Skip to main content
Ecology and Evolution logoLink to Ecology and Evolution
. 2015 Jan 20;5(3):781–798. doi: 10.1002/ece3.1400

Evolution of leaf warbler songs (Aves: Phylloscopidae)

Dieter Thomas Tietze 1,2,, Jochen Martens 3, Balduin S Fischer 1, Yue-Hua Sun 4, Annette Klussmann-Kolb 1,5, Martin Päckert 6
PMCID: PMC4328779  PMID: 25691998

Abstract

Songs in passerine birds are important for territory defense and mating. Speciation rates in oscine passerines are so high, due to cultural evolution, that this bird lineage makes up half of the extant bird species. Leaf warblers are a speciose Old-World passerine family of limited morphological differentiation, so that songs are even more important for species delimitation. We took 16 sonographic traits from song recordings of 80 leaf warbler taxa and correlated them with 15 potentially explanatory variables, pairwise, and in linear models. Based on a well-resolved molecular phylogeny of the same taxa, all pairwise correlations were corrected for relatedness with phylogenetically independent contrasts and phylogenetic generalized linear models were used. We found a phylogenetic signal for most song traits, but a strong one only for the duration of the longest and of the shortest element, which are presumably inherited instead of learned. Body size of a leaf warbler species is a constraint on song frequencies independent of phylogeny. At least in this study, habitat density had only marginal impact on song features, which even disappeared through phylogenetic correction. Maybe most leaf warblers avoid the deterioration through sound propagation in dense vegetation by singing from exposed perches. Latitudinal (and longitudinal) extension of the breeding ranges was correlated with most song features, especially verse duration (longer polewards and westwards) and complexity (lower polewards). Climate niche or expansion history might explain these correlations. The number of different element types per verse decreases with elevation, possibly due to fewer resources and congeneric species at higher elevations.

Keywords: Model of evolution, Phylloscopus, phylogenetic signal, Seicercus, song evolution

Introduction

Passerines sing in order to defend their territories and to advertise for mates (Catchpole and Slater 2008). The second reason implies that sexual selection might have a strong impact on the evolution of such vocal behavior (Price 2008). Nevertheless, species recognition must be maintained for both purposes. Songbirds learn their song from tutors (Baptista and Kroodsma 2001; Catchpole and Slater 2008), but it consists of innate elements (Catchpole and Slater 2008). Almost half of all bird species are passerines (Dickinson 2003), which is also due to the accelerated (cultural) evolution through learning and sexual selection (Thielcke 1970; Baptista and Trail 1992; Price 2008; Verzijden et al. 2012).

As bird song is such an important behavior, we must ask what drives the evolution of song traits (review in Wilkins et al. 2013). At the level of ontogeny, an interplay of genetic inheritance and social learning (Catchpole and Slater 2008) is assumed. Various environmental and organismic constraints act on both stages: Body size provides physical conditions for frequency range and speed of vocalisations (e.g., Wallschläger 1980; Ryan and Brenowitz 1985), while migratory behavior might enforce a trade-off with song performance (Read and Weary 1992). Acoustic properties of the habitat should necessitate adaptations to optimize the transmission of sound (e.g., Morton 1975; Ryan and Brenowitz 1985). Competition for acoustic niche space could limit the extent of such adaptations. Sexual selection could favor more complex songs, which on the other hand require a higher male investment. Obvious explanations might only reflect common ancestry so that neutral evolution needs to be disentangled from phylogenetically independent correlations. It is highly likely that more than one factor is responsible for a given trait, so that explanatory variables need to be incorporated in more complicated statistical models than just bivariate correlations.

Leaf warblers (Phylloscopidae sensu Alström et al. 2006b) are a large family of insectivorous passerines. The fact that external morphology differs only slightly among taxa emphasizes the importance of vocal communication in this clade (Alström et al. 2006a; Martens 2010). Leaf warblers live on wooden plants in Eurasia and Africa (with one species reaching high-latitude Nearctic); a maximum of 16 sympatric species can co-exist on a single Chinese mountain (Martens 2010; Fig. 1). Most species migrate seasonally (from seasonal elevational movements to long-distance migration between continents). Leaf warbler males vocalize a lot in the breeding period. Despite a remarkable interspecific variation in leaf warbler song, song characteristics are highly repeatable within species. All that makes phylloscopid warblers a good model to study vocal trait evolution.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Distribution map. Breeding distribution of leaf warbler (Phylloscopidae) species according to BirdLife International & NatureServe (2011); species richness increases from dark blue (1) via green and yellow to red (16).

Others have already tested various hypotheses regarding song evolution in leaf warblers (Badyaev and Leaf 1997; Mahler and Gil 2009), but these studies suffer from several weaknesses that we address here, as follows. The sample size was increased, and the phylogenetic data set improved (635 individuals of 80 taxa vs. 84 individuals of 30 species in Mahler and Gil 2009; almost fully resolved dated molecular tree). Any arbitrary selection of taxon sample may produce an outcome different from a fully sampled approach (Ackerly 2000; Pollock et al. 2002). Intraspecific genetic and acoustic variation was taken into account and was shown to be high and significant in several warbler species (e.g., P. [reguloides] represented by a single lineage in the previous study despite much higher differentiation up to species level; Päckert et al. 2009). A direct truly environmental measure of habitat was used (in contrast to an approximation by tarsus/beak ratio by Mahler and Gil 2009). Analyses that are more sophisticated were applied, disentangling historical and various ecological causes (linear models accounting for an interactive role played by explanatory variables, including models taking phylogenetic relationships into account).

The following hypotheses were tested:

Hypothesis 1: Song characters show significant phylogenetic signals, but are considerably more labile than morphological characters (Blomberg et al. 2003) and frequency song parameters are more conserved than temporal and structural ones (Mahler and Gil 2009).

Hypothesis 2: Body size is negatively correlated with frequency characteristics (Wallschläger 1980; Badyaev and Leaf 1997; Mahler and Gil 2009).

Hypothesis 3: Song characters (particularly frequency parameters) are strongly influenced by habitat characteristics (Badyaev and Leaf 1997; Rheindt et al. 2004).

Hypothesis 4: Song parameters vary significantly with geographic distribution, that is, with latitudinal and longitudinal extent of breeding areas (Mahler and Gil 2009) and with elevational preferences in the breeding season (Snell-Rood and Badyaev 2008).

Materials and Methods

Tree reconstruction

Several studies have used a modified leaf warbler phylogeny based on the data set by Johansson et al. (2007; including 55 taxa) for biogeographic reconstructions (Päckert et al. 2012), speciation rate analysis and ecological modeling (Price 2010). As a phylogenetic backbone, we used the three-marker data set (cytochrome b, 12S and myoglobin intron 2) from Päckert et al. (2012) including 69 taxa of Phylloscopidae and added original sequences for 13 taxa. Newly generated sequences were processed according to laboratory protocols given in Päckert et al. (2012; and references therein).

The total data set used for phylogenetic reconstructions comprised sequence data of 80 leaf warbler taxa compared with 30 taxa analyzed by Mahler and Gil (2009). GenBank sequences of Acrocephalus dumetorum were included in the analysis for hierarchical outgroup rooting.

The sequences for each gene were aligned by ClustalW using MEGA v5.1 (Tamura et al. 2011) and slightly adjusted by eye. All sequences used for the analysis were deposited at GenBank under the accession numbers provided in Table S4. The best-fit model for each locus was identified with the Akaike information criterion (AIC) implemented in MrModeltest v2.3 (Nylander 2004) in conjunction with PAUP* v4.0b10 (Swofford 2003; see Table S5). Phylogenetic relationships were reconstructed using Bayesian inference through BEAST v1.5.3 (Drummond and Rambaut 2007). In BEAST, the following settings were used: All three genes were treated as separate partitions with unlinked substitution and clock models. Substitution and heterogeneity models were set according to Table S5, and empirical base frequencies were used. Furthermore, cytochrome b was partitioned into three codon positions after clipping of the stop codon, and all parameters were unlinked. A relaxed uncorrelated log-normal clock was used with a birth–death process assumed as a tree prior. The reconstruction was for 10,000,000 generations. The log files were checked with Tracer v1.5 (Drummond and Rambaut 2007) in order to set the burn-in value. The BEAST trees were summarized with TreeAnnotator v1.5.3 using a burn-in value of 5000 and median node heights, and the final tree visualized in FigTree v1.3.1.

Song analysis

In the study group, song is usually composed of well-defined periods of singing, termed verses, which are separated from each other by pauses. In many species, individuals exhibit different verse variants called song types that may vary from 1 to 44. The variation among song types is discontinuous yet slight, following the same general species-typical song pattern. The specific set of song types varies among individuals while the sheer number of different individual song types, also known as the repertoire size, remains largely constant within taxa (cf. Fig. 2A–D; Martens 1980; Martens et al. 1999; Irwin 2000; Päckert et al. 2009; Ivanitskii et al. 2012).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Sonogram plate. Selection of sonograms highlighting variation and composition of leaf-warbler songs. Phylloscopus schwarzi a–d: four strophes of the same individual (pauses omitted) representing three different song types (Russia, Ussuri 1990, J. Martens); P. humei mandellii e: buzzing song pattern (China, Shaanxi 1997, J. Martens), f: call-like song pattern (China, Shaanxi 1997, J. Martens); P. forresti g: reeling song pattern (China, Gansu 2010, J. Martens); h: part of endless song pattern (China, Sichuan 2000, J. Martens); P. sibilatrix i: reeling song pattern (Germany, Saxony 2011, B. Fischer), j: call (Germany, Hesse 2011, B. Fischer), k: call-like song pattern (Germany, Saxony 2011, B. Fischer); P. collybita collybita l: (Germany, Saxony 2011, B. Fischer); P. collybita tristis m: (Russia, Novosibirsk 1986, J. Martens); P. tytleri n: (India, Kashmir 1976, J. Martens); P. trochilus o: (Germany, Hesse 2011, B. Fischer); P. borealoides p: (Japan, Hokkaido 1996, M. Päckert); P. calciatilis q: (Laos 2010, J. Martens); P. umbrovirens r: (Ethiopia, Oromia, B. Fischer); P. inornatus s: (Russia, Komi Republic 2006, A. Lindholm); P. magnirostris t: (China, Shaanxi 1997, J. Martens); P. plumbeitarsus u: (Russia, Ussuri 1996, M. Päckert).

Within a certain verse, further subdivisions can be made: The smallest unit of a verse is the element that is represented by a continuous line on the sonogram (Fig. 2). Element types differ from each other in structure and shape. Verses may be composed of fixed groups of elements, termed syllables or note groups. The composition and order of elements and syllables of a verse define the syntax. In some species, verses begin with a highly stereotyped motif, the introductory note (Martens 1980; Martens et al. 2004; Catchpole and Slater 2008, p. 9).

Seven leaf warbler species (nine taxa) exhibit two vastly different songs of distinct structural patterns. Divergence between distinct song patterns within a species equals the one found between songs of well-differentiated species, but does not result in prezygotic isolation. In most species concerned, males display songs of a rather stereotypical and invariable pattern and others of a more variable pattern including different song types alternately in the same behavioral context (e.g., continuous “endless song” vs. verse song in species of the P. proregulus group; Martens et al. 2004).

Almost all analyzed song recordings were taken from JM’s collection (for auditory impressions of the song of most taxa listen to Martens 2013), supplemented by recordings from commercial sound carriers, sound archives, and colleagues. For sonographic analysis, digitised recordings were converted to a sampling rate of 22.1 kHz and 16 bit. Measurements were performed manually on the sonograms using the software Avisoft-SASLab Lite (www.avisoft.com). The unit used for bioacoustic analysis was the verse. For taxa with low to medium repertoire sizes, a maximum of five verses per individual and five individuals per taxon was measured. To account for higher variation in taxa with large repertoires (>20 song types/individual), the number of both verses and individuals investigated was increased to a maximum of ten verses per male. Altogether, measurements of 3347 single verses from 635 individuals were used for analysis.

For any given verse, measurements of ten continuously varying song parameters were taken on the sonogram (Fig. S1). From the resulting data, six additional song parameters were derived. Song variables fall into two distinct categories: frequency and compositional parameters. The latter comprise temporal and structural parameters, which mutually depend upon each other. Precise definitions of all song parameters used for analysis are presented in Table 1. For each of the song parameters, taxon means were calculated from individual averages. Songs of the same species with different structural patterns were measured separately, and means were calculated for each of the two structurally different songs. However, in all nine taxa performing songs of two distinct patterns, only one of these patterns was used for analysis. As an example, the so called endless song of some species does not permit several timely song parameters to be measured. Therefore, the typical leaf warbler song pattern with clear-cut organization into verses was used for analysis for P. forresti, P. chloronotus, and P. yunnanensis (cf. Fig. 2G–H; Alström and Olsson 1990; Martens et al. 2004). Their close relative P. proregulus has only one song pattern, but distinct introductory notes delimit individual verses in its near-continuous song and allow for measurements of distinct verse units. In the remaining taxa, songs most similar to and putatively homologous to other Phylloscopus songs were analyzed, while those more similar to calls were omitted (P. humei, P. pulcher, P. sibilatrix, and P. subviridis; cf. Fig. 2E–F and I–K; Martens 1980; Irwin et al. 2001a). Variants in the song of P. coronatus are not considered to belong to different song patterns (cf. Martens 1980).

Table 1.

Song parameter definition and phylogenetic signal

Category Trait Unit Definition K P λ Model R label
Composition tges s Duration of verse (song period) from the beginning of the first to the end of the last element 0.426 0.001 0.874 λ tges
tmax s Duration of longest element 0.988 0.001 1.000 EB (BM) tmax
tmin s Duration of shortest element 0.932 0.001 0.998 BM (λ, EB) tmin
zel Number of distinct elements 0.553 0.001 0.862 OU (λ) zel
zel/tges s-1 Tempo defined as speed of delivery of elements (number of elements/s) 0.533 0.001 0.808 OU (λ) zeltges
zeltype Absolute element diversity defined as the number of unique element types 0.276 0.058 0.627 λ zeltype
Frequency fmax kHz Maximum frequency 0.428 0.001 0.800 λ fmax
fmin kHz Minimum frequency 0.299 0.020 0.877 λ fmin
fmean kHz Mean frequency ((fmin + fmax)/2) 0.371 0.001 0.966 λ fmean
▵f kHz Bandwidth, measured as the difference between maximum and minimum frequencies (fmax − fmin) 0.142 0.356 0.743 λ df
▵fmax kHz Maximum element bandwidth 0.407 0.001 0.850 λ dfmax
▵fmin kHz Minimum element bandwidth 0.374 0.001 0.923 λ dfmin
fmodend KHz Frequency gradient measured as the difference between maximum frequencies of first and last elements (fmaxend - fmax1) 0.128 0.511 0.355 λ (white) fmodend
Derived complexity1 Relative element dissimilarity as apparent from differences between maximum and minimum measures of bandwidth and duration according to the formula (▵fmax/▵fmin + tmax/tmin)/2 0.177 0.212 1.000 λ complexity1
complexity2 Relative element diversity measured as the fraction of unique element types (zeltype/zel) 0.646 0.001 0.977 λ complexity2
complexity3 Diversity-tempo index, combining relative element diversity and speed of element delivery according to the formula: complexity2 + zel/tges/30.268 s. Tempo component is adjusted to set the fastest tempo in the data set to 1.0 (P. borealis). 0.364 0.001 0.755 λ complexity3
PCall1 First principal component from an analysis of measures 1–4, 6–8, 11–13 0.569 0.001 0.986 λ HKstim1
PCall2 Second principal component from an analysis of measures 1–4, 6–8, 11–13 0.287 0.013 0.804 λ HKstim2
PCcomp1 First principal component from an analysis of measures 1–4, 6 0.719 0.001 0.994 λ HKzeit1
PCcomp2 Second principal component from an analysis of measures 1–4, 6 0.469 0.001 0.865 λ HKzeit2
PCfreq1 First principal component from an analysis of measures 7–8, 11–13 0.410 0.001 0.979 λ HKfreq1
PCfreq2 Second principal component from an analysis of measures 7–8, 11–13 0.199 0.126 0.850 λ HKfreq2
Explanatory length cm Body length from tip of bill to tip of tail 0.948 0.001 1.000 BM length
mass g Body mass 1.055 0.001 1.000 BM (EB) mass
migration Migratory behavior (see text) 0.386 0.002 0.511 OU migration
region Main biogeographic region of breeding range (see text) 0.659 0.001 0.993 λ region
latmax ° Maximal range extension to the North 0.318 0.001 0.588 λ lat_max
latmin ° Maximal range extension to the South 0.119 0.552 0.990 λ lat_min
latmean ° Mean latitude ((latmax–latmin)/2) 0.229 0.087 1.000 λ lat_mean
latequator ° Mean latitude from absolute values of the extremes 0.183 0.181 1.000 λ lat_equator
longmax ° Maximal range extension to the East 0.320 0.002 0.469 λ (OU) long_max
longmin ° Maximal range extension to the West 0.459 0.001 0.700 λ long_min
longmean ° Mean longitude ((longmax–longmin)/2) 0.396 0.001 0.642 λ long_mean
elemax m Highest elevation in the breeding season 0.385 0.001 0.663 OU ele_max
elemin m Lowest elevation in the breeding season 0.380 0.002 0.636 OU ele_min
elemean m Mean elevation ((elemax–elemin)/2) 0.432 0.001 0.783 OU ele_mean
habitat Habitat density (see text) 0.782 0.001 1.000 BM (OU) habitat

Precise definitions of all song parameters used for analysis and explanatory variables with phylogenetic signal (Blomberg’s K with P value, Pagel’s λ), estimated model of evolution (BM: Brownian motion, EB: early burst, OU: Ornstein–Uhlenbeck, λ: lambda; alternative models in parentheses, if ΔAICc < 2; for details see text) and R labels used in the Electronic Appendix. Temporal parameters were measured in seconds to three digits, frequency parameters in kilohertzes to three digits.

Explanatory variables

In order to correlate bioacoustic measures with morphological and ecological traits, data on body length and mass, migration, horizontal and elevational distribution as well as habitat were obtained from the literature for all taxa. Main source for the inference of all of the parameters mentioned above was Alström et al. (2006a). Further references were consulted to supplement missing data (indicated separately for each parameter). Data on mean total length (in centimeters) as measured from tip of bill to tip of tail (Svensson 1992) were complemented by Svensson et al. 2009. For recent taxonomic splits, data on the respective taxa under which they used to be combined were taken. Similarly, for missing subspecies, information for the whole species was used. Length of P. calciatilis was inferred from measurements published in Alström et al. (2010). For body mass (in grams), the mean value of the largest series of measurements for both sexes from Dunning (2008) was taken and complemented by data from Alström et al. (2006a). As before, if data on subspecies were missing, the species value was used. For further missing taxa, data from close relatives with similar size and proportions were used. Migration strategy of leaf warblers was classified into three discrete categories corresponding to the average amount of migrating behavior exhibited (data complemented by Alström et al. 2010, 2011): residents that are (largely) sedentary (score 0), partial migrants including altitudinal and short distance migrants (score 1) and genuine (long-distance) migrants with (usually) well-separated breeding and nonbreeding grounds (score 2). To classify the horizontal distribution of breeding grounds, two different approaches were pursued: bioregion and mean coordinates. Biogeographic regions allocated were (1) Palaearctic including Macaronesia, (2) Sino-Himalayas, (3) South-east Asia and (4) Afrotropic (according to classifications given in Päckert et al. 2012). Geographic coordinates of maximal extension of breeding areas (accurate to one degree) were inferred via Google Earth v6 from distribution maps and accounts given in relevant literature (Alström et al. 2006a; complemented by Irwin et al. 2001a; Olsson et al. 2005; Martens et al. 2008; Päckert et al. 2009; Alström et al. 2010, 2011; Rheindt 2010). Further data were retrieved from JM’s collection of sound recordings, specimens, blood samples, and tissue samples. Mean geographic coordinates were defined as the mean of the latitudinal and longitudinal distribution limits, respectively, (latmean = (latmax + latmin)/2); longmean = (longmax + longmin)/2). In addition, the mean distance from the equator (latequator; in degrees) was inferred from the mean latitude to better reflect an ecological gradient from tropical to temperate regions. Data on elevational distribution of breeding grounds (in meters above sea level) were compiled for minimum, maximum, and mean values (elemean = (elemax + elemin)/2; complemented by Vietinghoff-Scheel 1980; Glutz von Blotzheim and Bauer 1991; Clement and Helbig 1998; Alström et al. 2010, 2011; Päckert et al. 2012; and JM’s collection). Elevational extent of the breeding range of P. trivirgatus benguetensis was estimated from the distribution of appropriate habitat within its restricted range. Breeding habitat was classified into five discrete types from open to closed following Badyaev and Leaf (1997): (1) open with no or very sparse vegetation, (2) bushes and subalpine bushes, (3) intermediate between bushes and forest habitats, gardens, (4) coniferous, and (5) deciduous forests. Some species’ habitat requirements spanned more than one of the above-mentioned categories. In these cases, the habitat type most commonly occupied was used. As before, when data on subspecies were unavailable, species information was obtained (complemented by Gaston 1974; Alström et al. 2010, 2011).

Statistical analysis

Principal component analysis was conducted in R v3.0.2 (R Core Team 2013) with function prcomp with scaling for three sets of directly measured song parameters: frequency, composition, and both (Table 1). In the PCA with all measured song parameters, the first two components (PCAall1, PCAall2) had eigenvalues of 1.66 and 1.48, respectively, and together explained only 50% of total variance. PCAall1 was negatively loaded by element time parameters and PCAall2 with maximum frequency. The first two components on compositional parameters (PCAcomp1, PCAcomp2) accounted for 70% of total variance with eigenvalues of 1.51 and 1.12. PCAcomp1 was negatively loaded with element duration while PCAcomp2 was negatively loaded with element number and verse duration. Finally, the first component of PCA on frequency parameters (PCAfreq1) yielded an eigenvalue of 1.34 and singly made up 36% of total variance. It was positively loaded by maximum and to a lesser degree by minimum element bandwidth and maximum frequency. Note that PCAfreq2 is not considered due to a lack of phylogenetic signal. More information on the principal components can be found in the Supplementary Information (Tables S1S3).

Testing for phylogenetic signal was conducted for both song variables and explanatory variables. Following the guidelines set up by Blomberg et al. (2003), this was carried out in a two-step manner in R (package picante v1.6-1, Kembel et al. 2010): signal detection and quantification. First, it was tested whether the data deviates significantly from the basic assumption that character states are randomly distributed across the phylogenetic tree. If this was the case, the null hypothesis that characters evolved independently from their phylogenetic history was rejected. In a second step, the strength of phylogenetic signal was inferred using the K statistic (Blomberg et al. 2003). Blomberg’s K is a measure of signal strength where K = 0 means a random distribution (i.e., no signal, total phylogenetic independence) and K = 1 a character state distribution as expected under a Brownian motion model of evolution (i.e., strong phylogenetic signal). We also calculated Pagel’s lambda and tested each variable for the best evolutionary model given the phylogeny by choosing the model with the lowest AICc (sample size-corrected Akaike information criterion) value out of Brownian motion, Ornstein–Uhlenbeck, early-burst, lambda (Pagel 1999), and white-noise (nonphylogenetic) model (R package geiger v2.0.3, Harmon et al. 2008).

Bivariate correlations for all pairs of one song trait and one explanatory variable each were performed. As related taxa tend to resemble each other, the tip node data (i.e., measured in extant species) cannot a priori be assumed to represent independent data points. To address this problem, phylogenetically independent contrasts (PICs; Felsenstein 1985) were computed for each pair of variables (R package ape v3.0-11, Paradis et al. 2004). In addition to conventional bivariate correlations with the raw data set, a second correlation analysis was conducted based on these contrasts.

In order to account for multifactorial explanations for single song features, linear models were formulated in R and stepwise reduced from all to a minimum number of explanatory variables. Phylogenetic generalized linear models (pGLS; R package caper, Orme et al. 2012) were used to correct for phylogeny. Only the explanatory variables from the minimal corresponding linear model were fed into each pGLS, including those without significance. For example, the linear model for tges started with all potentially explanatory variables and was stepwise reduced by R to migration and lat_equator (< 0.001 for both), ele_max (< 0.05), ele_min (< 0.1), and habitat (> 0.1). All these five variables were used as explanatory variables in a pGLS, which returned only migration, lat_equator, and ele_min as significant components of the model with lat_equator having the highest significance (Table 2). For the remaining song traits, see Supplementary Data S2 and S3.

Table 2.

Correlation between variables

graphic file with name ece30005-0781-tbl-0002.jpg

Coefficients of correlation for all pairwise correlations. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. Values in bold stand for significant correlations in phylogenetically independent contrasts. Underlined values indicate significant contributions to minimal linear models. Values with a vertical line on the right side contributed significantly to the phylogenetic generalized linear model (pGLS). Explanatory traits with strongest contribution to the pGLS for a given song trait are marked in yellow. For full model output, see the Supplements.

Results

Phylogenetic tree

We obtained sequence data for all 80 taxa under consideration (Table S4). The BEAST tree (Fig. 3) was well resolved (48 nodes with full support). The Phylloscopidae were split into two major clades at an early stage. One clade with full node support contained all Seicercus species in two nonsister clades and all Phylloscopus species restricted to the tropics. The second clade consequently comprised Phylloscopus species with extant temperate distribution only. Species complexes with significant substructure were found in both major lineages. Some taxa with clearly different song had short divergence times (e.g., P. ogilviegranti subspecies, S. grammiceps/castaniceps, Chinese vs. Himalayan populations of P. pulcher).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Phylogeny of leaf-warblers (Phylloscopidae). Molecular phylogeny of leaf-warblers (Phylloscopidae) based on a 1900-bp alignment of three genes (for details see Table S5) reconstructed in BEAST (genes and codon positions partitioned, GTR + Γ + I model for cytochrome b and myoglobin, GTR + I model for 12S rDNA, 30 million generations).

Song features

The variability in phylloscopid song (Fig. 2) was reflected in an immense variance in song parameters among leaf warbler taxa (Supplementary Data S1): A complete verse in leaf warbler song lasted 1.88 ± 0.97 (0.46–5.31) s. Its longest element took 0.18 ± 0.20 (0.03–1.43) s, and its shortest element took 0.11 ± 0.20 (0.01–1.43) s. The verse consisted of 16.5 ± 17.7 (1.0–96.1) distinct elements. The speed was 8.4 ± 6.3 (0.7–30.3) elements per second. The number of unique element types was 3.7 ± 2.5 (1.0–14.3). A maximum frequency of 7.50 ± 1.18 (5.17–10.12) kHz was reached. The average minimum frequency was 3.11 ± 0.93 (1.30–6.85) kHz, and the average mean frequency was 5.30 ± 0.93 (3.41–8.33) kHz. The average verse covered a bandwidth of 4.44 ± 1.07 (1.52–7.39) kHz, the maximum element covered a bandwidth of 3.80 ± 1.00 (0.96–5.98) kHz, and the minimum element covered a bandwidth of 2.05 ± 0.90 (0.66–4.42) kHz. The frequency gradient from the first to the last element was −0.11 ± 0.80 (−2.90 to 2.47) kHz on average. The three complexity measures (defined in Table 1) yielded 2.17 ± 0.83 (1.00–4.83), 0.38 ± 0.27 (0.02–1.00), and 0.33 ± 0.12 (0.12–0.64), respectively.

The phylogenetic signal for song traits (Table 1) varied with Blomberg’s K between slightly over 0 and almost 1: A relatively strong signal (Blomberg’s K: 0.7–1.1) was only detected for the duration of the longest and of the shortest element – much larger than for any other song parameter. A medium signal strength (Blomberg’s K: 0.4–0.8) was found for all other compositional parameters but the element diversity, for the frequency parameters maximum frequency and maximum element bandwidth, and for complexity2. Element diversity and the remaining frequency parameters as well as complexity1 and complexity3 exhibited a weak signal (Blomberg’s K: 0.1–0.4) only and mostly failed the randomisation test (Table 1). Values for Pagel’s λ were closer to 1 except for complexity1 and significantly correlated with K values (Table 1). Almost all vocal traits evolved under a λ model, but element durations under a Brownian motion (or early-burst) model and element number and speed under the Ornstein–Uhlenbeck model.

Variation in explanatory traits

Leaf warbler attributes that could explain song features varied in variation breadth and degree of equipartition (Table S6): Leaf warblers are small passerine birds with 11.0 ± 0.8 (9.5–13.0) cm body length and 7.8 ± 1.7 (5.0–11.8) g body mass. Twelve resident (score 0), 31 partially migratory (score 1), and 37 long-distance migrants (score 2) led to an average migratoriness of 1.3 ± 0.7. The breeding ranges of 26 taxa were mainly in the Palaearctic including Macaronesia, of 39 taxa in the Sino-Himalayan region, of 13 taxa in South-east Asia, and of two taxa in tropical Africa (cf. Fig. 1). Breeding leaf warblers could be found between 34°S and 71°N and between 18°W and 41°W (across Eurasia and North America) with a diversity hotspot in Southwest China (Fig. 1). This resulted in a mean latitude of 31.0 ± 14.7 (−18 to 59) and a mean longitude of 90.0 ± 33.4 (−17 to 150). Leaf warblers were found breeding from sea level up to 4880 m on average. This resulted in a mean elevation of 1945.0 ± 868.5 (450–3965) m. Only three taxa were found in sparse vegetation, seven in bushes, 13 in bushes to forest, 13 in coniferous, and 44 in deciduous forests, resulting in average habitat density of 4.1 ± 1.2.

The phylogenetic signal for explanatory variables (Table 1) varied with Blomberg’s K between slightly over 0 and slightly over 1: A strong signal (Blomberg’s K: 0.7–1.1) was found in body length and mass as well as habitat. Mean elevation, maximal range extension to the West, and main biogeographic region exhibited medium signal strength (Blomberg’s K: 0.4–0.8). The remaining distributional parameters and migratoriness showed a weak signal (Blomberg’s K: 0.1–0.4), latmin, and latequator even failed the randomisation test (Table 1). Values for Pagel’s λ were either closer to 1 (body length and mass, region, latitudes except for maximum) or between 0.45 and 0.8 and correlated with K values (Table 1). Biogeographic region and horizontal distributional parameters evolved under a λ model, body length and mass and habitat under a Brownian motion model and migratory behavior and elevational distribution under the Ornstein–Uhlenbeck model.

Constraints on song parameters

We found a negative relationship between body size parameters with general frequency parameters of song across species that was backed by PICs for most bivariate correlations (fmax, fmin, fmean, and all PCA values that were strongly loaded by frequency parameters; Table 2). Body mass was furthermore positively associated with tempo (and diversity-tempo index, complexity3) with heavier species performing more complex songs with faster repetition rates (Table 2; again both correlations were backed by PICs). Minimal linear models supported these findings, because body length contributed significantly to the explanation of most frequency variables (and frequency-dependent variables) listed above. However, only three of those correlations between body length and frequency parameters contributed significantly to pGLSs (Table 2; strongest contribution to explanation of fmean, Fig. 4D). In contrast, body mass showed the strongest contribution to the explanation of tempo (and diversity-tempo index, complexity3) with pGLSs (Table 2; Fig. 4B).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Bivariate correlations. Selection of bivariate plots between explanatory and response variables: (A) duration of verse on distance from the equator, (B) song speed on body mass, (C) number of elements per verse on range extension to the East, (D) mean frequency on body length, (E) maximum element bandwidth on range extension to the South, (F) complexity1 on range extension to the North, (G) complexity2 on distance from the equator, (H) principal component 2 from the PC analysis of structural song traits on distance from the equator, (I) principal component 1 from the PC analysis of frequency parameters on range extension to the South. Regression lines were omitted, if direct correlations were insignificant. For trait definitions see Table 1, for coefficients of correlation and significance levels, see Table 2.

Spatial parameters of species distribution ranges (latitude, longitude, and elevation) correlated with a variety of song parameters; however, notably many of those correlations were significant only when corrected for phylogeny (Table 2).

All latitudinal variables were significantly correlated with song length (tges) and structural song variables (complexity2, PCcomp2), and most of these correlations were backed by PICs (Table 2). Mean latitude did not contribute significantly to linear models at all, and only two correlations of maximal range extension to the North (latmax) and song parameters (Δfmin and complexity1) contributed to pGLSs (Table 2; Fig. 4F). The two remaining latitudinal variables differed greatly in their contribution to pGLSs. Correlations of seven song parameters with maximum range extension to the South (latmin) contributed significantly to pGLSs, most of them being strongest contributions to the explanations of frequency or frequency-dependent variables (Table 2; Fig. 4E, I). In contrast, four correlations of mean range distance from the equator (latequator) with temporal structural song variables showed strongest contributions to pGLSs (Table 2). Generally, with increasing distance of breeding range from the equator to temperate regions, leaf warbler songs were longer and less complex across species (Table 2; Fig. 4A, G, H).

Of three longitudinal explanatory variables, only correlations between maximum range extension to the East (longmax) and five song parameters contributed significantly to pGLSs. Four of these correlations showed strongest contributions to pGLSs explaining element diversity (zeltype), minimum element bandwidth (Δfmin), frequency gradient (fmodend), and the second PC for frequency (Table 2; Fig. 4C).

Contribution of elevational extent of breeding ranges to linear models was less important. Only correlations of lower limits of elevational breeding ranges (elemin) with four song parameters contributed significantly to pGLSs (Table 2). Among these four, only element diversity (zeltype) showed a significant correlation with elemin that was backed by PICs (Table 2).

The three remaining explanatory variables did not contribute strongly to linear models: Surprisingly habitat did not show any correlations with song variables that would have been backed by PICs nor did habitat contribute to pGLSs for any song variable. Likewise, migratory behavior and biogeographic region of breeding did not show any significant correlation after phylogenetic contrasting (except migration and maximum frequency, fmax), but some correlations of these two variables with song parameters contributed to pGLSs (Table 2).

Discussion

Phylogenetic signal in song

The hypothesis that song characters show significant phylogenetic signals could be confirmed in general, although a few song parameters such as element diversity slightly and frequency bandwidth and frequency gradient clearly missed a significant deviation from random distribution across the phylogenetic tree.

The hypothesis that song characters are considerably more labile than morphological characters could be confirmed, too (average K values of 0.5 vs. 1.0 and λ values of 0.8 vs. 1.0 in Table 1, a much clearer contrast than in Mahler and Gil 2009). Only the length of the shortest and the longest element per verse approached K and λ values of 1 and evolved under a Brownian motion model that both indicates a high degree of trait conservation (such as for body length and mass). These findings are in accordance with previous studies documenting a generally low phylogenetic signal of passerine song traits, for example, in cardueline finches (Cardoso and Mota 2007; Cardoso et al. 2012; except presence of harmonics) or even an absence of phylogenetic signal in half of all parameters analyzed of wood warbler (Parulidae) flight calls (Farnsworth and Lovette 2008) and avian songs (or songlike vocalisations) in Amazon rainforest communities (Tobias et al. 2010).

The hypothesis that frequency song parameters are more conserved than temporal and structural ones (Mahler and Gil 2009) had to be rejected (average K values of 0.3 vs. 0.6 and λ values of 0.79 vs. 0.86 in Table 1). (Note that Mahler and Gil 2009 concluded that from differences in coefficients of variation and not from differences in K or λ values). This result is in accordance with the finding that temporal components were more congruent with phylogeny than frequency components in oropendolas (Icteridae, Psarocolius; Price and Lanyon 2002) and auklets (Alcidae; Seneviratne et al. 2012). One possible explanation for differences in strength of phylogenetic signal among song parameters is that some vocal traits have a strong genetic component (thus are rather innate) while the others are mainly learned. Such a relationship between signal strength and heritability has been demonstrated for syntax parameters and call-like song components in songs of goldcrests, Regulidae, and of treecreepers, Certhiidae (Päckert et al. 2003; Tietze et al. 2008).

In leaf warblers trait conservation of element duration might have a strong heritable component, too, at least with respect to the results of experiments with naïve birds reared in acoustic isolation showing that element length is largely innate (Schubert 1976; Thielcke 1983). Although these experiments were conducted with two leaf warbler species only (P. collybita, P. trochilus) and thus the results might not easily be generalized for the entire family, element parameters in these species seem to be the relevant song traits involved in species recognition (Schubert 1971; Helb 1973; Martens and Hänel 1981; Martens and Meincke 1989; Martens et al. 2004) and might therefore be more strongly conserved than other song traits.

Impact of body size on song frequency

The hypothesis that body size is negatively correlated with frequency characteristics could be confirmed. Nevertheless, not all such correlations were supported by phylogenetically independent contrasts. Body length significantly contributed to linear models explaining variation of maximum, minimum, and mean frequencies. At least one body measure significantly contributed to the corresponding pGLS. While neither of the two was the best predictor for the extreme frequencies, body length was for mean frequency.

As expected, measures of overall frequency are strongly correlated with body size in such a way that larger birds produce songs of lower pitch. This association seems to be a general phenomenon in avian vocalisations and has been demonstrated across a wide range of taxa (Wallschläger 1980; Ryan and Brenowitz 1985; Wiley 1991; Badyaev and Leaf 1997; Tubaro and Mahler 1998; Bertelli and Tubaro 2002; Seddon 2005; Snell-Rood and Badyaev 2008; Cardoso and Price 2010; Martin et al. 2011; Gonzalez-Voyer et al. 2013; Greig et al. 2013). A common explanation is that body size correlates either with the size of vibrating structures of the syringeal membrane which produce the sounds (Seneviratne et al. 2012) or with beak size and shape (Podos 2001; Podos et al. 2004; Derryberry et al. 2012). This prediction was recently shown to be valid even within species: In Purple-crowned Fairy-wrens (Malurus coronatus), larger males display significantly lower pitched songs; however, only the lower frequency bound of advertising songs was shown to be negatively correlated with body size (Hall et al. 2013). Also in Common Chiffchaffs (P. collybita), song frequencies decrease with male body size, and such slight individual differences of song frequency range were even shown to have a significant effect on the intensity of a male competitor’s territorial reaction (Linhart et al. 2012).

In addition to effects on overall frequency, body size of leaf warblers also explained measures of tempo in that heavier species sang faster and more complex. Similarly, in the Maluridae from Australia and New Guinea males of those species with larger testes sing shorter songs including more rapidly repeated and more variable notes (Greig et al. 2013). This is more difficult to interpret than frequency relationships, especially when considering beak size as a limiting factor of vocal traits – however, Mahler and Gil (2009) did not confirm that beak shape was a morphological constraint of leaf warbler song. But as the vocal apparatus and body size may not always be directly proportional to each other (Ryan and Brenowitz 1985), the impact of body dimensions on frequency or temporal song traits may be more intricate than generally thought. In fact, in other bird groups, the correlations among body size parameters and song tempo were shown to be the other way round: In Darwin’s finches, larger species produced slower-paced signals (Podos 2001) and in antbirds (Thamnophilidae) beak width was shown to be a strong predictor of song pace, such that species with broad bills performed songs with longer notes at a lower repetition rate (Seddon 2005). On the one hand, body mass was regarded as a morphological constraint of respiratory frequency and thus maximum note repetition rate (Suthers 2001). On the other hand, fast and complex songs require rapid and intricate muscle contractions of the vocal apparatus and hence are expected to be costly (Ballentine 2009). Likewise, a possible explanation for the negative correlation of body size and trill tempo in leaf warblers may be that heavier birds can produce songs of high energetic cost more easily. Thus, considering the conflicting results from bioacoustic studies, there is possibly no generalized rule on the effect of body dimensions on the pace of avian vocal signals, also taking into account that some studies found no significant correlation of body dimensions with any song parameter analyzed (Cardoso et al. 2012).

How habitat density constrains the song

Contrary to the results by Mahler and Gil (2009), the hypothesis that song characters (particularly frequency parameters) vary strongly with habitat characteristics had to be rejected for leaf warblers. We are well aware of singular adaptations to habitat such as P. magnirostris to mountain torrents (Martens and Geduldig 1988), but we here only considered vegetation density. The direct correlation of this habitat dimension with frequency bandwidth was highly significant, but due to phylogenetic relationships among the taxa. In fact, there is mixed evidence of habitat affecting vocal traits from previous studies. Mahler and Gil (2009) tested this hypothesis only indirectly using tarsus/beak ratio as an indicator of habitat use and found no effect after analysis of contrasts. In an earlier study, Badyaev and Leaf (1997) found for Phylloscopus and Hippolais warblers that temporal parameters are strongly correlated with habitat structure while frequency parameters are not. Rheindt et al. (2004; p. 385) confirmed an effect of habitat on frequency song parameters only if both traits were phylogenetically corrected (but after complex correction for autocorrelation the habitat effect was not detectable anymore!).

As a generalized rule, it has been proposed that higher frequencies (above 2 kHz) are more likely to be found in open habitat and that rapid repetition would be avoided in forests (Kroodsma and Miller 1982; chapter 5). Both assumptions were not supported by our data. In Amazonian bird communities, dense habitats seem to enhance songs of lower frequencies, higher pace and including a greater number of notes (lower pitch but higher temporal complexity; Tobias et al. 2010). Furthermore, from meta-analyses of 26 bioacoustic bird studies, there is no clear evidence that closed habitat means generally lower frequencies (Boncoraglio and Saino 2007).

Very plausibly, habitat characteristics other than density, not investigated here, might still be important and deliver potential ultimate causes for the correlations with distributional and vocal traits. For example, Medina and Francis (2012) showed that song complexity of Nearctic passerines increases with seasonality, particularly with precipitation (and temperature to a lesser extent) and is apparently not correlated with sexual selection indexes such as latitude, migration, and dichromatism. Most leaf warbler species display their songs from perches rather than from canopy cover or understorey like other species, and thus song characteristics might be less affected by habitat density. In that context, perch height was previously shown to have an effect on antbird songs in Neotropical rainforest communities with a trend of a minimization of signal degradation of songs toward lower frequency range and slower time structure near the ground (Nemeth et al. 2001).

Last, the frequency and temporal dimensions of song might undergo indirect evolutionary changes as a consequence of beak size changes due to ecological adaptation (Mahler and Gil 2009; Derryberry et al. 2012).

Song variation in space

The hypothesis that song parameters vary significantly with geographic distribution (latitudinal and longitudinal extent of breeding areas) could be confirmed. Distributional traits were the strongest contribution to linear models, explaining 8 of the 13 direct and 8 of the 9 derived song traits.

Song complexity (all three measures) decreased toward higher latitudes against the trend reported by Mahler and Gil (2009). Although it could be confirmed that species-specific mid-latitude is a labile trait (Price et al. 1997), it turned out to be a good predictor for various song features (even if extreme latitudes appear more influential) and this trait was used by Mahler and Gil (2009, p. 48) as a surrogate for the strength of sexual selection.

Prior to any explanation of these deviating results, two major differences between the latter study and ours have to be outlined. Most importantly, the data set by Mahler and Gil (2009) included almost exclusively Palaearctic species and boreal species in the Sino-Himalayas, but none of the subtropical and tropical species of the Afrotropic (n = 2 in our data set) and the Indomalaya (continental South-east Asia [n = 8] and the Greater Sundas [n = 5]) nor any member of genus Seicercus nested in the leaf warbler tree, also including several tropical species of the lower latitudes (n = 10).

Second, the northward increase of song complexity found by Mahler and Gil (2009) was inferred from a latitudinal gradient of their PC1 implying that northern Palaearctic species have larger repertoires, longer songs and more highly variable and complex syllables than species of lower latitudes. A comparable positive latitudinal gradient of song elaboration was found in cardueline finches of the Northern Hemisphere (with very similar loadings of PC1; Cardoso et al. 2012) and in the Maluridae of the Southern Hemisphere in such a way that “complexity may increase in association with more temperate or variable environments” (Greig et al. 2013). Furthermore, similar northward clinal variation of songs along population chains East and West of the Qinghai-Tibetan Plateau was demonstrated before for closest relatives of the Greenish Warbler clade (P. trochiloides and allies; Irwin 2000; Irwin et al. 2001b). In fact, one effect confirmed by our analyses is a significant northward increase of song duration, which was commonly interpreted as an effect of greater sexual selection at higher latitudes (Mahler and Gil 2009; Cardoso et al. 2012). As an example, in Willow Warblers (P. trochilus), long songs are an apparent indicator for male quality, because song length in that species was shown to be highly correlated with extra-pair paternity and paternity loss (while repertoire size was not; Gil et al. 2007).

In contrast to previous studies, song complexity indicated by both relative element dissimilarity and diversity of leaf warbler songs decreased northwards. In more detail, Greig et al. (2013) found the opposite latitudinal gradient for the same complexity measure (their “song versatility” is based on the same calculation as “element diversity” in our study), while complexity indices (PCs) used by Mahler and Gil (2009) and Cardoso et al. (2012) were more strongly influenced by syllable structure rather than by element dissimilarity and diversity (our study). Although we did not account for repertoire sizes as a measure of complexity in our study while Mahler and Gil (2009) did, by far the greatest individual male repertoires in the Phylloscopidae were documented from tropical Seicercus species, with no <44 distinct verse types per male (S. omeiensis; see Martens et al. 1999; Päckert et al. 2004). Thus, even considering repertoire sizes of tropical species, our results cast some doubt on a predicted greater selective pressure at temperate latitudes on male leaf warbler repertoires or on complexity of verse patterns.

Additionally, there is the tendency for more complex song further East in Eurasia where the diversity hotspot of leaf warblers is. This could be explained by some contrast reinforcement or acoustic niche partitioning within this bird family.

The hypothesis that song parameters vary significantly with elevational extent of breeding area could partially be confirmed. Elevational impact on verse length and element bandwidths seem to have historical reasons, but the positive impact on mean frequency and the negative impact on the number of element types appear to be causal since supported by PICs. That number of different element types per verse decreases with elevation maybe due to fewer resources (Price et al. 2014) and less competition by congeneric species at higher elevations (similar in Gonzalez-Voyer et al. 2013).

According to our analysis, element diversity and duration of leaf warbler songs decrease with elevation. Similar spatial variation of songs was found in cardueline finches toward longer and more elaborated songs with higher element diversity at lower elevations, and variation in strength of sexual selection along an elevational gradient was discussed as a trigger of song evolution in this passerine group (Snell-Rood and Badyaev 2008). However, elevation might be associated with a number of ecological factors affecting vocalisations that might not have been considered. For example, Afrotropical Green Hylias (Hylia prasina) sing at lower frequencies at higher elevations with reduced canopy cover and likewise avoid masking by insect sounds in these local habitats (Kirschel et al. 2009). In contrast, in Neotropical Grey-breasted Wood Wrens (Henicorhina leucophrys), local adaptation is assumed to have enhanced ecological speciation due to a link of morphological and acoustic variation: In this species, populations at high elevations have songs of a broad bandwidth including high-frequency notes (Caro et al. 2013). Consequently, there is not much of a clue for a generalized effect of elevation on avian vocal traits either (particularly for transcontinental comparisons) because local environmental conditions and ecological gradients affecting vocal variation might strongly differ among mountain systems. Additionally, traits of elevational distribution themselves evolved under a different model than almost all other explanatory traits.

Conclusion

Basic components of leaf warbler song evolve under a Brownian motion model, being possibly innate. Although body size is also phylogenetically constrained, it is strongly correlated with frequency even after phylogenetic correction. This indicates a causal correlation for physical reasons reported earlier. The habitat variable might still be too simplified, because it merely reflects increasing habitat density. The impact of habitat on leaf warbler song appears to be more complicated than could be tested in this approach. Habitat and geographical dimensions should be replaced by environmental-niche components in order to work out ecological–physiological causalities. This should be further combined with historical biogeography in order to trace song trait evolution more realistically.

Acknowledgments

For sample sources, see Päckert et al. (2012). DTT was funded by the German Research Foundation DFG (Ti 679/2-1). JM received annual grants from Feldbausch Foundation and Wagner Foundation at Fachbereich Biologie of Mainz University for fieldwork in Asia. C. Blume, S. Emmling, D. Garceag, and C. Waßmann digitised JM’s recordings. C. Tipp from the British Library sent additional recordings from the National Sound Archive and K.F. Jachmann contributed two recordings of P. examinandus. A. Rauh provided help in the DNA laboratory. Reviewers and the editors helped to improve the manuscript. Many cordial thanks are due to all friends, colleagues, and organizations mentioned. We acknowledge financial support by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Ruprecht-Karls-Universität Heidelberg within the funding programme Open Access Publishing.

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

Supporting Information

Data S1. Additional tables:

Tables S1–3. Principal component loadings for the principal component analyses for various, only structural and only frequency parameters, respectively (see Table 1).

Tables S4. Taxa treated in this study in alphabetical order with GenBank accession numbers.

Tables S5. Model settings as estimated with MrModeltest for the different gene regions analyzed.

Tables S6 Mean values of sonographic measurements and explanatory variables per taxon. For definitions and units of the latter, see Table 1.

ece30005-0781-sd1.xlsx (43.9KB, xlsx)

Data S2–S3. R output from the linear models and pGLSs, respectively.

ece30005-0781-sd2.txt (176.2KB, txt)
ece30005-0781-sd3.txt (25.8KB, txt)

Figure S1. Measurements taken from a sonogram. Note that fmax1 and fmaxend combine to describe fmodend (frequency gradient; cf. Table 1).

ece30005-0781-sd4.txt (315.7KB, txt)

References

  1. Ackerly DD. Taxon sampling, correlated evolution, and independent contrasts. Evolution. 2000;54:1480–1492. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00694.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alström P. Olsson U. Taxonomy of the Phylloscopus proregulus complex. Bull. Br. Ornithol. Club. 1990;110:38–43. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alström P, Clement P. Species accounts of Phylloscopus and Seicercus warblers. In: del Hoyo J, Elliot A, Sargatal J, editors; Madge SC, et al., editors. Handbook of the birds of the world. Vol. 11. Barcelona: Lynx; 2006a. pp. 646–679. [Google Scholar]
  4. Alström P, Ericson PG, Olsson U. Sundberg P. Phylogeny and classification of the avian superfamily Sylvioidea. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 2006b;38:381–397. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2005.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alström P, Davidson P, Duckworth JW, Eames JC, Le TT, Nguyen C, et al. Description of a new species of Phylloscopus warbler from Vietnam and Laos. The Ibis. 2010;152:145–168. [Google Scholar]
  6. Alström P, Saitoh T, Williams D, Nishiumi I, Shigeta Y, Ueda K, et al. The Arctic Warbler Phylloscopus borealis – three anciently separated cryptic species revealed. The Ibis. 2011;153:395–410. [Google Scholar]
  7. Badyaev AV. Leaf ES. Habitat associations of song characteristics in Phylloscopus and Hippolais warblers. Auk. 1997;114:40–46. [Google Scholar]
  8. Ballentine B. The ability to perform physically challenging songs predicts age and size in male swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana. Anim. Behav. 2009;77:973–978. [Google Scholar]
  9. Baptista LF. Kroodsma DE. Avian bioacoustics. In: del Hoyo J, Elliot A, Sargatal J, et al., editors; Handbook of the birds of the world. Vol. 6. Barcelona: Lynx; 2001. pp. 11–52. [Google Scholar]
  10. Baptista LF. Trail PW. The role of song in the evolution of passerine diversity. Syst. Biol. 1992;41:242–247. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bertelli S. Tubaro PL. Body mass and habitat correlates of song structure in a primitive group of birds. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2002;77:423–430. [Google Scholar]
  12. BirdLife International & NatureServe. Bird species distribution maps of the world. Cambridge: BirdLife International and NatureServe; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  13. Blomberg SP. Garland T., Jr Ives AR. Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 2003;57:717–745. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00285.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Boncoraglio G. Saino N. Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: a meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Funct. Ecol. 2007;21:134–142. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cardoso GC. Mota PG. Song diversification and complexity in canaries and seedeaters (Serinus spp.) Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2007;92:183–194. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cardoso GC. Price TD. Community convergence in bird song. Evol. Ecol. 2010;24:447–461. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cardoso GC, Hu Y. Mota PG. Birdsong, sexual selection, and the flawed taxonomy of canaries, goldfinches and allies. Anim. Behav. 2012;84:111–119. [Google Scholar]
  18. Caro LM, Caycedo-Rosales PC. Bowie R, Slabbekoorn H. Cadena CD. Ecological speciation along an elevational gradient in a tropical passerine bird? J. Evol. Biol. 2013;26:357–374. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Catchpole CK. Slater PJB. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge: University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  20. Clement P. Helbig AJ. Taxonomy and identification of chiffchaffs in the Western Palearctic. Br. Birds. 1998;91:361–376. [Google Scholar]
  21. Derryberry EP, Seddon N, Claramunt S, Tobias JA, Baker A, Aleixo A, et al. Correlated evolution of beak morphology and song in the neotropical woodcreeper radiation. Evolution. 2012;66:2784–2797. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01642.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dickinson EC, et al., editors. The Howard and Moore complete checklist of birds of the world. 3rd ed. London: Christopher Helm; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  23. Drummond AJ. Rambaut A. BEAST: Bayesian evolutionary analysis by sampling trees. BMC Evol. Biol. 2007;7:214. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-7-214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dunning JB. CRC handbook of avian body masses. 2nd ed. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  25. Farnsworth A. Lovette IJ. Phylogenetic and ecological effects on interspecific variation in structurally simple avian vocalizations. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2008;94:155–173. [Google Scholar]
  26. Felsenstein J. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am. Nat. 1985;125:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gaston AJ. Adaptation in the genus Phylloscopus. The Ibis. 1974;116:432–450. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gil D, Slater PJ. Graves JA. Extra-pair paternity and song characteristics in the willow warbler Phylloscopus trochilus. J. Avian Biol. 2007;38:291–297. [Google Scholar]
  29. Glutz von Blotzheim UN. Bauer KM. 1991. Wiesbaden Aula, and Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas. Vol. 12, part 2. Sylviidae.
  30. Gonzalez-Voyer A, den Tex R-J, Castelló A. Leonard JA. Evolution of acoustic and visual signals in Asian barbets. J. Evol. Biol. 2013;26:647–659. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Greig EI, Price JJ. Pruett-Jones S. Song evolution in Maluridae: influences of natural and sexual selection on acoustic structure. Emu. 2013;113:270–281. [Google Scholar]
  32. Hall ML, Kingma SA. Peters A. Male songbird indicates body size with low-pitched advertising songs. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e56717. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Harmon LJ, Weir JT, Brock CD, Glor RE. Challenger W. GEIGER: investigating evolutionary radiations. Bioinformatics. 2008;24:129–131. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btm538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Helb H-W. Analyse der artisolierenden Parameter im Gesang des Fitis (Phylloscopus t. trochilus) mit Untersuchungen zur Objektivierung der Methode. J. Ornithol. 1973;114:145–206. [Google Scholar]
  35. Irwin DE. Song variation in an avian ring species. Evolution. 2000;54:998–1010. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2000.tb00099.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Irwin DE, Alström P, Olsson U. Benowitz-Fredericks ZM. Cryptic species in the genus Phylloscopus (Old World leaf warblers) The Ibis. 2001a;143:233–247. [Google Scholar]
  37. Irwin DE, Bensch S. Price TD. Speciation in a ring. Nature. 2001b;409:333–337. doi: 10.1038/35053059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ivanitskii VV, Marova IM. Malykh IM. Between order and chaos: Contrasting syntax in the advertising song of dusky or Warblers (Phylloscopus fuscatus) and Radde’s (Ph. schwarzi) warblers. J. Ornithol. 2012;153:337–346. [Google Scholar]
  39. Johansson US, Alström P, Olsson U, Ericson PG, Sundberg P. Price TD. Build-up of the Himalayan avifauna through immigration: a biogeographical analysis of the Phylloscopus and Seicercus warblers. Evolution. 2007;61:324–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00024.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kembel SW, Cowan PD, Helmus MR, Cornwell WK, Morlon H, Ackerly DD, et al. Picante: R tools for integrating phylogenies and ecology. Bioinformatics. 2010;26:1463–1464. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kirschel AlexanderNG, Blumstein DT, Cohen RE, Buermann W, Smith TB. Slabbekoorn H. Birdsong tuned to the environment: green hylia song varies with elevation, tree cover, and noise. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2009;20:1089–1095. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kroodsma DE. In: Acoustic communication in birds. Miller EH, et al., editors. New York: Academic Press; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  43. Linhart P, Slabbekoorn H. Fuchs R. The communicative significance of song frequency and song length in territorial chiffchaffs. Behav. Ecol. 2012;23:1338–1347. [Google Scholar]
  44. Mahler B. Gil D. The evolution of song in the Phylloscopus leaf warblers (Aves: Sylviidae): a tale of sexual selection, habitat adaptation, and morphological constraints. Adv. Study Behav. 2009;40:35–66. [Google Scholar]
  45. Martens J. Lautäußerungen, verwandtschaftliche Beziehungen und Verbreitungsgeschichte asiatischer Laubsänger (Phylloscopus) Berlin & Hamburg: Paul Parey; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  46. Martens J. Systematic notes on Asian birds: 72. A preliminary review of the leaf warbler genera Phylloscopus and Seicercus. Br. Ornithol. Club Occ. Publ. 2010;5:41–116. [Google Scholar]
  47. Martens J. 2013. Vocalizations of leaf-warblers and spectacled warblers (Phylloscopus and Seicercus). Double audio CD, no. SX 419 726, syrinx Tonstudio Berlin [ http://www.syrinx-ton.de ]
  48. Martens J. Geduldig G. Acoustic adaptations of birds living close to Himalayan torrents. In: van den Elzen R, et al., editors; Current topics in avian biology. Bonn: Verl. d. Deutschen Ornithologen-Ges; 1988. pp. 123–131. Proceedings of the international centennial meeting of the Deutsche Ornithologen-Gesellschaft. [Google Scholar]
  49. Martens J. Hänel S. Gesangsformen und Verwandtschaft der asiatischen Zilpzalpe Phylloscopus collybita abietinus und Ph. c. sindianus. J. Ornithol. 1981;122:403–427. [Google Scholar]
  50. Martens J. Meincke C. Der sibirische Zilpzalp (Phylloscopus collybita tristis): Gesang und Reaktion einer mitteleuropäischen Population im Freilandversuch. J. Ornithol. 1989;130:455–473. [Google Scholar]
  51. Martens J, Eck S, Päckert M. Sun Y-H. The Golden-spectacled Warbler Seicercus burkii – a species swarm (Aves: Passeriformes: Sylviidae). Part 1. Zool. Abh. (Dresd.) 1999;50:281–327. [Google Scholar]
  52. Martens J, Tietze DT, Eck S. Veith M. Radiation and species limits in the Asian Pallas’s warbler complex (Phylloscopus proregulus s.l.) J. Ornithol. 2004;145:206–222. [Google Scholar]
  53. Martens J, Sun Y-H. Päckert M. Intraspecific differentiation of Sino-Himalayan bush-dwelling Phylloscopus leaf warblers, with description of two new taxa (P. fuscatus P. fuligiventer P. affinis P. armandii P. subaffinis. Vert. Zool. 2008;58:233–265. [Google Scholar]
  54. Martin JP, Doucet SM, Knox RC. Mennill DJ. Body size correlates negatively with the frequency of distress calls and songs of Neotropical birds. J. Field Ornithol. 2011;82:259–268. [Google Scholar]
  55. Medina I. Francis CD. Environmental variability and acoustic signals: a multi-level approach in songbirds. Biol. Lett. 2012;8:928–931. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Morton ES. Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am. Nat. 1975;109:17–34. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nemeth E, Winkler H. Dabelsteen T. Differential degradation of antbird songs in a Neotropical rainforest: Adaptation to perch height? J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2001;110:3263–3274. doi: 10.1121/1.1420385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nylander JAA. MrModeltest v2. Program distributed by the author. Uppsala: Evolutionary Biology Centre, Uppsala University; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  59. Olsson U, Alström P, Ericson PG. Sundberg P. Non-monophyletic taxa and cryptic species - evidence from a molecular phylogeny of leaf-warblers (Phylloscopus, Aves) Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 2005;36:261–276. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2005.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Orme D, Freckleton R, Thomas G, Petzoldt T, Fritz S, Isaac N, et al. 2012. caper: Comparative Analyses of Phylogenetics and Evolution in R. R package version 0.5 http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=caper.
  61. Päckert M, Martens J, Kosuch J, Nazarenko AA. Veith M. Phylogenetic signal in the song of crests and kinglets (Aves: Regulus. Evolution. 2003;57:616–629. doi: 10.1554/0014-3820(2003)057[0616:PSITSO]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Päckert M, Martens J, Sun Y-H. Veith M. The radiation of the Seicercus burkii complex and its congeners (Aves: Sylviidae): molecular genetics and bioacoustics. Org. Divers. Evol. 2004;4:341–364. [Google Scholar]
  63. Päckert M, Blume C, Sun Y-H, Wei L. Martens J. Acoustic differentiation reflects mitochondrial lineages in Blyth’s leaf warbler and white-tailed leaf warbler complexes (Aves: Phylloscopus reguloides Phylloscopus davisoni. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 2009;96:584–600. [Google Scholar]
  64. Päckert M, Martens J, Sun Y-H, Severinghaus LL, Nazarenko AA, Ji T, et al. Horizontal and elevational phylogeographic patterns of Himalayan and Southeast Asian forest passerines (Aves: Passeriformes) J. Biogeogr. 2012;39:556–573. [Google Scholar]
  65. Pagel M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature. 1999;401:877–884. doi: 10.1038/44766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Paradis E, Claude J. Strimmer K. APE: Analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics. 2004;20:289–290. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Podos J. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s finches. Nature. 2001;409:185–188. doi: 10.1038/35051570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Podos J, Southall JA. Rossi-Santos MR. Vocal mechanics in Darwin’s finches: correlation of beak gape and song frequency. J. Exp. Biol. 2004;207:607–619. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pollock DD, Zwickl DJ, McGuire JA. Hillis DM. Increased taxon sampling is advantageous for phylogenetic inference. Syst. Biol. 2002;51:664–671. doi: 10.1080/10635150290102357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Price T. Speciation in birds. USA: Roberts and Company, Greenwood Village, CO; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  71. Price TD. The roles of time and ecology in the continental radiation of the Old World leaf warblers (Phylloscopus and Seicercus. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2010;365:1749–1762. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Price JJ. Lanyon SM. Reconstructing the evolution of complex bird song in the oropendolas. Evolution. 2002;56:1514–1529. doi: 10.1554/0014-3820(2002)056[1514:RTEOCB]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Price TD, Helbig AJ. Richman AD. Evolution of breeding distributions in the Old World leaf warblers (genus Phylloscopus. Evolution. 1997;51:552–561. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02442.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Price TD, Hooper DM, Buchanan CD, Johansson US, Tietze DT, Alström P, et al. Niche filling slows the diversification of Himalayan songbirds. Nature. 2014;509:222–225. doi: 10.1038/nature13272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. http://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  76. Read AF. Weary DM. The evolution of bird song: comparative analyses. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B. 1992;338:165–187. [Google Scholar]
  77. Rheindt FE. New biogeographic records for the avifauna of Taliabu (Sula Islands, Indonesia), with the preliminary documentation of two previously undiscovered taxa. Bull. Br. Ornithol. Club. 2010;130:33–51. [Google Scholar]
  78. Rheindt FE, Grafe TU. Abouheif E. Rapidly evolving traits and the comparative method: how important is testing for phylogenetic signal? Evol. Ecol. Res. 2004;6:377–396. [Google Scholar]
  79. Ryan MJ. Brenowitz EA. The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am. Nat. 1985;126:87–100. [Google Scholar]
  80. Schubert G. Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die artkennzeichnenden Parameter im Gesang des Zilpzalps, Phylloscopus c. collybita (Vieillot) Behaviour. 1971;38:289–314. [Google Scholar]
  81. Schubert M. Das akustische Repertoire des Fitislaubsängers (Phylloscopus t. trochilus) und seine erblichen und durch Lernen erworbenen Bestandteile. Beitr. Vogelkd. 1976;22:167–200. [Google Scholar]
  82. Seddon N. Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in Neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution. 2005;59:200–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Seneviratne SS, Jones IL. Carr SM. Patterns of vocal divergence in a group of non-oscine birds (auklets; Alcidae, Charadriiformes) Evol. Ecol. Res. 2012;14:95–112. [Google Scholar]
  84. Snell-Rood EC. Badyaev AV. Ecological gradient of sexual selection: elevation and song elaboration in finches. Oecologia. 2008;157:545–551. doi: 10.1007/s00442-008-1092-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Suthers RA. Peripheral vocal mechanisms in birds: Are songbirds special? J. Morph. 2001;248:289–290. [Google Scholar]
  86. Svensson L. Identification guide to European passerines. 4th ed. Stockholm: British Trust of Ornithology; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  87. Svensson L, Mullarney K. Zetterström D. Collins bird guide. 2nd ed. London: HarperCollins; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  88. Swofford DL. PAUP*. Phylogenetic analysis using parsimony (*and other methods), vers. 4. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  89. Tamura K, Peterson D, Peterson N, Stecher G, Nei M. Kumar S. MEGA5: Molecular Evolutionary Genetics Analysis Using Maximum Likelihood, Evolutionary Distance, and Maximum Parsimony Methods. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2011;28:2731–2739. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msr121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Thielcke G. Lernen von Gesang als möglicher Schrittmacher der Evolution. Z. Zool. Syst. Evolutionsforsch. 1970;8:309–320. [Google Scholar]
  91. Thielcke G. Entstanden Dialekte des Zilpzalps Phylloscopus collybita durch Lernentzug? J. Ornithol. 1983;124:333–368. [Google Scholar]
  92. Tietze DT, Martens J, Sun Y-H. Päckert M. Evolutionary history of treecreeper vocalisations (Aves: Certhia. Org. Divers. Evol. 2008;8:305–324. [Google Scholar]
  93. Tobias JA, Aben J, Brumfield RT, Derryberry EP, Halfwerk W, Slabbekoorn H, et al. Song divergence by sensory drive in Amazonian birds. Evolution. 2010;64:2820–2839. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01067.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tubaro PL. Mahler B. Acoustic frequencies and body mass in new world doves. Condor. 1998;100:54–61. [Google Scholar]
  95. Verzijden MN, ten Cate C, Servedio MR, Kozak GM, Boughman JW. Svensson EI. The impact of learning on sexual selection and speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2012;27:511–519. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vietinghoff-Scheel EV. Phylloscopus ijimae (Stejneger) Atlas Verbr. palaearkt. Vögel. 1980;8:1–3. [Google Scholar]
  97. Wallschläger D. Correlation of song frequency and body weight in passerine birds. Experientia. 1980;36:412. [Google Scholar]
  98. Wiley RH. Associations of song properties with habitats for territorial oscine birds of eastern North America. Am. Nat. 1991;138:973–993. [Google Scholar]
  99. Wilkins MR, Seddon N. Safran RJ. Evolutionary divergence in acoustic signals: causes and consequences. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2013;28:156–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data S1. Additional tables:

Tables S1–3. Principal component loadings for the principal component analyses for various, only structural and only frequency parameters, respectively (see Table 1).

Tables S4. Taxa treated in this study in alphabetical order with GenBank accession numbers.

Tables S5. Model settings as estimated with MrModeltest for the different gene regions analyzed.

Tables S6 Mean values of sonographic measurements and explanatory variables per taxon. For definitions and units of the latter, see Table 1.

ece30005-0781-sd1.xlsx (43.9KB, xlsx)

Data S2–S3. R output from the linear models and pGLSs, respectively.

ece30005-0781-sd2.txt (176.2KB, txt)
ece30005-0781-sd3.txt (25.8KB, txt)

Figure S1. Measurements taken from a sonogram. Note that fmax1 and fmaxend combine to describe fmodend (frequency gradient; cf. Table 1).

ece30005-0781-sd4.txt (315.7KB, txt)

Articles from Ecology and Evolution are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES