Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2015 Feb 11;35(6):2432–2437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0532-14.2015

Norepinephrine Is Necessary for Experience-Dependent Plasticity in the Developing Mouse Auditory Cortex

Kathryn N Shepard 1,2, L Cameron Liles 3, David Weinshenker 1,3, Robert C Liu 1,2,
PMCID: PMC4323528  PMID: 25673838

Abstract

Critical periods are developmental windows during which the stimuli an animal encounters can reshape response properties in the affected system to a profound degree. Despite this window's importance, the neural mechanisms that regulate it are not completely understood. Pioneering studies in visual cortex initially indicated that norepinephrine (NE) permits ocular dominance column plasticity during the critical period, but later research has suggested otherwise. More recent work implicating NE in experience-dependent plasticity in the adult auditory cortex led us to re-examine the role of NE in critical period plasticity. Here, we exposed dopamine β-hydroxylase knock-out (Dbh−/−) mice, which lack NE completely from birth, to a biased acoustic environment during the auditory cortical critical period. This manipulation led to a redistribution of best frequencies (BFs) across auditory cortex in our control mice, consistent with prior work. By contrast, Dbh−/− mice failed to exhibit the expected redistribution of BFs, even though NE-deficient and NE-competent mice showed comparable auditory cortical organization when reared in a quiet colony environment. These data suggest that while intrinsic tonotopic patterning of auditory cortical circuitry occurs independently from NE, NE is required for critical period plasticity in auditory cortex.

Keywords: auditory cortex, critical period, norepinephrine, sound exposure, tonotopy

Introduction

Sensory systems often undergo critical periods during postnatal development, during which neural response properties can be easily reshaped by sensory experience. The pioneering work of Hubel and Wiesel, for example, showed that the proportion of visual cortex responding to stimulation of a previously occluded eye could be reduced if occlusion occurred during a circumscribed period of development (Wiesel and Hubel, 1963). In the wake of Hubel and Wiesel's findings, such critical periods have also been described for the auditory (Zhang et al., 2001; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2007), somatosensory (Fox, 1992; Crair and Malenka, 1995), and vestibular (Jones et al., 2000; Moorman et al., 2002) systems.

Early studies by Pettigrew and colleagues indicated that norepinephrine (NE) was necessary for critical period plasticity. Depleting it from kitten visual cortex inhibited the shift in responsiveness toward the unoccluded eye following monocular occlusion (Kasamatsu and Pettigrew, 1976; Daw et al., 1983), and replenishing it during the critical period restored plasticity (Kasamatsu et al., 1979). However, subsequent studies reported that lesioning either the noradrenergic or cholinergic system alone failed to block plasticity (Bear and Daniels, 1983; Adrien et al., 1985), suggesting functional redundancy between the two systems (Bear and Singer, 1986).

NE's involvement in critical period regulation remains unclear today, though more recent studies have implicated NE in experience-dependent plasticity in the adult auditory cortex (Edeline et al., 2011; Martins and Froemke, 2013). This motivated us to revisit NE's involvement in experience-dependent developmental plasticity, using the auditory cortex of the NE-deficient Dbh−/− mouse as our model. In these mice, we attempted to drive the redistribution of best frequencies (BFs) in the auditory cortical frequency map through pure tone exposure during the critical period (Zhang et al., 2001; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2007; Insanally et al., 2009). We found that while auditory cortical organization was comparable between unperturbed Dbh−/− mice and controls, critical period plasticity was inhibited in Dbh−/− mice, suggesting that NE is required for frequency map plasticity in the auditory cortical critical period.

Materials and Methods

All procedures were approved by the Emory Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Electrophysiological recordings were taken from 6–8-week-old female Dbh−/− mice bred on a mixed C57BL/6J and 129SvEv background (Research Resource Identifier: MGI:1857556). Because NE deficiency is fatal to embryonic Dbh−/− mice (Thomas et al., 1995), the NE precursor l-3,4-dihydroxyphenylserine (l-DOPS) was administered via maternal drinking water from embryonic day 14.5 until birth. This intervention allows Dbh−/− embryos to synthesize enough NE for normal fetal development (Thomas et al., 1998). Once born, Dbh−/− mice do not require l-DOPS for survival despite complete NE deficiency. Furthermore, other than lacking NE, their noradrenergic system is largely comparable to NE-competent controls (Weinshenker et al., 2002a; Jin et al., 2004). Dbh+/− littermates, which are heterozygous for the dopamine β-hydroxylase gene and have NE levels comparable to wild-type (Dbh+/+) mice (Thomas et al., 1998; Bourdélat-Parks et al., 2005; Sanders et al., 2006), were used as controls.

“Baseline” animals of both genotypes were housed in a standard colony environment until electrophysiological recording between postnatal day (P) 42 and P56. “8 kHz-exposed” animals of both genotypes were housed in this colony except during sound exposure. That occurred in a sound-attenuating chamber (IAC Acoustics) for 16–18 h/d from P7 to P21, which encompasses the P12–P15 critical period for frequency tuning, as defined in the C57BL/6J mouse (Barkat et al., 2011). Stimuli consisted of 8 kHz pure tones (80 dB SPL, 100 ms, 1 s trains at 5 Hz, alternated with 2 s silence). A 4 W white LED bulb and an automatic timer were used to maintain the same light cycle (12 h light/dark) as the colony.

Peripheral hearing thresholds were assessed via auditory brainstem response (ABR). Anesthesia was induced with a 6:1 ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (5 mg/kg) mixture, and maintained with 12:1 ketamine (30 mg/kg) and xylazine (1 mg/kg). A presurgical dose of dexamethasone (10 mg/kg) was given to prevent cortical edema. Needle electrodes were inserted subdermally behind each bulla and at the skull vertex. Signals were sampled at 25 kilosamples/s, amplified 200,000×, and bandpass filtered between 100 and 3000 Hz. ABRs were assessed for clicks and tone pips at 8, 16, 24, and 32 kHz (3 ms, 21 Hz repetition rate), presented through a free-field speaker (HiVi RT1.3 Planar Isodynamic Tweeter). Stimuli were presented at decreasing sound intensities (5 dB interval) until sound-evoked ABR peaks were no longer apparent. ABR waveforms were averaged over 500 stimulus repeats. Off-line, a blinded observer determined the threshold of the ABR's first peak, which represents activity in the auditory nerve. Peak amplitudes were also measured in BioSigRP (Tucker-Davis Technologies) by comparing the height of manually identified ABR peaks to their preceding troughs. Peak I was omitted from analysis because it was not preceded by a trough.

Frequency responses were next mapped electrophysiologically across the left auditory cortex. After removing the muscle over the left parietal bone, a craniotomy was performed and a headpost was cemented to the animal's skull at bregma to maintain a consistent head position throughout the experiment.

Multiunits were recorded across the left auditory cortex using a 4 MΩ 3 × 1 tungsten matrix microelectrode (FHC) with 305 μm interelectrode spacing. The electrode was driven into layer 4 (∼400 μm), and pure tones were played back (60 ms, seven intensities from 5 to 65 dB SPL, 30 frequencies log-spaced 2–32 kHz) in pseudorandom order, with five repetitions of each frequency–intensity combination. Stimuli were presented with a free-field speaker positioned 20 cm from and 45° anterolateral to the right ear. We aimed to capture the entire spatial extent of core auditory cortex, consisting of primary auditory cortex and anterior auditory field. In identifying these, we looked for clear frequency tuning and a peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) peak <40 ms from sound onset; weak or unapparent PSTH peaks, or latencies >40 ms indicated that a site was outside core auditory cortex. Using vascular features as references, spatial coordinates of each recording site were tracked on a high-resolution photo of the craniotomy.

Offline, frequency response areas (FRAs) were constructed for each multiunit by plotting the magnitude of the response (spikes per second) against the frequency and amplitude of the stimulus. A blinded observer referenced each unit's FRA and PSTH to manually determine its response latency, duration, threshold, and bandwidth at 20 dB below threshold. BFs were defined as the frequency that generated the highest average spike rate over all the intensities equal to or less than the threshold intensity. Using a method similar to that described in de Villers-Sidani et al. (2007), we also analyzed rate-level functions in an automated fashion. Using custom Matlab code, we found the sound intensity that evoked the strongest response to the BF. Then, using a least-squares method, we fitted a line to the segment of the rate-level function between the loudest sound intensity and the intensity eliciting the peak response. If the peak sound intensity was also the loudest sound intensity, we simply fitted the entire rate-level function. Units with positive slopes were classified as monotonic and units with negative slopes were classified as nonmonotonic.

Individual animals' BF maps were generated by Voronoi tessellation (Matlab: voronoin) of all recording sites for a given animal. Average BF maps were constructed to give a qualitative impression of tonotopic organization within groups. Individual animals' BF maps were normalized such that the rostrocaudal and dorsoventral extents of primary cortex fit a 100 × 100 grid, and each grid cell was colored depending on which Voronoi polygon its center would fall within. These “pixelated” maps for each animal within a group were “stacked” and the BFs averaged for each cell in the z direction. If <6 maps contributed a BF measurement to a particular cell (i.e., if that normalized location was outside the core auditory cortex for a significant proportion of animals), that cell was excluded from the average map, such that all colored cells display the average BF at that site for ≥6 animals.

To quantitatively analyze the proportionate area of cortex tuned to a particular frequency band, we used the areas of the (un-normalized) Voronoi polygons tuned to that band, expressed as a proportion of the sum of all core polygons. A fraction of recording sites were associated with polygons of infinite or spuriously large areas because for technical reasons we were unable to “enclose” that area with a perimeter of nonauditory sites. To ensure that these areas did not bias our analyses, we reassigned the median area measurement to the 5% of sites with the largest areas.

For comparisons between two groups, two-sample t tests were used, provided the data were normally distributed and had equal variance; otherwise, a Mann–Whitney U test was used. For comparisons across all four groups, 2 × 2 ANOVAs were used with genotype and rearing condition as factors. To compare best frequency distributions, a two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used. The significance level was set to 0.001 to correct for this test's high power, and for the artificially inflated sample size we incurred by pooling multiunits by group. For all other tests, p < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Because Dbh−/− mice experience high rates of ear infection and deafness relative to NE-competent controls (Maison et al., 2010), we performed ABRs to assess hearing thresholds before cortical electrophysiology. Mice that showed clear ABR peaks (Fig. 1A) in response to clicks, 8 kHz tones, and 16 kHz tones were mapped electrophysiologically. Consistent with prior research, more Dbh+/− (baseline: 9 of 11; 8 kHz-exposed: 12 of 18) than Dbh−/− (baseline: 14 of 22; 8 kHz-exposed: 13 of 26) mice met these criteria. Among animals that were included, there was no effect of genotype on ABR threshold for peak 1 (Fig. 1B; clicks: Fgeno = 0.12, p = 0.73; 8 kHz: Fgeno = 0.63, p = 0.43; 16 kHz: Fgeno = 1.2, p = 0.28), which reflects activity in the auditory nerve, for clicks, 8 kHz tones, or 16 kHz tones. Animals that underwent developmental sound exposure had thresholds comparable to baseline (clicks: Fexperience = 0.84, p = 0.37; 8 kHz: Fexperience = 1.79, p = 0.19; 16 kHz: Fexperience = 1.92, p = 0.18). Further, no significant interactions between genotype and experience were observed (clicks: Finteraction = 4.18, p = 0.05; 8 kHz: Finteraction = 2.96, p = 0.10; 16 kHz: Finteraction = 2.6, p = 0.12).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Auditory brainstem responses. A, Representative ABR waveforms for 55 dB SPL click from individual Dbh+/−, Dbh−/−, and Dbh+/+ mice. Arrows indicate the first ABR peak. B, Group average thresholds in response to clicks, 8 kHz tones. and 16 kHz tones. Error bars represent mean ± 1 SE.

Twenty-four and 32 kHz ABR stimuli were played to these animals, though most animals of both genotypes were deaf to these frequencies at the sound intensities tested (≤75 dB SPL). ABRs were also recorded from Dbh+/+ mice (Fig. 1A, n = 3). While thresholds for ABRs evoked by clicks and low-frequency tones were comparable to those seen in Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− mice (clicks: 37 ± 13 dB SPL; 8 kHz: 63 ± 19 dB SPL; 16 kHz: 70 ± 18 dB SPL), thresholds for 24 and 32 kHz tones were again beyond our loudest tested intensity, indicating that strain background rather than dopamine β-hydroxylase deficiency is responsible for these animals' limited hearing range.

We next characterized auditory cortex in mice reared in a quiet colony environment. Tone-locked responses were obtained from animals in each group (Fig. 2A,B, example multiunits), and FRAs (Fig. 2A,B) showed classic V-shaped tuning profiles in Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− animals alike. BFs were estimated, and complete BF maps (i.e., encircled by nonresponsive sites) were constructed for the left auditory cortices of 8 Dbh+/− (284 total multiunits) and 7 Dbh−/− (220 total multiunits) mice. Spatial organization of BFs was heterogeneous across mice, and maps were often dominated by sites tuned to 13–16 kHz, though a coarse tonotopic frequency gradient could usually be discerned (Fig. 2C,D). Mapping a wild-type mouse confirmed that strain background itself, rather than loss of dopamine β-hydroxylase, can manifest nonuniform, 13–16 kHz-dominated frequency maps (Fig. 2E). Moreover, averaging maps for a particular group together allowed the expected tonotopic gradient to become more apparent for both Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− mice (Fig. 2F,G).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Frequency responses in baseline mice. A, B, Representative example PSTHs and FRAs from individual Dbh+/− (A) and Dbh−/− (B) multiunits in response to all (pooled) experimental stimuli. In PSTH, black horizontal bar indicates stimulus. C–E, Representative BF maps for individual Dbh+/− (C), Dbh−/− (D), and Dbh+/+ (E) mice. Each polygon represents a single multiunit, colored by its BF. X's represent noncore or nonresponsive recording sites. F, G, Group average BF maps. Cell color represents the average BF for all constituent maps that have core multiunits at that normalized position. Color scale as in C–E. Tonotopic gradients for primary auditory cortex (A1) and anterior auditory field (AAF), shown with arrows. H, Cumulative distribution of pooled BFs for each group.

Consistent with these qualitative impressions, quantitative comparisons of baseline Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− multiunit spatial organization were similar. Total auditory cortex size and the proportion of multiunits tuned to each frequency tested did not differ between groups (Fig. 2H; Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, 0.16; p > 0.001). Unit response properties were also not significantly different (Table 1). These data show that, for mice reared in a quiet colony, loss of NE does not appreciably affect the organization of core auditory cortex. Though this suggests NE is not required for the initial patterning of auditory circuitry and its maintenance in an unperturbed environment, it does not preclude the possibility that NE might be involved in adaptation of the auditory cortex to an environmental manipulation during the critical period.

Table 1.

Response properties of baseline Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− multiunits

Parameter Dbh+/− (n = 8)a Dbh−/− (n = 7)a t test p
Latency (ms) 21 ± 0.9 21 ± 0.8 0.07 0.94
Duration (ms) 66 ± 4.6 71 ± 6.5 0.65 0.53
Threshold (dB SPL) 33.5 ± 2.7 34.6 ± 1.0 0.38 0.71
Bandwidth at 20 dB above threshold (octave) 1.08 ± 0.06 0.99 ± 0.07 1.05 0.31
Evoked at BF (spikes/s) 90.2 ± 6.8 81.7 ± 3.5 1.06 0.31
Spontaneous (spikes/s) 7.4 ± 1.1 7.3 ± 0.9 0.12 0.91
Body mass (g) 19.5 17.0 3.56 0.004
Auditory cortex size (mm2)b 2.93 2.73 18 0.28

aData in Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− columns show pooled multiunit means ± SEM.

bFor auditory cortex size, medians are shown and test statistic is Mann–Whitney U.

We therefore attempted to drive developmental plasticity directly by altering the acoustic environment during the critical period for frequency tuning in the mouse. Cohorts of mixed Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− littermates were exposed to repeated presentations of 8 kHz tones from P7 to P21. An 8 kHz frequency was selected for exposure because all animals mapped in our baseline study showed BFs at or around 8 kHz, but in no animal was it the most prominent frequency. This provided existing cortical territory to expand into under appropriate conditions, while minimizing the chance of a ceiling effect at the exposure frequency.

Complete BF maps were obtained from 10 Dbh+/− (356 total multiunits) and 7 Dbh−/− mice (214 total multiunits). Consistent with prior work, exposure to 8 kHz tones during development drove an approximate doubling of the core cortical area devoted to that frequency in NE-competent animals (Fig. 3C, middle; 8.8 ± 6.6% of total auditory cortex area in baseline Dbh+/− vs 20.5 ± 8.4% in 8 kHz-exposed Dbh+/−). This effect was apparent at the level of individual cortical maps in many cases (Fig. 3A), as well as in the average map (Fig. 3B). In contrast, there was no evidence of such an expansion in NE-deficient animals (11.7 ± 6.6% of total auditory cortex area in baseline Dbh−/− vs 11.5 ± 7.1% in 8 kHz-exposed Dbh−/−). The proportion of cortex tuned to 8 kHz ± 0.35 octave was not significantly different between exposed and unexposed cohorts of Dbh−/− mice. Thus, there was a significant interaction between genotype and experience for the 8 kHz band (Finteraction = 5.09, p = 0.03).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Effect of developmental sound exposure on frequency responses in Dbh+/− and Dbh−/− mice. A, As in Figure 2C–E, representative BF maps for individual 8 kHz-exposed mice. Magenta outline highlights multiunits with BFs at 8 kHz ± 0.35 octaves. Magenta box on color bar highlights frequency of developmental exposure. B, As in Figure 2F,G, group average BF maps for 8 kHz-exposed mice. Cell color represents the average BF for all constituent maps that have core multiunits at that normalized position. Color scale as in A. C, Percentage area of core auditory cortex (AC area) tuned within different frequency bins. Each bin contains six tested frequencies, spanning ∼0.7 octaves. Color scheme is same as in Figure 1B. Error bars represent means ± SEM. Asterisk indicates significant genotype × experience interaction, p < 0.05. D, Bandwidth at 20 dB above threshold (BW20), for multiunits with characteristic frequencies (CFs) within the specified ranges. “Base.” refers to baseline group; “8kHz” refers to 8 kHz-exposed group. Error bars represent means ± SEM.

Proportionate area devoted to any frequency band other than ∼8 kHz did not differ significantly between exposed and unexposed cohorts, despite a nonsignificant trend for 19.7–31.6 kHz, likely arising from random variability in high-frequency hearing. That bin for sound-exposed Dbh+/− mice contained a slightly higher proportion of animals with higher-frequency hearing limits (by ABR), and these animals also happened to have large cortical representation of those frequencies. In any case, in no bin besides 8 kHz was a significant interaction observed for genotype and rearing experience (2–3.39 kHz band: Finteraction = 0.01, p = 0.93; 3.4–5.99 kHz band: Finteraction = 0.84, p = 0.37; 10.5–18.73 kHz band: Finteraction = 3.52, p = 0.07; 19.7–31.6 kHz band: Finteraction = 1.01, p = 0.32). The specificity of our plasticity effects for the band of exposure therefore implicates NE as a regulator of experience-dependent cortical map plasticity during the critical period.

Additional evidence was sought to determine the specificity of this plasticity to the auditory cortical map. We found that peak amplitudes of 8 kHz tone-evoked ABRs, which are generated subcortically, lacked significant genotype × experience interactions (Table 2). This indicates that plasticity for the exposed frequency is restricted to the cortex. That cortical multiunit firing rates in response to 8 kHz tones are also comparable across groups (baseline Dbh+/−: 26.8 ± 27.2 spikes/s; baseline Dbh−/−: 24.9 ± 27.0 spikes/s; 8 kHz-exposed Dbh+/−: 26.4 ± 25.1 spikes/s; 8 kHz-exposed Dbh−/−: 27.1 ± 23.3 spikes/s; Finteraction = 0.60, p = 0.44) further suggests plasticity just at the level of the cortical map.

Table 2.

8 kHz tone-evoked ABR peak amplitudesa

Baseline
8 kHz-exposed
Finteraction p
Dbh+/− Dbh−/− Dbh+/− Dbh−/−
Peak II (nV) 898 873 1077 772 0.81 0.38
Peak III (nV) 261 348 541 182 2.83 0.11
Peak IV (nV) 2049 2191 1871 1582 0.42 0.53
Peak V (nV) 1701 1153 871 1011 1.9 0.18

aStatistical results are reported for genotype × experience ANOVAs conducted for each peak.

Finally, we examined our results further to assess whether some previously characterized forms of developmental exposure-induced plasticity were expressed in our model. We first turned to tuning bandwidth, as others have observed changes in this response parameter following developmental sound exposure (Han et al., 2007; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2007). We focused on multiunits tuned to the 8 kHz bin of exposure (6.3–10.1 kHz), and the adjacent higher-frequency bin (11.1–17.9 kHz) that contained the large majority of the rest of the multiunits. Bandwidths were indeed narrower in sound-exposed Dbh+/− animals (Fig. 3D; Fexperience = 10.68, p = 0.001), though this was not specific for the sound-exposed frequency bin (Finteraction = 0.6, p = 0.44), as Han et al. (2007) showed. In Dbh−/− mice, we did not find evidence of an effect of sound exposure on bandwidth either across all frequencies (Fexperience = 0.31, p = 0.58), or in a frequency-dependent manner (Finteraction < 0.01, p = 0.95). This echoes the pattern of plasticity we observed for frequency tuning, in which sound exposure-dependent changes were observed in NE-competent Dbh+/− mice, but not NE-deficient Dbh−/− mice. We next compared the proportions of Dbh +/− units showing monotonic versus nonmonotonic rate-level functions, as in de Villers-Sidani et al. (2007). Consistent with that study, most of the units recorded from both baseline and sound-exposed mice showed monotonic rate-level functions. Unlike de Villers-Sidani et al., though, we observed only a slight and nonsignificant increase in the proportion of nonmonotonic rate-level functions in sound-exposed animals (baseline Dbh+/−: 35%; 8 kHz-exposed Dbh+/−: 39%; two-sample z test, z = 1.04, p = 0.29). Because no significant effect was observed for the Dbh+/− mouse, we did not extend our investigation to the Dbh−/− groups. Thus, the clear map plasticity observed in Dbh+/− mice is accompanied by a nonfrequency-specific narrowing of tuning bandwidths, but not a change in rate-level function.

Discussion

Here, we sought to re-examine whether NE is required for critical period plasticity using the core auditory cortex of NE-deficient Dbh−/− mice as our model. We found that while the baseline organization of auditory cortex in the Dbh−/− mouse was comparable to that of its Dbh+/− counterparts, we were unable to drive a redistribution of BFs by exposing these animals to a biased acoustic environment during development. This effect was not accompanied by changes in the cortical or subcortical magnitude of the response to the stimulus of exposure. This implies that NE is required for plasticity during the critical period for frequency tuning in the auditory cortical map.

A number of prior studies in visual cortex have argued against the necessity of NE for critical period plasticity (Bear and Daniels, 1983; Adrien et al., 1985; Bear and Singer, 1986). One possible reason for this discrepancy is these studies' use of neurotoxins, particularly 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA), to destroy noradrenergic neurons. 6-OHDA is somewhat nonspecific, as it kills dopaminergic (Uretsky and Iversen, 1970; Walker et al., 2013) as well as noradrenergic neurons, and acutely compromises cholinergic signaling (Bear and Singer, 1986). Further, depending on the route of administration and dosage, 6-OHDA lesions may not include all noradrenergic projections in the area of interest.

The Dbh−/− mouse does not carry these risks of nonspecificity and incompletion, but its use here does introduce some caveats. First, Dbh−/− mice experience delayed growth through approximately week 5 of postnatal development (Thomas et al., 1995), raising the possibility that critical period plasticity could be delayed, rather than blocked, in these mice. Our long duration of sound exposure deliberately overshoots the critical period in the mouse (Barkat et al., 2011) by ∼1 week to largely protect against the possibility that the critical period occurs later. It could also be that closure of the critical period window is delayed in these mice, and that between finishing developmental sound exposure and adult mapping, their tonotopic maps renormalize. In principle, this could be addressed by mapping animals immediately after sound exposure. However, completing anesthetized mapping studies in 3-week-old Dbh−/− mice is very difficult technically due to their small size and fragility. In any case, that renormalization does not apparently happen in Dbh+/− mice would still indicate a difference in plasticity mechanisms.

Second, Dbh−/− mice show hypersensitive dopaminergic signaling in some regions, raising the question of whether this or the primary NE deficit is responsible for the observed plasticity phenotype. This seems unlikely as dopaminergic signaling abnormalities were restricted to the striatum and not present in neocortex (Weinshenker et al., 2002b; Schank et al., 2006). Nevertheless, a NE replacement study, though experimentally challenging, would help rule out the possibility that dopamine is responsible for the effect we observed. We note though that stimulating the dopaminergic system during sound exposure in adult rodents actually induces map expansion (Bao et al., 2001), making it less likely that this mechanism explains the lack of plasticity in our Dbh−/− mice.

Finally, since NE loss is systemic in the Dbh−/− mouse, our results do not elucidate the site at which NE acts to enable developmental plasticity. Replacing NE in the auditory cortex of the Dbh−/− mouse during the critical period would help determine whether NE's action is direct or indirect, though achieving this pharmacologically is difficult because mouse pups are so small and fragile. A site-specific, conditional Dbh knock-out that permits DBH synthesis only in cortex, and only during early postnatal development, could provide more definitive results, but would also be more technically challenging to produce.

It is appealing to speculate that, if NE acts directly on the auditory cortex to enable critical period map plasticity, it does so by gating or facilitating the synaptic plasticity events that likely underlie map plasticity. There is ample evidence of noradrenergic involvement in synaptic plasticity: in other systems, NE has been shown to facilitate synaptic plasticity directly through its actions on AMPA receptor trafficking (Hu et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2010) and neurotransmitter release (Carey and Regehr, 2009). It also permits long-term potentiation in thalamorecipient neurons in the amygdala indirectly by reducing GABAergic inhibition of these cells (Tully et al., 2007). The precise synaptic changes that ultimately produce map plasticity have not yet been characterized, but recent in vitro work suggests there is a critical period for plasticity in layer IV thalamocortical synapses that occurs concurrently with and potentially underlies the critical period for map plasticity in auditory cortex (Barkat et al., 2011). Follow-up work assessing the necessity of NE for this critical period for thalamocortical connectivity could be useful for not only identifying the substrate upon which NE acts to enable map plasticity, but could also for further clarifying the extent to which the critical periods for map plasticity and thalamocortical connectivity share common mechanisms.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants F31 DC011987 (K.N.S.), R01 DC008343 (R.C.L.), and R01 DA027535 (D.W.), and Emory University's PRISM (Problems and Research to Integrate Science & Mathematics) Fellowship DGE0536941, a GK-12 program of the National Science Foundation (K.N.S.). We also thank Sarah Pallas for her feedback on the manuscript, and Laura Mariani for technical assistance.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Adrien J, Blanc G, Buisseret P, Frégnac Y, Gary-Bobo E, Imbert M, Tassin JP, Trotter Y. Noradrenaline and functional plasticity in kitten visual cortex: a re-examination. J Physiol. 1985;367:73–98. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1985.sp015815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bao S, Chan VT, Merzenich MM. Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature. 2001;412:79–83. doi: 10.1038/35083586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barkat TR, Polley DB, Hensch TK. A critical period for auditory thalamocortical connectivity. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1189–1194. doi: 10.1038/nn.2882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bear MF, Daniels JD. The plastic response to monocular deprivation persists in kitten visual cortex after chronic depletion of norepinephrine. J Neurosci. 1983;3:407–416. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-02-00407.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bear MF, Singer W. Modulation of visual cortical plasticity by acetylcholine and noradrenaline. Nature. 1986;320:172–176. doi: 10.1038/320172a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bourdélat-Parks BN, Anderson GM, Donaldson ZR, Weiss JM, Bonsall RW, Emery MS, Liles LC, Weinshenker D. Effects of dopamine beta-hydroxylase genotype and disulfiram inhibition on catecholamine homeostasis in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:72–80. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0139-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carey MR, Regehr WG. Noradrenergic control of associative synaptic plasticity by selective modulation of instructive signals. Neuron. 2009;62:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crair MC, Malenka RC. A critical period for long-term potentiation at thalamocortical synapses. Nature. 1995;375:325–328. doi: 10.1038/375325a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Daw NW, Rader RK, Robertson TW, Ariel M. Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine on visual deprivation in the kitten striate cortex. J Neurosci. 1983;3:907–914. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-05-00907.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. de Villers-Sidani E, Chang EF, Bao S, Merzenich MM. Critical period window for spectral tuning defined in the primary auditory cortex (A1) in the rat. J Neurosci. 2007;27:180–189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3227-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Edeline JM, Manunta Y, Hennevin E. Induction of selective plasticity in the frequency tuning of auditory cortex and auditory thalamus neurons by locus coeruleus stimulation. Hear Res. 2011;274:75–84. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fox K. A critical period for experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in rat barrel cortex. J Neurosci. 1992;12:1826–1838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-05-01826.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Han YK, Köver H, Insanally MN, Semerdjian JH, Bao S. Early experience impairs perceptual discrimination. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1191–1197. doi: 10.1038/nn1941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hu H, Real E, Takamiya K, Kang MG, Ledoux J, Huganir RL, Malinow R. Emotion enhances learning via norepinephrine regulation of AMPA-receptor trafficking. Cell. 2007;131:160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Insanally MN, Köver H, Kim H, Bao S. Feature-dependent sensitive periods in the development of complex sound representation. J Neurosci. 2009;29:5456–5462. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5311-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Jin SH, Kim HJ, Harris DC, Thomas SA. Postnatal development of the cerebellum and the CNS adrenergic system is independent of norepinephrine and epinephrine. J Comp Neurol. 2004;477:300–309. doi: 10.1002/cne.20263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jones SM, Warren LE, Shukla R, Browning A, Fuller CA, Jones TA. The effects of hypergravity and substrate vibration on vestibular function in developing chickens. J Gravit Physiol. 2000;7:31–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kasamatsu T, Pettigrew JD. Depletion of brain catecholamines: failure of ocular dominance shift after monocular occlusion in kittens. Science. 1976;194:206–209. doi: 10.1126/science.959850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kasamatsu T, Pettigrew JD, Ary M. Restoration of visual cortical plasticity by local microperfusion of norepinephrine. J Comp Neurol. 1979;185:163–181. doi: 10.1002/cne.901850110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Liu Y, Formisano L, Savtchouk I, Takayasu Y, Szabó G, Zukin RS, Liu SJ. A single fear-inducing stimulus induces a transcription-dependent switch in synaptic AMPAR phenotype. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:223–231. doi: 10.1038/nn.2474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Maison SF, Le M, Larsen E, Lee SK, Rosowski JJ, Thomas SA, Liberman MC. Mice lacking adrenergic signaling have normal cochlear responses and normal resistance to acoustic injury but enhanced susceptibility to middle-ear infection. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2010;11:449–461. doi: 10.1007/s10162-010-0220-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Martins A, Froemke RC. Neuromodulatory plasticity governs cortical plasticity. Presented at the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting; November; San Diego. 2013. [Google Scholar]
  23. Moorman SJ, Cordova R, Davies SA. A critical period for functional vestibular development in zebrafish. Dev Dyn. 2002;223:285–291. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.10052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Sanders JD, Szot P, Weinshenker D, Happe HK, Bylund DB, Murrin LC. Analysis of brain adrenergic receptors in dopamine-beta-hydroxylase knockout mice. Brain Res. 2006;1109:45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Schank JR, Ventura R, Puglisi-Allegra S, Alcaro A, Cole CD, Liles LC, Seeman P, Weinshenker D. Dopamine beta-hydroxylase knockout mice have alterations in dopamine signaling and are hypersensitive to cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2221–2230. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Thomas SA, Matsumoto AM, Palmiter RD. Noradrenaline is essential for mouse fetal development. Nature. 1995;374:643–646. doi: 10.1038/374643a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Thomas SA, Marck BT, Palmiter RD, Matsumoto AM. Restoration of norepinephrine and reversal of phenotypes in mice lacking dopamine beta-hydroxylase. J Neurochem. 1998;70:2468–2476. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1998.70062468.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Tully K, Li Y, Tsvetkov E, Bolshakov VY. Norepinephrine enables the induction of associative long-term potentiation at thalamo-amygdala synapses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:14146–14150. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704621104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Uretsky NJ, Iversen LL. Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine on catecholamine containing neurones in the rat brain. J Neurochem. 1970;17:269–278. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1970.tb02210.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Walker MD, Dinelle K, Kornelsen R, Lee A, Farrer MJ, Stoessl AJ, Sossi V. Measuring dopaminergic function in the 6-OHDA-lesioned rat: a comparison of PET and microdialysis. EJNMMI Res. 2013;3:69. doi: 10.1186/2191-219X-3-69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Weinshenker D, White SS, Javors MA, Palmiter RD, Szot P. Regulation of norepinephrine transporter abundance by catecholamines and desipramine in vivo. Brain Res. 2002a;946:239–246. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02889-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Weinshenker D, Miller NS, Blizinsky K, Laughlin ML, Palmiter RD. Mice with norepinephrine deficiency resemble amphetamine-sensitized animals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002b;99:13873–13877. doi: 10.1073/pnas.212519999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wiesel TN, Hubel DH. Single-cell response in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J Neurophysiol. 1963;26:1003–1017. doi: 10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Zhang LI, Bao S, Merzenich MM. Persistent and specific influences of early acoustic environments on primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1123–1130. doi: 10.1038/nn745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES