Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jan 31.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurovirol. 2014 Nov 12;21(1):24–31. doi: 10.1007/s13365-014-0290-2

No Association Between Apoε4 Alleles, HIV Infection, Age, Neuropsychological Outcome or Death

James T Becker 1,2,3, Jeremy J Martinson 4, Sudhir Penugonda 5, Lawrence Kingsley 4, Samantha Molsberry 4, Steven Wolinsky 5, Sandra Reynolds 6, Aaron Aronow 7, Karl Goodkin 8, Andrew Levine 9, Eileen Martin 10, Eric N Miller 11, Cynthia A Munro 12, Ann Ragin 13, Ned Sacktor 14
PMCID: PMC4319994  NIHMSID: NIHMS641972  PMID: 25388225

Abstract

The ε4 allele of the ApoE gene may have important interactions with physical health and cognitive function among individuals with HIV disease. The purpose of this study is to examine the relationships between ε4, HIV disease, age, neuropsychological impairment and death in a large, well-characterized study sample. 2,846 men participating in the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study had ApoE genotyping and neuropsychological test data available for analysis. We found a significant association between HIV infection and time to death (from any cause), as well as older age, race, and education. But, ApoE status was not significantly associated with time to death. Similarly, we found a significant association between HIV infection and time to incident cognitive impairment, as well as age, education, and HIV serostatus; Apoε4 status was not related to incident cognitive impairment. There were no significant interactions between ApoE, HIV infection, and age on cognitive impairment. These data replicate and strengthen prior findings of the lack of association between ApoE ε4 and cognitive outcomes in HIV disease. We conclude that within the specific constraints of an exclusively male study in which the majority of participants were less than 65 years of age (range: 22-87 years), it appears reasonable to conclude that the ε4 allele is not significantly interacting with HIV serostatus.

Keywords: HIV disease, APOE, Cognition, Age

Introduction

Apolipoprotein E (ApoE) is a class of apolipoproteins that mediate cholesterol metabolism (Mahley 1988). In the central nervous system, ApoE is produced primarily by astrocytes and transports cholesterol to neurons via ApoE receptors (Liu, Kanekiyo et al. 2013). The gene, APOE, is mapped to chromosome 19, and the ε4 allele of this gene is found in approximately 14 percent of the general population (Wardell, Suckling et al. 1982, Sing and Davignon 1985, Jarvik 1997). The frequency of the ε4 allele is elevated among individuals with atherosclerosis (Menzel, Kladetzky et al. 1983), Alzheimer's disease (AD) (Corder, Saunders et al. 1993, Strittmatter, Saunders et al. 1993), and with impaired cognitive function (Brayne, Harrington et al. 1996). APOE is also associated with mortality (Lane, Gao et al. 2003, Drenos and Kirkwood 2010, Beydoun, Beydoun et al. 2013), coronary artery disease and death (Tiret, de Knijff et al. 1994, Guang-da, You-ying et al. 2004).

Because of its links with cholesterol metabolism, heart disease and with increased risk for AD, it is reasonable to conjecture that ε4 might have important interactions with physical health and cognitive function in individuals with HIV disease. Indeed, there are some limited data linking ε4 to mortality in HIV disease (Valcour, Shiramizu et al. 2008). Reports from cross-sectional studies found no association between the presence of an ε4 allele and cognitive impairment in HIV disease (Andres, Feger et al. 2010, Joska, Combrinck et al. 2010, Sun, Abadjian et al. 2010, Morgan, Woods et al. 2013), although this is not a consistent finding (Spector, Singh et al. 2010, Chang, Andres et al. 2011, Hoare, Westgarth-Taylor et al. 2012, Soontornniyomkij, Moore et al. 2012). However, it should be noted that studies that did find an association between HIV disease and ε4 include chronological age in their models (Panos, Hinkin et al. 2013). The purpose of this study is to replicate and extend these previous findings in a larger sample, and specifically to examine incident cognitive impairment (i.e. from a state of normal cognition) as well as time to death (Rosvall, Rizzuto et al. 2009).

Methods

This research was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards at each of the clinical sites of the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS). Each participant signed a written statement of informed consent prior to starting any research-related activities.

Subjects

The MACS is an ongoing, prospective cohort study in the United States of the natural and treated history of HIV infection among men who have sex with men. A total of 6,972 men have been enrolled in the study at four study sites since its inception in April, 1984 (4,954 in 1984–1985, 668 in 1987–1991, and 1,350 in 2001–2003). Participants return every 6 months for an interview, physical examination, and collection of blood for laboratory testing. The interview covers physical health, medical treatments, and sexual and substance use behaviors (see: http://www.statepi.jhsph.edu/macs/macs.html). All men complete the Centers for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (Radloff 1977).

We refer to the men who enrolled in 1984-1985 or 1987-1991 as Cohort 1 (C1), and those who enrolled between 2001-2003 as Cohort 2 (C2). C1 was the original sample of 4,954 men, and C2 was a “New Recruit Cohort” that focused on enrolling minority and special target groups such as the partners of the men in the original cohort. C2 enrollment also focused on recruiting racial/ethnic minorities as well as a special target group of uninfected men who had been censored from C1 in 1995.

Genomic DNA extraction

Genomic DNA was isolated from lysates of either buffy-coat or B-cell immortalized cell lines from individuals within the MACS. Genomic DNA concentration and quality was assessed using UV spectrophotometry (NanoDrop, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) and/or by fluorometric quantitation (Quant-IT PicoGreen, Life Technologies, Carlsbad, USA).

Genotyping

End-point genotyping of APOE Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) rs429358 [C/T] and rs7412 [C/T] was performed using TaqMan and OpenArray technologies (Life Technologies, Carlsbad, USA). Genomic DNA was introduced to nanoliter reaction wells on a high-density microplate containing validated TaqMan primers and probes. PCR amplification was completed as recommended by the manufacturer. Water non-template and known positive controls were plated on each array for internal quality assurance and quality checks. Arrays were imaged after amplification on one of two OpenArray NT imagers. SNP genotypes were ascribed after clustering VIC and FAM signals (STATA 12.1, College Station, TX), and were then used to determine the ApoE alleles present(Zuo, van Dyck et al. 2006).

Neuropsychological (NP) Evaluation

The neuropsychological component of the MACS, and the recruitment cohort and survivorship difference have been described in detail (Becker, Kingsley et al. 2014). The NP evaluation includes measures from multiple cognitive domains related to the classification of HIV-Associated Neurocognitive Disorder (HAND) (Miller, Selnes et al. 1990, Miller, Satz et al. 1991, Selnes and Miller 1994, Woods, Rippeth et al. 2004, Antinori, Arendt et al. 2007). Because we did not assess activities of daily living at the earlier study visits, we were unable to assign HAND classifications, but were nevertheless able to provide evidence of existing cognitive deficits.

Classifications of cognitive impairment were operationalized using the criteria outlined by Woods et al (Woods, Rippeth et al. 2004) and Antinori et al. (Antinori, Arendt et al. 2007). Six cognitive domains were defined using measures from the MACS neuropsychological test battery: 1) Executive Function (Trail-Making Part B, Stroop Interference); 2) Speed of Information Processing (Symbol Digit Modalities Test, Stroop Color Naming); 3) Attention and Working Memory (two measures of one-back reaction time using the CalCAP Reaction Time program); 4) Learning (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test – Sum of Trials 1 through 5, Rey Complex Figure Immediate Recall); 5) Memory (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Delayed Recall, Rey Complex Figure Delayed Recall); and 6) Motor (most impaired hand on the Grooved Pegboard). We used data from the MACS seronegative men to create the statistical models to derived T-scores for each participant (i.e., both infected and uninfected men) for each test adjusting for age, years of education, ethnicity (Caucasian vs. other), and number of times the test had been administered. For each domain a summary T-score was calculated by averaging the two T-scores or, in the case of the Motor domain, using the lowest T-score on either the dominant or nondominant hand of the Grooved Pegboard. If the T-score for one of the tests was >= 40 and the other one was <40 then the domain was assigned the lowest obtained T-score plus five. If only one test in a domain was completed, the T-score for that test was used. For individuals who had T-scores for at least four of the six cognitive domains, classifications of cognitive functioning were made as follows (Antinori, Arendt et al. 2007): 1) Within Normal Limits if one or fewer domains had T-scores 1 SD or more below the mean (i.e., T ≤ 40); 2) Mild Impairment if two or more domains had T-scores ≤ 40, and the individual did not meet criteria for the more severe category 3 that follows; and 3) Severe Cognitive Impairment if two or more domains had T-scores ≤ 30, or one domain had a T-score ≤ 25.

It is important to note that the neuropsychological classification was fully automated. The results were not reviewed to determine the probable cause of the impairment. However, we created a binary variable (present/absent) to indicate the presence of a confounding condition at any time during follow-up that could, in and of itself, alter cognitive functions. These included serious medical diagnoses (e.g., heart failure), CNS infections, stroke, serious psychiatric disorders (e.g., affective, substance abuse, etc.), among others (See Supplemental Table for list of conditions). 31.9% of the uninfected men, and 34.1% of the infected had at least one confounding condition.

Statistical Analysis

We examined the unadjusted associations between ApoE genotype and other variables using t-tests and contingency tables as appropriate. Time to event in years (i.e., cognitive impairment or death) was tested using Cox proportional hazard models. And we identified and accounted for any differences in association as a function of recruitment cohort differences (Becker, Kingsley et al. 2014).

Results

Of the 4,275 men who had ApoE genotyping available for this analysis, 2,846 also had neuropsychological test data. The individuals who contributed data to this study were more likely to represent racial minorities, less likely to be HIV-infected, and more likely to be alive and active in the study as of December 31, 2012, relative to those who did not contribute data (See Supplemental E-Tables 1A and 1B). 2,040 men from C1 (59.8% of the cohort), and 806 men from C2 (93.4%) had ApoE genotyping data available. C1 contributed 71.7% of the ApoE data, and C2 contributed 28.3% for this analysis (See Table 1).

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics by Cohort, Race, and ApoE ε4 Status at First NP Visit

Cohort 1 2
Race Minority White Minority White
ε4 Present No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
N (%) 231 (70.2) 98 (29.8) 2377 (77.1) 705 (22.9) 342 (65.4) 181(34.6) 262 (76.8) 79 (23.2)
Age 4 32.09 (7.1) 32.14 (7.1) 33.79 (7.5) 33.55 (7.6) 38.29 (8.6) 39.02 (8.6) 36.78 (10) 38.47 (9.8)
Study
Status1
31.60 (73) 26.53 (26) 33.49 (796) 34.04 (240) 77.19 (264) 74.03 (134) 82.82 (217) 82.82 (65)
HIV
Infected 2
51.08 (118) 50.00 (49) 34.29 (815) 34.33 (242) 57.60 (197) 58.01 (105) 46.95 (123) 50.63 (40)
AIDS 2 23.38 (54) 22.45 (22) 28.19 (670) 31.21 (220) 6.43 (22) 7.18 (13) 3.44 (9) 2.53 (2)
AIDS
Death 5
81.33 (61) 80.56 (29) 80.42 (649) 84.40 (211) 29.63 (8) 55.56 (5) 28.57 (2) 0.00 (0)
cART 3
(% of HIV+)
0.00 (0) 0.00 (0) 0.00 (0) 0.00 (0) 65.48 (129??) 70.48 (74) 65.85 (81) 80.00 (32)
Education
(Years) 4
15.54 (2.4) 15.15 (2.5) 15.96 (2.3) 15.93 (2.4) 13.06 (2.3) 12.96 (2.2) 15.53 (2.9) 15.12 (2.7)
CES-D 10.93 (9.5) 9.96 (6.8) 9.57 (8.6) 9.49 (8.4) 14.83 (12) 15.42 (12) 12.78 (11) 11.81 (12)
Ever
Classified
Cognitively
Impaired2
30.83 (37) 38.78 (19) 46.71 (674) 48.83 (209) 59.18 (187) 67.65 (115) 43.44 (106) 43.42 (33)
1

Active as of 12/31/2012 (Percent (N))

2

Percent (N) Yes or Present

3

Combination antiretroviral therapy (cART)

4

Mean (standard deviation)

5

Death due to AIDS related condition.

There was a significant association between race and ApoE genotype; 23.2% of the men who identified themselves as White had at least one copy of the ε4 allele. By contrast, 31.2% of the men in other races (primarily African-American, but including Hispanics) had at least one ε4 allele [χ2 = 20.8, df=1, p<.001, Relative Risk = 1.54 (1.27 – 1.85)]. Therefore, all subsequent associations were evaluated in the White and non-White groups separately.

The allele frequencies for ApoE among the White participants were: ε2 .075, ε3 .80, and ε4 .13. Among the minority participants, the allele frequencies were: ε2 .11, ε3 .72, and ε4 .18. These rates are comparable with population estimates (i.e., both Caucasian and other races) of the allele distributions: ε2 .09, ε3 .77, and ε4 .15 (Sing and Davignon 1985).

There were also differences in the distribution of ε4 as a joint function of cohort and minority status. Among the White participants, the level of ε4 was .23 in C1 and .23 in C2 (χ2 = 0.002, p>0.05). By contrast, among the minority participants, the level of ε4 differed: from .25 in C1 to .36 in C2 (χ2 = 11.53, p<.001). In unadjusted models, we found no significant associations between the presence of an ε4 allele and age, as either a continuous variable (Table 1) or a dichotomous variable [≤40 vs. ≥ 50 years, Supplemental Tables E3A and 3B (Valcour, Shiramizu et al. 2008)]. We further found no unadjusted associations between ApoE status and HIV serostatus or death (See Table 1). With regard to the neuropsychological outcomes, there was a borderline association between ε4 and having incident impairment only in the minority participants in C2 (χ2 = 3.45, p>0.06, RR= 1.16 (1.00-1.35)); there were no other associations between ε4 and incident cognitive dysfunction.

We used Cox Proportional Hazard Models to investigate the association between time to death (measured from study entry) and ApoE ε4, HIV serostatus, age at entry, white race, and cohort of entry. We found a significant association between HIV infection and time to death (from any cause), as well as older age, and race. However, ApoE status was not significantly associated with time to death (See Table 3).

Table 3.

Cox Regression Models of Time to Death Hazard Ratios and 95% Confidence Interval

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Age at Entry .95(.94-.96) * .95(.94-.96) * .97(.96-.98) *
Race (Caucasian) 2.48(1.9-3.2) * 2.47(1.9-3.2) * 1.71(1.3-2.2) *
HIV Infected 5.00(4.2-6.0) * 5.00(4.2-6.0) * 5.44(4.6-6.5) *
ApoE ε4 (present) .99(.82-1.2) 1.01(.84-1.2)
Cohort (C2) .24(.17-.35) *

p<.05

Finally, we examined time to incident cognitive impairment (either mild or severe) among the 1,481 individuals classified as cognitively normal at their first neuropsychological evaluation and not having identified confounding conditions. There was no association between ε4 and time to develop cognitive impairment (See Table 4). In none of our analyses did we find significant interactions between ε4, HIV infection, age, and either death or cognitive impairment (See Supplemental Table E4). We repeated these analyses including all 2,204 participants who were cognitively normal at their first evaluation and included “potentially confounding conditions” as a covariate in the models. Again, ApoE was not related to time to cognitive impairment.

Table 4.

Cox Regression Models of Incident Cognitive Impairment Hazard Ratios and 95% Confidence Interval

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Age 1.04 (1.03-1.06)* 1.04 (1.03-1.06)* 1.04 (1.03-1.05)*
Education (Years) .98 (.94-1.02) .98 (.94-1.02) .99 (.95-1.03)
Race (Caucasian) .93 (.74-1.2) .93 (.74-1.2) 1.09 (.86-1.4)
HIV Infected 1.24 (1.0-1.5)* 1.24 (1.0-1.5)* 1.24 (1.0-1.5)*
ApoE ε4 (Present) 1.11 (.89-1.4) 1.11 (.89-1.4)
Cohort (C2) .98 (.76 – 1.26)
*

p < .05

We repeated the analyses using data only from the 1,134 White participants without potentially confounding conditions. There was no association between ApoE ε4 and time to impairment (Hazard = .99, 95% Confidence Interval = .78-1.3). When we further restricted the analysis to those men who were older than 49 years at the time of their last evaluation, we again found no effect of ApoE ε4 on time to impairment (HR=1.06, .66-1.7). Among the non-White participants only, we also found no association between ApoE ε4 and time to impairment (HR= 1.48, .97-2.3).

Finally, we repeated the analyses separately for the White and non-White participants coding ApoE ε4 as either absent, one copy or two copies. Among the White participants the presence of either one or two copies of the ε4 allele was not associated with cognitive impairment (one copy= .99(.77-1.3); two copies= 1.04 (.49-2.2). Among the non-Whites, there was a paradoxical effect of one copy [HR= 1.60 (1.0 – 2.5)] compared to carrying two copies of the ε4 allele [HR=.45 (.06-3.02)], which may be due to the difference in sample sizes (no copies: n = 244; 1 copy: 93; 2 copies: 10).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to replicate and extend previous findings related to the (lack of) association between ApoE ε4 and critical outcomes in the context of HIV disease. We found a significant increase in the rate of ε4 among non-White participants (primarily African-American), and -- once we controlled for race -- we found no significant associations between ApoE genotype and either HIV infection, age, death, or neuropsychological dysfunction. In particular, our analysis of incident neuropsychological impairment among those men who were classified as cognitively normal at their first NP study visit found no association between ApoE genotype and the development of cognitive dysfunction.

There are a variety of factors that can affect time to NP impairment, and these factors are themselves inter-correlated. However, within the constraints of this study, including differences in recruitment cohorts, age, race and potentially confounding medical conditions, we did not find an effect of ApoE genotype on time to cognitive impairment from a state of normal cognition. This does not preclude the possibility that ApoE may have an impact on specific cognitive functions or cognitive domains. However, our findings indicate that the transition from normal to impaired cognition is unaffected by ApoE ε4. These findings are consistent with the report from the Wiulfred Hall Study reporting no effect of ε4 on time to HIV-Associated Dementia (Burt, Agan et al. 2008).

While future, more fine-grained analyses might well find important roles of ApoE in various outcomes in HIV disease (e.g., cardiovascular), it is reassuring that -- at least to the extent that we were able to examine it here in terms of cognition -- ApoE appears to behave the same in HIV-infected individuals as it does in the uninfected men. It is important to note that the men in our study were in their mid-30s at the time of entry, and it is only recently (and in the context of combination antiretroviral therapy) that factors related to cholesterol metabolism and cardiovascular risk may become critical outcome variables (cf. (Kingsley, Cuervo-Rojas et al. 2008). However, it is important to reiterate that the follow-up of these participants averaged of 12.6 years (range: 0.8 – 26.4) between the first and last evaluations, and the average age was 48.1 years (range: 21.7 – 87.05) at the last evaluation.

Our failure to find an association between ApoE genotype, age, and cognitive impairment is at odds with some reports (Spector, Singh et al. 2010, Hoare, Westgarth-Taylor et al. 2012, Soontornniyomkij, Moore et al. 2012, Panos, Hinkin et al. 2013). However, the discrepancy may be, in part, due to the fact that these other reports have detailed cross-sectional analyses which may be affected by selective survival (i.e., the older infected men may be more likely to have had a longer history of uncontrolled HIV replication and thus entered any study with greater degree of CNS involvement). Our longitudinal analysis of time to develop cognitive impairment thus strengthens the conclusions by others (Andres, Feger et al. 2010, Joska, Combrinck et al. 2010, Sun, Abadjian et al. 2010, Morgan, Woods et al.) that within the age band tested (i.e., primarily younger than 65 years), ApoE ε4 genotype does not differentially impact HIV-infected individuals.

Further, some studies have reported finding interactions between HIV disease, age, and ApoE status [cf. (Valcour, Shiramizu et al. 2008, Panos, Hinkin et al. 2013)]; we found no such associations here. Again, this may be due, in part, to the fact that we adjusted our estimates for race as well as for cohort of entry, which adjusts for survivor bias. However, when we analyzed data only from men alive in December, 2012, (i.e., ignoring effects of selective survival), we still found no association with age (See Supplemental Table E3B).

One weakness of the MACS cohort relates to the racial distribution between the two recruitment cohorts (Becker, Kingsley et al. 2014); C1 was primarily White men having sex with men, and C2 had a significantly higher proportion of minority participants, many of whom were injection drug users. We also found that the rate of ε4 was higher among minority participants in C2 than among minority participants in C1; the reason for this is unclear (and may be a sampling artifact). These factors likely play an important role in explaining the small effects of ApoE on incident cognitive function in the minority subjects only, and primarily in C2. It will be important to replicate these analyses in large samples of African-Americans with and without HIV disease to determine whether any of the APOE alleles confer differential risk of impairment or death [cf., (Lee, Tang et al. 2001)].

Another weakness of our study is that we did not complete an a priori power analysis, in part because a pilot study (to estimate the effect size) wasn’t feasible (however, see (Hoenig and Heisey 2001, Kraemer, Mintz et al. 2006, Moore, Carter et al. 2011). However, if we were to assume that our measured Hazard Ratio of 1.11 is reasonable, and use a raw standard deviation of (log(1.4)-log(1.11))/1.96=0.11, with α = 0.05, we would have needed 60,000 men for 80% power to detect an association (http://www.stata.com/manuals13/ststpowercox.pdf). From this we conclude that, based on our data, any effect of ε4 is small.

In conclusion, our results have replicated and strengthened prior findings of the lack of association between ApoE ε4 and cognitive outcomes in HIV disease. This does not suggest that Apoε4 is unimportant among individuals over the age of 65 years or that it will not confer greater risk for non-HIV-associated neurodegenerative conditions. It does suggest that the phenomenon of accelerated aging we know applies to HIV infection does not seem to extend to the ApoE ε4 effect. Further, given that the MACS includes only men, there may be sex differences in ApoE ε4 effects that could not be examined here [cf. (Beydoun, Beydoun et al. 2013, Kulminski, Arbeev et al. 2014)]. However, within the specific constraints of the various outcomes examined in the literature, it appears reasonable to conclude that the ApoE ε4 allele is not significantly interacting with HIV serostatus to increase risk of incident cognitive impairment or death.

Supplementary Material

13365_2014_290_MOESM1_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM2_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM3_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM4_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM5_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM6_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM7_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM8_ESM

Table 2.

Unadjusted Associations Between APOE ε4 Genotype, Cognition and Death as Function of Race 1

Caucasian Minority
ε4− ε4+ Effect Size 2 ε4− ε4+ Effect Size 2
N= 1301 380 376 147
Age 37.7 (8.4) 38.3 (8.9) .06 42.0
(10.7)
40.5 (10.4) .13
Education 3 15.8 (2.4) 15.8 (2.5) .03 14.1 (2.7) 13.8 (2.5) .22
HIV Status 4 39.8 (518) 41.8 (159) 1.09 (.86-1.37) 43.9 (165) 51.7 (76) 1.37 (.93-2.00)
Alive 5 70.8 (921) 73.7 (280) 1.15 (.89-1.49) 88.8 (334) 87.1 (128) .85 (.47-1.52)
Cognitive
Impairment5
33.8 (440) 33.7 (128) .99 (.78-1.27) 31.4 (118) 35.4 (52) 1.20 (.80-1.79)
Confounding
Conditions 4
32.0 (416) 34.5 (131) 1.12 (.88-1.43) 35.1 (132) 29.9 (44) .79 (.52-1.19)
1

Among individual cognitively normal at first evaluation.

2

Relative Risk for categorical variables; Cohen’s d for continuous variables.

3

Mean (standard deviation)

4

Percent (N) infected.

5

Percent (N) Yes or Present.

Acknowledgements

Data in this manuscript were collected by the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS) with centers at Baltimore (U01-AI35042): The Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health: Joseph B. Margolick (PI), Barbara Crain, Adrian Dobs, Homayoon Farzadegan, Joel Gallant, Lisette Johnson-Hill, Cynthia Munro, Michael W. Plankey, Ned Sacktor, James Shepard, Chloe Thio; Chicago (U01-AI35039): Feinberg School of Medicine, Northwestern University, and Cook County Bureau of Health Services: Steven M. Wolinsky (PI), John P. Phair, Sheila Badri, Maurice O'Gorman, David Ostrow, Frank Palella, Ann Ragin; Los Angeles (U01-AI35040): University of California, UCLA Schools of Public Health and Medicine: Roger Detels (PI), Otoniel Martínez-Maza (Co-P I), Aaron Aronow, Robert Bolan, Elizabeth Breen, Anthony Butch, Beth Jamieson, Eric N. Miller, John Oishi, Harry Vinters, Dorothy Wiley, Mallory Witt, Otto Yang, Stephen Young, Zuo Feng Zhang; Pittsburgh (U01-AI35041): University of Pittsburgh, Graduate School of Public Health: Charles R. Rinaldo (PI), Lawrence A. Kingsley (Co-PI), James T. Becker, Ross D. Cranston, Jeremy J. Martinson, John W. Mellors, Anthony J. Silvestre, Ronald D. Stall; and the Data Coordinating Center (UM1-AI35043): The Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health: Lisa P. Jacobson (PI), Alvaro Munoz (Co-PI), Alison, Abraham, Keri Althoff, Christopher Cox, Jennifer Deal, Gypsyamber D’Souza, Priya Duggal, Janet Schollenberger, Eric C. Seaberg, Sol Su, Pamela Surkan. The MACS is funded primarily by the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (NIAID), with additional co-funding from the National Cancer Institute (NCI). The National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI), and the National Institute on Deafness and Communication Disorders (NIDCD) also provided targeted supplemental funding for specific projects. MACS data collection is also supported by UL1-TR000424 (JHU CTSA). Website located at http://www.statepi.jhsph.edu/macs/macs.html. The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Additional support for the analysis of these data and preparation of the manuscript was provided by funds from the NIH to J.T.B. (AG034852 and MH098745).

The members of the Neuropsychology Working Group include James T. Becker, Pim Brouwers, Christopher Cox, Jenna Fahey, Rebecca Godfrey, Karl Goodkin, Robin Huebner, Andrew J. Levine, Eileen M. Martin, Donna M. Martineck, Eric M. Miller, Ann Ragin, Sandra Reynolds, JoanaDarc Roe, Ned Sacktor, Janet Schollenberger, Eric Seaberg, Ola A. Selnes, and Matthew Wright.

Footnotes

Disclosure: Eric N. Miller is the author of the reaction time software used in this study (CalCAP) and has a financial interest in the software.

References

  1. Andres MA, Feger U, Nath A, Munsaka S, Jiang CS, Chang L. APOE epsilon 4 allele and CSF APOE on cognition in HIV-infected subjects. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol. 2010;6(3):389–398. doi: 10.1007/s11481-010-9254-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, Brew BJ, Byrd DA, Cherner M, Clifford DB, Cinque P, Epstein LG, Goodkin K, Gisslen M, Grant I, Heaton RK, Joseph J, Marder K, Marra CM, McArthur JC, Nunn M, Price RW, Pulliam L, Robertson KR, Sacktor N, Valcour V, Wojna VE. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. 2007;69(18):1789–1799. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Becker JT, Kingsley LA, Molsberry S, Reynolds S, Aronow A, Levine AJ, Martin E, Miller EN, Munro CA, Ragin A, Sacktor N, Selnes OA. Cohort Profile: Recruitment cohorts in the neuropsychological substudy of the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. International journal of epidemiology. 2014 doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Kaufman JS, An Y, Resnick SM, O'Brien R, Ferrucci L, Zonderman AB. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele interacts with sex and cognitive status to influence all-cause and cause-specific mortality in U.S. older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2013;61(4):525–534. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brayne C, Harrington CR, Wischik CM, Huppert FA, Chi LY, Xuereb JH, O'Connor DW, Paykel ES. Apolipoprotein E genotype in the prediction of cognitive decline and dementia in a prospectively studied elderly population. Dementia. 1996;7(3):169–174. doi: 10.1159/000106873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burt TD, Agan BK, Marconi VC, He W, Kulkarni H, Mold JE, Cavrois M, Huang Y, Mahley RW, Dolan MJ, McCune JM, Ahuja SK. Apolipoprotein (apo) E4 enhances HIV-1 cell entry in vitro, and the APOE epsilon4/epsilon4 genotype accelerates HIV disease progression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(25):8718–8723. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803526105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chang L, Andres M, Sadino J, Jiang CS, Nakama H, Miller E, Ernst T. Impact of apolipoprotein E epsilon4 and HIV on cognition and brain atrophy: antagonistic pleiotropy and premature brain aging. Neuroimage. 2011;58(4):1017–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, Roses AD, Haines JL, Pericak-Vance MA. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261(5123):921–923. doi: 10.1126/science.8346443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Drenos F, Kirkwood TB. Selection on alleles affecting human longevity and late-life disease: the example of apolipoprotein E. PLoS One. 2010;5(4):e10022. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Guang-da X, You-ying L, Zhi-song C, Yu-sheng H, Xiang-jiu Y. Apolipoprotein e4 allele is predictor of coronary artery disease death in elderly patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Atherosclerosis. 2004;175(1):77–81. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2004.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hoare J, Westgarth-Taylor J, Fouche JP, Combrinck M, Spottiswoode B, Stein DJ, Joska JA. Relationship between apolipoprotein E4 genotype and white matter integrity in HIV-positive young adults in South Africa. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2012 doi: 10.1007/s00406-012-0341-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hoenig JM, Heisey DM. The abuse of power: The pervasive fallacy of power calculations in data analysis. . The American Statistician. 2001;55:19–24. [Google Scholar]
  13. Jarvik GP. Genetic predictors of common disease: apolipoprotein E genotype as a paradigm. Ann Epidemiol. 1997;7(5):357–362. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(97)00028-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Joska JA, Combrinck M, Valcour VG, Hoare J, Leisegang F, Mahne AC, Myer L, Stein DJ. Association between apolipoprotein E4 genotype and human immunodeficiency virus-associated dementia in younger adults starting antiretroviral therapy in South Africa. J Neurovirol. 2010;16(5):377–383. doi: 10.3109/13550284.2010.513365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kingsley LA, Cuervo-Rojas J, Munoz A, Palella FJ, Post W, Witt MD, Budoff M, Kuller L. Subclinical coronary atherosclerosis, HIV infection and antiretroviral therapy: Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study. Aids. 2008;22(13):1589–1599. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e328306a6c5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kraemer HC, Mintz J, Noda A, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage JA. Caution regarding the use of pilot studies to guide power calculations for study proposals. Archives of general psychiatry. 2006;63(5):484–489. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kulminski AM, Arbeev KG, Culminskaya I, Arbeeva L, Ukraintseva SV, Stallard E, Christensen K, Schupf N, Province MA, Yashin AI. Age, gender, and cancer but not neurodegenerative and cardiovascular diseases strongly modulate systemic effect of the Apolipoprotein E4 allele on lifespan. PLoS genetics. 2014;10(1):e1004141. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lane KA, Gao S, Hui SL, Murrell JR, Hall KS, Hendrie HC. Apolipoprotein E and mortality in African-Americans and Yoruba. Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD. 2003;5(5):383–390. doi: 10.3233/jad-2003-5505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lee JH, Tang MX, Schupf N, Stern Y, Jacobs DM, Tycko B, Mayeux R. Mortality and apolipoprotein E in Hispanic, African-American, and Caucasian elders. American journal of medical genetics. 2001;103(2):121–127. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Liu CC, Kanekiyo T, Xu H, Bu G. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: risk, mechanisms and therapy. Nat Rev Neurol. 2013;9(2):106–118. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Mahley RW. Apolipoprotein E: cholesterol transport protein with expanding role in cell biology. Science. 1988;240(4852):622–630. doi: 10.1126/science.3283935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Menzel HJ, Kladetzky RG, Assmann G. Apolipoprotein E polymorphism and coronary artery disease. Arteriosclerosis. 1983;3(4):310–315. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.3.4.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Miller EN, Satz P, Visscher B. Computerized and conventional neuropsychological assessment of HIV1-infected homosexual men. Neurology. 1991;41(10):1608–1616. doi: 10.1212/wnl.41.10.1608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Miller EN, Selnes OA, McArthur MB. Neuropsychological test performance in HIV1-infected homosexual men: The Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS) Neurology. 1990;40:197–203. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Moore CG, Carter RE, Nietert PJ, Stewart PW. Recommendations for planning pilot studies in clinical and translational research. Clinical and translational science. 2011;4(5):332–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-8062.2011.00347.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Morgan EE, Woods SP, Letendre SL, Franklin DR, Bloss C, Goate A, Heaton RK, Collier AC, Marra CM, Gelman BB, McArthur JC, Morgello S, Simpson DM, McCutchan JA, Ellis RJ, Abramson I, Gamst A, Fennema-Notestine C, Smith DM, Grant I, Vaida F, Clifford DB. Apolipoprotein E4 genotype does not increase risk of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. J Neurovirol. 2013 doi: 10.1007/s13365-013-0152-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Panos SE, Hinkin CH, Singer EJ, Thames AD, Patel SM, Sinsheimer JS, Del Re AC, Gelman BB, Morgello S, Moore DJ, Levine AJ. Apolipoprotein-E genotype and human immunodeficiency virus-associated neurocognitive disorder: the modulating effects of older age and disease severity. Neurobehavioral HIV Medicine. 2013;5:11–22. doi: 10.2147/NBHIV.S39573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  29. Rosvall L, Rizzuto D, Wang HX, Winblad B, Graff C, Fratiglioni L. APOE-related mortality: effect of dementia, cardiovascular disease and gender. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30(10):1545–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Selnes OA, Miller EN. Development of a screening battery for HIV-related cognitive impairment: The MACS experience. In: Grant I, Martin A, editors. Neuropsychology of HIV Infection. Oxford University Press; New York: 1994. pp. 176–190. [Google Scholar]
  31. Sing CF, Davignon J. Role of the apolipoprotein E polymorphism in determining normal plasma lipid and lipoprotein variation. Am J Hum Genet. 1985;37(2):268–285. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Soontornniyomkij V, Moore DJ, Gouaux B, Soontornniyomkij B, Tatro ET, Umlauf A, Masliah E, Levine AJ, Singer EJ, Vinters HV, Gelman BB, Morgello S, Cherner M, Grant I, Achim CL. Cerebral beta-amyloid deposition predicts HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders in APOE epsilon4 carriers. AIDS. 2012;26(18):2327–2335. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32835a117c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Spector SA, Singh KK, Gupta S, Cystique LA, Jin H, Letendre S, Schrier R, Wu Z, Hong KX, Yu X, Shi C, Heaton RK. APOE epsilon4 and MBL-2 O/O genotypes are associated with neurocognitive impairment in HIV-infected plasma donors. AIDS. 2010;24(10):1471–1479. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e328339e25c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Strittmatter WJ, Saunders AM, Schmechel D, Pericak-Vance M, Enghild J, Salvesen GS, Roses AD. Apolipoprotein E: high-avidity binding to beta-amyloid and increased frequency of type 4 allele in late-onset familial Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90(5):1977–1981. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.5.1977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sun B, Abadjian L, Rempel H, Calosing C, Rothlind J, Pulliam L. Peripheral biomarkers do not correlate with cognitive impairment in highly active antiretroviral therapy-treated subjects with human immunodeficiency virus type 1 infection. J Neurovirol. 2010;16(2):115–124. doi: 10.3109/13550280903559789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Tiret L, de Knijff P, Menzel HJ, Ehnholm C, Nicaud V, Havekes LM. ApoE polymorphism and predisposition to coronary heart disease in youths of different European populations. The EARS Study. European Atherosclerosis Research Study. Arteriosclerosis and thrombosis : a journal of vascular biology / American Heart Association. 1994;14(10):1617–1624. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.14.10.1617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Valcour V, Shiramizu B, Shikuma C. Frequency of apolipoprotein E4 among older compared with younger HIV patients: support for detrimental effect of E4 on survival. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(41):E66. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806919105. author reply E67-68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wardell MR, Suckling PA, Janus ED. Genetic variation in human apolipoprotein E. J Lipid Res. 1982;23(8):1174–1182. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Woods SP, Rippeth JD, Frol AB, Levy JK, Ryan E, Soukup VM, Hinkin CH, Lazzaretto D, Cherner M, Marcotte TD, Gelman BB, Morgello S, Singer EJ, Grant I, Heaton RK. Interrater reliability of clinical ratings and neurocognitive diagnoses in HIV. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004;26(6):759–778. doi: 10.1080/13803390490509565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Zuo L, van Dyck CH, Luo X, Kranzler HR, Yang BZ, Gelernter J. Variation at APOE and STH loci and Alzheimer's disease. Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:13. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

13365_2014_290_MOESM1_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM2_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM3_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM4_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM5_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM6_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM7_ESM
13365_2014_290_MOESM8_ESM

RESOURCES