Abstract
Processes through which parental ideology is transmitted to children—especially at a young age prior to the formation of political beliefs—remain poorly understood. Given recent evidence that political ideology is associated with neural responses to cognitive conflict in adults, we tested the exploratory hypothesis that children’s neurocognitive responses to conflict may also differ depending on their parents’ ideology. We assessed relations between parental political ideology and children’s neurocognitive responses to conflict, as measured by the N2 component of the event-related potential. Children aged 5–7 completed an age-appropriate flanker task while EEG was recorded, and the N2 was scored to incongruent versus congruent flankers to index conflict processing. Because previous research documents heightened liberal-conservative differences in threat-relevant contexts, each trial of the task was preceded by an angry face (threat-relevant) or comparison face (happy or neutral). An effect of parental ideology on the conflict-related N2 emerged in the threat condition, such that the N2 was larger among children of liberals compared with children of moderates and conservatives. These findings suggest that individual differences in neurocognitive responses to conflict, heightened in the context of threat, may reflect a more general pattern of individual differences that, in adults, relates to political ideology.
Keywords: political ideology, conflict monitoring, conflict sensitivity, N2, children
Effects of Parental Ideology on Neural Sensitivity to Cognitive Conflict in Children
As political ideologies become increasingly polarized, the differences between liberals and conservatives are often thought to extend beyond policy preferences to fundamental differences in psychological function. Although a growing body of evidence has linked difference in political views to more general forms of psychological and neural processing, the important question of whether these effects are due to political beliefs, as opposed to dispositions already in place prior to the development of political beliefs, remains (Hibbing, Smith, & Alford, 2014; Jost & Amodio, 2012)
In the current study, we took a developmental approach to exploring this issue by examining a pattern of neurocognitive function, previously associated with ideology in adults, in the children of liberals, moderates, and conservatives. We reasoned that while these children were unlikely to have refined political beliefs, they would share more basic psychological propensities of their parents (Olson, Vernon, Harris, & Jang, 2001). If a similar pattern of neurocognitive response were observed in the children of liberals versus conservatives, then this evidence would weigh against the possibility that previously-observed ideology effects were due to political beliefs, per se. Furthermore, this observation would provide further support for the idea that psychological patterns associated with political ideology in adults emerge from cognitive styles already in place at a younger age.
Psychologists have long associated political beliefs with broader cognitive, motivational, and personality styles (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950). More recently, Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, and Sulloway (2003) proposed that certain ideological positions—such as advocacy versus resistance to social change and rejection versus acceptance of inequality—are adopted in part because they serve more general motivations to reduce uncertainty, ambiguity, and threat. For example, research has revealed that liberals exhibit a greater tolerance of ambiguity, uncertainty, and complexity, whereas conservatives exhibit less cognitive flexibility and more structured and persistent approaches to decision making (Jost et al., 2003), as well as less openness to new experiences (Carney, Jost, Gosling, & Potter, 2008; Gerber, Huber, Doherty, Dowling, & Ha, 2010). These patterns suggest that political ideology may emerge from a broader set of psychological processes involved in motivation and self-regulation, in particular (Jost & Amodio, 2012).
Research on the neurocognitive correlates of ideology have focused on individual differences in the processing of conflict—a core component of self-regulation (e.g., Amodio, Jost, Master, & Yee, 2007; Kanai, Feilden, Firth, & Rees, 2011; Weissflog, Choma, Dywan, van Noordt, & Segalowitz, 2013). Broadly speaking, conflict processing involves the detection of discrepancies when two or more incompatible response options are activated simultaneously and the recruitment of neural resources to resolve conflicting or complex information (Barch, Braver, Sabb, & Noll, 2000; Botvinick, Brayer, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001). This mechanism of cognitive control supports the self-regulation of behavior in the face of complex response options in order to facilitate higher-order goals and intentions.
Conflict processing has been associated with a scalp-recorded event-related potential (ERP), the N2. The N2 occurs over the frontal-midline region beginning approximately 200 to 350 ms following stimulus onset. Importantly, the amplitude of the N2 is larger to stimuli that evoke a response conflict, such as the presentation of incongruent response options (e.g., incongruent flanker displays in a visual flanker task) or tasks that require the inhibition of prepotent responses, such as No-Go trials in a Go/No-Go task (Kopp, Rist, & Mattler, 1996; Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, van den Wildenberg, & Ridderinkhof, 2003; Van Veen & Carter, 2002a). Source analyses suggest that the N2 is generated by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) under circumstances that elicit conflicting response tendencies (Banich et al., 2001; Carter et al., 1998; Van Veen & Carter, 2002b). The N2 amplitude is therefore believed to in part signal the extent to which conflict-related cognitive processes are engaged during tasks.
If political liberals exhibit greater awareness of and responsiveness to complex and potentially conflicting pieces of information, in comparison with conservatives, this difference should be associated with increased ACC activity and its corresponding N2 amplitude. Amodio et al. (2007) investigated this hypothesis in a sample of young adults. After participants indicated their political orientation, they completed a Go/No-Go task in which a small proportion of trials required them to withhold the habitual “Go” response. Amodio and colleagues (2007) observed that liberals exhibited greater accuracy and larger N2 amplitudes on No-Go trials, in comparison with conservatives. This result was consistent with the hypothesis that liberalism is associated with greater neurocognitive activity under conditions of response conflict. These findings have been conceptually replicated and extended in other studies (Inzlicht, McGregor, Hirsh, & Nash, 2009; Shook & Fazio, 2009; Weissflog et al., 2013). Taken in conjunction, this work suggests that the endorsement of a more liberal ideology is associated with enhanced responsiveness of neural systems to cognitive conflict. Given this past evidence, and given prior evidence that individual differences in conflict processing have a significant heritable component, we hypothesized that children of more liberal parents would exhibit larger N2 amplitudes during a conflict monitoring task.
Ideological differences in conflict processing might also be moderated by the emotional context in which cognitive processes operate (McLean et al., 2013). The past decade has seen an explosion of research demonstrating that individual differences, such as enhanced sensitivity to negative and threat-related stimuli, moderate the impact of emotional factors on cognition (e.g., Dennis & Chen, 2007; Hofmann, Ellard, & Siegle, 2012; Ohrmann et al., 2007). For individuals showing this enhanced sensitivity, threat-relevant stimuli such as human faces displaying fear and anger can disrupt neurocognitive processes, especially when the threatening stimuli are distracting or irrelevant to task performance (Fox, Russo, & Dutton, 2002; Mogg, Philippot, & Bradley, 2004). For example, Dennis and Chen (2009) found that moderately anxious adults, who are thought to show elevated threat sensitivity, evidenced reduced sensitivity of the N2 to incongruent versus congruent flankers following the presentation of task-irrelevant fearful faces.
Political conservatism is associated with greater sensitivity to negative stimuli (Carraro, Castelli, & Macchiella, 2011; Jost et al., 2007; Oxley et al., 2008; Shook & Clay, 2011) and greater vigilance for potential threats in the environment (Dodd, Balzer, & Jacobs, 2012; McLean et al., 2013). For instance, conservatives are more likely than liberals to respond to negative or threatening images with increased physiological arousal and to rapidly fixate upon the images, suggesting increased cognitive resources devoted to threat (Dodd et al., 2012; Hibbing et al., 2014). Thus, it is possible that threat-related contexts may amplify neurocognitive differences between liberals and conservatives, whereas pleasant or neutral contexts would not. Consistent with this notion, McLean and colleagues (2013) found that conservatives, but not liberals, displayed enhanced sensitivity to angry targets, measured by reaction times indexing greater attentional focusing. In the present study, we varied the emotional context of conflict monitoring by displaying an angry face (threat-relevant) or a comparison face (happy or neutral) prior to each trial of a flanker task. These faces were task-irrelevant because they provided no information about the target flanker task. We considered the possibility that liberal-conservative individual differences in the N2 response to conflict (greater N2 amplitudes to incongruent flankers among children of liberals versus conservatives) would be heightened in the threat-relevant context, which follows from the idea that children of liberals, relative to conservatives, would be less reactive to the task-irrelevant angry faces and thus more able to cognitively engage with the target flanker task.
The goal of the present study was to test whether enhanced neurocognitive responses to conflict, which has been observed in more liberal versus conservative adults (e.g., Amodio et al., 2007), are exhibited by children of liberals, as compared to children of conservatives. We recruited early school-aged children (ages 5–7) and obtained parents’ reports of their own political orientation. Child participants then completed the Attention Network Test (ANT) while their brain activity was recorded using EEG. The ANT is a cued version of the Eriksen Flanker Task, which has been shown to elicit ACC activity and can be used to measure attentional processing in response to congruent and incongruent responses options (Fan, Wu, Fossella, & Posner, 2001). Moreover, the Flanker Task has been identified as an especially promising measurement tool for identifying core cognitive processes associated with ideological differences, especially in emotional contexts (McLean et al., 2013). In the present study, we used the ANT to estimate the influence of parental political orientation on both N2 amplitudes and attention performance under conditions of high conflict (incongruent flankers) and low conflict (congruent flankers). A manipulation of emotional context was introduced so that every trial was preceded by the presentation of an angry, pleasant, or neutral face (see Dennis, Malone, & Chen, 2009). We chose human faces because of their salience and social significance (Johnson, 2005) and compared the effects of angry faces to pleasant and neutral faces to explore whether the effects of parental ideology on conflict monitoring would be strengthened in a threat-relevant emotional context.
Our focal hypothesis was that children of more liberal, as opposed to more conservative, parents would exhibit larger N2 amplitudes, indicating enhanced recruitment of cognitive resources to track and resolve conflict on incongruent trials. In addition, we tested the exploratory hypothesis that these individual differences linked to parental ideology would be stronger when participants were exposed to angry (threat-relevant) faces as compared with happy and neutral faces.
Method
Participants
Fifty-one children aged 5 to 7 years old (M = 75.33 months, SD = 6.29) from the New York City area completed the study. Of these 51 children, eight were excluded because of EEG artifacts produced by excessive movement (on 60% of trials or more). These eight children were all 5 years old, which is consistent with the fact that younger children have more difficulty remaining still during EEG recording (Banaschewski & Brandeis, 2007). Ten children were excluded because of excessive response errors or outlying scores with respect to response latencies (more than 3 standard deviations above the mean), yielding a final sample of 33 participants. Biological parents (30 mothers and three fathers) and children were ethnically diverse: 42% (n= 14) were White/Caucasian; 36% (n= 12) were Black or African-American; 12% (n= 4) were Hispanic/Latino; 3% (n= 1) was Asian; and 6% (n= 2) were biracial (i.e., they chose Hispanic and another category). The education level of parents was 6% completed 11th grade (n = 2);3% completed 12th grade with no diploma (n = 1);15% were high school graduates (n = 5);15% completed some college but did not have a degree (n = 5); 24% had an associate’s degree (n = 8);18% had a bachelor’s degree (n = 6);15% had a master’s degree (n = 5); and 3% had professional degree (n = 1). Median annual household income (before taxes) was “between $40,000–50,000” with a sample range from “$10,000–$20,000” to “$150,000 and above.” The relationship status of parents was 64% married (n = 21), 6% (n = 2) unmarried but living with a partner, 9% (n= 3) separated, 3% (n= 1) divorced, and 18% (n = 6) never married. Eighty-five percent of the children were right-handed (n= 28).
Procedures
Prior to beginning the EEG portion of the experiment, informed consent was obtained from parents and children. All consented to participation. The entire laboratory visit lasted approximately 2.5 hours, and children were informed that they could take a break at any time.
Attention Network Test
EEG was recorded while children completed a modified version of the Child Attention Network Test (ANT; Dennis et al., 2009; Rueda et al., 2004) in the experimental booth. Participants were seated approximately 65 cm from a personal IBM computer with a 14-inch monitor (Windows XP) in a dimly lit room. The ANT, which was presented using E-Prime software (Psychological Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA), involves a combination of cued reaction time (Posner, 1980) and a flanker task. In the flanker task, participants must respond to a central target stimulus, such as an arrow, that points toward either the left or right side of the screen. Participants are instructed to press a button to indicate the direction that the arrow (left or right). However, the target stimulus is flanked by additional stimuli on both sides. On some trials, the flanker stimuli point in the same direction of the target; hence, these trials are referred to as “congruent” flankers. On other trials, the flankers point toward the opposite direction, and are thus referred to as “incongruent.” Comparing responses to incongruent versus congruent flankers provides a measure of conflict monitoring.
The original ANT designed for adults used arrows to represent the focal stimuli and flankers (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002). To make the task more relevant and interesting to young children, the target (central) and flanking stimuli (two on each side) were changed to fish (Rueda et al., 2004). Children were instructed to “catch the fish named Fun” in the center of the screen by indicating the direction in which the fish’s nose was pointing (selecting the right or left mouse button as quickly as they could). Targets with fish all pointing in the same direction are congruent and those with the central fish pointing in a different direction than the flanking fish are incongruent. There were equal numbers of congruent and incongruent trials. Children were given a six-trial practice block before starting the three experimental blocks and were given feedback on their performance. Between blocks, children were given short breaks in which to rest.
Trials were preceded by cueing conditions that served to alert the child to the impending target or to orient children to a location on the screen where the target will appear. These cueing conditions can also be used to calculate performance efficiency scores in multiple domains of attention (alerting, orienting, and executive). However, because the present study’s hypotheses focused on speed and accuracy under conditions of high versus low conflict (incongruent versus congruent flankers), reaction times were collapsed across cueing conditions, separately by flanker type.
Based on previous studies (Dennis & Chen, 2009; Dennis et al., 2009), the task was modified by briefly presenting angry, happy, or neutral faces for 200ms prior to each trial to determine the extent to which threat-relevant (versus pleasant or neutral) stimuli influence N2 amplitudes and attentional performance. Participants were presented with three experimental blocks, with each block presenting angry, happy, or neutral faces prior to each trial. Order of blocks was counterbalanced across participants. Each block included 64 trials, which were equally divided between congruent and incongruent flanker displays. There were 48 grayscale photographs of faces, and these were equally divided, so there were 16 from each of the three categories of facial expression (Tottenham et al., 2009).
Each trial consisted of a series of events (see Figure 1). First, an emotional face stimulus (angry, happy, or neutral) was presented for 200ms, followed by a fixation cross, which was variably presented on the screen for 400–1600ms. Subsequently, one of the four cueing conditions (no cue, center cue, double cue, or spatial cue) was presented for 150ms. This was again followed by the fixation cross that was presented for 450ms. After the fixation period, the central target (along with the flankers pointing in either congruent or incongruent directions in comparison to the central target) was displayed for up to 1700ms or until a response was made. The flanker display disappeared when the button was pressed. The final event consisted of a fixation period that was variably presented for 1000ms. No feedback on performance was given. Each trial lasted for a maximum of 5,100ms. Prior to averaging reaction times (RTs) for each stimulus condition, trials with outliers (RTs ± 3 standard deviations from each participant’s mean) were discarded. RT and error rates to congruent and incongruent flankers were the target measures of interest.
Figure 1.
Experimental procedure for the modified ANT (see also Dennis, Malone, & Chen, 2009). Participants indicate the direction in which the nose of the fish in the center of the target display is pointing. Targets with fish all pointing in the same direction are congruent and those with the central fish pointing in a different direction than the flanking fish are incongruent.
EEG Recording
Using the Biosemi system (BioSemi; Amsterdam, NL), EEG activity was recorded continuously via 64 Ag/AgCl scalp electrodes embedded in an elasticized nylon cap based on the international 10/20 system. Eye movements were monitored by electro-oculogram (EOG) from electrodes placed 1 cm above and below the left eye (to measure vertical eye movements) and 1 cm on the outer corner of each eye (to measure horizontal eye movements). The EEG signal was preamplified at the electrode to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. EEG was recorded at a sampling rate of 512 Hz. The voltage from each active electrode was referenced online with respect to a common mode sense active electrode producing a monopolar (non differential) channel.
Offline data processing was conducted using Brain Vision Analyzer (Version 2.2, GmbH; Munich, DE). Data were re-referenced offline to an average reference and filtered with a high pass frequency of .1 Hz and a low pass frequency of 30 Hz. The EEG was segmented for each trial beginning 400 ms prior to flanker onset and continuing for 800 ms. Baseline correction was performed for each trial, using the 200 ms prior to flanker onset.
EEG was corrected for blinks using independent components analysis (ICA). Artifacts were identified using the following criteria: any data with voltage steps exceeding 75 μV, changes within a segment that were greater than 200 μV, amplitude differences greater than ±120 μV within a segment, and activity lower than .2 μV per 100ms. Such artifacts were excluded from analysis. After ICA, data were visually inspected to confirm successful eye blink removal and to detect any remaining artifacts. Data from individual channels containing artifacts were rejected on a trial-by-trial basis.
Prior to analyses, outliers were removed from raw RT and N2 data using a 90% Winsorisation: data falling below the 5th percentile were set to the value of the 5th percentile and data falling above the 95th percentile were set to the 95th percentile. Analyses targeted the conflict-related N2 component, which was found by Amodio et al. (2007) to be associated with political ideology in adults. The N2 was quantified for correct trials only as the peak amplitude between 300–370 ms post stimulus onset to the flankers at electrode Fz, where N2 amplitudes were maximal. All participants showed their maximal amplitude within this window. Figure 2 illustrates the scalp distribution of the N2 for incongruent and congruent flankers, separately. Peak amplitudes were calculated for each flanker type, separately for each face condition (angry, happy, neutral). In addition, N2 difference scores were calculated as the difference between incongruent and congruent flankers following each face type. This difference score was used as an indicator of neural sensitivity to stimulus-response conflict. The average number of trials used to calculate the N2 for incongruent flankers was 20.41 (SD = 6.09) and for congruent flankers was 23.63 (SD = 5.67).
Figure 2.
Scalp distribution of the N2 for incongruent and congruent flankers, averaged across the three emotion conditions.
Questionnaires
Parental ideology
To assess political orientation (or ideology), we asked parents to place themselves on a scale ranging from −5 (Extremely Liberal) to 5 (Extremely Conservative). A response of zero was described as “neither liberal nor conservative” and for the present study was considered to indicate moderate (i.e., centrist) political views. This single item strongly predicts presidential voting intentions and a number of other social and political attitudes and behaviors (Jost, 2006). Parental ideology was examined as a continuous variable and also in terms of liberal, moderate, and conservative groups, so that negative scores were coded as liberal, scores of zero as moderate, and positive scores as conservative. Using this scoring system, roughly three equal groups emerged: liberal (n = 12), moderate (n = 9), and conservative (n = 12). The mean score for all parents on this scale was −0.15 (SD = 2.31) with a wide range of scores from −4 to 5.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Descriptive statistics concerning children’s task performance and N2 amplitudes are presented in Table 1. Mean values are shown separately by flanker type (incongruent or congruent) and whether the trial was preceded by angry, happy, or neutral faces. Figure 3 shows N2 amplitudes for congruent and incongruent trials for children of conservative, moderate, and liberal parents separately for each face condition. Children’s age was unrelated to parental political orientation: for conservative parents, Mage in months = 75.92 (SD = 4.54), for moderate parents, M = 75.44 (SD = 6.15), and for liberal parents, M = 74.67 (SD = 8.14).
Table 1.
Means and Standard Deviations for N2 Amplitudes and Attention Performance on the Attention Network Test Following Each Emotional Face Condition
Angry Faces | Happy Faces | Neutral Faces | Average | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N2 Amplitudes (mean mV) | ||||
Incongruent | −7.07 (4.70) | −6.89 (4.74) | −5.60 (5.55) | −6.52 (3.98) |
Congruent | −4.75 (5.39) | −5.08 (4.31) | −5.31 (5.53) | −5.05 (3.70) |
Error Rate (%) | ||||
Incongruent | 0.32 (0.21) | 0.31 (0.20) | 0.34 (0.22) | 0.32 (0.19) |
Congruent | 0.22 (0.19) | 0.19 (0.17) | 0.23 (0.20) | 0.22 (0.17) |
Reaction Time (in ms) | ||||
Incongruent | 918.03 (159.76) | 912.22 (166.28) | 905.99 (188.6) | 912.08 (154.28) |
Congruent | 854.61 (155.26) | 847.46 (148.52) | 851.47 (172.13) | 851.18 (144.17) |
Note. Means are presented with standard deviations in parentheses. N2 amplitudes and reaction times are based on correct trials only.
Figure 3.
N2 amplitudes for congruent and incongruent trials for the children of liberals, moderates, and conservatives separately for each face condition.
Correlations among Study Variables
Bivariate correlations were computed for all study variables to determine whether parental ideology was associated with conflict-related neurocognitive functioning (N2 amplitudes to incongruent trials) and attentional performance (RTs and error rates on incongruent trials) for a total of 9 correlations.1 Consistent with predictions, greater parental liberalism (lower scores on ideology) was associated with larger N2 amplitudes following angry faces, r(33) = .39, p = .03 (see Figure 4). No other significant correlations between parental ideology and N2 amplitudes emerged, and no other correlations among study variables reached significance.
Figure 4.
Children of more liberal parents showed greater N2 amplitudes to incongruent flankers following angry faces.
Associations between Parental Ideology and Child Attentional Performance
We explored whether children of liberals, as compared with those of conservatives, would exhibit performance differences on high conflict (incongruent) stimulus trials, as a function of emotional context. Reaction time and error rate scores were submitted separately to 3 (Parental ideology: liberal, moderate, conservative) X 2 (Flanker: incongruent, congruent) X 3 (Emotion: angry, happy, neutral) repeated measures ANOVAs.
Main effects of Flanker congruence emerged for each dependent variable, indicating that, unsurprisingly, performance on the ANT was poorer under conditions of response conflict. Specifically, the analysis of error rates yielded a main effect of Flanker [F(1,30) = 23.47, p = .001 η2 = .44] indicating that errors were greater for incongruent [M = 0.33 ± 0.06] than congruent flankers [M = 0.22 ± 0.06, p = .001], as did the analysis of reaction times [F(1,30) = 43.92, p = .001 η2 = .59] indicating that reaction times were longer for incongruent [M = 915 ± 54] than congruent flankers [M = 855 ± 50, p = .001].
Additionally, the Emotion X Parental ideology interaction was significant for reaction times [F(4, 60) = 2.72, p = .04, η2 = .15] indicating that children of conservatives had faster reaction times following neutral faces, regardless of flanker type [M = 880 ± 90], than children of moderates [M = 970 ± 102, p = .04]. Among children of conservatives only, reaction times were faster following neutral versus angry faces [M = 880 ± 90, p = .03].
Associations between Parental Ideology and Child N2 Amplitudes
To investigate our focal prediction that the conflict-related N2 amplitudes would be larger among children with more politically liberal parents, we conducted a 3 (Parental ideology: liberal, moderate, conservative) X 2 (Flanker: incongruent, congruent) X 3 (Emotional expression: angry, happy, neutral) repeated measures ANOVA. This analysis tested the prediction that ideological differences would be greater under conditions of threat (i.e., following exposure to angry faces) relative to non-threat (happy or neutral faces). It also enabled us to explore whether children of political moderates would exhibit patterns of response that more closely resembled those of liberals or conservatives.
The ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of Flanker [F(1, 30) = 7.74, p = .009, η2= .21] indicating that N2 amplitudes were greater to incongruent [M = −6.47 ± 1.39] versus congruent flankers [M = −4.95 ± 1.20, p = .009]. Additionally, the Parental ideology X Emotion interaction reached significance, F(4, 60) = 2.76, p = .04, η2= .16 (see Figure 5). Simple effects analyses revealed that in the angry face condition children of liberals [M = −8.66 ± 2.26] exhibited significantly larger N2 amplitudes than both children of moderates [M = −3.73 ± 2.60, p = .008] and children of conservatives [M = −4.80 ± 2.26, p = .02]. Furthermore, children of liberals exhibited significantly larger N2 amplitudes in the angry versus neutral [M = −5.02 ± 2.76, p = .01] face conditions.2
Figure 5.
Interaction between parental ideology and emotion type for N2 amplitudes. Errors bars show ± 1 standard error.
Discussion
As evidence for the neurocognitive correlates of political ideology builds, researchers are increasingly faced with the questions of whether these correlates relate to political views per se or whether they reflect a more general cognitive style that likely developed earlier in life (e.g., Block & Block, 2006). Findings from the current study raise the possibility that neurocognitive responses to conflict—a form of processing that has been associated with ideology in multiple studies—reflect a more general pattern of individual differences present in childhood that relates, later in adulthood, to political ideology. Specifically, children of liberal parents exhibited larger N2 amplitudes during both incongruent and congruent trials of a flanker task, indicating enhanced recruitment of cognitive resources to detect and resolve conflict, in comparison with children of moderate and conservative parents. This effect only emerged in trials following threat-relevant angry faces. This pattern conceptually replicates, in children, an effect of ideology on neurocognitive functioning that has been documented in studies involving adults (e.g., Amodio et al., 2007; Shook & Fazio, 2009; Weissflog et al., 2013), and points to the potential role of the emotional context, in particular that of threat-relevant information. These findings are broadly consistent with the idea that psychological patterns associated with political ideology in adults may emerge from cognitive styles already in place at a younger age, rather than reflecting political beliefs per se.
Predicted effects of parental ideology on the N2 emerged, but were only observed in the presence of threat-relevant stimuli (angry faces), which previous research suggests may elicit reduced reactivity in liberal as compared to conservative adults (Jost et al., 2007; Oxley et al., 2008). The specificity of this effect in the context of negative and threat-relevant stimuli has several implications. Consistent with the view that liberalism is associated with greater ability to integrate and process aversive, complex, or threat-relevant stimuli (Jost et al., 2007; Oxley et al., 2008), it is possible that the offspring of liberal adults share this basic individual difference prior to the formation of political beliefs. This would explain the finding that neurocognitive processing of conflict in the children of liberals versus more conservative adults remained relatively robust in the context of stimuli that were task-irrelevant and threatening (in the present study, angry faces). Indeed, previous research suggests that threat sensitivity in childhood predicts ideology in adulthood: rigid and anxious qualities in early childhood predict conservatism in early adulthood (Block & Block, 2006), and early temperamental fearfulness predicts conservatism in late adolescence (Fraley, Griffin, Belsky, & Roisman, 2012). This idea that heightened sensitivity to a threat-relevant emotional context is associated with disruptions in the sensitivity of the N2 to conflict is consistent with research showing such disruptions in trait anxious adults (Dennis & Chen, 2009).
On the other hand, other research suggests that low arousal, threatening stimuli may facilitate cognitive performance in general (Fenske & Eastwood, 2003; Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2010; O’Toole, DeCicco, Hong, & Dennis, 2011). Conflict monitoring, which is rapidly developing during this age period (Jonkman, 2006; Rueda et al., 2004; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005), may have been facilitated in children of liberals because they, like their parents, have greater ability to devote resources to a task despite task-irrelevant and distracting negative stimuli. At the same time, following angry faces, children of liberals showed a greater N2 response to both incongruent and congruent trials, the latter in which reduced N2 amplitudes are expected. An important question for future research is whether this reduced discrimination between distinct levels of conflict represents general increased engagement with the task or other processes, such as increase cognitive vigilance. Overall, the specificity of the effects of parental ideology that we observed on child N2 responses suggests that future research should systematically investigate the emotional contexts in which neurocognitive differences between liberals and conservatives do and do not emerge, as well as the developmental trajectory of these differences. Moreover, it supports the idea that these individual differences in cognitive responses precede the development of discrete political views.
In general, previous studies suggest that liberalism is associated with greater cognitive flexibility and integrative complexity (e.g., Jost et al., 2003). In the present study, however, we observed only one, unexpected, link between parental political orientation and attentional performance in children: both children of liberal and conservative parents showed faster reaction times following neutral faces. This effect was unrelated to ERP findings in that the N2 was sensitive to parental ideology only in the context of angry faces. Thus, this reaction time effect may reveal that neutral faces simply interfere less with task performance compared to valenced stimuli (happy and angry faces in this study). Moreover, neurocognitive differences in conflict processing in young children may reflect relatively subtle difference in cognitive function that may not have a direct impact on standard metrics of attention performance during a moderately difficult conflict monitoring task (in this study, errors ranged from around 20–30%). Thus, this divergent finding of distinct effects of emotion in the behavioral and neurophysiological realm could reflect a developmental sequence in which the N2 is sensitive to early individual differences in neurocognitive responses to threat that are not yet readily apparent in behavior, but which may have a systematic impact on the development of other functional capacities later in life (Banaschewski & Brandeis, 2007; Burghy et al., 2012). Tracking such epigenetic processes in cognitive responses associated with the emergence of political ideology is a crucial goal for future research.
Although our study provided new data on the role of conflict processing in children as it relates to their parent’s ideology, several limitations are notable. For example, parental ideology was assessed using a single-item measure. While this single item has been shown to strongly predict major political behaviors, such as voting decisions (Jost, 2006), it is possible that a multidimensional assessment of parental ideology may have revealed additional complexity to the observed relation with children’s conflict processing. In addition, the primary task, the modified flanker task, used to generate both the N2 and measures of attention performance, may have limited our ability to compare our findings with those from other studies on ideology and the N2, which have typically used the go-/no-go task (e.g., Amodio et al., 2007).
It is further notable that this study was not designed to examine the process through which political orientation is passed from a parent to a child. Moreover, children aged five to seven are unlikely to have developed substantial political beliefs (Hess & Torney, 2005). Rather, this study speaks to the important question of whether concordances between parental ideology and individual differences in children’s cognitive responses to conflict are already in place at a young age, prior to the likely development of political beliefs, and thus are not due to political views per se. The results of the current study suggest that neurocognitive patterns associated with political ideology in adults may emerge from cognitive styles already in place at a younger age.
It is important to stress that neurodevelopmental issues should be taken into account when interpreting results. The sensitivity of the N2 to conflict may not yet be stable in early childhood (Buss, Dennis, Brooker, & Sippel, 2011) —the target age of the present study. A possible implication of this developmental variability is that the greater N2 responses in children of liberals (versus moderates and conservatives) might reflect neurodevelopmental maturity rather than responsivity to conflict. Yet, the sample as a whole evidenced an “adult-like” flanker effects (i.e., greater N2 amplitudes to incongruent versus congruent flankers). Group differences specific to the angry face context suggest that children’s neurocognitive responses, because they are developing so rapidly, may be especially sensitive to affective contexts. In other words, a threat-related context might have either bolstered the recruitment of cognitive resources in the service of conflict processing among children of liberals or interfered with this capacity in children of moderates and conservatives. Because the amplitude of the N2 is believed to signal the extent to which cognitive resources are recruited under conditions of conflict, this further shows that a threat-related context has distinct neurocognitive implications for children of liberal versus conservative parents.
In summary, the present study provides the first evidence to our knowledge that individual differences in conflict processing associated with political ideology in adults is present in the young children of liberals, moderates and conservative—even though these children presumably have not yet developed refined political beliefs. This evidence weighs against the conclusion that previously-observed ideology effects on conflict processing in adults were due to political beliefs, per se, and instead suggests that psychological patterns associated with political ideology in adults may emerge from cognitive styles already in place at a younger age. These findings help to lay the groundwork for theoretical and empirical research addressing the origins, development, and cross-generational transmission of political ideology. Such research also has broader implications for the creation of developmental cognitive neuroscience models that identify biomarkers and neural mechanisms in the development of beliefs, values, and opinions.
Footnotes
When race/ethnicity and education were entered as covariates, the results remained the same.
The pattern of effects was the same with mean N2 amplitudes (calculated as the area under the curve from 300–370 ms. There were no significant effects for N2 latency when using the peak data.
References
- Adorno TW, Frenkel-Brunswik E, Levinson DJ, Sanford RN. The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Amodio D, Jost J, Master S, Yee C. Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nature Neuroscience. 2007;10(10):1246–1247. doi: 10.1038/nn1979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banaschewski T, Brandeis D. Annotation: What electrical brain activity tells us about brain function that other techniques cannot tell us - a child psychiatric perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(5):415–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01681.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banich MT, Milham MP, Jacobson BL, Webb A, Wszalek T, Cohen NJ, Kramer AF. Attentional selection and the processing of task-irrelevant information: Insights from fMRI examinations of the Stroop task. Progress in Brain Research. 2001;134:459–470. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(01)34030-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barch DM, Braver TS, Sabb FW, Noll DC. Anterior cingulate and the monitoring of response conflict: evidence from an fMRI study of overt verb generation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12(2):298–309. doi: 10.1162/089892900562110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Block J, Block JH. Nursery school personality and political orientation two decades later. Journal of Research in Personality. 2006;40(5):734–749. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.09.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Brayer TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review. 2001;108(3):624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burghy CA, Stodola DE, Ruttle RP, Molloy EK, Armstrong JM, Oler JA, Birn RM. Developmental pathways to amygdala-prefrontal function and internalizing symptoms in adolescence. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15(12):1736–1741. doi: 10.1038/nn.3257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss KA, Dennis TA, Brooker RJ, Sippel LM. An ERP study of conflict monitoring in 4–8-year old children: Associations with temperament. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;1(2):131–140. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carney DR, Jost JT, Gosling SD, Potter J. The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology. 2008;29(6):807–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00668.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carraro L, Castelli L, Macchiella C. The automatic conservative: Ideology-based attentional asymmetries in the processing of valenced information. Plos ONE. 2011;6(11):1–6. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter C, Braver T, Barch D, Botvinick M, Noll D, Cohen J. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science. 1998;280(5364):747–749. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis TA, Chen C. Neurophysiological mechanisms in the emotional modulation of attention: the interplay between threat sensitivity and attentional control. Biological Psychology. 2007;76:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis TA, Chen C. Trait anxiety and conflict monitoring following threat: An ERP study. Psychophysiology. 2009;46:122–131. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00758.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis TA, Malone MM, Chen C. Emotional face processing and emotion regulation in children: An ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2009;34(1):85–102. doi: 10.1080/87565640802564887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodd MD, Balzer AB, Jacobs CM. The political left rolls with the good and the political right confronts the bad: Connecting physiology and cognition to preferences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367:640–649. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14(3):340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fan J, Wu Y, Fossella JA, Posner MI. Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BMC Neuroscience. 2001;2(1):14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-2-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenske MJ, Eastwood JD. Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: evidence from flanker tasks. Emotion. 2003;3(4):327–343. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox E, Russo R, Dutton K. Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagment from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion. 2002;16(3):355–379. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraley RC, Griffin BN, Belsky J, Roisman GI. Developmental antecedents of political Ideology a longitudinal lnvestigation from birth to age 18 years. Psychological science. 2012;23(11):1425–1431. doi: 10.1177/0956797612440102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gable P, Harmon-Jones E. The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Review. 2010;21(2):211–215. doi: 10.1177/0956797609359622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerber AS, Huber GA, Doherty D, Dowling CM, Ha SE. Personality and political attitudes: Relationships across issue domains and political contexts. American Political Science Review. 2010;104:111–133. doi: 10.1017/S0003055410000031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hess RD, Torney J. The development of political attitudes in children. Chicago: Aldine Transaction; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Hibbing JR, Smith KB, Alford JR. Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2014;37(3):297–307. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13001192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann SG, Ellard KK, Siegle GJ. Neurobiological correlates of cognitions in fear and anxiety: A cognitive neurobiological information-processing model. Cognition and Emotion. 2012;26(2):282–299. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.579414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inzlicht M, McGregor I, Hirsh JB, Nash K. Neural Markers of Religious Conviction. Psychological science. 2009;20(3):385–392. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02305.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson M. Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience. 2005;6(10):766–774. doi: 10.1038/nrn1766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonkman LM. The development of preparation, conflict monitoring and inhibition from early childhood to young adulthood: a Go/Nogo ERP study. Brain Research. 2006;1097:181–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jost JT. The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist. 2006;61(7):651–670. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jost JT, Amodio DM. Political ideology as motivated social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence. Motivation and Emotion. 2012;36:55–64. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jost JT, Glaser J, Kruglanski AW, Sulloway FJ. Exceptions that prove the rule--Using a theory of motivated social cognition to account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies: Reply to Greenberg and Jonas (2003) Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(3):383–393. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jost JT, Napier JL, Thorisdottir H, Gosling SD, Palfai TP, Ostafin B. Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2007;33(7):989–1007. doi: 10.1177/0146167207301028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanai R, Feilden T, Firth C, Rees G. Political orientations are correlated with brain structure in young adults. Current Biology. 2011;21(8):677–680. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopp B, Rist F, Mattler U. N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology. 1996;33(3):282–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean SP, Garza JP, Wiebe SA, Dodd MD, Smith KB, Hibbing JR, Espy KA. Applying the flanker task to political psychology: A research note. Political Psychology. 2013 doi: 10.1111/pops.12056. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mogg K, Philippot P, Bradley BP. Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113(1):160–165. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, van den Wildenberg W, Ridderinkhof KR. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3(1):17–26. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Toole LJ, DeCicco JM, Hong M, Dennis TA. The impact of task-irrelevant emotional stimuli on attention in three domains. Emotion. 2011;11(6):1322–1330. doi: 10.1037/a0024369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohrmann P, Rauch AV, Bauer J, Kugel H, Arolt V, Heindel W, Suslow T. Threat sensitivity as assessed by automatic amygdala response to fearful faces predicts speed of visual search for facial expression. Experimental Brain Research. 2007;183:51–59. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1022-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson JM, Vernon PA, Harris JA, Jang KL. The heritability of attitudes: A study of twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80(6):845–860. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oxley DR, Smith KB, Alford JR, Hibbing MV, Miller JL, Scalora M, Hibbing JR. Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science. 2008;321(5896):1667–1670. doi: 10.1126/science.1157627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posner MI. Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1980;32:3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, Halparin JD, Gruber DB, Lercari LP, Posner MI. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(8):1029–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: Contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28(2):573–594. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shook NJ, Clay R. Valence Asymmetry in Attitude Formation A Correlate of Political Ideology. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2011;2(6):650–655. doi: 10.1177/1948550611405219. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shook NJ, Fazio RH. Political ideology, exploration of novel stimuli, and attitude formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2009;45(4):995–998. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Nelson C. The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research. 2009;168(3):242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Veen V, Carter C. The anterior cingulate cortex as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior. 2002a;77(4–5):477–482. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Veen V, Carter C. The timing of action-monitoring processes in the antrior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002b;14(4):593–602. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissflog M, Choma BL, Dywan J, van Noordt SJR, Segalowitz SJ. The political (and physiological) divide: Political orientation, performance monitoring, and the anterior cingulate response. Social Neuroscience. 2013;8(5):434–447. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.833549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]