Skip to main content
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2015 Mar 19;370(1664):20140096. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0096

Cross-cultural perspectives on music and musicality

Sandra E Trehub 1,, Judith Becker 2, Iain Morley 3
PMCID: PMC4321137  PMID: 25646519

Abstract

Musical behaviours are universal across human populations and, at the same time, highly diverse in their structures, roles and cultural interpretations. Although laboratory studies of isolated listeners and music-makers have yielded important insights into sensorimotor and cognitive skills and their neural underpinnings, they have revealed little about the broader significance of music for individuals, peer groups and communities. This review presents a sampling of musical forms and coordinated musical activity across cultures, with the aim of highlighting key similarities and differences. The focus is on scholarly and everyday ideas about music—what it is and where it originates—as well the antiquity of music and the contribution of musical behaviour to ritual activity, social organization, caregiving and group cohesion. Synchronous arousal, action synchrony and imitative behaviours are among the means by which music facilitates social bonding. The commonalities and differences in musical forms and functions across cultures suggest new directions for ethnomusicology, music cognition and neuroscience, and a pivot away from the predominant scientific focus on instrumental music in the Western European tradition.

Keywords: music, cross-cultural, ritual, antiquity, social cohesion

1. Universals and contrasts in musical systems

Music is universal, transmitted through generations, usually performed in the presence of others, and of extreme antiquity.

While there is no inter-culturally valid definition of music, and the cover term music is found in only selected cultures, a number of presumptive universals indicate that musicality is a prominent and distinctive characteristic of humankind. All peoples engage in activities that we would call music, often in relation to play, and everywhere in relation to ritual. All peoples of the world sing, an activity recognized on the basis of context or by cultural consensus as different from speech. All peoples have some form of instrumental music as well, however rudimentary.

Music-making is necessarily a cultural performance because conventions about the structure of music, its instrumentation, context of performance and meaning are all learned. Music-making is a system of communication transmitted through ongoing transgenerational interaction.

Music-making is or implies a social performance, even when performed or listened to alone. The solitary performer often has an audience in mind. For example, when music students at North American universities practise alone in hermetic practice rooms, they are usually imagining an audience (J. Becker 2014, personal communication) [1]. Solitary music-making for personal pleasure is likely to evoke memories of learning the piece (when, where and from whom) and previous experiences of playing and hearing it. Musical pieces, like performers, are saturated with contextual, social memory.

In the Western industrialized world, music listening is commonly solitary, and iPods, earbuds and headphones are ubiquitous. The listener seems alone, but the music has an implied social context such as a symphony orchestra, a rapper before a live audience or a DJ at a club. Even a solitary basement musician producing a multi-track composite performance is likely to invoke or imitate a social context.

An exception to the truism that music-making is a social performance is the deliberate use of music to shut out the world and create an alternate reality. At times, the iPod-solitary-listening reality can produce something close to a deeply spiritual or mystical experience.

I've walked that way for—I don't know how many years … and it's very boring, so having the music makes me see things that I would see everyday in a kind of new way—like a leaf falling or something. It might be like, ‘Wow, a leaf has fallen!’

[1, p. 58]

Except for solitary, self-directed performances, music performances are political in the sense that they are situated and embedded within structures of power and influence. Where they are performed, by whom, and for whom reveal a great deal about the cultural and social status of the performer and the performance.

Who has creative and economic control?

Which people and assumptions dictate the terms, not only of the creative work produced by artists but also of the critical and scholarly writing about it?

What are the ways that the race, gender, class, sexual orientation, and generation of an artist shape his or her professional experiences and aesthetic choices?

[2, p. 328]

Because musical performances are socially and culturally situated, they come to be ethically saturated as well. Our deepest values may be implied by participation in a particular genre of music such as a hootenanny (populist), heavy metal concert (male machismo), rave (youth culture plus drugs) or European classical concert (upper-middle-class values and lifestyle). There are exceptions, of course, but the aforementioned stereotypes are more likely than not to apply to any given listener at a musical event. As Wittgenstein claimed, ‘Ethics and aesthetics are one and the same’ [3, p. 421].

Music everywhere is believed to affect our emotions, to involve some kind of arousal, ranging all the way from mild pleasure or displeasure to profoundly transformed states of consciousness such as trancing [4,5]. Nevertheless, the scientific study of music and emotion has focused primarily on solitary listening, which is but one of many modes of listening across cultures [6]. Cross-cultural research in this domain is limited and largely restricted to listeners' categorization of intended and felt emotions by means of a small set of emotion words (e.g. happy, sad and scared) or facial expressions [79]. Even within a culture, listeners' perception of emotion differs markedly from their felt emotion, and the latter varies considerably with differences in listening context, experimenter- versus self-selected music and number of response alternatives [1012].

(a). What is called music?

Many languages, including most North American Indian languages [13] and several languages in Africa such as that of the Basongye of Zaire [14] or the Tiv of Nigeria [15], have names for individual genres of music but do not have a cover term that includes all of their musical genres. Among the most important commonalities among musical systems of the world is the conjoining of music and ritual, often including dance and speech. The term for music frequently includes other activities as well. For example, Sanskrit sangīta, Thai wai khruu [16] and nkwa of the Igbo of Nigeria [7] encompass music and dance as facets of the same activity, making no clear terminological distinction between them.

Honest observers are hard pressed to find a single indigenous group in Africa that has a term congruent to the usual Western notion of ‘music’. There are terms for more specific acts like singing, playing instruments, and more broadly performing (dance, games, music); but the isolation of musical sound from other arts proves a Western abstraction, of which we should be aware when we approach the study of performance in Africa.

[17, p. 7]

The synonymity of music and dance may be recognized by the participants themselves, as in the case of the ritual dance of the Maring of Papua New Guinea at the Kaiku festival, in which the dance and ritual pledge are interdependent in transforming the natural order [18], and the Candomblé Afro-Brazilian religion, in which music is so ubiquitous that the same term encompasses what we recognize as music and religion [16].

By contrast, the chanting of religious texts in Judaism (ta'ameh ha-mikrah) and Islam (qirā'ah) would be classified as musical by external observers but as recitation by the practitioners [16]. As with the preceding examples, however, the musical and ritual elements are mutually dependent facets of the performative activity.

(b). Statistical universals

Strictly speaking, there are no structural characteristics that have been identified in all known musical systems. Nonetheless, some characteristics have a wide distribution globally and are considered statistical universals [19].

Perhaps foremost among statistical universals is the idea of musicality itself, that everyone has the capacity or potential for engaging in a range of musical activities. Moreoever, one does not just sing, but one sings something and that something can be identified in one way or another [20]. What is performed has an identity, a name or moniker that sets it apart from other musical acts and from any other kind of utterance. For example, what is sung may be considered a unit in a ritual, the creation of someone, performed by someone or performed at some particular place. Another statistical universal is the presence of musical units or phrases, identifiable by repetition or some form of redundancy. Repetition of units or phrases, whether identical or with variation, is widely represented in musical systems across the globe [21,22]. Musical units or phrases can be transposed and still retain their identity [19]. The musical phrase, or combination of phrases, is frequently made up of unequal intervals, often major seconds and thirds, and commonly combining to produce pentatonic scales [19,23,24]. While the idea of a scale is by no means universal, the analytical construction of ‘scale’ indicates that most musical systems in the world do not exceed seven notes within the octave.

The sense of octave equivalence is found wherever men, women and children sing together in unison. In Bali, Indonesia, the sense of octave equivalence is underlined by being deliberately undermined. Equivalent pairs of keys of Balinese bronze xylophones are carefully tuned so that the octave across each pair is ‘out-of-tune’ or ‘stretched’ to produce the desired ‘beats’ when both keys are struck simultaneously [25]. The deliberate use of beats in Bali and other musical cultures [26] is inconsistent with the claim that beats, which are typically avoided in Western music, are innately unpleasant [27,28].

Most musical systems are predominantly isorhythmic, which means that the same rhythmic configuration, once established, tends to continue throughout. A given beat within an isorhythmic configuration is commonly subdivided into two or three units [19].

Certain ideas concerning music are also frequent across the globe. One of these is the belief that one's own system of music is natural. To consider one's music system as natural is to endow it with a kind of necessity or power that it might not be able to claim otherwise. But naturalness can be located in many different realms. One way in which naturalness is identified in Western music, as well as in India, the Middle East, and parts of sub-Saharan Africa, is on the basis of acoustics, the building of scales on the overtone structure of any single tone [29,30].

A different kind of link with nature, and thus with spiritual power, can be found in ideas about music of the Kaluli, an ethnic group of the highlands of Papua New Guinea. The Kaluli believe that human composers are reworking bird songs. Their most important ceremonial song genre consists of four descending tones in imitation of the call of the muni bird that can be heard as the voices of their deceased ancestors [31].

Another system of ideas concerning the naturalness of musical systems can be found in the lore surrounding Central Javanese court gamelan ensembles. Indic conceptions of power were imported into Java in the first millennium CE and were drawn upon by the sultans of Central Java to enhance their status. According to Indic cosmology, female energy, sakti, permeates the universe, is found everywhere for all time and is morally neutral. While we should consider this system of ideas a construct, for its practitioners it was a description of the natural world and the universe. To be an effective ruler in Java, the male sultan, through meditation, added to his effectiveness by attracting and assimilating cosmic sakti. In modern Indonesian, with its Indic past receding, sakti is simply translated as power. Any gamelan ensemble, but especially old palace gamelans, is believed to possess or embody sakti, in part because they are made of bronze, an alloy of copper and tin, which comes from the Earth and must be transformed by fire. In Java, blacksmiths and gong-makers are believed to embody sakti, a necessary power to transform substances of the Earth, through fire, into wondrous objects such as the dagger, kris, or a gamelan instrument. A large palace ensemble will always include two large gongs, one female and one male. The female gong is considered to be older and more primal than the male gong and will always be used to end a piece. The division of a pair of male and female instruments extends to other paired instruments, the bronze xylophones of the ensemble as well as the gongs. Thus, the naturalness and power of a gamelan ensemble is attributed, in part, to its metaphoric iconicity with cosmic power [32].

(c). Scholarly and everyday ideas about the origins of music

Music is often ascribed to spiritual or supernatural aspects of the natural world. In the ancient and mediaeval worlds, East and West, music or sound was conceived as a primal source of the phenomenal world. Pythagoras and Plato formulated the Occidental theory of cosmic music, the relations between music intervals and numbers, the character of scales, the ‘harmony of the spheres’ and their influence on nature and society [33]. Scholars in ancient China created a complex system of relations between social phenomena, elements of the calendar and relations between tones. ‘The origin of music is in the very remote past. It was … rooted in the Grand Unity. The Grand Unity gave birth to … heaven and earth’ (ca 239 BCE) [34, p. 56].

In ancient India, sound itself was considered sacred in origin [35], and the Hindu aspiring for liberation or for association with a chosen deity performed a ‘sonic act’ informed by a ‘sonic theology’ [36].

Ancient theories of the relation of music to the cosmos have continuing influence, but commonplace notions about music and the supernatural also abound, such as Native American and African beliefs that dreams or visions are the sources of particular songs [3740]. Related to the link of musical performance with special powers or supernatural forces is the widespread belief that music has profound effects on our minds and bodies and can ameliorate physical and mental problems [36,4042].

(d) The antiquity of musical activities

A deep history to musical activities is suggested by their existence in all known human societies. They are key components of ritual activities, cosmologies and the management of social relationships—all core elements of maintaining human groups. This suggests shared mechanisms by which recognizable musical activities emerge inevitably in human societies despite divergent histories or that musical activities themselves are part of a shared history of human groups that pre-dates their divergence from one another.

The earliest direct evidence of musical activities is necessarily limited to the earliest evidence of musical instrument use. In many musical traditions, however, musical behaviours are not synonymous with instrumentation, and much instrumentation would not preserve archaeologically. For example, among the Blackfoot and Sioux Native Americans [38,43], the Aka and Mbuti African Pygmies [44,45], the Yupik of southwest Alaska [46] and the Pintupi-speaking Australian Aborigines [47], the melodic content of music is largely provided by the voice, with instrumentation primarily being percussive. When instruments are used, they are often made from natural, biodegradable materials that would not preserve archaeologically under most conditions and are supplemental to the use of the body for producing melodic and rhythmic content.

This means that archaeological evidence for musical activities, in the form of preserved instruments, is unlikely to represent the earliest musical activities or their extent. Nevertheless, it is clear that the earliest populations of modern humans (Homo sapiens) to enter Europe, more than 40 000 years ago, were engaged in musical activities. The oldest known pipes or flutes come from the sites of Geissenklösterle, Hohle Fels and Vogelherd in the Swabian Jura of Germany [4851]. These are made from swan bone, vulture bone and mammoth ivory. All come from layers associated with Aurignacian technologies, among the earliest tool types produced by modern humans in Europe, and in the case of the Geissenklösterle examples, the layers have recently been re-dated to 43 150–39 370 cal bp (calibrated, or calendar, years before present) [52]. The bird bone examples are made from modified radius and ulna bones (lower arm/wing bones). Bird bones are light and naturally hollow, so are relatively easily worked. The lower wing bones of large birds, such as vultures, eagles, swans and some geese, are of sufficient size to function as multi-pitch pipes. These have had the ends (epiphyses) removed and finger holes prepared by thinning the bone surface and piercing or boring a hole with a sharp tool [4,53]. For the mammoth ivory examples, a far more laborious process was used to produce a somewhat larger equivalent to the bird bones. The ivory was soaked to separate its laminar layers, cut in half along its length, the central core removed and the two halves re-sealed with airtight resin glue, having had finger holes bored along the length [48,49].

Further examples associated with Aurignacian technologies are known from Spy, Belgium (what may be ‘panpipes’) [54,55], Abri Blanchard [56,57] and Isturitz, France [53,5860], which suggest widespread musical activities at that time. Given the sophistication of the production techniques and the association with some of the earliest Homo sapiens populations in Europe, it seems likely that these represent part of a behavioural repertoire that pre-dates their arrival in Europe. These instruments constitute the first of an extensive record of pipes and other instruments from throughout the Upper Palaeolithic period (from around 45 000 until around 12 000 years ago, at the end of the last ice age). In several cases (e.g. Isturitz, Mas d'Azil), these instruments were found at sites that were focal points for large gatherings at particular times of the year [55,61].

As noted, melodic instruments were a small part of many sound-producing traditions, with melody being principally provided by the voice, and instruments being primarily percussive. Were this the case among Palaeolithic human populations as well, then the record of bone pipes would represent a small proportion of the instruments produced and used, and that, in turn, would represent a small proportion of the musical activities carried out.

While pipes are perhaps the most readily recognized and best preserved instruments from Upper Palaeolithic contexts, other possible sound-producers are known from this period. These include rasps (‘scraped idiophones’) [6266], bullroarers (‘free aerophones’) [61,62], struck bones (osseophones) [67,68], rocks, and stalactites and stalagmites (lithophones) [6973]. All are well-known forms of sound production in ethnographic contexts. For example, bullroarers have been used in Australia, Africa, North America, New Guinea, and among the Maori of New Zealand and the Sami of Scandinavia. Their use often has sacred and religious associations, and their powerful religious roles in ancient Greece are also well documented [7479]. Similarly, the use of ‘rock gongs’ or ‘singing stones’ has been documented in historical and contemporary contexts in the Canary Islands [80], Sweden [81], India [82,83], Bolivia, southeast Asia, Australia and Africa [78,84]. ‘Rock gongs’ produce a single tone or multiple tones depending upon where they are struck. As would be expected from their extensive geographical and temporal range, their uses are varied, including signalling (the sounds can be heard over distances of several miles) and accompanying ritual, singing and dancing [78,82,83].

(e). Music and social organization

Conjoining music, dance and ritual language within an event that addresses the existential concerns of the community is the most universally valued of musical activities. Owing to the entrainment of human bodies in a group, a musical event that is a ceremony or a ritual, even a small communal or family gathering, necessarily becomes an enhancer of community spirit. Making music together is simultaneously building a community together, which is considered by many to be the most adaptive and evolutionarily significant aspect of musical experience worldwide [8588].

Among the Pintupi-speaking Aborigines of Australia [89], musical activities are an important part of interactions with neighbouring groups during the dry seasons [90,91]. The Yupik of southwest Alaska achieve ‘socially lubricating’ functions through music whose lyrics and dance actions diffuse tension through teasing or relating comic events [46]. Both of these peoples, hunter–gatherers in very different environments and separated by many thousands of miles, believe that their musical activities directly influence the world around them, that they have come from the land and that they are akin to the other fauna in their environment [43,44,46,89].

Although there is some variation in the permissible roles of individuals and genders in musical activities, the activities are often inclusive, with little distinction between performers and audience. All who are present participate in the activity in some capacity. For the Aka and Mbuti equatorial African Pygmies, music is a communal, cooperative activity, with no one considered a specialist musician [44]. Songs related to hunting are typically performed by men [91], and performances related to rites of passage are typically performed by women [44]. For all other music and dance performances, persons of any age or gender may participate [92]. Similarly, for the Blackfoot and Sioux native Americans, gender roles and specific responsibilities were delineated in music used for ritual purposes, but a wide cross-section of the community participated in the second-most common use, social dancing [43].

Songs also function as a repository of knowledge and cultural values that can be transmitted across persons and generations, as with the Alaskan Yupik, the Australian Pintupi-speaking Aborigines and many other groups [46,89]. In rural Vietnam, for example, a number of traditional songs provide detailed guidance for planting and harvesting crops [93]. The ABC song and counting songs in many cultures continue to play an important didactic role in childhood.

2. Music and group cohesion

Although it is highly likely that the social aspects of musical engagement account for the perpetuation of music across cultures and millennia [86,88], these aspects have received considerably less attention in psychology and neuroscience than the perceptual, cognitive and emotional processes of solitary listeners and the neural correlates of these processes [9497]. Progress in documenting the everyday music listening habits of Western adults [1,41,98,99] has not been matched by progress in delineating the consequences of various listening contexts for arousal, emotion regulation and well-being. Even when emotional responsiveness is of principal interest, music that evokes episodic memories or extra-musical associations is commonly excluded [11] despite the impact of such associations on listeners' emotional experience.

(a). Musical caregiving

Music is ubiquitous in caregiving. Carers across cultures sing to infants and have done so from time immemorial [100]. Their lullabies to soothe infants and induce sleep are typically simple, melodically, rhythmically and lyrically [101104], and they are readily identifiable across cultures [105]. Lullabies are commonly performed with rhythmic movement (e.g. rocking) and touch [106], so the child's introduction to music is multimodal even in cultures where solitary listening is the norm.

Lullabies are the songs of choice in caregiving contexts with almost constant physical contact between carer and infant, which often involves limited face-to-face contact [107]. High levels of contact maximize infants’ comfort and safety in the challenging conditions of the developing world [108,109]. An exception to this practice involves the BaYaka Pygmies of Central Africa where a mother typically responds to a howling baby by yodelling even louder while rhythmically patting the baby's back [110]. Lullaby lyrics in most cultures express praise and affection for infants but, at times, stressed carers use the privacy of the dyadic context and non-comprehending listener to give lyrical expression to their personal difficulties [100,111], with potentially soothing consequences for singers and listeners.

Middle-class European and American mothers, having the luxury of focusing on intellectual and social stimulation, place infants in secure devices (e.g. seats and swings) and engage in face-to-face play [112]. Their singing and melodious speech enables them to ‘keep in touch’ with infants [113]. The songs of choice in such contexts are lively play songs, with lullabies commonly reserved for bedtime routines [100]. Regardless of carers' choice of songs, their manner of performance, featuring higher than usual pitch level, slower than usual tempo and warm vocal tone, is discernible to naive listeners within and across cultures [114117]. The maternal performing style is also individually distinctive. Mothers sing a few songs repeatedly and in a highly stereotyped manner—typically with identical pitch level and tempo [118]. The result is a distinctive dyadic ritual that eases the burdens of caregiving, enhances infants’ sense of security and cements carer–infant bonds.

Aspects of the maternal singing style are similar to the intimate, group-involving style of singing that Lomax [119] identified in highly integrated communities around the world. That style has been linked to the blurring of boundaries between the self and others [120,121]. Mothers' face-to-face singing modulates the arousal levels of contented infants [122] and is more effective than maternal speech at reducing the high arousal of distressed infants [123]. Even with auditory-only materials from unfamiliar mothers, songs recorded in an infant's presence engage infants more readily than comparable recordings that lack an infant audience [114,124].

(b). Music in communal contexts

Music listening evokes pleasure even in the solitary, unnatural context of the laboratory or scanner [97,125,126]. The pleasure and social consequences are amplified, however, when music is experienced with family or peers [127] and even more so when music features active involvement as in singing, drumming or dancing [128]. Communal musical experiences and collective rituals in general enhance social cohesion and prosocial behaviour [86,129,130], presumably through jointly experienced elation and synchronized action [87,131,132].

(c). Synchronous arousal

Heightened arousal strengthens social bonds among those who engage in collaborative rituals regardless of the presence or absence of music, positive affect or overt physical coordination. For example, extreme rituals that include beating, mutilation or excessive exertion intensify social bonds among participants and empathic observers, as reflected in the extent of their identification, cooperation and coordinated physiological activity [133135]. In the annual fire-walking ritual of a small rural village in Spain, fire-walkers walk barefoot across glowing red coals as friends, relatives and thousands of visiting spectators look on. The arousal of the fire-walkers' friends and relatives becomes synchronized with that of the fire-walkers, as reflected in the microstructure of their cardiac activity [134]. The arousal of unrelated spectators, although elevated, does not exhibit the signature cardiac features of the fire-walkers and related observers. Incidentally, music may well prime participants (fire-walkers and audience) for the main event because it is preceded by the fire-walkers dancing around the glowing coals, and each fire-walker is summoned to the challenge by a trumpet call.

Communal musical experiences are typically of moderate emotional intensity, but examples of intense performances with positive valence are common among the BaYaka Pygmies of the Central African Republic [110]. They engage in extended bouts of vigorous singing, dancing and story-telling, in contrast to the reported low frequency of high-intensity rituals and high frequency of low-intensity rituals [130,133]. For enjoyable musical rituals rather than punitive non-musical rituals, intensity may be compatible with high frequency. For the BaYaka Pygmies, musical activities play a critical role in the maintenance and intergenerational transmission of their egalitarian and cooperative values and practices [110].

High-intensity rituals, including loud, long-sustained singing and trancing, are also found in charismatic Christian congregations, especially Pentecostals. Originally from the US, Pentecostal congregations, with hours-long musical services, are now found worldwide [136].

(d). Synchronous action

Although synchronous action is not a prerequisite for the formation or maintenance of social bonds, synchronous activity in groups of Buddhist chanters and Hindu devotional singers results in greater identification, trust and generosity among participants than does the non-synchronized activity of cross-country running groups [137]. The lesser prosocial consequences of cross-country running are inconsistent with the contention that social bonding arises from the energetic aspects of music-making [128].

Prosocial consequences are evident even when synchronous activity occurs without music. For example, three initially unfamiliar individuals who walk in step evaluate one another more favourably than similar threesomes who walk together but not in synchrony [138]. According to historian William McNeill [139], the military practice of marching in step no longer has the significance it once had for battle formation, but it is maintained because of the sense of unity it fosters among marchers from diverse political, religious and socio-economic backgrounds.

The prosocial consequences of synchronous activity are evident in early childhood. After 4-year-old children engage in cooperative play that includes singing and playing percussion instruments, they exhibit more prosocial behaviour than 4-year-olds who engage in similar cooperative play without music [140]. Infants move rhythmically but not synchronously to rhythmic music [141]. By seven months of age, however, they are sensitive to specific patterns of movement experienced when moved by others but not to movement patterns that they simply observe [142]. By 14 months of age, they are sensitive to movement patterns observed as well as experienced. After observing an adult bouncing synchronously with their own bouncing, they offer more assistance to that adult than to one who engaged in non-synchronous bouncing [143].

(e). Imitation

Synchronous action is one route to social bonding and prosocial behaviour in childhood and adulthood. Imitation is another. An important pillar of our musicality and of our ritual propensity in general is our ability to imitate or provide a high-fidelity copy of the vocal and bodily actions of others [144,145]. Ordinary interpersonal interaction often involves unconscious mimicry or low-fidelity copying that is prompted by prosocial attitudes of the mimic and has prosocial consequences for the person mimicked [146]. Such mimicry is presumed to arise from our motivation to conform or be like others [145,147,148]. Similar affiliative motivations seem to underlie young children's production of exact imitations of the actions of others [149] and their enhanced responsiveness to individuals who imitate their actions [150]. For example, 18-month-old infants initiate more play with adults who imitate their actions than with those who interact without action imitation [151]. The motivation to act like others reaches a high plane in musical and ritual activity where the form is primary and the immediate function is secondary or inconsequential [145].

(f). Music, meaning and communication

For members of many small communities, past and present, musical rituals have meanings that are transparent within the native community but opaque to others. There may be different levels of meaning as well, holistic meanings about particular rituals, like a call for rain or for animals to present themselves for the hunt, and specific meanings linked to individual elements, like the gender of a gong. Music theorists and scientists consider these meanings extra-musical, according them lesser significance than intra-musical meanings involving the internal structure of music. But these so-called extra-musical meanings are highly conventionalized, unlike the idiosyncratic extra-musical meanings (e.g. ‘our song’) that are typically subsumed under this category. Although music lacks communicative specificity in comparison with language, its power sometimes exceeds that of language in social, emotional and spiritual domains. In live contexts, moreover, music can communicate to greater numbers of individuals and over greater distances than language can.

Laboratory studies of isolated listeners and music-makers have yielded important insights into sensorimotor and cognitive skills and their neural underpinnings, but they have illuminated little about the significance of music for individuals, peer groups and communities, and for cultural evolution. Some experimental anthropologists, evolutionary biologists and evolutionary psychologists are beginning to make inroads into these issues by using precise measurement techniques with naturally occurring groups of music-makers and listeners [128,137].

The commonalities and differences in musical forms and functions across cultures suggest new directions for ethnomusicology, music cognition and neuroscience, and a pivot away from the predominant scientific focus on instrumental music in the Western European tradition. Empirical and field studies of ecologically valid music in diverse settings can complement traditional laboratory research, generating richer conceptions of human musicality than those currently available (e.g. see other papers in this volume). Social cognition [152] and iterated (person-to-person) learning [153] are considered to make vital contributions to the evolution of language. Their role may be even greater in the evolution of music.

Acknowledgement

The authors acknowledge assistance from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and AIRS (Advancing Interdisciplinary Research in Singing) in the preparation of this manuscript.

References

  • 1.Herbert R. 2011. Everyday musical listening: absorption, dissociation and trancing, p. 58 Farnham, UK: Ashgate. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mahon M. 2014. Music, power, and practice. Ethnomusicology 58, 327–333. ( 10.5406/ethnomusicology.58.2.0327) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wittgenstein L. 2001. Tractatus logico-philosophicus, p. 421 London, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Becker J. 2004. Deep listeners. Bloomington, IN: University of Indiana Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Juslin PN, Sloboda JA. (eds). 2010. Handbook of music and emotion: theory, research, applications. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Becker J. 2010. Exploring the habitus of listening: anthropological perspectives. In Handbook of music and emotion: theory, research, applications (eds Juslin PN, Sloboda JA.), pp. 127–157. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Balkwill L-L, Thompson WB. 1999. A cross-cultural investigation of emotion in music: psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Percept. 17, 43–64. ( 10.2307/40285811) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Balkwill L-L, Thompson WB, Matsunaga R. 2003. Recognition of emotion in Japanese, Western, and Hindustani music by Japanese listeners. Jpn Psychol. Res. 46, 337–349. ( 10.1111/j.1468-5584.2004.00265.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fritz T, Jentschke S, Gosselin N, Sammler D, Peretz I, Turner I, Friederici A, Koelsch S. 2009. Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Curr. Biol. 19, 573–576. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.058) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Liljeström S, Juslin PN, Västfäll D. 2013. Experimental evidence of the roles of music choice, social context, and listener personality in emotional reactions to music. Psychol. Music 41, 579–599. ( 10.1177/0305735612440615) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Salimpoor VN, Benovoy N, Longo G, Cooperstock JR, Zatorre RJ. 2009. The rewarding acts of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal. PLoS ONE 4, e7487 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0007487) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Zentner M, Grandjean D, Scherer KR. 2008. Emotions evoked by the sounds of music: characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion 8, 492–521. ( 10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Leon Portilla M. 2007. La musica de los Aztecas [Music among Aztecs]. Pauta 103, 7–19. [In Spanish.] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Merriam A. 1964. The anthropology of music. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Keil C. 1979. Tiv song. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bohlman P. 2002. World music. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stone R. 1982. Let the inside be sweet: the interpretation of music event among the Kpelle of Liberia, p. 7 Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rappaport R. 1999. Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brown S, Jordania J. 2013. Universals in the world's musics. Psychol. Music 41, 229–248. ( 10.1177/0305735611425896) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Nettl B. 2000. An ethnomusicologist contemplates musical universals. In The origins of music (eds Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S.), pp. 464–472. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Huron D. 2006. Sweet anticipation: music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Margulis EH. 2014. On repeat: how music plays the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nettl B. 2005. The study of ethnomusicology. Evanston, IL: University of Illinois Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Trehub SE. 2000. Human processing predispositions and musical universals. In The origins of music (eds Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S.), pp. 427–448. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tenzer M. 1991. Balinese music. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Jordania J. 2006. Who asked the first question? The origins of human choral singing, intelligence, language and speech. Tblisi, Georgia: Logos. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Trainor LJ, Heinmiller BM. 1998. The development of evaluative responses to music: infants prefer to listen to consonance over dissonance. Infant Behav. Dev. 21, 77–88. ( 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90055-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Zentner M, Kagan J. 1998. Infants’ perception of consonance and dissonance in music. Infant Behav. Dev. 21, 483–492. ( 10.1016/s0163-6383(98)90021-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gill KZ, Purves D. 2009. A biological rationale for musical scales. PLoS ONE 4, e8144 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0008144) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mukhergee P. 2004. The scales of Indian music. New Delhi, India: Aryan Books International. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Feld S. 2012. Sound and sentiment: birds, weeping, poetics, and song in Kaluli expression. Durham, NC: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Becker J. 1988. Earth, fire, sakti, and the Javanese gamelan. Ethnomusicology 32, 388–391. ( 10.2307/851938) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.James J. 1993. The music of the spheres: music, science and the natural order of the universe. New York, NY: Grove. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.DeWoskin K. 1982. A song for one or two: music and the concept of art in early China, p. 56 Michigan Pap. Chinese Stud., no. 42. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rowell L. 1992. Music and musical thought in early India. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sullivan L. 1997. Enchanting powers: music in the world's religions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Boas F. 1888/1964. The central Eskimo. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution; (Reprinted by University of Nebraska Press 1964.) [Google Scholar]
  • 38.McAllester D. 1964. Enemy way music: a study of social and esthetic values as seen in Navaho music. Reports of the Rimrock Project, Cambridge, MA: The Museum.
  • 39.Nettl B. 1989. Blackfoot musical thought: comparative perspectives. Kent, OH: Kent State University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Owomoyele O. 2002. Cultures and customs of Zimbabwe. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.DeNora T. 2013. Music asylums: wellbeing through music in everyday life. Farnham, UK: Ashgate. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sacks O. 2008. Musicophilia: tales of music and the brain. New York, NY: Knopf. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Nettl B. 1992. North American Indian music. In Excursions in world music (eds Nettl B, Capwell C, Bohlman P, Wong I, Turino T.), pp. 260–277. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ichikawa M. 1999. The Mbuti of northern Congo, pp. c210–c214. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Locke D. 1996. Africa: Ewe, Mande, Dagbamba, Shona and BaAka. In Worlds of music: an introduction to the music of the world‘s people, 3rd edn (ed. Titon J.), pp. 83–144. New York, NY: Schirmer. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Johnston T. 1989. Song categories and musical style of the Yupik Eskimo. Anthropos 84, 423–431. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Breen M. 1994. I have a dreamtime: aboriginal music and black rights in Australia. In World music: the rough guide (eds Broughton S, Ellingham M, Muddyman D, Trillo R.), pp. 655–662. London, UK: Rough Guides. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Conard N, Malina M, Münzel S, Seeberger F. 2004. Eine Mammutelfenbeinflöte aus dem Aurignacien des Geissenklösterle: neue Belege für eine musikalische Tradition im Frühen Jungpaläolithikum auf der Schwäbischen Alb [A mammoth ivory flute from the Aurignacian of the Geissenklösterle: new evidence for a musical tradition in the earliest Upper Palaeolithic of the Schwäbian Alb]. Archäol. Korresp. 34, 447–462. [In German.] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Conard N, Malina M, Münzel S. 2009. New flutes document the earliest musical tradition in southwestern Germany. Nature 460, 737–740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.d'Errico F, et al. 2003. Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism and music—an alternative multidisciplinary perspective. J. World Prehist. 17, 1–70. ( 10.1023/A:1023980201043) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hahn J, Münzel S. 1995. Knochenflöten aus den Aurignacien des Geissenklösterle bei Blaubeuren, Alb-Donau-Kreis [Bone flutes from the Aurignacian of the Geissenklösterle, near Blaubeuren, Alb-Donau-Kreis]. Fundberichte Baden-Würtemberg 20, 1–12. [In German.] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Higham T, Basell L, Jacobi R, Wood R, Bronk Ramsey C, Conard N. 2012. Testing models for the beginnings of the Aurignacian and the advent of figurative art and music: the radiocarbon chronology of Geißenklösterle. J. Hum. Evol. 62, 664–676. ( 10.1016/j.jhevol2012.03.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Buisson D. 1990. Les flûtes paléolithiques d'Isturitz. Bull. Soc. Préhistorique Française 87, 420–433. ( 10.3406/bspf.1990.9925) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Otte M. 1979. Le Paléolithique Supérieur Ancien en Belgique [The early Upper Palaeolithic in Belgium]. Monographies d'Archéologie Nationale 5 Brussels, Belgium: Musées Royaux d'Art et d'Histoire. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Scothern P. 1992. The music-archaeology of the Palaeolithic within its cultural setting. PhD thesis, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Harrold F. 1988. The Chatelperronian and the Early Aurignacian in France. In The early Upper Palaeolithic; BAR International Series 437 (eds Hoffecker J, Wolf C.), pp. 157–191. Oxford, UK: Archaeopress. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jelinek J. 1990. Art in the mirror of ages: the beginnings of artistic activities. Brno, Czech Republic: Moravian Museum, Anthropos Institute. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lawson G, d'Errico F. 2002. Microscopic, experimental and theoretical re-assessment of Upper Palaeolithic bird-bone pipes from Isturitz, France: ergonomics of design, systems of notation and the origins of musical traditions. In Studien zur Musikarchäologie III (eds Hickman E, Kilmer A, Eichman R.), pp. 119–142. Rahden, Germany: Verlag Marie Leidorf. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Passemard E. 1923. Une flûte aurignacienne d'Isturitz. Association Française pour l'Avancement des Sciences, pp. 474–476. Montpelier: Association Française pour l'Avancement des Sciences.
  • 60.Passemard E. 1944. La cavern d'Isturitz en Pays Basque [The Isturitz Cave in Pays Basque]. Préhistoire 9, 7–95. [In French.] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bahn P. 1983. Late Pleistocene economies in the French Pyrenees. In Hunter-gatherer economy in prehistory: a European perspective (ed. Bailey G.), pp. 167–185. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Dauvois M. 1989. Son et musique Paléolithiques Palaeolithic sound and music. Doss. Archéol. 142, 2–11. [In French.] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Dauvois M. 1999. Mesures acoustiques at témoins sonores osseux Paléolithiques. In Préhistoire d'os, recuil d’études sur l'industrie osseuse préhistorique offert à Mme Henriette Camps-Febrer. Aix-en-Provence, France: Publications de l'Université de Provence. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Huyge D. 1990. Mousterian skiffle? Note on a Middle Palaeolithic engraved bone from Schulen, Belgium. Rock Art Res. 7, 125–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Huyge D. 1991. The ‘Venus’ of Laussel in the light of ethnomusicology. Archeologie Vlaanderen 1, 11–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kuhn S, Stiner M. 1998. The earliest Aurignacian of Riparo Mochi (Liguria, Italy). Curr. Anthropol. 39(Suppl. 3), 175–189. ( 10.1086/204694) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bibikov S. 1978. A stone age orchestra. In Readings in physical anthropology and archaeology (eds Hunter D, Whitten P.), pp. 134–148. London, UK: Harper and Row. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Hadingham E. 1980. Secrets of the Ice Age: the world of the cave artists. London, UK: Heinemann. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Glory A. 1964. La Grotte de Roucador [The Cave of Roucador]. Bull. Soc. Préhist. Fr.61, clxvi-clxix.
  • 70.Glory A. 1965. Nouvelles découvertes de dessins rupestres sur le causse de Gramat [New discoveries of rock art on the Gramat plateau]. Bull. Soc. Préhist. Fr. 62, 528–536. ( 10.3406/bspf.1965.4049) [In French.] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Glory A, Vaultier M, Farinha Dos Santos M. 1965. La grotte ornée d'Escoural (Portugal) [The painted cave of Escoural (Portugal)]. Bull. Soc. Préhist. Fr. 62, 110–117. ( 10.3406/bspf.1965.4017) [In French.] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Dams L. 1984. Preliminary findings at the ‘Organ Sanctuary’ in the cave of Nerja, Malaga, Spain. Oxford J. Archaeol. 3, 1–14. ( 10.1111/j.1468-0092.1984.tb00112.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dams L. 1985. Palaeolithic lithophones: descriptions and comparisons. Oxford J. Archaeol. 4, 31–46. ( 10.1111/j.1468-0092.1985.tb00229.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Harding J. 1973. The bull-roarer in history and in antiquity. Afr. Music 5, 40–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Lang A. 1884. Custom and myth. London, UK: Harper. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Haddon A. 1898. The study of man. New York, NY: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Maioli W. 1991. Le origini il suono e la musica [The origins of sound and music] Milan, Italy: Jaca Book. [In Italian.] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Montagu J. 2007. Origins and development of musical instruments. Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Victor P, Robert-Lamblin J. 1989. La civilisation du phoque. Paris, France: Armand Colin-Raymond Chabaud. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Álvarez R, Siemens L. 1988. The lithophonic use of large natural rocks in the prehistoric Canary Islands. In The archaeology of early music cultures; third international meeting of the ICTM study group on music archaeology (ed. Hickman E, Hughes D.), pp. 1–10. Bonn, Germany: Verlag für systematische Musikwissenschaft. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Henschen-Nyman O. 1988. Cup-marked standing stones in Sweden. In The archaeology of early music cultures; third international meeting of the ICTM study group on music archaeology (eds Hickman E, Hughes D.), pp. 11–16. Bonn, Germany: Verlag für systematische Musikwissenschaft. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Boivin N. 2004. Rock art and rock music: petroglyphs of the South Indian Neolithic. Antiquity 78, 38–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Boivin N, Brumm A, Lewis H, Robinson D, Korisettar R. 2007. Sensual, material, and technological understanding: exploring prehistoric soundscapes in south India. J. R. Anthropol. Inst. 13, 267–294. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9655.2007.00428.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Fagg M. 1997. Rock music. Pitt-Rivers Mus. Occ. Pap., no. 14. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Benzon WL. 2001. Beethoven's anvil: music in mind and culture. New York, NY: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Dunbar RIM. 2012. On the evolutionary function of song and dance. In Music, language, and human evolution (ed. Bannan N.), pp. 201–214. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Freeman W. 2000. A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. In The origins of music (eds Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S.), pp. 411–424. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Loersch C, Arbuckle NL. 2013. Unraveling the mystery of music: music as an evolved group process. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 777–798. ( 10.1037/a0033691) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Myers F. 1999. Pintupi-speaking Aboriginals of the Western Desert. In The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (eds Lee R, Daly R.), pp. 348–357. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Kehoe A. 1999. Blackfoot and other hunters of the North American Plains. In The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (eds Lee R, Daly R.), pp. 36–40. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Bahuchet S. 1999. Aka Pygmies. In The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (eds Lee R, Daly R.), pp. 190–194. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Turino T. 1992. The music of sub-Saharan Africa. In Excursions in world music (eds Nettl B, Capwell C, Bohlman P, Wong I, Turino T.), pp. 165–195. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Cong-Huyen-Ton-Nu NT. 1979. The functions of folk songs in Vietnam. In The performing arts: music and dance (ed. Blacking J.), pp. 141–151. The Hague, NL: Mouton. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Deutsch D. (ed.) 2013. The psychology of music, 3rd edn San Diego, CA: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Patel AD. 2008. Music, language, and the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Juslin PN, Sloboda JA. 2013. Music and emotion. In The psychology of music, 3rd edn (ed. Deutsch D.), pp. 583–645. San Diego, CA: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Zatorre RJ, Salimpoor VN. 2013. From perception to pleasure: music and its neural substrates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 10 430–10 438. ( 10.1073/pnas.1301228110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Juslin PN, Laukka P. 2004. Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: a review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. J. New Music Res. 33, 217–238. ( 10.1080/0929821042000317813) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.North AC, Hargreaves DJ, Hargreaves JJ. 2004. Uses of music in everyday life. Music Percept. 22, 41–77. ( 10.1525/mp.2004.22.1.41) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Trehub SE, Trainor LJ. 1998. Singing to infants: lullabies and play songs. Adv. Infancy Res. 12, 43–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Brown MJE. 1980. Lullaby. In The new Grove dictionary of music and musicians (ed. Sadie S.), pp. 313–314. London, UK: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Hawes BL. 1974. Folksongs and function: some thoughts on the American lullaby. J. Am. Folklore 87, 140–148. ( 10.2307/539474) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Sakata HL. 1987. Hazara women in Afghanistan: innovators and preservers of musical tradition. In Women and music in cross-cultural perspective (ed. Koskoff E.), pp. 85–95. Westport, CT: Greenwood. [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Unyk AM, Trehub SE, Trainor LJ, Schellenberg EG. 1992. Lullabies and simplicity: a cross-cultural perspective. Psychol. Music 20, 15–28. ( 10.1177/0305735692201002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Trehub SE, Unyk AM, Trainor LJ. 1993. Adults identify infant-directed music across cultures. Infant Behav. Dev. 16, 193–211. ( 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80017-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Ayres B. 1973. Effects of infant carrying practices on rhythm in music. Ethos 1, 381–404. ( 10.1525/eth.1973.1.4.02a00020) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Trehub SE, Gudmundsdottir HR. In press Mothers as singing mentors for infants. In The Oxford handbook of singing (eds Welch G, Nix J.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Hrdy SB. 2009. Mothers and others: the evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 109.LeVine RA. 1988. Human parental care: universal goals, cultural strategies, individual behavior. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 1988, 3–12. ( 10.1002/cd.23219884003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Lewis J. 2013. A cross-cultural perspective on the significance of music and dance to culture and society: insight from BaYaka Pygmies. In Language, music, and the brain: a mysterious relationship (ed. Arbib M.), pp. 45–65. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Masuyama EE. 1989. Desire and discontent in Japanese lullabies. Western Folklore 48, 144–148. ( 10.2307/1499687) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Richman AL, Miller PM, Solomon MJ. 1988. The socialization of infants in suburban Boston. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 40, 65–74. ( 10.1002/cd.23219884008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Falk D. 2004. Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 491–503. ( 10.1017/S0140525X04000111) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Trainor LJ. 1996. Infant preferences for infant-directed versus noninfant-directed playsongs and lullabies. Infant Behav. Dev. 19, 83–92. ( 10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90046-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Trainor LJ, Clark ED, Huntley A, Adams BA. 1997. The acoustic basis of preferences for infant-directed singing. Infant Behav. Dev. 20, 383–396. ( 10.1016/S0163-6383(97)90009-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Trehub SE, Unyk AM, Trainor LJ. 1993. Maternal singing in cross-cultural perspective. Infant Behav. Dev. 16, 285–295. ( 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80036-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Trehub SE, Unyk AM, Kamenetsky SB, Hill DS, Trainor LJ, Henderson JL, Saraza M. 1997. Mothers’ and fathers’ singing to infants. Dev. Psychol. 33, 500–507. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.33.3.500) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Bergeson TR, Trehub SE. 2002. Absolute pitch and tempo in mothers’ songs to infants. Psychol. Sci. 13, 72–75. ( 10.1111/1467-9280.00413) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Lomax A. 1968. Folk song style and culture. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science. [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Pantaleoni H. 1985. On the nature of music. Oneonta, NY: Welkin. [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Zuckerkandl V. 1973. Sound and symbol: man the musician, vol. 2. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Shenfield T, Trehub SE, Nakata T. 2003. Maternal singing modulates infant arousal. Psychol. Music 31, 365–375. ( 10.1177/03057356030314002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Ghazban N. 2013. Emotion regulation in infants using maternal singing and speech. PhD thesis, Ryerson University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Masataka N. 1999. Preference for infant-directed singing in 2-day-old hearing infants of deaf parents. Dev. Psychol. 35, 1001–1005. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ. 2001. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion . Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11 818–11 823. ( 10.1073/pnas.191355898) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Janata P, Tomic ST, Haberman JM. 2012. Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 54–75. ( 10.1037/a0024208) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Boer B, Abubakar A. 2014. Music listening in families and peer groups: benefits for young people's social cohesion and emotional well-being across four cultures. Front. Psychol. 5, 392 ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00392) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Dunbar RIM, Kaskatis K, MacDonald I, Barra V. 2012. Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evol. Psychol. 10, 688–702. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Sosis R, Ruffle B. 2004. Religious ritual and cooperation: testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 713–722. ( 10.1086/379260) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Whitehouse H. 2004. Modes of religiosity: a cognitive theory of religious transmission. Lanham, MD: AltaMira Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Durkheim E. 1995. The elementary forms of religious life. New York, NY: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Janata P, Parsons LM. 2013. Neural mechanisms of music, singing, and dancing. In Language, music, and the brain: a mysterious relationship (ed. Arbib M.), pp. 307–328. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Atkinson QD, Whitehouse H. 2011. The cultural morphospace of ritual form: examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evol. Hum. Behav. 32, 50–62. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Konvalinka I, Xygalatas D, Bulbulia J, Schjødt U, Jegindø E, Wallot S, Van Orden G, Roepstorff A. 2011. Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 8514–8519. ( 10.1073/pnas.1016955108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Xygalatas D, Mitkidis P, Fischer R, Reddish P, Skewes J, Geertz A, Roepstorff A, Bulbulia J. 2013. Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1602–1605. ( 10.1177/0956797612472910) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Anderson AH. 2013. To the ends of the Earth: Pentecostalism and the transformation of world Christianity. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Fischer R, Callander R, Reddish P, Bulbulia J. 2013. How do rituals affect cooperation? Hum. Nat. 24, 115–125. ( 10.1007/s12110-013-9167-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Wiltermuth SS, Heath C. 2009. Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.McNeill W. 1995. Keeping together in time: dance and drill in human history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Kirschner S, Tomasello M. 2010. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 354–364. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Zentner M, Eerola T. 2010. Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 5768–5773. ( 10.1073/pnas.1000121107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Phillips-Silver J, Trainor LJ. 2005. Feeling the beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science 308, 1430 ( 10.1126/science.1110922) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ. 2014. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 1003–1011. ( 10.1111/desc.12193) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Donald M. 1991. Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Merker B. 2009. Ritual foundations of human uniqueness. In Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human uniqueness (ed. Malloch S, Trevarthen C.), pp. 45–59. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 146.van Baaren RB, Decety J, Dijksterhuis A, van der Leij A, van Leeuwen ML. 2011. Being imitated: consequences of nonconsciously showing empathy. In The social neuroscience of empathy (eds Decety J, Ickes WJ.), pp. 31–42. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Meltzoff AN. 2007. ‘Like me’: a foundation for social cognition. Dev. Sci. 10, 126–134. ( 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Tomasello M. 2003. Constructing a language: a usage based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Over H, Carpenter M. 2013. The social side of imitation. Child Dev. Pers. 7, 6–11. ( 10.1111/cdep.12006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Agnetta B, Rochat P. 2004. Imitative games by 9-, 14-, and 18-month-old infants. Infancy 6, 1–36. ( 10.1207/s15327078in0601_1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Fawcett C, Liszkowski U. 2012. Mimicry and play initiation in 18-month-old infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 689–696. ( 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2014. The evolution of language from social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28, 5–9. ( 10.1016/j.conb.2014.04.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Kirby S, Griffiths T, Smith K. 2014. Iterated learning and the evolution of language. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28, 108–114. ( 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES