Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Feb 14.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Brain Sci. 2012 Jun;35(3):167–168. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11001762

What’s in a baby-cry? Locationist and constructionist frameworks in parental brain responses

James E Swain 1, S Shaun Ho 1
PMCID: PMC4329017  NIHMSID: NIHMS659847  PMID: 22617675

Abstract

Parental brain responses to baby stimuli constitute a unique model to study brain-basis frameworks of emotion. Results for baby-cry and picture stimuli may fit with both locationist and psychological constructionist hypotheses. Furthermore, the utility of either model may depend on postpartum timing and relationship. Endocrine effects may also be critical for accurate models to assess mental health risk and treatment.


To differ with Lindquist et al., perhaps both locationist and constructionist frameworks for the brain basis of parental emotion have utility, depending on stimulus parameters, timing, and relationship. For example, consider the baby-cry, a single primal signal that for an infant, not capable of complex interactive social signaling, must serve to attract the attention of new parents at the risk of adverse parental responses or drawing predators (Soltis 2004; Swain et al. 2004). Thus far, there is evidence that the parental brain basis of baby-cry-elicited emotions may fit with locationism; involving a simple set of circuits, including the amygdala and insula. Several groups have been studying the brain basis of maternal response to baby-cry and related behaviors within a relatively locationist framework (Swain et al. 2007). Initially, the thalamocingulate circuit for emotion response and regulation in mammals was supported (Lorberbaum et al. 2002; MacLean 1990), but several other brain regions, including parts of the amygdala, insula, and striatum/nucleus accumbens (NA) have also been shown to respond to baby-cry (Swain et al. 2011). Responses in the medial frontal cortex and striatum also predict parental mood and anxiety (Swain et al. 2008).

However, as the infant develops nuanced social signals in the context of a growing attachment, psychological constructionist frameworks to understand parental brain responses to baby stimuli, in accordance with Lindquist et al., may be more appropriate, in which emotions can be elicited that are intimately tied to prior experiences and conceptualization. For example, early life events may influence current brain activity in cortical regions, that have not been the subject of rodent literature. Some such connections have recently been discussed with respect to maternal sensitivity. (For thorough reviews, see Barrett & Fleming 2011; Swain 2011). In support of such early-life effects on complex brain functions, a recent study has shown that maternal brain structure and functional responses to the mother’s own baby’s crying in the early postpartum period varied according to mothers’ perceived maternal care quality in their own childhood in accord with a constructionist framework (Kim et al. 2010b). In this study, mothers who reported having received greater maternal care in childhood showed higher gray matter density, in a range of higher cortical and executive function areas, including the insula, superior and middle frontal gyri, orbital gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and fusiform gyrus.

Strikingly, some of these morphological and functional changes in the maternal brain are also associated with the concurrent conceptualization of positive maternal thoughts toward their babies (Kim et al. 2010a). In this first prospective longitudinal study, gray matter volume increased over the first few months postpartum (from 2–4 weeks to 3–4 months) in the insula, pre-frontal cortex, parietal lobes, and midbrain areas. Further work is required to elaborate the constructionist framework of relation between concurrent positive thoughts about one’s baby and increased gray matter volume in multiple core affect regions of the hypothalamus, substantia nigra, and amygdala, or to support locationist approaches for certain experimental paradigms (Kim et al. 2010a).

In evaluating locationist versus constructionist frameworks of the brain basis of emotion, a meta-analysis of brain imaging studies may be informed by the critical contributions of certain hormones just beginning to be included in such studies. For example, we contend that multiple hormonal systems related to parental motivation, including oxytocin, must be considered in frameworks of the brain basis of emotion. The neurohormone oxytocin, for example, is one of the major factors that accounts for variations in regulating parental emotions during parent–infant interactions (Feldman et al. 2010). Another aspect of a new mother’s emotion regulation is her recent mode of delivery, such that vaginal versus cesarean deliveries are associated with higher oxytocin (Marchini et al. 1988). Consistent with this, mothers who have had vaginal deliveries show greater brain responses to baby-cries in the insula, striatum, and anterior cingulate cortex (Swain et al. 2008) than do mothers who have had cesarean deliveries. Furthermore, regardless of delivery type, mothers known to show higher oxytocin during breastfeeding (Nissen et al. 1996), also have higher brain responses to their own baby’s crying than do formula-feeding mothers, in the insula, striatum, amygdala, and superior frontal gyrus (Kim et al. 2011). Although requiring replication, these experiments support a constructionist framework that includes hormone levels – in addition to maternal brain responses in the cingulate, striatum, and hypothalamus when shown baby pictures, some of which responses vary with individual differences in oxytocin and concurrent attachment (Strathearn et al. 2009).

Indeed, the key dimension of hormone responses in developing a comprehensive framework for understanding the brain basis of emotions is underlined by a causal relationship between oxytocin and women’s brain responses, in two recent studies. First, in a randomized control trial (Riem et al. 2011), experimentally elevated oxytocin versus placebo resulted in increased responses to the cries of unrelated babies, in the inferior frontal gyrus and insula, and decreased responses in the right amygdala. This study shows that oxytocin may modulate maternal behaviors, that is, enhance positive motivation by sensitizing care-related insula activity and reduce negative motivation by desensitizing anxiety-related amygdala responses. Second, in a study by Naber et al. (2010), administration of oxytocin increased sensitive parental emotions and behaviors, assessed by videotaped play session. In fact, infant pictures by themselves have been shown to exert similar modulation of maternal brains (Bartels & Zeki 2004; Strathearn et al. 2008).

The use of baby-cry to stimulate emotions in parents may present some challenges to the psychological constructionist framework, and require locationism for certain paradigms. Perhaps then also, locationism may still be useful in understanding the effects of interventions – such as for complex multi-system and time-delayed effects. Indeed, some recent studies interpret brain imaging data on basic face responses (Canli et al. 2005) or intervention effects in emotion response circuits according to a locationist model, whether using structural or functional approaches (Chua et al. 2011; Peterson & Weissman 2011). Ultimately, more research, probably including endocrine parameters, is needed to clarify the utility of locationist versus constructionist frameworks according to different paradigms.

Acknowledgments

James E. Swain is supported by a grant from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, the Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation. Both Dr. Swain and Dr. Ho are supported by grants from the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (NIMHD) grant no. IRC2MD004767-01, and the and the Michigan Institute for Clinical Health Research, grant no. UL1RR024986.

Contributor Information

James E. Swain, Email: jamesswa@med.umich.edu.

S. Shaun Ho, Email: hosh@med.umich.edu.

References

  1. Barclay L, Everitt L, Rogan F, Schmied V, Wyllie A. Becoming a mother--an analysis of women’s experience of early motherhood. J Adv Nurs. 1997;25(4):719–728. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.1997.t01-1-1997025719.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barrett J, Fleming AS. Annual Research Review: All mothers are not created equal: neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(4):368–397. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bowlby J. Attachment theory and its therapeutic implications. Adolesc Psychiatry. 1978;6:5–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brown SL, Brown RM, Preston S. A model of human caregiving motivation. In: Brown SL, Brown RM, Penner L, editors. Moving Beyond Self Interest: Perspectives from Evolutionary Biology, Neuroscience, and the Social Sciences. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  5. Buss C, Lord C, Wadiwalla M, Hellhammer DH, Lupien SJ, Meaney MJ, et al. Maternal care modulates the relationship between prenatal risk and hippocampal volume in women but not in men. J Neurosci. 2007;27(10):2592–2595. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cassidy J, Shaver PR. Handbook of Attachment. Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  7. Champagne FA. Epigenetic influence of social experiences across the lifespan. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52(4):299–311. doi: 10.1002/dev.20436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chong S, Vickaryous N, Ashe A, Zamudio N, Youngson N, Hemley S, et al. Modifiers of epigenetic reprogramming show paternal effects in the mouse. Nat Genet. 2007;39(5):614–622. doi: 10.1038/ng2031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:175–199. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dawson G, Ashman SB, Carver LJ. The role of early experience in shaping behavioral and brain development and its implications for social policy. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12(4):695–712. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400004089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dietz DM, Laplant Q, Watts EL, Hodes GE, Russo SJ, Feng J, et al. Paternal transmission of stress-induced pathologies. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(5):408–414. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dietz DM, Nestler EJ. From father to offspring: paternal transmission of depressive-like behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(1):311–312. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans GW. A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(5):924–933. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(3–4):329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Francis DD, Diorio J, Liu D, Meaney MJ. Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science. 1999;286(5442):1155–1158. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5442.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gianaros PJ, Horenstein JA, Cohen S, Matthews KA, Brown SM, Flory JD, et al. Perigenual anterior cingulate morphology covaries with perceived social standing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(3):161–173. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gianaros PJ, Horenstein JA, Hariri AR, Sheu LK, Manuck SB, Matthews KA, et al. Potential neural embedding of parental social standing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3(2):91–96. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Goodman JH. Becoming an Involved Father of an Infant. Jognn-Journal of Obstetric Gynecologic and Neonatal Nursing. 2002;34(2):190–200. doi: 10.1177/0884217505274581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(9):651–659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kim P, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Wang X, Swain JE. The plasticity of human maternal brain: longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124(5):695–700. doi: 10.1037/a0020884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kim P, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Newman MA, Feldman R, Swain JE. Perceived quality of maternal care in childhood and structure and function of mothers’ brain. Dev Sci. 2010;13(4):662–673. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00923.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Korosi A, Baram TZ. Plasticity of the stress response early in life: mechanisms and significance. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52(7):661–670. doi: 10.1002/dev.20490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186:190–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am Psychol. 1998;53(2):185–204. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Meaney MJ. Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:1161–1192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Mercer RT. The process of maternal role attainment over the first year. Nurs Res. 1985;34(4):198–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Muscatell KA, Morelli SA, Falk EB, Way BM, Pfeifer JH, Galinsky AD, et al. Social status modulates neural activity in the mentalizing network. Neuroimage. 2012;60(3):1771–1777. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. NICHD Early Care Research Network. Infant-mother attachment classification: risk and protection in relation to changing maternal caregiving quality. Dev Psychol. 2006;42(1):38–58. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Swain JE. The human parental brain: in vivo neuroimaging. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35(5):1242–1254. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Swain JE, Konrath S, Brown S, Finegood ELB-A, Ho SS. Parenting and beyond: common neurocircuits underlying parental and altruistic caregiving. Parenting, Science and Practice. 2012 doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.680409. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Swain JE, Leckman JF, Mayes LC, Feldman R, Hoyt E, Kang H, et al. Functional Brain Activations of Parents Listening to Their Own Baby-Cry Change over the Early Postpartum. Developmental Psychobiology. 2012 in submission. [Google Scholar]
  32. Taylor SE, Eisenberger NI, Saxbe D, Lehman BJ, Lieberman MD. Neural responses to emotional stimuli are associated with childhood family stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(3):296–301. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Tomoda A, Suzuki H, Rabi K, Sheu YS, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Reduced prefrontal cortical gray matter volume in young adults exposed to harsh corporal punishment. Neuroimage. 2009;47(Suppl 2):T66–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Woon FL, Hedges DW. Hippocampal and amygdala volumes in children and adults with childhood maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Hippocampus. 2008;18(8):729–736. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES