Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014 Aug 17;31:33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.024

More than spikes: Common oscillatory mechanisms for content specific neural representations during perception and memory

Andrew J Watrous 1, Juergen Fell 1, Arne D Ekstrom 3,4, Nikolai Axmacher 1,2
PMCID: PMC4329113  NIHMSID: NIHMS625176  PMID: 25129044

Abstract

Although previous research into the mechanisms underlying sensory and episodic representations has primarily focused on changes in neural firing rate, more recent evidence suggests that neural oscillations also contribute to these representations. Here, we argue that multiplexed oscillatory power and phase contribute to neural representations at the mesoscopic scale, complementary to neuronal firing. Reviewing recent studies which used oscillatory activity to decipher content-specific neural representations, we identify oscillatory mechanisms common to both sensory and episodic memory representations and incorporate these into a model of episodic encoding and retrieval. This model advances the idea that oscillations provide a reference frame for phase-coded item representations during memory encoding and that shifts in oscillatory frequency and phase coordinate ensemble activity during memory retrieval.

Keywords: neural oscillations, phase coding, episodic memory, content specific representation

Introduction

Reliable, flexible representations of the external world are crucial for encoding sensory perceptions into memories. Decades of research have documented changes in neural firing rate to varied and unique (“content specific”) environmental features. Beyond studies of single neuron firing rates, more recent work has investigated how the synchronized activity of many neurons, which results in oscillations of the local field potential (LFP) and the electroencephalogram (EEG), contributes to neural representation. Local and inter-areal synchronization supports dynamic coordination of incoming sensory information [1,2], inter-areal interactions [3,4], and synaptic plasticity [5,6], each of which are integral to both perception and memory. Thus, we argue that oscillations may be particularly relevant for encoding perceptual experiences into memory representations by coordinating spatially distributed and co-activated neuronal groups (i.e. cell assemblies). These coordinated representations thus constitute memory traces, or “engrams” [7,8].

Neural oscillations influence neural activity and human behavior

We begin by addressing general concerns regarding the role of oscillations in the “neural code”, which is distinguishable in two senses. Coding in a “strong” sense implies that neural computation is causally driven by some configuration of spikes or extracellular signal, which implies that the brain is using this code to represent information. Coding in a “weak” sense is descriptive and refers to the decodability of some mental state by observing a brain state characterized by different metrics such as power, phase, and frequency. Although there is limited evidence for oscillatory coding via ephaptic effects in the “strong” sense, considerable evidence using pattern classification and information theoretic approaches indicates a role for oscillatory coding in the “weak” sense, as discussed below.

The LFP is primarily a manifestation of aggregated synaptic activity and other transmembrane potentials [9,10]. Given that neurons process information through spiking, the LFP may only reflect a byproduct of neuronal firing without any further functional significance [11]. However, a small but growing body of evidence challenges this idea [12-14] , finding that the local oscillatory environment causally affects neural firing via ephaptic coupling (reviewed in [10]). While there is ongoing debate regarding the functional role of ephaptic coupling and a lack of evidence for similar effects in humans, these studies nonetheless suggest that changes in the LFP can impact neural firing [15].

Human stimulation studies also implicate specific oscillatory frequencies and phases in perception and memory. Helfrich et al [16] used transcranial alternating current stimulation (TACS) to entrain endogenous 10Hz posterior alpha activity during a target detection task. Targets presented during different oscillatory phases of the entrained rhythm led to differential behavioral performance, causally implicating oscillatory phase in sensory processing. In another study, Polonia et al [17] used TACS to induce different patterns of frontal-parietal theta phase relations during a letter recognition task. Whereas zero-lag (in-phase) patterns improved behavioral performance, 180° phase lag (anti-phasic) patterns diminished performance. Similarly, it has been observed that memory formation for words can be modulated by in-phase vs. anti-phasic electric stimulation of rhinal cortex and hippocampus [18] and that entorhinal stimulation modifies hippocampal theta phase to enhance spatial learning [19]. Although stimulation strength in these studies are orders of magnitude larger than that which occurs endogenously, these studies indicate that specific patterns of oscillatory phase support memory encoding and recognition, probably by promoting task-relevant neuronal communication and synaptic plasticity (reviewed in [5]). In sum, recent animal and human studies suggest that oscillations extend beyond a correlational epiphenomenon and are thus a plausible substrate contributing to the neural representation of an engram.

Independent contributions of LFP power and phase to neural representation

There is now abundant evidence for stimulus specific single neuron activity in humans (reviewed in [20]). Several studies have bridged the gap from single neurons to content-specific patterns in the LFP, showing both category [21,22] and stimulus [23,24] specific neuronal firing and LFP responses at low (below 10 Hz) and high (>30 Hz) frequencies in the human temporal lobe. Thus, activity in the theta and gamma bands appear particularly relevant for neural representation [25] (Figure 1A).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Content-specific sensory and episodic information can be decoded from frequency and phase-specific oscillations. A) Importance map for decoding visual letters using human intracranial recordings as a function of time and frequency. Decoding is best when using power in the theta and high-gamma bands. Modified with permission from [25]. B) Decoding accuracy for naturalistic auditory stimuli using power or phase in monkeys (left) and humans (right). Note that decoding accuracy is largest using theta phase in both species and that theta phase is better than firing rate in monkeys. Modified with permission from [28]. C) Heat map depicting the relative contribution of power and phase (represented jointly in the complex plane) to representations of facial expressions. Maps are shown separately in different bins of the complex plane at two different frequencies. Note that representations of different aspects of the faces (eyes vs. mouths) are multiplexed at different frequencies and phases. Reproduced with permission from [29]. D) Phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) occurs at a variety of phases (left) and frequency combinations (right) in humans, fulfilling two of the requirements for a coding scheme based on frequency and phase-specific neural firing/PAC. The distribution of coupling phases may be either monophasic or biphasic (left). Whereas phase-providing frequencies varied in the delta-alpha band, amplitude-providing frequencies varied in the beta-gamma bands. Reproduced with permission from [47]. E) Difference in phase-locking for items encoded at a flickering frequency of 6Hz versus 10Hz (left). Successful recognition of items encoded at 6Hz was accompanied by frequency-specific phase-locking (right), implicating frequency-specificity in both encoding and retrieval. Modified with permission from [55]. F) Primate neurons fire to frequency-specific (lower) inter-regional oscillatory phase coupling patterns, which constitute an “internal receptive field” (IRF) for the neuron (upper left) and are a strong predictor of firing rate (upper right). Recreating an assembly’s IRF may be sufficient to activate it in support of memory retrieval for a content-specific event. Modified with permission from [36]. See original text for full details in each panel.

Power and phase (the squared-amplitude and momentary deflection angle of an oscillation, respectively) are theoretically independent aspects of the LFP [26] which could allow one to decode complementary information (see Box 1, B-C). Analyzing recordings from visual and auditory cortex during presentation of naturalistic stimuli, Belitski et al [27] demonstrated that power at different frequencies in the 1-10Hz band conveys unique information about the stimuli from both spike rate and from each other. Activity at higher frequencies (>50 Hz) carried more redundant information between bands and with spike rate. In another study, power and phase measurements from human electroencephalography (EEG) and monkey LFP recordings were used to decode naturalistic auditory stimuli [28]. Low-frequency oscillatory phase predicted representations for different sounds better than power in both species (Figure 1B), and in monkeys, oscillatory phase was a better predictor of perceived sound than firing rate. Finally, Schyns et al [29] directly compared the information in oscillatory frequency, power, and phase to the representation of different faces using human EEG, finding that low frequency (below ~15Hz) phase represented significantly more information than power. Moreover, they found that the combination of power and phase was better than either measure alone (Figure 1C), indicating a multiplexed coding scheme (discussed below). Together, these and other studies [30,31] confirm that content-specific representations can be decoded from aggregated neural activity.

Box 1.

Content specific decoding via oscillations. A) Hypothetical firing rate of a single neuron showing content specific firing for the orange stimulus (upper). Alternatively, firing rate may not predict content specific representation in perceptual and memory tasks (lower; as in [28,33,35]). Under these conditions, consideration of the power (B), phase (C), or both (D) of the LFP signal may provide additional information. B) Power and frequency-specific neuronal firing. Traces show idealized 2 and 4 Hz oscillations elicited by the red and blue stimuli along with associated spiking of a single neuron. Content-specific neurons fire at specific frequencies, but across an extended and therefore unspecific phase range. Content specific information (right panel) in this scenario can be recovered via frequency-specific power increases or by considering neuronal firing as a function of power [22,24,25,27,31]. C) Layout similar to B for a phase-specific firing scheme. In this case, the neuron fires at a specific narrow oscillatory phase across several frequencies. Content specific information is conveyed by the phase of oscillatory activity or by considering neuronal firing as a function of phase [26,28]. D) Layout similar to B for a power- and phase-specific firing scheme. In this case, neurons fire at specific frequencies and phases. Content specific information is conveyed by neural firing as a function of frequency and the phase of oscillations or by these measures alone [29,31,36,38,45-48]). For clarity, low-frequency oscillations are shown, although the principles depicted here generalize to other frequencies. Similarly, other possible mechanisms of content-specific representation, such as population coding or inter-regional measures [60,61], are not shown.

graphic file with name nihms-625176-f0001.jpg

For human memory, phase-based intracranial EEG measures are better predictors for successful memory encoding of words than power-based measures [32]. Moreover, during a card-matching task, low-frequency (~2Hz) phase was a better predictor of correct performance than power in several brain regions [26]. Finally, hippocampal theta-band phase locking of single unit firing, instead of firing rate or power, predicts successful memory encoding [33]. These findings underscore phase as a coding mechanism and suggest that, in some cases, the LFP phase (which partially indexes the spatiotemporal coordination of spiking) may reflect behaviorally-relevant information which is not identifiable solely based on the spiking activity of the sampled neurons. [21,27,28,34,35]. We now explore how oscillations in various frequencies may combine to form content-specific representations.

Content-specific representations manifest in multiplexed oscillatory power and phase

Oscillations can occur simultaneously at multiple frequencies, raising the possibility that power and phase at different frequencies could represent distinct information (i.e. signal multiplexing, as in FM radio; Box 1D). Increasing empirical evidence suggests such a frequency-multiplexed coding scheme [29,36-39], which may allow for multiple processing streams [2,37,40] to code information with enhanced capacity [41,42]. During perception, frequency-specific power and phase represent different aspects of facial features [29] (Figure 1C) and human speech [38]. Regarding memory, inter-areal phase synchronization in distinct low frequency bands predicts different types of memory retrieval [37] and rodent low and high gamma activity convey unique spatial information [43]. Thus, multiplexing may be a general motif in neural representation during perception and memory [44]. We next consider how subcomponents of the LFP may combine to form content-specific representations during episodic encoding and retrieval.

Frequency and phase specific assembly coding for percepts and engrams

Several lines of evidence support a model whereby sensory and episodic experiences are encoded by cell assemblies firing at specific frequencies and phases of an underlying low-frequency rhythm (Box 1D, reviewed in [7]). First, low-frequency oscillations convey unique information about sensory stimuli between adjacent frequencies [27]. Both neuronal firing and gamma responses may be locked to specific phases of low-frequency oscillations [24,45], contributing to phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) between low-frequency phase and gamma amplitude [10]. In turn, spiking and PAC exhibits the diversity of frequencies and phases necessary for a frequency and phase-dependent coding scheme [33,36,46,47] (Figure 1D). Second, in accord with the notion of multiplexing, some human neurons phase-lock to multiple frequencies [46] and high-gamma amplitude may phase-lock to multiple low frequency phases simultaneously [39,47]. Third, prior work has found that content-specific representations manifest as neuronal firing and/or PAC at distinct phases [45,48-50], similar to the well-known phase precession of hippocampal place cells. Thus, single neuron firing and gamma are largest and convey the most content-specific information at particular frequencies and phases, suggesting that assemblies are organized by the frequency and phase of low-frequency oscillations [47,51]. Given that single-unit phase-locking [33] and PAC [52-54] are strongly associated with learning, they may also serve to transform sensory representations into frequency and phase-coded engrams.

Two mechanisms, phase entrainment and phase resetting, track rhythmic structure in the environment and could optimize encoding by aligning periods of local neuronal excitability with incoming rhythmic or arrhythmic sensory inputs, respectively. First, phase entrainment tracks rhythmic components of the external world such that the LFP mirrors the specific frequencies and phases of the sensory input [1,2,55]. Entrainment manifests as an increase in power and phase consistency at low frequencies [2,55] and may primarily determine the frequency at which cell assemblies are phase-coded. Second, phase resetting (i.e. stimulus-related phase shifting) occurs in response to both arrhythmic sensory inputs [38] and with “active” environmental sensing, such as rhythmic sampling of the environment [1,56]. Importantly, resetting is commonly observed in memory tasks [26,32,56,57] and is enhanced for items which are subsequently remembered [56], probably by promoting synaptic plasticity (reviewed in [58]). Thus, phase resetting may primarily specify the phase for an assembly by coordinating sensory inputs onto different phases of the underlying rhythm, forming cell assemblies which fire at unique phases for different sensory inputs. Together, entrainment and resetting can produce consistent oscillatory frequency and phase patterns across repetitions for a particular sensory input yet distinct patterns for different inputs, thereby organizing the frequency and phase of cell assembly formation, respectively.

Decoding oscillatory engrams during memory retrieval

How might phase and frequency coded cell assemblies be reactivated during retrieval? It has recently been reported that frequency-specific patterns of sensory inputs which occur during encoding reoccur during successful memory retrieval [55] (Figure 1E). Similarly, Canolty et al [36] found that spatially distributed groups of primate neurons are phase-locked to narrowband low-frequency oscillations, which have a particularly strong effect on neuronal spiking [12,36]. These neurons increased their firing to frequency-specific patterns of inter-regional oscillatory phase which acted as an “internal receptive field” (IRF) for the assembly (Figure 1F). Other studies have shown that IRFs vary with different behavioral conditions [59] and that inter-regional patterns of oscillatory phase allow for decoding of content-specific information in both monkeys [60] and humans [61]. Thus, recreating the IRF of an assembly should be sufficient to activate it.

We posit that retrieval is driven by dynamically shifting the frequency and phase of the LFP to the internal receptive field “IRF” [36] of a cell assembly [7], leading to its reactivation. While initial evidence for this idea has been reported using PCA analyses of intracranial EEG data [62], our model predicts that subtle changes in frequency and phase within the canonical bands may also be important (i.e. 2Hz vs. 4Hz, Box 1). Other findings also support this view. First, the LFP frequency helps determine several neuronal properties, including spike threshold, spike timing, and coincidence detection, and may also reflect coordination between neuronal groups [63]. Such frequency modulations are therefore likely to contribute to both encoding and retrieval likelihood. Second, human low-frequency phase coordinates single neuron firing [33,46] and exhibits rich spatiotemporal structure which varies with behavior [64]. Third, remembering spatial versus temporal information leads to frequency-specific changes in large scale patterns of oscillatory phase in humans [37]. Finally and perhaps most importantly, inter-regional oscillatory phase patterns are related to performance during retrieval [17,18] and successful retrieval is accompanied by frequency-specific input patterns which occurred during encoding [55]. These findings indicate that one aspect of the engram may be reflected in large scale patterns of oscillatory phase. It may therefore be possible to identify engrams by deciphering these frequency and phase-specific oscillatory patterns.

Concluding remarks and outstanding questions

Here, we have argued that oscillations and spike rates may each be useful for detecting neural representations of percepts and engrams. The firing of an assembly codes for content-specific memory traces and the large scale oscillatory environment reflects assembly communication and activation. Cell assembly firing at different frequencies and phases of low-frequency oscillations provide a mechanism for content-specific representation during perception and episodic encoding. Frequency and phase-tuned cell assembly organization arises out of oscillatory phase entrainment and phase-resetting, which together track environmental rhythmicity during initial perception. Retrieval is possible via the reinstatement of the large scale patterns of frequency-specific oscillatory phase which occurred during encoding, serving to select a cell assembly representing content specific information. This model therefore provides an account of how oscillations may reflect the organization of cell assemblies during initial sensory experience, encoding, and retrieval. Thus, it provides a more unified account of how oscillations coordinate cell assemblies in both perception and memory.

Given that the neuronal recording techniques necessary to identify distributed cell assemblies are not available in humans, testing these ideas may provide an alternate way to deduce content specific memory representations in humans. Yet, the above ideas and available literature also leave many questions unanswered. Do ephaptic effects exist in humans? Can frequency and phase-specific neural stimulation evoke content specific memory traces in humans? What are the relative contributions of synchronization and desynchronization at different frequencies to content specific representations [37,48,65]? How sparse or spatially distributed are these representations [25,36] and how do different neural codes spanning different areas combine [26,34]? What is the relation between intra- and inter-regional content specific representation [60,61]? Resolving these issues may ultimately lead to tracking distributed content-specific memory representations in humans – i.e., to finding the “engram”.

Highlights.

  • Oscillations causally influence human perception and memory

  • Decoding multiplexed power and phase reveals content specific representations

  • Percepts and engrams are encoded by frequency and phase-coded cell assembly firing

  • Reinstating an assembly’s frequency and phase reactivates it during retrieval

Acknowledgements

We thank the original authors for permission to reprint the findings depicted in Figure 1 and the reviewer for their insightful comments. We also thank Douglas Totten, Andrew Heusser, and Kevin Hill for helpful discussion on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Annotated Bibliography

  • 1.Schroeder CE, Wilson DA, Radman T, Scharfman H, Lakatos P. Dynamics of Active Sensing and perceptual selection. Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 2010;20:172–176. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Giraud AL, Poeppel D. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:511–517. doi: 10.1038/nn.3063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Siegel M, Donner TH, Engel AK. Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fell J, Axmacher N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:105–118. doi: 10.1038/nrn2979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jutras MJ, Buffalo EA. Synchronous neural activity and memory formation. Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 2010;20:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Watrous AJ, Ekstrom AD. The spectro-contextual encoding and retrieval theory of episodic memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:75. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lee H, Fell J, Axmacher N. Electrical engram: how deep brain stimulation affects memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:574–584. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mitzdorf U. Current source-density method and application in cat cerebral cortex: investigation of evoked potentials and EEG phenomena. Physiol Rev. 1985;65:37–100. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1985.65.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Buzsaki G, Anastassiou CA, Koch C. The origin of extracellular fields and currents--EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:407–420. doi: 10.1038/nrn3241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ray S, Maunsell JH. Differences in gamma frequencies across visual cortex restrict their possible use in computation. Neuron. 2010;67:885–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Anastassiou C, Perin R, Markram H, Koch C. Ephaptic coupling of cortical neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:217–223. doi: 10.1038/nn.2727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Anastassiou CA, Montgomery SM, Barahona M, Buzsaki G, Koch C. The effect of spatially inhomogeneous extracellular electric fields on neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:1925–1936. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3635-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Frohlich F, McCormick DA. Endogenous electric fields may guide neocortical network activity. Neuron. 2010;67:129–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ozen S, Sirota A, Belluscio MA, Anastassiou CA, Stark E, Koch C, Buzsaki G. Transcranial electric stimulation entrains cortical neuronal populations in rats. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:11476–11485. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5252-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Helfrich RF, Schneider TR, Rach S, Trautmann-Lengsfeld SA, Engel AK, Herrmann CS. Entrainment of brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Current Biology. 2014;24:333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Polanía R, Nitsche M, Korman C, Batsikadze G, Paulus W. The importance of timing in segregated theta phase-coupling for cognitive performance. Current Biology : CB. 2012;22:1314–1318. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *. This study used 6Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation simultaneously over left frontal and parietal areas during a letter recognition task. Experimentally induced synchronization (0 degree phase lag) or desynchronization (180 degree phase lag) between areas led to improved and diminished behavioral performance, respectively. This study thus causally implicates inter-areal low-frequency synchronization in a simple form of human memory. TACS entrainment is likely to prove an invaluable tool for determining the causal relation between endogenous brain rhythms and a wide variety of behaviors.
  • 18.Fell J, Staresina BP, Do Lam AT, Widman G, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE, Axmacher N. Memory modulation by weak synchronous deep brain stimulation: a pilot study. Brain Stimulation. 2013;6:270–273. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Suthana N, Haneef Z, Stern J, Mukamel R, Behnke E, Knowlton B, Fried I. Memory enhancement and deep-brain stimulation of the entorhinal area. N Engl J Med. 2012;366:502–510. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1107212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Quiroga R. Concept cells: the building blocks of declarative memory functions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:587–597. doi: 10.1038/nrn3251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kraskov A, Quiroga R, Reddy L, Fried I, Koch C. Local field potentials and spikes in the human medial temporal lobe are selective to image category. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:479–492. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Chan A, Baker J, Eskandar E, Schomer D, Ulbert I, Marinkovic K, Cash S, Halgren E. First-pass selectivity for semantic categories in human anteroventral temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:18119–18129. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3122-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ekstrom A, Viskontas I, Kahana M, Jacobs J, Upchurch K, Bookheimer S, Fried I. Contrasting roles of neural firing rate and local field potentials in human memory. Hippocampus. 2007;17:606–617. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rey H, Fried I, Quian Quiroga R. Timing of single-neuron and local field potential responses in the human medial temporal lobe. Current Biology : CB. 2014;24:299–304. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.van Gerven M, Maris E, Sperling M, Sharan A, Litt B, Anderson C, Baltuch G, Jacobs J. Decoding the memorization of individual stimuli with direct human brain recordings. NeuroImage. 2013;70:223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **. This paper applied machine learning algorithms to ECoG data to identify patterns of activity distributed across time, frequency, and brain region associated with individual letters. The authors found spatially localized theta and high gamma power in the occipital and temporal lobe to carry the most content specific information. The authors also identified considerable inter-subject variability in the content specific activity patterns, highlighting the need to identify oscillatory engrams at the individual level.
  • 26.Lopour B, Tavassoli A, Fried I, Ringach D. Coding of information in the phase of local field potentials within human medial temporal lobe. Neuron. 2013;79:594–606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Belitski A, Panzeri S, Magri C, Logothetis N, Kayser C. Sensory information in local field potentials and spikes from visual and auditory cortices: time scales and frequency bands. Journal of Computational Neuroscience. 2010;29:533–545. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0230-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *. This paper shows that low and high frequency oscillatory power contribute independent information during coding of natural stimuli. Whereas low frequency and spiking activity conveyed separate information, gamma band activity showed redundant representation between adjacent frequencies and with spiking. This paper thus suggests that low-frequency activity codes separate information between adjacent bands and additionally supports a link between neuronal firing and PAC.
  • 28.Ng B, Logothetis N, Kayser C. EEG phase patterns reflect the selectivity of neural firing. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:389–398. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **. This study assessed decoding performance of natural sounds using both EEG/LFP power and phase along with firing rates in human EEG and macaque LFP recordings. Phase, rather than power, carried the most information about acoustic stimuli and showed similar decoding performance when compared to auditory cortex neuronal firing rates. This paper therefore highlights dissociated contributions of oscillatory power and phase to firing rate in sensory representations.
  • 29.Schyns P, Thut G, Gross J. Cracking the code of oscillatory activity. PLoS Biology. 2011;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **. This paper identified the relative contributions of oscillatory power and phase at various frequencies to human face representations. The authors found that facial features were represented most strongly by a combination of power and phase and, remarkably, that different facial features were encoded in multiplexed combinations of power and phase at various frequencies.
  • 30.Jafarpour A, Horner A, Fuentemilla L, Penny W, Duzel E. Decoding oscillatory representations and mechanisms in memory. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:772–780. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jacobs J, Kahana M. Neural representations of individual stimuli in humans revealed by gamma-band electrocorticographic activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:10203–10214. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2187-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fell J, Ludowig E, Rosburg T, Axmacher N, Elger CE. Phase-locking within human mediotemporal lobe predicts memory formation. Neuroimage. 2008;43:410–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rutishauser U, Ross I, Mamelak A, Schuman E. Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking of single neurons. Nature. 2010;464:903–907. doi: 10.1038/nature08860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kayser C, Montemurro M, Logothetis N, Panzeri S. Spike-phase coding boosts and stabilizes information carried by spatial and temporal spike patterns. Neuron. 2009;61:597–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hyman J, Zilli E, Paley A, Hasselmo M. Working Memory Performance Correlates with Prefrontal-Hippocampal Theta Interactions but not with Prefrontal Neuron Firing Rates. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2010;4:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.002.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *. This study measured single neuron phase locking of medial prefrontal neurons to the hippocampal theta rhythm in rodents performing a delayed non-match to sample task. The authors found that correct performance depended on mPFC phase-locking to the hippocampal rhythm and not to mPFC firing rates, thus dissociating oscillatory phase and firing rate contributions to task performance. By showing inter-regional phase-locking of single neurons, this study is consistent with the notion of “internal receptive fields” during correct memory retrieval.
  • 36.Canolty R, Ganguly K, Kennerley S, Cadieu C, Koepsell K, Wallis J, Carmena J. Oscillatory phase coupling coordinates anatomically dispersed functional cell assemblies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:17356–17361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008306107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Watrous AJ, Tandon N, Conner CR, Pieters T, Ekstrom AD. Frequency-specific network connectivity increases underlie accurate spatiotemporal memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:349–356. doi: 10.1038/nn.3315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gross J, Hoogenboom N, Thut G, Schyns P, Panzeri S, Belin P, Garrod S. Speech rhythms and multiplexed oscillatory sensory coding in the human brain. PLoS Biology. 2013;11 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • *. The authors observed human auditory cortex entrainment to acoustic stimulus properties in the delta-theta band along with concomitant phase resetting to abrupt transitions in the acoustic envelope, providing an excellent example of how LFP phase may follow rhythmicity in sensory inputs. This paper also demonstrates dissociated roles of low and high-frequency oscillations in speech processing, implicating oscillatory multiplexing in sensory representation.
  • 39.Voytek B, Canolty RT, Shestyuk A, Crone NE, Parvizi J, Knight RT. Shifts in gamma phase-amplitude coupling frequency from theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:191. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Benchenane K, Tiesinga P, Battaglia F. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: a gateway to memory and attention. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2011;21:475–485. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Akam T, Kullmann D. Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014 doi: 10.1038/nrn3668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Panzeri S, Brunel N, Logothetis N, Kayser C. Sensory neural codes using multiplexed temporal scales. Trends in Neurosciences. 2010;33:111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Cabral H, Vinck M, Fouquet C, Pennartz C, Rondi-Reig L, Battaglia F. Oscillatory dynamics and place field maps reflect hippocampal ensemble processing of sequence and place memory under NMDA receptor control. Neuron. 2014;81:402–415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ekstrom AD, Watrous AJ. Multifaceted roles for low-frequency oscillations in bottom-up and top-down processing during navigation and memory. Neuroimage. 2014;85 Pt 2:667–677. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Siegel M, Warden M, Miller E. Phase-dependent neuronal coding of objects in short-term memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:21341–21346. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908193106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jacobs J, Kahana M, Ekstrom A, Fried I. Brain oscillations control timing of single-neuron activity in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:3839–3844. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4636-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.van der Meij R, Kahana M, Maris E. Phase-amplitude coupling in human electrocorticography is spatially distributed and phase diverse. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:111–123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4816-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **. The authors assessed phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) in humans and found that PAC occurs at a variety of frequencies and phases between spatially distributed electrode pairs. This study thus identifies PAC as a plausible substrate for encoding sensory and memory representations.
  • 48.Jacobs J, Lega B, Anderson C. Explaining how brain stimulation can evoke memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;24:553–563. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Fuentemilla L, Penny WD, Cashdollar N, Bunzeck N, Duzel E. Theta-coupled periodic replay in working memory. Curr Biol. 2010;20:606–612. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lisman JE, Jensen O. The theta-gamma neural code. Neuron. 2013;77:1002–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Buzsaki G. Neural syntax: cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron. 2010;68:362–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Tort AB, Komorowski RW, Manns JR, Kopell NJ, Eichenbaum H. Theta-gamma coupling increases during the learning of item-context associations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:20942–20947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911331106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Benchenane K, Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Tierney PL, Gioanni Y, Battaglia FP, Wiener SI. Coherent theta oscillations and reorganization of spike timing in the hippocampal-prefrontal network upon learning. Neuron. 2010;66:921–936. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kendrick KM, Zhan Y, Fischer H, Nicol AU, Zhang X, Feng J. Learning alters theta amplitude, theta-gamma coupling and neuronal synchronization in inferotemporal cortex. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wimber M, Maass A, Staudigl T, Richardson-Klavehn A, Hanslmayr S. Rapid memory reactivation revealed by oscillatory entrainment. Current Biology. 2012;22:1482–1486. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **. Recording human electroencephalography, the authors found that frequency-specific patterns of sensory inputs which occur during memory encoding reoccur during successful memory retrieval. These results underscore the causal role of oscillations during memory encoding and retrieval.
  • 56.Jutras MJ, Fries P, Buffalo EA. Oscillatory activity in the monkey hippocampus during visual exploration and memory formation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110:13144–13149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302351110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rizzuto DS, Madsen JR, Bromfield EB, Schulze-Bonhage A, Kahana MJ. Human neocortical oscillations exhibit theta phase differences between encoding and retrieval. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1352–1358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Axmacher N, Mormann F, Fernandez G, Elger CE, Fell J. Memory formation by neuronal synchronization. Brain Research Reviews. 2006;52:170–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Canolty RT, Ganguly K, Carmena JM. Task-dependent changes in cross-level coupling between single neurons and oscillatory activity in multiscale networks. PLoS Computational Biology. 2012;8:e1002809. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Salazar R, Dotson N, Bressler S, Gray C. Content-specific fronto-parietal synchronization during visual working memory. Science. 2012;338:1097–1100. doi: 10.1126/science.1224000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Majima K, Matsuo T, Kawasaki K, Kawai K, Saito N, Hasegawa I, Kamitani Y. Decoding visual object categories from temporal correlations of ECoG signals. Neuroimage. 2014;90:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • **. Recording human electroencephalography, the authors found that frequency-specific patterns of sensory inputs which occur during memory encoding reoccur during successful memory retrieval. These results underscore the causal role of oscillations during memory encoding and retrieval.
  • 62.Manning JR, Sperling MR, Sharan A, Rosenberg EA, Kahana MJ. Spontaneously reactivated patterns in frontal and temporal lobe predict semantic clustering during memory search. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:8871–8878. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5321-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Cohen MX. Fluctuations in oscillation frequency control spike timing and coordinate neural networks. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:8988–8998. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0261-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Panagiotides H, Freeman WJ, Holmes MD, Pantazis D. Behavioral states may be associated with distinct spatial patterns in electrocorticogram. Cogn Neurodyn. 2011;5:55–66. doi: 10.1007/s11571-010-9139-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hanslmayr S, Staudigl T, Fellner MC. Oscillatory power decreases and long-term memory: the information via desynchronization hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:74. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES