, Natividad A (2000) Tuna fishing and a review of payaos in the Philippines Pêche thonière et dispositifs de concentration de poissons. Caribbean-Martinique, 15–19 Octobre 1999 http://archimer.ifremer.fr/doc/00042/15284/. 10.1016/j.virol.2014.12.023 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Rey-Valette H, Cillaurren E, David G (2000) Multidisciplinary assessment of the sustainability of small-scale fishery around anchored FADs. Aquatic Living Resources 13: 241–252. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Gomes C, Mahon R, Hunte W, Singh-Renton S (1998) The role of drifting objects in pelagic fisheries in the Southeastern Caribbean. Fisheries Research 34: 47–58. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sebastian R (2007) National report of the Commonwealth of Dominica. National reports and technical papers presented at the First Meeting of the WECAFC Ad Hoc Working Group on the Development of Sustainable Moored Fish Aggregating Device Fishing in the Lesser Antilles 8–11 October 2001; Le Robert, Martinique.
- 10. Morgan A (2011) Fish Aggregating Devices and Tuna: Impacts and Management Options. Washington, DC: Ocean Science Division, Pew Environment Group. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Dagorn L, Holland KN, Restrepo V, Moreno G (2013) Is it good or bad to fish with FADs? What are the real impacts of the use of drifting FADs on pelagic marine ecosystems? Fish and Fisheries 14: 391–415. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Ostrom E (1990) Governing the commons: the evolution of institutions for collective action. New York: Cambridge University Press; 280 p. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Beverly S, Griffiths D, Lee R (2012) Anchored fish aggregating devices for artisanal fisheries in South and Southeast Asia: benefits and risks. Bangkok, Thailand: FAO Regional Office for Asia and the Pacific. [Google Scholar]
- 14. Laland K, Odling-Smee J, Feldman M (2001) Cultural niche construction and human evolution. Journal Of Evolutionary Biology 14: 22–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Odling-Smee F, Laland K, Feldman M (2003) Niche construction: the neglected process in evolution. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press; 472 p. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Stephens D, Krebs J (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press; 247 p. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Stephens D, Brown J, Ydenberg R (2007) Foraging: behavior and ecology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 608 p. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Dray S, Royer-Carenzi M, Calenge C (2010) The exploratory analysis of autocorrelation in animal-movement studies. Ecological Research 25: 673–681. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Radeloff V, Miller T, He H, Mladenoff D (2000) Periodicity in spatial data and geostatistical models: autocorrelation between patches. Ecography 23: 81–91. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Benhamou S (1992) Efficiency of area-concentrated searching behaviour in a continuous patchy environment. Journal of Theoretical Biology 159: 67–81. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Kareiva P, Odell G (1987) Swarms of predators exhibit ‘preytaxis’ if individual predators use area-restricted search. American Naturalist 130: 233–270. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Weimerskirch H, Pinaud D, Pawlowski F, Bost C (2007) Does prey capture induce area-restricted search? A fine-scale study using GPS in a marine predator, the wandering albatross. American Naturalist 170: 734–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Sommerfeld J, Kato A, Ropert-Coudert Y, Garthe S, Hindell M (2013) Foraging Parameters Influencing the Detection and Interpretation of Area-Restricted Search Behaviour in Marine Predators: A Case Study with the Masked Booby. PLoS ONE 8: e63742 10.1371/journal.pone.0063742 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hills T, Kalff C, Wiener J (2013) Adaptive Lévy Processes and Area-Restricted Search in Human Foraging. PLoS ONE 8: e60488 10.1371/journal.pone.0060488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Papastamatiou Y, DeSalles P, McCauley D (2012) Area-restricted searching by manta rays and their response to spatial scale in lagoon habitats. Marine Ecology Progress Series 456: 233–244. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Eifler D, Baipidi K, Eifler M, Dittmer D, Nguluka L (2012) Influence of prey encounter and prey identity on area-restricted searching in the lizard Pedioplanis namaquensis. Journal of Ethology 30: 197–200. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Bartumeus F, Catalan J (2009) Optimal search behavior and classic foraging theory. Journal of Physics A: Mathematical and Theoretical 42: 1–12. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Charnov E (1976) Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology 9: 129–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Knell A, Codling E (2012) Classifying area-restricted search (ARS) using a partial sum approach. Theoretical Ecology 5: 325–339. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Knell A (2011) Identifying Behavioural Changes in Movement Path Data, PhD dissertation. Department of Mathematical Sciences: University of Essex. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Pauly D (2006) Major Trands in Small-Scale Marine Fisheries, With Emphasis on Developing Countries and some Implications for the Social Sciences. MAST 4: 7–22. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Defoe J (2007) National report of Dominica: FAD management systems and their impact on fisheries sustainable development. Second meeting of the WECAFC ad hoc working group on sustainable moored FAD fishing. Guadeloupe, 5–10 July 2004: FAO Fisheries Report N 683.
- 33. Alvard M, McGaffey E, Carlson D (2014) A method for measuring fishing effort by small-scale FAD (Fish aggregating devices) fishers from the Commonwealth of Dominica. Field Methods: in press. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Bird D, Bird R, Codding B (2009) In pursuit of mobile prey: Martu hunting strategies and archaeofaunal interpretation. American Antiquity 74: 3–29. [Google Scholar]
- 35. Rhine R, Flanigon M (1978) An empirical comparison of one-zero, focal-animal, and instantaneous methods of sampling spontaneous primate social behavior. Primates 19: 353–361. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Li N, Zhang Z (2012) Crowd behavior analysis based on velocity field simplification. Gaojishu Tongxin/Chinese High Technology Letters 22: 490–496. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Jensen J (2007) Remote sensing of the environment: an Earth resource perspective. New York: Prentice Hall; 608 p. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Hjørland B, Albrechtsen H (1995) Toward a new horizon in information science: Domain-analysis. Journal of the American Society for Information Science 46: 400–425. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Fronhofer E, Hovestadt T, Poethke H (2013) From random walks to informed movement. Oikos 122: 857–866. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Boyer D, Walsh PD (2010) Modelling the mobility of living organisms in heterogeneous landscapes: Does memory improve foraging success? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences 368: 5645–5659. 10.1098/rsta.2010.0275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Boyd R, Richerson P (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 340 p. [Google Scholar]
- 43. Ben-Yami M, De Jesus AS, Peters C, Bjarnason B (1989) How to make and set FADs (Fish Aggregating Devices). Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. 68 p. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Valone TJ (1989) Group Foraging, Public Information, and Patch Estimation. Oikos 56: 357–363. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Gabriel O, Brandt A (2005) Fish catching methods of the world. Oxford, UK Blackwell; 523 p. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Solana-Sansores R (2001) Floating objects of the Eastern Pacific: Types, spatial distribution and temporal changes. Ciencias Marinas 27: 423–443. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Orians G, Pearson N (1977) On the theory of central place foraging In: Horn D, Stairs G, Mitchell R, editors. Analysis of Ecological Systems. Columbus: Ohio State University Press; pp. 155–177. [Google Scholar]
- 48. Freeman R, Dennis T, Landers T, Thompson D, Bell E, et al. (2010) Black Petrels (Procellaria parkinsoni) Patrol the Ocean Shelf-Break: GPS Tracking of a Vulnerable Procellariiform Seabird. PLoS ONE 5: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Zavalaga C, Dell’Omo G, Becciu P, Yoda K (2011) Patterns of GPS Tracks Suggest Nocturnal Foraging by Incubating Peruvian Pelicans (Pelecanus thagus). PLoS ONE 6: e19966 10.1371/journal.pone.0019966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Daw T (2011) The spatial behaviour of artisanal fishers: Implications for fisheries management and development.
- 51. Lee J, South A, Jennings S (2010) Developing reliable, repeatable, and accessible methods to provide high-resolution estimates of fishing-effort distributions from vessel monitoring system (VMS) data. ICES Journal of Marine Science 67: 1260–1271. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Walker E, Bez N (2010) A pioneer validation of a state-space model of vessel trajectories (VMS) with observers’ data. Ecological Modelling 221: 2008–2017. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Nathan R (2008) An emerging movement ecology paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105: 19050–19051. 10.1073/pnas.0808918105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Buchin M, Driemel A, van Kreveld M, Sacrist V (2011) Segmenting trajectories: A framework and algorithms using spatiotemporal criteria. Journal of Spatial Information Science 3: 33–63. [Google Scholar]
- 55. Fauchald P, Tveraa T (2003) Using first-passage time in the analysis of area-restricted search and habitat selection. Ecology 84: 282–288. [Google Scholar]
- 56. Tremblay Y, Roberts A, Costa D (2007) Fractal landscape method: An alternative approach to measuring area-restricted searching behavior. Journal of Experimental Biology 210: 935–945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Postlethwaite C, Brown P, Dennis T (2013) A new multi-scale measure for analysing animal movement data. Journal of Theoretical Biology 317: 175–185. 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Papworth S, Bunnefeld N, Slocombe K, Milner-Gulland E (2012) Movement ecology of human resource users: Using net squared displacement, biased random bridges and resource utilization functions to quantify hunter and gatherer behaviour. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 3: 584–594. [Google Scholar]
- 59. Gurarie E, Andrews R, Laidre K (2009) A novel method for identifying behavioural changes in animal movement data. Ecology Letters 12: 395–408. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01293.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Breed G, Costa D, Jonsen I, Robinson P, Mills-Flemming J (2012) State-space methods for more completely capturing behavioral dynamics from animal tracks. Ecological Modelling 235–236: 49–58. [Google Scholar]
- 61. Hawkins M, Olwell H (1998) Cumulative sum charts and charting for quality improvement. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Page E (1954) Continous inspection schemes. Biometrika 41: 100–115. [Google Scholar]
- 63. Laube P, Purves R (2011) How fast is a cow? Cross-Scale Analysis of Movement Data. Transactions in GIS 15: 401–418. [Google Scholar]
- 64. Levin S (1992) The problem of pattern and scale in ecology. Ecology 73: 1943–1967. [Google Scholar]
- 65. R Core Team (2014) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; http://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- 66. Smith E (1991) Inujjuamiut foraging strategies: evolutionary ecology of an arctic hunting economy. New York: A. de Gruyter; 455 p. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Sosis R (2001) Sharing, consumption, and patch choice on Ifaluk Atoll—Evaluating an explanatory hypothesis. Human Nature-an Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 12: 221–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Lambert G, Jennings S, Hiddink J, Hintzen N, Hinz H, et al. (2012) Implications of using alternative methods of vessel monitoring system (VMS) data analysis to describe fishing activities and impacts. ICES Journal of Marine Science 69: 682–693. [Google Scholar]
- 69. Chang S, Yuan T (2014) Deriving high-resolution spatiotemporal fishing effort of large-scale longline fishery from vessel monitoring system (VMS) data and validated by observer data. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 71: 1363–1370. [Google Scholar]
- 70. Stewart K, Lewison R, Dunn DC, Bjorkland R, Kelez S, et al. (2010) Characterizing fishing effort and spatial extent of coastal fisheries. PLoS ONE 5 10.1371/journal.pone.0014462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Aswani S (1998) Patterns of marine harvest effort in southwestern New Georgia, Solomon Islands: Resource management or optimal foraging? Ocean and Coastal Management 40: 207–235. [Google Scholar]
- 72. Bene C, Tewfik A (2001) Fishing effort allocation and fishermen’s decision making process in a multi-species small-scale fishery: Analysis of the conch and lobster fishery in Turks and Caicos Islands. Human Ecology 29: 157–186. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Daw T (2008) Spatial distribution of effort by artisanal fishers: Exploring economic factors affecting the lobster fisheries of the Corn Islands, Nicaragua. Fisheries Research 90: 17–25. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Furusawa T (2012) Tracking Fishing Activities of the Roviana Population in the Solomon Islands Using a Portable Global Positioning System Unit and a Heart Rate Monitor. Field Methods 24: 216–229. [Google Scholar]
- 75. Bush S, Mol A (2014) Governing in a placeless environment: Sustainability and fish aggregating devices. Environmental Science & Policy In press. [Google Scholar]
- 76. Webster D (2008) Adaptive Governance: The Dynamics of Atlantic Fisheries Management. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 77. Shainee M, Leira B (2011) On the cause of premature FAD loss in the Maldives. Fisheries Research 109: 42–53. [Google Scholar]
- 78. Grant S, Berkes F (2007) Fisher knowledge as expert system: A case from the longline fishery of Grenada, the Eastern Caribbean. Fisheries Research 84: 162–170. [Google Scholar]
- 79. Kotzerka J, Garthe S, Hatch SA (2010) GPS tracking devices reveal foraging strategies of Black-legged Kittiwakes. Journal of Ornithology 151: 459–467. [Google Scholar]
- 80. Wajnberg E, Hoffmeister TS, Coquillard P (2013) Optimal within-patch movement strategies for optimising patch residence time: an agent-based modelling approach. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 2053–2063. [Google Scholar]
- 81. Marshall H, Carter A, Ashford A, Rowcliffe J, Cowlishaw G (2013) How do foragers decide when to leave a patch? A test of alternative models under natural and experimental conditions. Journal of Animal Ecology 82: 894–902. 10.1111/1365-2656.12089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Hutchinson J, Wilke A, Todd P (2008) Patch leaving in humans: can a generalist adapt its rules to dispersal of items across patches? Animal Behaviour 75: 1331–1349. [Google Scholar]
- 83. Wajnberg E, Bernhard P, Hamelin F, Boivin G (2006) Optimal patch time allocation for time-limited foragers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60: 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- 84. Alvard M (1993) Testing The Ecologically Noble Savage Hypothesis—Interspecific Prey Choice By Piro Hunters Of Amazonian Peru. Human Ecology 21: 355–387. [Google Scholar]
- 85. Hill K, Kaplan H, Hawkes K, Hurtado A (1987) Foraging Decisions Among Ache Hunter-Gatherers—New Data and Implications For Optimal Foraging Models. Ethology and Sociobiology 8: 1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 86. Smith E (1983) Anthropological applications of optimal foraging theory—a critical-review. Current Anthropology 24: 625–651. [Google Scholar]
- 87. Raichlen D, Wood B, Gordon A, Mabulla A, Marlowe F, et al. (2013) Evidence of Lévy walk foraging patterns in human hunter—gatherers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111: 728–733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
GPS data files are available from the Texas A&M University Digital Repository (http://hdl.handle.net/1969.1/152005).