Skip to main content
Balkan Medical Journal logoLink to Balkan Medical Journal
. 2014 Dec 1;31(4):328–334. doi: 10.5152/balkanmedj.2014.14171

The Investigation of Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials Responses in Young Adults Having Musical Education

Zahra Polat 1,, Ahmet Ataş 2
PMCID: PMC4318404  PMID: 25667787

Abstract

Background:

In the literature, music education has been shown to enhance auditory perception for children and young adults. When compared to young adult non-musicians, young adult musicians demonstrate increased auditory processing, and enhanced sensitivity to acoustic changes. The evoked response potentials associated with the interpretation of sound are enhanced in musicians. Studies show that training also changes sound perception and cortical responses. The earlier training appears to lead to larger changes in the auditory cortex.

Aims:

Most cortical studies in the literature have used pure tones or musical instrument sounds as stimuli signals. The aim of those studies was to investigate whether musical education would enhance auditory cortical responses when speech signals were used. In this study, the speech sounds extracted from running speech were used as sound stimuli.

Study Design:

Non-randomized controlled study.

Methods:

The experimental group consists of young adults up to 21 years-old, all with a minimum of 4 years of musical education. The control group was selected from young adults of the same age without any musical education. The experiments were conducted by using a cortical evoked potential analyser and /m/, /t/ /g/ sound stimulation at the level of 65 dB SPL. In this study, P1 / N1 / P2 amplitude and latency values were measured.

Results:

Significant differences were found in the amplitude values of P1 and P2 (p<0.05). The differences among the latencies were not found to be significantly important (p>0.05).

Conclusion:

The results obtained in our study indicate that musical experience has an effect on the nervous system and this can be seen in cortical auditory evoked potentials recorded when the subjects hear speech.

Keywords: Auditory cortex, cortical auditory, evoked potentials, evoked responses, musical training


In the literature, music education has been shown to enhance auditory perception for children and young adults (13). Compared to young adult non-musicians, young adult musicians demonstrate increased auditory processing, and enhanced sensitivity to acoustic changes. The evoked response potentials associated with the interpretation of sound are enhanced in musicians. Studies show that training also changes sound perception and cortical responses. Earlier training appears to lead to larger changes in the auditory cortex (46). Learning to read in a language involves auditory processing because, in order to learn to read, children must be able to break a word into its phonemes. Hence, if phonemic awareness is increased in children, this may lead to increased reading skills. In the literature, it has been reported that pre-school children’s phonemic awareness and early reading skills are correlated with musical training.

Studies have also shown that the earlier maturation of evoked potentials in children is correlated to musical training (5, 7, 8). Most cortical studies in the literature used pure tones or musical instrument sounds as stimuli. However, in the literature, there are studies showing musicians’ advantages for encoding both music and speech (9, 10). A neural basis for this musician benefit has been demonstrated in the sub cortical encoding of sounds. The brain stem responses for musicians and non-musicians were similar when quiet, but noise had a more disruptive effect on the morphology, size, timing, and frequency of non-musicians’ responses compared to musicians’ (10, 11). The aim of that study was to investigate whether musical education would enhance auditory cortical responses when speech signals were used. Therefore, speech sounds extracted from running speech were used as sound stimuli in this study.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

This work has been approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee. A signed informed consent form was obtained from each participant following a detailed explanation of the procedures that they may undergo. The thirteen subjects in the experimental group were young adults between the ages of 15.6 and 23.6 years. The average age was 18.61 and the standard deviation was 2.1 years. They were students in the musical high school or in the musical department of the university and had received a minimum of 4 years of musical education. The longest duration of musical educations was 13 years, and all individuals regularly play their musical instruments for a minimum of 3 hours per day. The control group consisted of nine subjects selected from young adults of the same age without any musical education. All of the participants in the control group were students in the Audiology Department of the University. The age of the control group was between 18.3 and 20 years. The average and standard deviations were 18.68 and 0.3, respectively. The characteristics of the groups are given in Table 1.

TABLE 1.

The characteristics of the control and experimental groups

Control group Experimental group
n 9 13
Gender 7 Female 7 Female
2 Male 6 Male
Age (years) 18.68±0.3 18.61±2.1
Musical experience (years) 0 7.73±3.74

The subjects participating in this study were selected according to a procedure. For this purpose, for all cases, standard Ear, Nose and Throat (ENT) examination, audiometric and impedansmetric test batteries were carried out. Subjects with any hearing problem were discarded.

The experiments were conducted by using The HEARLab System Cortical Evoked Potential Analyser (Frye Electronics, Inc; Tigard, Oregon, USA). The /m/, /t/ and /g/ speech sounds from running speech were used as sound stimulation. The presentation level of sound stimuli was at 65 dB Sound Pressure Level (SPL). The measurement parameters of the HEARlab System Cortical Evoked Potential Analyser used during this study are given in Table 2.

TABLE 2.

The measurement parameters of the HEARlab System Cortical Evoked Potential Analyzer

Acoustic stimulation ACA measurement parameters
Stimuli type Speech sounds from running speech
Duration /m/ 30 ms
/g/ 20 ms
/t/ 30 ms
Repetition period 1125 ms
Number of epochs 200–220
Polarity Alternate
Level 65 dB

ACA: aided cortical assessment

Measurement set up

Listeners were seated in a double-walled sound chamber with the one loudspeaker positioned 30 inches from the test ear at an azimuth of 90 degrees. The stimuli were presented through the front loudspeaker at the 65dB SPL level. Measurement set up is shown in Figure 1.

FIG. 1.

FIG. 1.

The CAEP measurement setup

Cortical auditory evoked potentials (CAEP) data were recorded from Ag-AgCl scalp electrode Fz (10–20 International System, mastoid earlobe reference, forehead ground). Placements of the electrodes are given in Figure 2.

FIG. 2.

FIG. 2.

Electrode placement diagram

If the subject was right-handed, A1 was used as a reference point; if the subject was left-handed, A2 was the reference.

In this study, P1/N1 /P2 amplitudes and the latency values were examined. Cortical responses were epoched and averaged in each condition with the HEARLab measurement system. Then, cortical response peaks (P1, N1 and P2) were chosen from each subject’s averaged waveform displayed at the screen of the system. Finally, amplitude and latency information were determined according to the chosen cortical response peak.

Statistical analysis

First, the normality of parameters was tested by Shapiro-Wilk analysis using SPSS 13.0 for Windows (IBM Corporation, New York, USA) and found to be normal. Then, Independent-Samples T Test was performed using SPSS 13.0 for Windows to test the statistical significance of the peak latency and the amplitude value differences between groups. The value of p≤0.05 was considered to be significant.

RESULTS

The bar graph in Figure 3 gives the mean amplitudes of P1, N1, and P2 components when /m/ was used as the speech sound stimulus. The value of mean amplitude of P1 when /m/ was used as the stimulus (P1m) was 1.0061μV for the control group and 2.1115μV for the experimental group. The value of mean amplitude of N1 when /m/ was used as the stimulus (N1m) was −3.5411μV for the control group and −3.0685μV for the experimental group. The value of the mean amplitude of P2 when /m/ was used as the stimulus (P2m) was 1.6533μV for the control group and 4.0538μV for the experimental group.

FIG. 3.

FIG. 3.

Mean amplitudes of P1m, N1m, and P2m components

The mean amplitudes of P1, N1, and P2 components are given in the bar graph in Figure 4 when /g/ was used as the speech sound stimulus. The mean amplitude of P1 when /g/ was used as the stimulus (P1g) was 1.0067μV for the control group and 3.3954μV for the experimental group. The value for N1 when /g/ was used as the stimulus (N1g) was −1.4944μV for the control group and −0.7477μV for the experimental group. The mean amplitude of P2 when /g/ was used as the stimulus (P2g) was 3.2422μV for the control group and 5.3700μV for the experimental group.

FIG. 4.

FIG. 4.

Mean amplitudes of P1g, N1g, and P2g components

The bar graph in Figure 5 shows the mean amplitudes of P1, N1, and P2 components when /t/ was used as the speech sound stimulus. The value of mean amplitude of P1 when /t/ was used as the stimulus (P1t) was 0.9567μV for the control group and 2.9700μV for the experimental group. The value of mean amplitude of N1 when /t/ was used as the stimulus (N1t) was −2.9278μV for the control group and −2.3662μV for the experimental group. The value of mean amplitude of P2 when /t/ was used as a stimulus (P2t) was 2.4833μV for the control group and 4.8454μV for the experimental group.

FIG. 5.

FIG. 5.

Mean amplitudes of P1t, N1t, and P2t components

The latency values were also measured and given in the following figures. The bar graph in Figure 6 gives the mean latencies of P1, N1, and P2 components when /m/ was used as the sound stimulus. The value of mean latencies of P1 was 62 milliseconds (ms) for the control group and 77.46 ms for the experimental group. The value of mean latencies of N1 was 100.445 ms for the control group and 123.38 ms for the experimental group. The value of mean latencies of P2 was 167.78 ms for the control group and 185.77 ms for the experimental group.

FIG. 6.

FIG. 6.

Mean latencies of P1m, N1m, and P2m components

When /g/ was used as the speech sound stimulus, the mean latencies of P1, N1, and P2 components were found, as shown in the bar graph in Figure 7. The value of mean latencies of P1 was 42.55 ms for the control group and 64.46 ms for the experimental group. The value of mean latencies of N1 was 78.22 ms for the control group and 104.15 ms for the experimental group. The value of mean latencies of P2 was 151.67 ms for the control group and 170.23 ms for the experimental group.

FIG. 7.

FIG. 7.

Mean latencies of P1g, N1g, and P2g components

The bar graph in Figure 8 gives the mean latencies of P1, N1, and P2 components when /t/ was used as the speech sound stimulus. The value of mean latencies of P1 was 46.33 ms for the control group and 64.077 ms for the experimental group. The value of mean latencies of N1 was 86.33 ms for the control group and 103 ms for the experimental group. The value of mean latencies of P2 was 158.89 ms for the control group and 165 ms for the experimental group.

FIG. 8.

FIG. 8.

Mean latencies of P1t, N1t, and P2t components

The T test revealed a significant main effect (p<0.05) between groups for amplitude values. Amplitude differences between groups are shown in Table 3. P1g, P1t, P2m, P2g, and P2t amplitude values were found to be significantly greater in musical young adults (p<0.05).

TABLE 3.

T test results of amplitudes of P1, N1, and P2 components

Group n Mean (μV) Std. Deviation t df p
P1m Control 9 1.0061 1.82165 1.3897 20 0.1799
Experimental 13 2.1115 1.84274 (p>0.05)
P1g Control 9 1.0067 1.84765 2.4943 20 0.0215
Experimental 13 3.3954 2.41938 (p<0.05)
P1t Control 9 0.9567 1.94247 2.1424 20 0.0446
Experimental 13 2.9700 2.30487 (p<0.05)
N1m Control 9 −3.5411 1.86358 0.6222 20 0.5408
Experimental 13 −3.0685 1.67317 (p>0.05)
N1g Control 9 −1.4944 2.48813 0.8296 20 0.4166
Experimental 13 −0.7477 1.74765 (p>0.05)
N1t Control 9 −2.9278 1.24963 0.7047 20 0.4891
Experimental 13 −2.3662 2.14208 (p>0.05)
P2m Control 9 1.6533 1.31937 3.3099 20 0.0035
Experimental 13 4.0538 1.87130 (p<0.05)
P2g Control 9 3.2422 2.46911 2.1756 20 0.0417
Experimental 13 5.3700 2.10093 (p<0.05)
P2t Control 9 2.4833 2.71420 2.2083 20 0.0391
Experimental 13 4.8454 2.28685 (p<0.05)

P1m: The mean amplitude of P1 when /m/ was used as stimulus; P1g: The mean amplitude of P1 when /g/ was used as stimulus; P1t: The mean amplitude of P1 when /t/ was used as stimulus; N1m: The mean amplitude of N1 when /m/ was used as stimulus; N1g: The mean amplitude of N1 when /g/ was used as stimulus; N1t: The mean amplitude of P1 when /t/ was used as stimulus; P2m: The mean amplitude of P2 when /m/ was used as stimulus; P2g: The mean amplitude of P2 when /g/ was used as stimulus; P2t: The mean amplitude of P2 when /t/ was used as stimulus

Latency differences between groups are shown in Table 4. None of the component latencies were found to be significantly different between groups (p>0.05).

TABLE 4.

T test results of the latencies of P1, N1, and P2 components

Group N Mean (ms) Std. Deviation t df p
P1m Control 9 62.0000 16.27882 −1.55120322 20 0.136533678
Experimental 13 77.4615 26.53179 p>0.05
P1g Control 9 42.5556 20.18112 1.961249395 20 0.063922822
Experimental 13 64.4615 28.88372 p>0.05
P1t Control 9 46.3333 20.71835 1.458833621 20 0.160138341
Experimental 13 64.0769 32.01682 p>0.05
N1m Control 9 100.4444 21.34895 −1.7929891 20 0.088113118
Experimental 13 123.3846 33.86871 p>0.05
N1g Control 9 78.2222 21.74729 1.761332729 20 0.093463744
Experimental 13 104.1538 40.07461 p>0.05
N1t Control 9 86.3333 20.97618 1.119844432 20 0.276048661
Experimental 13 103.0000 40.86563 p>0.05
P2m Control 9 167.7778 11.99768 1.573263245 20 0.131344595
Experimental 13 185.7692 32.60663 p>0.05
P2g Control 9 151.6667 10.74709 1.548528665 20 0.137174226
Experimental 13 170.2308 34.59565 p>0.05
P2t Control 9 158.8889 7.70462 −0.37458929 20 0.711908345
Experimental 13 165.0000 48.16119 p>0.05

P1m: The mean amplitude of P1 when /m/ was used as stimulus; P1g: The mean amplitude of P1 when /g/ was used as stimulus; P1t: The mean amplitude of P1 when /t/ was used as stimulus; N1m: The mean amplitude of N1 when /m/ was used as stimulus; N1g: The mean amplitude of N1 when /g/ was used as stimulus; N1t: The mean amplitude of P1 when /t/ was used as stimulus; P2m: The mean amplitude of P2 when /m/ was used as stimulus; P2g: The mean amplitude of P2 when /g/ was used as stimulus; P2t: The mean amplitude of P2 when /t/ was used as stimulus

DISCUSSION

In this study, the effects of professional musical education on cortical auditory evoked potentials were investigated. According to the results obtained in this study, statistically significant differences between the experimental and control groups were found to be within the amplitude value of P2 when /m/, /g/ and /t/ speech stimuli were used (p<0.05). These results are consistent with the studies in the literature. For example, Trainor et al. (5) used piano, pure ton and violin tones as stimuli signal. They compared the evoked response of adult and child musicians with non-musician counterparts. It was found that the P2-evoked response was larger in both adult and child musicians than in non-musicians and that auditory training enhances this component in non-musician adults. Although speech stimuli were used in our study, the results are similar to those in the literature.

In the study of Shahin et al. (12), highly skilled violinists and pianists and non-musician controls listened under conditions of passive attention to violin tones, piano tones, and pure tones matched in fundamental frequency to the musical tones. They found that, compared with non-musician controls, both musician groups evidenced larger P2 responses to the three types of tonal stimuli. In contrast, the amplitude of the N1 evoked by musical or pure tones did not differ between musicians and non-musicians. Kuriki et al. (2) examined the cortical auditory evoked responses in long-term trained musicians and compared them with those in the non-musician group. The single tone or accord tones of piano sounds were used as the stimuli. They found that the amplitude of the P2 response of auditory evoked potentials is modified by musical experience. In contrast, there was no significant alteration at the amplitude value of N1. The result of this study is also similar to our results. Here, in contrast to the amplitude value of P2, no significant difference in the amplitude of the N1 component was found between musicians and non-musicians. This observation of modification of the P2 amplitude is in agreement with the results of a previous CAEP study showing an enhancement of the P2 component in skilled musicians with long-term experience of the use of instruments (12). The amplitude of P2 was also enhanced after auditory training in non-musicians (1315) and after cochlear implantation in patients (16). Thus, it can be concluded that the cortical activity underlying P2 is amenable to modulation by long-term musical experience.

In the study of Musacchia et al. (3), the speech syllable “da” was presented to 14 musicians and 12 non-musicians, and cortical response peak values were compared. The amplitude value of P1 was found to be significantly higher in musicians. Similarly, in our study, statistically significant differences between the experimental and control groups were found in the amplitude value of P1 when /g/ and /t/ speech stimuli were used (p<0.05). For /m/ speech stimulation, there was a difference, but it was not found to be statistically significant (p>0.05).

According to the results of our study, latency values were found to be significantly different between groups (p>0.05). In the literature, there are studies reporting latency differences (9, 3, 17). Musacchia et al. (3) found P1 and N1 peaks earlier in the musician group. However, similar results were not found in this study. In the study of Nikjeh et al. (17), it was found that musicians had longer P1 latencies for pure tones and smaller P1 amplitudes for harmonic tones than non-musicians (17). There were no P1 group differences for speech stimuli. Similarly, in our study, there were no statically significant differences for speech stimuli in the latency values between musicians and non-musicians.

Findings in the literature and our study support a general influence of music training on central auditory function and illustrate experience-facilitated modulation of the auditory neural system. Interestingly, the effects of experience are not limited to the children. Even adult non-musicians given auditory training showed an enhancement of cortical responses, as seen in adult musicians and children with musical education. These results imply that musical training in adulthood and even in old age may provide a benefit (5).

The result obtained in our study indicate that musical experiences have effects on the nervous system; this can be seen in cortical auditory evoked potentials recorded when the subjects heard speech. Studies in the literature also show that musical experience enhances encoding mechanisms that are relevant for musical sounds as well as for the processing of linguistic cues and multisensory information. It can be concluded that:

  • Musical training enhances auditory perception.

  • Musicians are more sensitive than non-musicians to instrument sounds that are played.

  • Musicians are also more sensitive than non-musicians to speech stimuli.

  • Musical training may enhance phonetic awareness. This may also lead to enhanced language and reading performance in children.

Footnotes

This study was presented at the 11th Congress of The European Federation of Audiology Societies (EFAS), 19–22 June 2013, Budapest, Hungary.

Ethics Committee Approval: Ethics committee approval was received for this study from institutional ethics committee.

Informed Consent: Written informed consent was obtained from patients who participated in this study.

Peer-review: Externally peer-reviewed.

Author contributions: Concept - Z.P., A.A.; Design - Z.P.; Supervision - A.A.; Resource - Z.P., A.A.; Materials - Z.P.; Data Collection&/or Processing - Z.P.; Analysis&/or Interpretation - Z.P.; Literature Search - Z.P.; Writing - Z.P.; Critical Reviews - Z.P., A.A.

Conflict of Interest: No conflict of interest was declared by the authors.

Financial Disclosure: The authors declared that this study has received no financial support.

REFERENCES


Articles from Balkan Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of Trakya University Faculty of Medicine

RESOURCES