Abstract
Dysregulation of the immune system contributes to the pathogenesis of neuropsychiatric disorders including schizophrenia. Here, we demonstrated that toll-like receptor (TLR)-2, a family of pattern-recognition receptors, is involved in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia-like symptoms. Psychotic symptoms such as hyperlocomotion, anxiolytic-like behaviors, prepulse inhibition deficits, social withdrawal, and cognitive impairments were observed in TLR-2 knock-out (KO) mice. Ventricle enlargement, a hallmark of schizophrenia, was also observed in TLR-2 KO mouse brains. Levels of p-Akt and p-GSK-3α/β were markedly higher in the brain of TLR-2 KO than wild-type (WT) mice. Antipsychotic drugs such as haloperidol or clozapine reversed behavioral and biochemical alterations in TLR-2 KO mice. Furthermore, p-Akt and p-GSK-3α/β were decreased by treatment with a TLR-2 ligand, lipoteichoic acid, in WT mice. Thus, our data suggest that the dysregulation of the innate immune system by a TLR-2 deficiency may contribute to the development and/or pathophysiology of schizophrenia-like behaviors via Akt-GSK-3α/β signaling.
Schizophrenia is a debilitating psychiatric disorder with a prevalence of 1% worldwide, and is characterized by positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and cognitive impairments1. Recent studies have revealed that neuronal abnormalities, including altered glutamatergic function, GABAergic deficits, altered dopaminergic function, and impaired neurodevelopment, contribute to the etiopathophysiology of schizophrenia2. Specially, growing evidences suggest that abnormal neurodevelopment during perinatal and early adolescence may lead to a dysfunction of neural networks which may later develop into a full-blown schizophrenic disorder3,4. Furthermore, dysregulation of the immune system, along with genetic and environmental factors, is known to contribute to abnormal neurodevelopment processes, which leads to the pathogenesis of schizophrenia5. For example, an imbalance in inflammatory cytokines, such as interleukins, interferon, tumor necrosis factor-α, and chemokines, has been implicated in the negative and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia6,7. Prenatal immune activation is also known to induce schizophrenia-like behaviors in offspring, which are associated with abnormal neuronal development and synaptic transmission in an animal model of schizophrenia8. Thus, a dysregulation of the immune system appears to play an important role in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia.
A recent study has shown the close relationship between mental illness and the immune system, especially the innate immune system, in which a dysregulated immune system causes psychiatric disorders9. In general, the activation of TLR by pathogen-associated molecular patterns initiates an intracellular kinase cascade by inducing the translocation of transcription factor, NF-κB, which leads to the production of a variety of inflammatory mediators and cytokines10. Twelve functional TLRs have been identified in humans and mice, respectively, and they recognize different microbial ligands during infection11. In addition to the involvement in inflammatory processes, TLRs are also known to play important roles in neurodevelopment, adult neurogenesis, neuroplasticity, and learning and memory in the absence of any underlying immune activation12,13,14. For example, TLR-3 inhibits hippocampus-dependent working memory retention and adult hippocampal neurogenesis12. TLR-2 deficiency impairs hippocampal neurogenesis, whereas TLR-4 deficiency enhances neuronal cell proliferation and differentiation14. TLRs have also been shown to be involved in higher brain functions, such as cognition or mood-related behaviors12,15. However, there is no study demonstrating that the genetic mutation of TLRs may induce psychiatric disorder-like symptoms.
Here, we tried to examine whether the dysfunction of TLR genes such as TLR-2, TLR-3, or TLR-4 by using knock-out (KO) mice would affect aggressive behavior, one of psychotic behaviors16. Surprisingly, we found that only TLR-2 KO mice, but not TLR-3 or TLR-4 KO mice, showed abnormal and aggressive behaviors in a preliminary experiment. Thus, to examine the abnormal behavior observed in TLR-2 KO mice in detail, we measured behavioral outcomes, histological alterations, and the level of neuroplasticity in TLR-2 KO mice. Furthermore, numerous studies have demonstrated the abnormalities in Akt (protein kinase B)-glycogen synthase kinase (GSK)-3 signaling in schizophrenia17,18. Therefore, we also investigated abnormalities of Akt-GSK-3 signaling and the effects of typical or atypical antipsychotic drugs such as haloperidol and clozapine on the abnormal signaling cascade and behavior in TLR-2 KO mice. Finally, we tried to elucidate the linkage between TLR-2 activation and Akt-GSK-3 signaling using lipoteichoic acid (LTA) as a TLR-2 ligand. Conclusively, we showed in this study that the deletion of the TLR-2 gene in mice causes the typical behavioral, histological, and pathophysiological characteristics observed in schizophrenia patients.
Results
TLR-2 KO mice show hyperlocomotion and anxiolytic behavior
In our preliminary studies, we found that only TLR-2 KO mice exhibited abnormal and aggressive behavior; whereas these effects were not observed in TLR-3 and 4 KO mice (Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). Notably, the aggressive behavior of TLR-2 KO mice began to be observed apparently in 8 weeks old, whereas not in 3 or 5 weeks old (Supplementary Fig. 1c, d). Therefore, TLR-2 KO mice in 8–20 weeks old were employed in this study to examine behavioral, morphological, and biochemical characteristics.
Although the three major symptoms of schizophrenia are difficult to replicate in rodents, hyperactivity in a novel environment has been used as a translational feature of psychotic agitation19. Thus, we first determined the spontaneous locomotor activity of TLR-2 KO and WT mice in an open field test. TLR-2 KO mice showed higher locomotor activity than WT mice (Fig. 1a). The distance traveled was also significantly longer in TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice (Fig. 1b, c). TLR-2 KO mice showed a marked increase in the number of crossings in the anxiety-provoking center of the open field compared to WT mice (Fig. 1d, e). Next, we conducted an elevated plus-maze test and a marble-burying test to confirm the anxiety-like behavior in TLR-2 KO mice. In the elevated plus-maze test, TLR-2 KO mice spent significantly longer times in the open arms of the maze (Fig. 1f) and entered open arms more frequently (Supplementary Fig. 2a) than WT mice, but no significant differences in locomotor activity were found (Supplementary Fig. 2b). In the marble-burying test, TLR-2 KO mice buried significantly less marbles compared with WT mice (Fig. 1g). However, TLR-2 KO mice did not show any change in immobility in the forced swimming test (Supplementary Fig. 3), which is used to detect anti-depressant-like activity. Collectively, these results indicate that TLR-2 KO mice show hyperlocomotion and reduced anxiety-like behaviors in a novel environment.
Social function in TLR-2 KO mice
Next, we investigated whether TLR-2 KO mice would exhibit deficits in social function such as reduced social interaction as well as increased aggressive behavior, which are considered to negative symptoms of schizophrenia20. In the three-chamber social novelty preference test, both WT and TLR-2 KO mice had no preference for the two empty cages located in the right and left chambers during a habituation period (Supplementary Fig. 4). When the stranger mouse was put in the left cage, both WT and TLR-2 KO mice significantly spent more time in the left chamber (Fig. 1h), suggesting that there is no significant difference in the social ability between WT and TLR-2 KO. When a second stranger mouse was placed in the right cage, however, WT mice showed a strong preference for the new mouse, whereas TLR-2 KO mice did not (Fig. 1i), suggesting that social recognition may be impaired in TLR-2 KO mice. We next examined the reciprocal social interaction of freely moving WT and TLR-2 KO mice with same strain (C57/BL) or different strain mice (CD1 ICR). Both the duration and the number of contact to the C57BL (data not shown) as well as CD1 ICR test mice were significantly lower in TLR-2 KO mice than to WT mice (Fig. 1j, k). However, attacking behavior against the test mice was not observed in either TLR-2 KO or WT mice during this test (data not shown). In addition, to test aggressive behavior against an intruder in TLR-2 KO mice, we conducted the cotton bud biting test, which can directly provoke the test mice. TLR-2 KO mice showed a significant increase in the number of biting attacks and the duration of attacks against a cotton swab compared to WT mice (Fig. 1l). Collectively, these data indicate that TLR-2 KO mice show significant deficits in social behaviors, including impaired social recognition, reduced social interaction, and aggressive behavior.
Impaired cognitive and sensorimotor gating function in TLR-2 KO mice
It is well known that schizophrenia patients exhibit broad-based cognitive impairments across a range of cognitive abilities, such as attention, speed of processing, working and long-term memory, executive function, and social cognition1. Therefore, we attempted to assess the level of cognitive abilities in WT and TLR-2 KO mice using various behavioral tests. On day 1 of the fear conditioning test, TLR-2 KO mice showed a significantly reduced freezing response immediately after the 2nd and 3rd foot shocks compared to WT mice (Fig. 2a). During the test, we did not observe any difference of pain sensitivity between TLR-2 KO and WT mice. In addition, the impairment in the motor control of freezing or reduction in shock sensitivity compared to WT mice was not observed in TLR-2 KO mice (data not shown). TLR-2 KO mice also showed a significantly reduced freezing behavior both in the context test on day 2 (Fig. 2b) and in the cued test on day 3 (Fig. 2c) compared to WT mice, suggesting that the attention and conditioning memory processes may be disrupted in TLR-2 KO mice. In the novel object recognition test, the exploration time for a new object at 4 h after training was significantly reduced in TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice (Fig. 2d), suggesting that the recognition memory may also be impaired in TLR-2 KO mice. However, both WT and TLR-2 KO mice showed similar exploratory activity and preference for the two identical objects in the training phase (Supplementary Fig. 5a and b). In short-term memory performance using the Y-maze test, however, both WT and TLR-2 KO mice had similar spontaneous alternations (Fig. 2e), suggesting that short-term memory may not be impaired in TLR-2 KO mice. To assess spatial working learning and memory, we performed the Barnes circular maze test, which is commonly used to evaluate learning ability and the acquisition of spatial memory. TLR-2 KO mice took a longer time to escape (Fig. 2f) and showed a higher number of errors (Fig. 2g) than WT mice during the training trials, which indicate that the learning ability and the acquisition of spatial memory may be impaired in TLR-2 KO mice. In the probe test, the time spent in the target zone was significantly shorter in TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice (Fig. 2h). However, there was no difference in the distance traveled between WT and TLR-2 KO mice (data not shown). Thus, our data suggest that spatial learning and memory may be impaired in TLR-2 KO mice.
Schizophrenia patients show a loss of sensorimotor gating function, which is reflected in a reduced prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the startle reflex as well as deficits in motor coordination21. To assess sensorimotor gating function in TLR-2 KO mice, we performed an acoustic startle response test. Two-way ANOVA analysis revealed that there were no significant differences in the acoustic startle response between TLR-2 KO and WT mice (Fig. 2i), suggesting that hearing ability may be not impaired in TLR-2 KO mice. Although PPI monotonically increased with increased prepulse intensities in all genotypes, TLR-2 KO mice showed a significantly lower PPI level than WT mice at all prepulse levels (Fig. 2j). In the rota-rod test, TLR-2 KO mice also showed faster falling on the rod (Fig. 2k) and an increased number of falls than WT mice on the first day (Fig. 2l), indicating that TLR-2 KO mice may have deficits in motor coordination. Taken together, these results suggest that TLR-2 KO mice exhibit PPI deficits likely due to sensorimotor gating dysfunction and motor incoordination.
Several lines of evidences have suggested that the deficits in learning and memory in schizophrenia may be mediated through disrupted synaptic plasticity including long-term potentiation (LTP) or long-term depression (LTD)22. Thus, we characterized the neural plasticity of LTP in hippocampal slice cultures as well as LTD in cerebellar slice cultures of WT and TLR-2 KO mice. LTP at hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses was not altered (Supplementary Fig. 6a), whereas cerebellar LTD was inhibited in TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice (Supplementary Fig. 6b). It was reported that an impairment of parallel fiber LTD is sufficient to disrupt the encoding and storage of cerebellum-dependent motor memory23. Collectively, our data suggest the possibility that deficits in the wide ranges of the learning and memory system, including attention, conditioning, recognition, and spatial memories, in TLR-2 KO mice may be, at the very least, associated with the disrupted cerebellar LTD.
TLR-2 KO mice show ventricle enlargement and higher cell death than WT
Neuroimaging and postmortem studies have shown that ventricular enlargement has been observed in the schizophrenic brain, which have significant correlations with the severity of symptoms of schizophrenia2,24. To examine whether TLR-2 KO mice would show the morphological characteristics of schizophrenia including ventricle enlargement, we performed histochemical and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies. Nissl staining revealed that both lateral and dorsal 3rd ventricles were significantly enlarged in the brains of adult TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice (Fig. 3a). MRI data also showed that the volume of ventricles including the lateral, third, and fourth were significantly larger in TLR-2 KO mice than WT mice (Fig. 3b, c). Because the number of cell deaths may contribute these morphological changes in the mouse brain, we examined whether progressive cell death occurred in TLR-2 KO mice. TUNEL assay revealed that a higher cell death was observed in the ependymal layer of the lateral ventricle, cerebral cortex, CA1 region of hippocampus, and cerebellum in the TLR-2 KO mice than in WT mice (Fig. 3d). In addition, the intensity of Iba-1+ or GFAP+-glial cells in the hippocampal region were higher in TLR-2 KO mice than WT mice (Supplementary Fig. 7), which imply abnormal activation of glial cells in TLR-2 KO mice. Thus, these data provide the possibility that the higher cell death in the various brain regions, including the ependymal layer of the ventricles and abnormal activation of glial cells may be linked to the ventricle enlargement in TLR-2 KO mice.
Alteration of Akt-GSK-3 signaling in TLR-2 KO mice
Recent evidence suggests that an altered Akt-GSK-3 signaling cascade is closely associated with the pathogenesis of schizophrenia as well as the psychotomimetic effects in the animal models of schizophrenia18. Moreover, it has been implicated that TLR-2 dependent signaling is directly linked to the phosphoinositide-3-kinase (PI3K)-Akt signaling pathway25. Therefore, we examined whether Akt and GSK-3 activities were altered in TLR-2 KO mice. By Western blot analysis, we found that the levels of phosphorylated Akt (p-Akt), phosphorylated GSK-3α (p-GSK-3α), and phosphorylated GSK-3β (p-GSK-3β) were significantly higher in the cortex, hippocampus, midbrain, or cerebellum region of TLR-2 KO mice than WT mice (Fig. 4a–e). However, the total protein levels of Akt, GSK-3α, or GSK-3β were not different between TLR-2 KO and WT mice. Thus, these data indicate that TLR-2 KO mice show altered Akt and GSK-3 activities in the brain.
Anti-psychotics attenuate the PPI deficits and reduce Akt-GSK3 activation in TLR-2 KO mice
To investigate whether PPI deficits in TLR-2 KO mice can be reversed by typical (haloperidol) or atypical (clozapine) antipsychotics, we conducted the acoustic startle response test. In line with Figure 2i–j, PPI deficits were observed in TLR-2 KO mice, although the acoustic startle response was unchanged (Fig. 5a–d). As expected, the PPI deficits in TLR-2 KO mice were significantly rescued by the acute administration of haloperidol (1 mg per kg body weight) or clozapine (1 mg per kg body weight) (Fig. 5b, d). However, acute treatment with haloperidol or clozapine did not affect the level of acoustic startle response in TLR-2 KO mice (Fig. 5a, c). In addition, acute treatment with haloperidol or clozapine did not affect the level of acoustic startle amplitude and PPI in WT mice (Supplementary Fig. 8a, b). These results are in agreement with the previous reports that PPI deficits could be attenuated by antipsychotics in animal models or clinical cases of schizophrenia26.
Next, because abnormalities in Akt-GSK-3 signaling have been shown to be closely associated with sensorimotor gating function27, we evaluated whether the increased levels of p-Akt or p-GSK-3α/β in TLR-2 KO mice would be also reduced by treatment with such antipsychotics. Western blot analysis revealed that the increased expression level of p-GSK-3α/β, but not p-Akt, was markedly reduced in the cortex, hippocampus, midbrain, and cerebellum region of TLR-2 KO mice when treated with haloperidol or clozapine (Fig. 5e–i). However, the expression levels of p-Akt, p-GSK-3α, and p-GSK-3β in the cortex, hippocampus, midbrain, or cerebellum region of WT mice were not changed by the acute administration of haloperidol or clozapine compared to the vehicle-treated controls (Supplementary Fig. 8c). These results suggest that PPI deficits of sensorimotor gating function in TLR-2 KO mice may be associated with the alteration in GSK-3α/β activities.
Direct linkages between TLR-2 and Akt-GSK-3 signaling
To clarify the direct linkage between TLR-2 and Akt-GSK-3 signaling, we further investigated whether the systemic injection of LTA, a TLR-2 ligand, would affect Akt-GSK signaling in WT and TLR-2 KO mice. In WT mice, Western blot analysis revealed that the levels of p-Akt or p-GSK-3α/β were decreased at 1 h after acute administration of LTA (50 μg, i.p.) in the cortex, hippocampus, midbrain, or cerebellum region when compared to vehicle-treated controls and, thereafter, recovered to basal level (Fig. 6a–e). However, the increased levels of p-Akt (Supplementary Fig. 9) or p-GSK-3α/β (data not shown) in the cortex, hippocampus, midbrain, or cerebellum region of TLR-2 KO mice were not changed by LTA. These results suggest that TLR-2 signaling may be closely linked to the Akt-GSK-3 signaling pathway.
Discussion
Until now, most studies on the roles of TLRs have been focused on the immune system and the mechanisms underlying pathological conditions, such as pathogen recognition and the initiation of inflammatory and immune responses that trigger both innate and adaptive immune responses28,29. In the central nervous system (CNS), TLRs have also been known to play important roles in the pathogenesis of brain diseases, such as stroke, Alzheimer's disease, and multiple sclerosis13,30. Recently, emerging evidences have shown the novel roles of TLRs in the CNS without any immune challenge, such as the regulation of neurodevelopment, adult neurogenesis, neuronal plasticity, mood-related behaviors, and learning and memory system12,13,14,15,31,32. Notably, a multi-stage schizophrenia genome-wide association study provides support for the speculated link between the immune system and schizophrenia33. In the present study, we first demonstrated that a deficiency of the TLR-2 gene caused schizophrenia-like behavioral, histological, and biochemical characteristics in mice. Importantly, several antipsychotics were effective in mitigating the disruption of sensorimotor gating in TLR-2 KO mice. Moreover, TLR-2 KO mice exhibited morphological changes, including ventricle enlargements, which are relevant to brains of schizophrenia24,34. However, it should be pointed out that this study is limited to provide the direct evidence which TLR-2 is associated with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia is a complex multigenic disorder whose etiology can vary between ethnicities and subpopulations. Loss of a single gene in a mouse model does not replicate all aspects of schizophrenia. For example, Muller et al. reported that TLR-2 was not elevated in the blood samples of schizophrenia patients compared to healthy controls35. On the other hand, the TLR-2 gene is located at chromosome 4q32, a region that has been shown to be linked to positive schizophrenia-interpersonal sensitivity in schizophrenia patients36. A significant association between TLR-2 gene polymorphisms and clinical cognitive symptoms has also been observed in schizophrenia patients37. Thus, these reports demonstrate that TLR-2 changes may not be present in all populations of schizophrenia. Nevertheless, it would be noteworthy that our study provides first evidences on the correlation of innate immune system and psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia.
We found that TLR-2 KO mice exhibited hyperactivity during exposure to the open field. It is well known that increased locomotion or stereotype behaviors are observed in a subset of schizophrenia patients, as well as in animal models elicited by pharmacological and genetic approaches38,39. Thus, the persistent locomotor hyperactivity in TLR-2 KO mice may mimic the positive symptom of “psychomotor agitation” in schizophrenic patients40. In addition to hyperlocomotion, TLR-2 KO mice showed reduced anxiety-like behaviors, but not a depression-like behavior. A similar study showed that TLR-3 deficient mice exhibit an impaired amygdala-related anxiety behavior, suggesting the possibility that TLR signaling may be normally involved in amygdala plasticity12. Given that anxiety and depressive symptoms are frequently observed in the course of schizophrenia41, therefore, our data suggested that TLR-2 mediated signaling is likely not to relevant to the mood-related symptoms of the illness. Additionally, negative symptoms of schizophrenia are characterized as social withdrawal, avolition, and anhedonia, which are extremely difficult to replicate in animal models16,40. Nevertheless, deficits in social functions, e.g., social interaction, social cognition, and aggression, can be represented in both human and animal cases42. By social behavioral tests, we found that TLR-2 KO mice showed a tendency to avoid stranger mouse and an impaired social recognition. Furthermore, TLR-2 KO mice responded to the direct stimulation by the cotton bud, which led to provoking the mice and inciting the aggressive behavior. Taken together, TLR-2 KO mice exhibited persistent hyper-locomotion behavior and abnormal social behavior, which are similar to the positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia20. Notably, the aggressive behaviors began to be observed from 8 weeks old, but were not presented in younger mice, which is similar to the effects of age in some cases of schizophrenia in humans1.
PPI is widely regarded as an endophenotypic marker for sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia and is sensitive to treatments generating hyperdopaminergic and hypoglutamatergic states43. Although several other neuropsychiatric disorders, including obsessive-compulsive disorder, autism spectrum disorder, and bipolar disorder, are also associated with decreased PPI21,44,45, PPI deficit is the best characterized and the most widely replicated characteristic in the schizophrenia patients46. To our knowledge, we are the first to observe the disruption in sensorimotor gating in TLR-2 KO mice as measured by PPI. PPI deficits in TLR-2 KO mice were also reversed by an acute administration of typical or atypical antipsychotics. In addition, various cognitive tests revealed that broad learning and memory systems were impaired in TLR-2 KO mice, which may be due to sensorimotor gating dysfunction. Moreover, TLR-2 KO mice also showed a deficit in motor coordination in the rota-rod test. Recent evidence has shown that motor abnormalities and cognitive impairments in schizophrenia are closely related with the abnormalities in the structure and function of the cerebellum47,48. Furthermore, we observed that cerebellar LTD was impaired in TLR-2 KO mice. Thus, these results suggest the possibility that TLR-2 signaling may be involved in cerebellum plasticity.
Based on morphological studies, ventricle enlargement was observed in the brains of TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice. Furthermore, compared to WT mice, TLR-2 KO mice showed higher numbers of apoptotic cell deaths in the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, cerebellum, and the ependymal layer of the ventricles as well as the activation of glial cells, including astrocyte and microglia, in the hippocampus in the absence of apparent tissue abnormality. It has been demonstrated that differentiation of neural progenitor cell into neurons is disrupted in TLR-2 KO mice, leading to increased glial cell numbers rather than neurons14, similar with our observation. Therefore, these results indicate that TLR-2 may play an important role in neuronal cell fate and the development of the ventricular system32. Moreover, several lines of evidence show that abnormal adult neurogenesis is closely associated with not only disruption in sensorimotor gating (PPI), mood, and memory function, but also the pathogenesis of psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia49,50,51. Given that PPI deficits and impaired broad learning and memory system were presented in TLR-2 KO mice, impaired hippocampal neurogenesis in TLR-2 KO mice may contribute to sensorimotor gating dysfunction, resulting in PPI deficits and cognitive impairment.
The innate immune initiation by TLR-2 is known to be mediated by MyD88-dependent NF-κB activation. In contrast to our results from TLR-2 KO mice, it has been reported that the Myd88 deficiency in C57BL/6 mice showed more anxiety, better motor coordination, and improved spatial learning than WT mice52. On the other hand, recent studies have shown that the Akt-GSK-3 signaling pathway is closely associated with the function of TLRs in the immune system. The stimulation of TLR-2 is known to regulate Akt activity through Mal/PI3K signaling cascade25, which directly activates NF-κB, an orchestrator of the inflammatory response53. However, it is still unclear whether deletion of TLR-2 gene also affects the Akt activity and its downstream signaling cascade, especially in the brain. In the present study, we first observed that the activities of Akt and GSK-3α/β were markedly changed in the brains of TLR-2 KO mice. Moreover, the activation of TLR-2 by LTA caused the dephosphorylation of Akt and GSK-3α/β in WT mice, but failed to show those effects in TLR-2 KO mice. These data indicate that the Akt-GSK-3 signaling pathway may be directly linked to TLR-2 signaling in both WT and TLR-2 KO mice. However, the mechanism underlying how TLR-2 gene deletion changes Akt activity is unclear, and further study would be required to clarify these issues. In addition, PPI deficits as well as the inhibition of GSK-3α/β in TLR-2 KO mice were attenuated by both clozapine and haloperidol. It is well documented that the Akt-GSK-3 signaling pathway plays crucial roles in mediating diverse neuronal processes including brain development, regulation of synaptic plasticity, and determination of cell fate54. Moreover, dysregulation of the Akt-GSK-3 signaling pathway is associated with the pathogenesis of numerous neurological and psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia17,18,54. Collectively, these findings suggest that our observations, such as behavioral alterations (at least sensorimotor gating disruption), ventricle enlargement, and abnormal synaptic plasticity, in TLR-2 KO mice may be linked in part to the Akt/GSK-3 signaling cascade. Additionally, Emaminan et al. showed a decrease in Akt1 protein levels and, consequently, a reduced phosphorylation activity of GSK-3β in the peripheral lymphocytes and brains of individuals with schizophrenia18. They also showed that Akt deficient mice did not exhibit any change in PPI level compared with WT mice. On the contrary, we observed a significant increase in the phosphorylation levels of Akt and GSK-3α/β, but not in the level of total form of Akt or GSK-3, in the brains of TLR-2 KO mice compared to WT mice. It would be notable that acute administration of psychostimulants, such as DA receptor agonists, 5-HT receptor agonists, and NMDA receptor antagonists, induces phosphorylation of Akt or GSK-3β in the frontal cortex, which may lead to psychotomimetic activity such as sensorimotor gating disruption and repetitive movements in mice55. The GSK3 inhibitor, including SB216763 and linopirdine, reduces PPI when directly infused into the medial prefrontal cortex in mice27. Thus, the role of Akt-GSK-3 signaling in psychotic symptoms such as PPI may be different in rodents and in humans, especially in schizophrenia. Additional works would be required to resolve these issues. In addition, Emaminan et al. showed that chronic administration of haloperidol (for 12 days) increased the phosphorylation of Akt and GSK-3β in the C57Bl/6 mice brain, but acute treatment with haloperidol (at 2 h) did not affect the Akt phosphorylation of Ser 47318. In this study, we did not find any significant changes in the phosphorylation of Akt (Ser 473) and GSK-3α/β at 1 h after haloperidol treatment. The discrepancy in the effect of haloperidol may be attributable to the injection paradigm (1 h-acute versus 12 days-chronic).
In conclusion, we have conducted comprehensive behavioral, biochemical, and morphological analyses of TLR-2 KO mice. Behavioral alterations in TLR-2 KO mice were relevant to the symptoms of schizophrenia, such as the positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms. In addition, ventricles enlargements, apoptotic cell death, and marked dysregulation of Akt-GSK-3 signaling were also observed in the brain of TLR-2 KO mice. Although the critical issue of whether prenatal immune activation could induce schizophrenia-like symptoms should be investigated, the present study suggests that an alteration of TLR-2 functions in the CNS may contribute to schizophrenia etiology.
Methods
Materials
Lipoteichoic acid (LTA), haloperidol, and clozapine were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Chemical Co. (St. Louis, MO). Haloperidol was dissolved in saline with 1% dimethyl sulfoxide. Clozapine was dissolved in saline with 0.1 M hydrochloric acid. LTA were dissolved in saline. Antibodies to GSK-3α, GSK-3β, Akt, and β-actin were purchased from Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Inc. (Santa Cruz, CA). Antibodies to p-Akt at serine 473, p-GSK-3β at serine 9, and p-GSK-3α at serine 21 were purchased from Cell Signaling Technology (Danvers, MA). All other materials consisted of the highest grades available and were obtained from typical commercial sources.
Animals
TLR-2 KO mice backgrounds from C57BL/6 mice were obtained from Dr. Sung J. Lee (Seoul National University, Seoul, Korea)56. C57BL/6J mice used as wild-type (WT) control and CD1 ICR mice used for the reciprocal social interaction test were purchased from Daehan BioLink Inc. (Eumseong, Korea). Mice (8–20 weeks old) used for all behavioral tests were performed blind to genotypes with age-matched littermate pairs of mice. The mouse cages were maintained at a constant temperature (23 ± 1°C) and relative humidity (60 ± 10%) under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on from 07:30 to 19:30), and the facilities were approved by the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care International (AAALAC-i). Animals were allowed access to water and food ad libitum. Animal maintenance and treatment were carried out in accordance with the Animal Care and Use Guidelines issued by Kyung Hee University, Korea. All experiments with mice were performed according to the protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Kyung Hee University (Approved protocol No. KHP-2010-12-11). The number of mice used in each experiment is stated in Supplementary Table 1. The naive mice were used for sampling of biochemical or physiological studies.
Tests for mood-related behaviors
Open field test
To determine the spontaneous horizontal locomotor activity of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, an open-field test was performed as described below. The test was carried out in clear black Plexiglas boxes (41.5 cm × 41.5 cm × 41.5 cm) equipped with the video-based Ethovision System (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands). Mice were placed in the center of the apparatus and locomotor behaviors were recorded for 1 h using a video-tracking system. Horizontal locomotor activity was expressed as total ambulatory distance. The number of animals that visited the center zone (15 cm × 15 cm) was recorded. The test box was cleaned with 70% ethanol between each test.
Elevated plus-maze test
To evaluate the anxiety-like behavior of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, an elevated plus-maze test was conducted as described below. The mouse was placed at the center of the maze platform with its head facing an open arm. Animals were tested individually, once only, for 5 min, and the maze was cleaned after each trial to remove any residue or odors. The number of entries into the open and closed arms, the time spent in each arm, and the total distance moved was measured and analyzed using the video-based Ethovision System (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands).
Marble-burying test
To assess the anxiety-like behavior of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, a marble-burying test was conducted as described below. The test was conducted in a new cage (equally sized and illuminated as the home cage) with 20 clear glass marbles (20 mm diameter) evenly spaced in 6 cm depth of sawdust57. During the test, the mice had access to food and water, and the test cage was covered with a metal grid. After 30 min, the number of marbles that were buried in more than two-thirds of sawdust was counted.
Forced swimming test
To assess the depressive-like behavior of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, a forced swimming test was performed as described below. A mouse was placed in a 25 cm glass cylinder (14 cm diameter) containing a 20 cm height of water maintained at 24 ± 2°C; the mouse was forced to swim for 6 min. The immobility duration was recorded using a video-based Ethovision System (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands) during 6 min.
Tests for social behavior
Three-chamber social novelty preference test
To evaluate the social function of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, the three-chamber social novelty preference test was conducted as described in a previous study with minor modifications58. The setup consisted of a rectangular Plexiglas box (w × d × h: 52 × 25 × 23 cm) divided into 3 chambers. In the habituation trial (5 min), empty wire cages (height × diameter: 12 × 8 cm) that can provide contact with another mouse were present in the left and right chambers visible from the middle chamber. In the sociability trial (10 min), a novel mouse from same strain that has never been observed before was placed in the wire cage of the left chamber, whereas no mouse was placed in the wire cage of the right chamber. Immediately after the sociability trial, the social recognition trial (10 min) was conducted wherein another novel mouse was placed in the wire cage of the right chamber. Each trial, the test mouse was placed in the central chamber and explored all three chambers. The time spent in each chamber was recorded. At the end of each test, the apparatus and wire cages were cleaned with paper towels using 70% ethanol.
Reciprocal social interaction test
To evaluate the social interaction of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, the reciprocal social interaction test was performed as described below. The test mouse and an age- and gender-matched stimulus mouse (CD1 ICR or C57/BL) were introduced in a neutral cage with fresh bedding. The cage was used only once. The mice had not interacted previously with other mice. The social interactions between mice were recorded by video camera and PC-based video capture software for 10 min. The time spent in aggressive interactions, such as attacking, wrestling, and biting the dorsal surface, and the time spent in non-aggressive interactions, such as nose-to-nose sniffing, anogenital sniffing, and grooming, were measured manually using the event-recording function in the video-tracking software by a researcher who was blind to the genotype of test mice.
Cotton bud biting test
To test the aggressive behavior of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, the mice were held with the hand without pressing on the trachea, and a sterilized cotton bud was put to the face of the mice. Aggressive behavior was characterized by a violent biting attack against the cotton bud. The mice were given 10 trials. Analysis was performed by counting the total number of biting attacks and the average duration of biting. Five second was set as the maximum time of biting (cut-off time).
Tests for learning and memory
Fear conditioning test
To assess the conditioning memory of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, a fear-conditioning test was performed as previously described59. All training and testing took place in two modular test chambers. The two chambers differed in several aspects: chamber A (13 cm × 14 cm × 30 cm) was a square black acryl box, whereas chamber B (13 cm × 16 cm × 30 cm) was a rectangular plain acryl box. The two chambers were kept in different rooms and cleaned with different substances. Each chamber was located inside a larger, insulated plastic cabinet that provided protection from outside light and noise. The behavior of mice was recorded by digital video cameras mounted above the conditioning chamber. To assess freezing, distal videos were scored by two observers using a stopwatch, and the average of freezing time was analyzed. Freezing was defined as the complete absence of motion, including the motion of the vibrissae, for a minimum of 0.5 s. Each test session was scored continuously in its entirety. The fear conditioning procedure was conducted over 3 days. On day 1, animals were individually placed in the conditioning chamber A and allowed to freely explore for 90 s. The conditioned stimulus (CS) was a 30 s tone of 90 dB, and the unconditioned stimulus (US) was a 0.7 mA electric foot-shock for 1 s delivered at the end of the CS from the floor bar in chamber A. CS-US paring trials were delivered 3 times, and the inter-trial interval was 60 s and 120 s, respectively. The chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol between each set of mice. Each chamber was scented by a paper towel dabbed with mint solution and placed underneath the chamber floor. On day 2, mice were tested for conditioned fear of the training context. Animals were individually placed in the conditioning chamber A. The testing procedure and context were identical to those used on day 1, except the CS and US were not presented. Mice were placed into the chambers for 10 min. The freezing was scored for the entire session. On day 3, the procedure and context were changed in several ways to test the conditioned fear of the tone, the CS, in the absence of contextual cues associated with shock. Animals were individually placed in conditioning chamber B; the chamber was scented with 0.3% acetic acid; the ventilation fan was not operated; the experimenter wore a different style of gloves; the chambers were cleaned with a non-alcohol disinfectant between runs; and the mice were kept in a different holding room before testing. The tone was presented once for 30 s into the session. No shocks were administered. The freezing time was measured for the entire session.
Novel object recognition test
To assess the object recognition memory of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, the novel object recognition task was carried out as previously described60. The experimental apparatus consisted of a black polyvinyl plastic square open field (25 cm × 25 cm × 25 cm). Habituation training was conducted by exposing the animal to the experimental apparatus for 5 min per day in the absence of objects for 2 days. During the training session, mice were placed in the experimental apparatus in the presence of two identical objects and allowed to explore for 5 min. After a retention interval of 4 h, mice were again placed in the apparatus; however, one of the objects was replaced with a novel object. Mice were also allowed to explore for 5 min. The objects chosen for this experiment included a metal cylinder and a plastic rectangular block, both approximately the same height. The durations of time mice spent exploring each object (familiar object, Tfamiliar; novel object, Tnovel) were recorded.
Y-maze test
To assess the working memory of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, a Y-maze test was conducted in a three-arm maze with angles of 120° between the arms; the arms were 40 cm long and 3 cm wide with walls that were each 12 cm high. The maze floor and walls were constructed from dark opaque polyvinyl plastic as previously described61. Mice were initially placed within one arm, and the sequence and number of arm entries were recorded manually for each mouse over an 8 min period. The percentage of triads in which all three arms were represented, i.e., ABC, CAB, or BCA but not BAB, was recorded as an ‘alternation’ to estimate short-term memory. Arms were cleaned with 70% ethanol between each test to remove odors and residues. The alternation score (%) for each mouse was defined as the ratio of the actual number of alternations to the possible number (defined as the total number of arm entries minus two) multiplied by 100 as shown by the following equation: % Alternation = (Number of alternations)/(Total arm entries - 2) × 100. The number of arm entries was used as an indicator of locomotor activity.
Barnes circular maze test
To assess the spatial learning and memory of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, a Barnes circular maze test was performed as described below. The Barnes circular maze is a planar, round white Plexiglas platform (90 cm diameter), 1 m above the floor, with 20 evenly spaced holes (7 cm diameter) located 5 cm from the perimeter. A black escape box (15 × 7 × 7 cm) was placed under one hole. Spatial cues with distinct patterns and shapes were placed on the wall of the testing room. A 60 w light was turned on during the trial. An experimenter remained in the same place with minimal movement throughout the trials. The platform and the escape box were cleaned thoroughly with 70% ethanol and paper towels between each trial to remove olfactory cues. One day before the training trials began, test mice were habituated in the target box for 3 min. The training trials were repeated for 4 consecutive days, and 3 trials per day were performed with 20 min inter-trial intervals. At the beginning of each trial, the mouse was placed in a cylindrical holding chamber (10 cm diameter) located in the center of the maze. After 10 s of holding time, the mouse was allowed to search for the target hole for 3 min. If the mouse failed to find the target hole in 3 min, it was gently guided into the target hole by the experimenter's hands. When the mouse entered the escape box, the light was turned off and the mouse remained undisturbed for 1 min. The movement of the mouse was recorded, and the number of errors made and the latency to find the target hole were measured during the training trials by a video tracking software (Nodulus, Wageningen, Netherlands). On day 5, the probe trial was performed with each mouse. The escape box was removed, and the test mouse was allowed to find the target hole freely for 90 s. During the probe trial, the % time in the target area, the total distance travelled and the latency to find the target hole were measured using the video tracking software.
Tests for sensorimotor gating and motor coordination
Acoustic startle response and prepulse inhibition procedure
To assess the sensorimotor gating function of WT and TLR-2 KO mice, acoustic startle response (ASR) and PPI testing were performed in SR-LAB startle chambers (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) as previously described60. In brief, each chamber consisted of a clear non-restrictive Plexiglas cylinder resting on a platform inside a ventilated box. All ASR test sessions consisted of startle trials (pulse alone) and nostimulus trials (NS). The pulse alone trial consisted of a 40 ms 80, 90, 100, 110, or 120 dB pulse of broad-band noise. After the end of the ASR test sessions, the PPI test sessions were started, and all PPI test sessions consisted of pulse alone, prepulse trials (prepulse + pulse), and NS. The pulse alone trial of the PPI test consisted of a 40 ms 120 dB pulse of broad-band noise. Prepulse + pulse trials consisted of a 20 ms noise prepulse, an 80 ms delay, then a 40 ms 120 dB startle pulse (100 ms onset to onset). The acoustic prepulse intensities were 73, 76, and 82 dB (i.e., 3, 6, and 12 dB above, respectively, the 70 dB background noise). The NS trial consisted of background noise only. There was an average of 21 s (range: 12–30 s) between trials. The amount of PPI was calculated as a percentage score for each acoustic prepulse trial type: % PPI = 100 − (startle response for prepulse + pulse)/(startle response for pulse alone of the PPI test)/100. To investigate whether typical or atypical antipsychotic agents affect sensorimotor gating function in TLR-2 KO mice, haloperidol (1 mg per kg body weight) or clozapine (1 mg per kg body weight) was intraperitoneally administered into mice 1 h before the test.
Rota-rod test
The rota-rod test is a useful method to study the integrity of cerebellar function, as well as the lower motor unit function. The mice were trained to stay on the rotating rod (diameter, 7.3 cm). Following the training period, the mice were tested twice using a paradigm where the rotational speed was increased from 4 to 20 rpm during 25 min. The test trials were conducted once per day for 2 days, and the latency to first falling off the rod or the falling frequency during 25 min was recorded.
Electrophysiological measurements
To examine hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP), a block of the hippocampus of WT and TLR-2 KO mouse brain was removed and sectioned in the coronal plane into 400 μm thick slices using a Microsliver (DTK 1000, Ted Pella, Inc. Redding, CA). These slices were rapidly placed in ice-cold, oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) low-Ca2+/high-Mg2+ dissection buffer containing 5 mM KCl, 1.23 mM NaH2PO4, 26 mM NaHCO3, 10 mM dextrose, 0.5 mM CaCl2, 10 mM MgSO4, and 212.7 mM sucrose and transferred to oxygenated (95% O2 and 5% CO2) artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) containing 124 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 1.23 mM NaH2PO4, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgSO4, 26 mM NaHCO3 and 10 mM dextrose at 28–30°C for at least 1 h before recording. Slices were transferred to a recording chamber where they were perfused continuously with oxygenated ACSF (28–30°C) at a flow rate of 2 ml/min. A hippocampal CA1 field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) was evoked by Schaffer collateral stimulation (0.2 ms current pulses) using a concentric bipolar electrode. Synaptic responses were recorded with ACSF-filled microelectrodes (1–3 MΩ) and were quantified as the initial slope of fEPSP in CA1 as described previously62. Recordings were performed using an AM-1800 microelectrode amplifier (A-M systems, Sequim, WA), a PG 4000A stimulator (Cygnus Technology, Delaware Water Gap, PA), and a SIU-90 isolated current source (Cygnus Technology, Delaware Water Gap, PA). Baseline responses were collected at 0.07 Hz with a stimulation intensity that yielded a 40–60% maximal response. LTP was induced by four episodes of theta burst stimulation (TBS) with 10 s intervals. TBS consisted of ten stimulus trains delivered at 5 Hz; each train consisted of four pulses at 100 Hz. Data from slices with stable recordings (<5% change over the baseline period) were included in the analysis. All data are presented as the mean ± S.E.M. normalized to the preconditioning baseline (at least 20 min of stable responses). The experimenters were blind to mouse genotypes. IGOR software (Wavemetrics, Lake Oswego, OR) was used for digitizing and analyzing the responses.
To measure cerebellar long-term depression (LTD), parasagittal cerebellar brain slices (250 μm thick) were cut at 2–3°C using a vibrating tissue slicer. Brain slices were incubated for 30 min at 35°C and then kept at room temperature before recording. ACSF used for cutting contained the following (mM): 110 choline chloride, 2.5 KCl, 7 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 2.4 sodium-pyruvate, 1.3 sodium-ascorbate, 1.2 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, and 20 d-glucose. ACSF used for incubating contained the following (mM): 124 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 1.3 MgCl2, 2.5 CaCl2, 2.5 NaH2PO4, 26.2 NaHCO3, and 20 d-glucose. Brain slices were placed on an Olympus BX-50WI upright light microscope and perfused with standard ACSF. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were obtained from lobules 3–6 in Purkinje cells using an EPC-9 amplifier (HEKA Elektronik) at 30–32°C as described previously63. A patch electrode (2.5–3.0 MΩ) was filled with the following (in mM): 135 CsMS, 10 CsCl, 10 HEPES, 0.2 EGTA, 4 Na2-ATP, and 0.4 Na3-GTP for sEPSC experiments; and 130 K-gluconate, 10 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.3 EGTA, 2 MgCl2, 4 Na2-ATP, 0.4 Na3-GTP, and 10 Tri-phosphocreatine for the other experiments. The holding potential was −70 mV in voltage-clamp mode. Recordings were excluded if the series resistance varied by >15% and the injection current for the holding potential exceeded 600 pA. Parallel fiber (PF) inputs were activated by an ACSF-filled electrode in the molecular layer and climbing fiber (CF) inputs in the granule cell layer. LTD was induced by 30 repeats of a pairing stimulation for 5 min in the current-clamp mode. Pairing stimulation consisted of 7 PF stimuli at 100 Hz with 1 CF stimulus delayed by 150 ms. Differences in LTP or LTD between WT and TLR-2 KO mice were assessed by two-way ANOVA analysis on the average values of the last 10 min of the recording.
Histological and biochemical measurements
Tissue preparation, Nissl staining, and immunohistochemistry
Mice were anesthetized with an intramuscular injection of Zoletil 50® (10 mg per kg body weight) and perfused transcardially with phosphate buffer (100 mM, pH 7.4) followed by ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde (wt/vol). The brains were removed and post-fixed overnight in phosphate buffer (50 mM, pH 7.4) containing 4% paraformaldehyde. The brains were then immersed in a 30% sucrose solution (in 50 mM phosphate-buffered saline, PBS, wt/vol) and stored at 4°C until sectioning. Frozen brains were sectioned along the coronal plane (30 μm) using a cryostat (Leica Microsystems AG, Germany) and maintained in a storage solution at 4°C. For the Nissl staining, sections were mounted onto gelatin-coated slides, and they were stained with 0.5% cresyl violet (wt/vol), dehydrated through graded alcohols (70%, 80%, 90%, and 100% × 2; vol/vol), placed in xylene, and covered with a coverslip after the addition of Histomount media. To quantify the volume of each brain region, the area of each brain region (6 slices per mouse) was measured using a computerized image-analysis system (Leica Microsystems AG, Wetzlar, Germany). For the immunohistochemistry, free-floating sections were incubated for 24 h in PBS (4°C) containing primary antibody, 0.3% Triton X-100 (vol/vol), 0.5 mg/ml bovine serum albumin, and 1.5% normal horse serum (vol/vol). Afterwards, the sections were incubated for 2 h with biotinylated secondary antibody (1:200 dilution) and then with avidin–biotin–peroxidase complex (1:100 dilution) for 1 h at room temperature. The sections were then reacted with 0.02% diaminobenzidine (DAB, wt/vol) and 0.01% H2O2 (vol/vol) for 3 min. After each incubation step, the sections were washed three times with PBS. Finally, the sections were mounted on gelatin-coated slides, dehydrated in increasing alcohol concentrations, and cleared in xylene.
Volumetric study using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)
MRI images were obtained using a 9.4 T/160 mm animal MRI system (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA). Radiofrequency excitation and signal detection were accomplished with a 72 mm quadrature volume coil and a two channel phased array coil, respectively. The imaging protocol included a T2-weighted Image (T2WI) (TR = 4500 ms; TE = 80 ms; slice thickness = 0.5 mm; and matrix = 256 × 256 (no gap)). Volumetric analyses of MRI images were performed using the VnmrJ 4.0 software provided with the scanner (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA) according to the adult mouse brain atlas64.
Terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase-mediated deoxyuridine triphosphate-biotin nick end labeling staining
The brain sections were processed for terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase-mediated deoxyuridine triphosphate-biotin nick end labeling (TUNEL) staining using an ApopTag® in situ kit (Chemicon International, Billerica, MA). A DAB substrate kit (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) was used for peroxidase staining, and the sections were then counterstained with methyl green. Control sections were incubated in the absence of terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase enzyme. Investigators who were blind to the experimental conditions carried out all TUNEL analyses.
Western blotting
Haloperidol, clozapine, or LTA was injected intraperitoneally into mice 1 h before the sacrifice to acquire samples. Mice were sacrificed by decapitation, and the brain was immediately removed to isolate cortex, hippocampus, midbrain, or cerebellum tissue. Isolated tissues were homogenized in ice-chilled Tris-HCl buffer (20 mM, pH 7.4). Samples of homogenates (15 μg of protein) were subjected to SDS-PAGE under reducing conditions. Proteins were transferred onto PVDF membranes in transfer buffer and further separated at 100 V for 2 h at 4°C to determine expression levels. The Western blots were incubated for 2 h with blocking solution (5% skim milk, wt/vol) at 4°C, then overnight with primary antibodies. The blots were then washed twice with Tween 20/Tris-buffered saline (TTBS), incubated with a 1:5000 dilution of horseradish peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibody for 1 h at room temperature, washed three times with TTBS, and developed by enhanced chemiluminescence (Amersham Life Science, Arlington Heights, IL). Immunoblots were imaged using a bio-imaging program on a LAS-4000 mini imager (Fujifilm Lifescience USA, Stamford, CT) and analyzed using Multi Gauge version 3.2 (Fujifilm Holdings Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). The levels of pAkt and pGSK-3α/β were determined by calculating the ratios of phosphorylated protein to the corresponding total protein on the same membranes. These mean values were normalized to the ratios obtained for the WT animals.
Statistical analysis
All data are shown as the mean ± S.E.M. and were analyzed using unpaired two-tailed Student's t-test, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Tukey's post hoc comparison or two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni's post hoc test. All statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism 5.0 (Graphpad, La Jolla, CA). The statistical analyses are presented in Supplementary Table 1.
Author Contributions
S.J.P., J.Y.L., J.H.R. and T.Y.Y. designed the research. S.J.P. and J.Y.L. performed the experiments and analyzed the data. S.J.K. and S.C. provided the electrophysiological experiments. S.J.P., J.Y.L., T.Y.Y. and J.H.R. wrote the manuscript.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) and funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning (No. 2012R1A2A2A02012332, No. 2011-0010884 (the Basic Science Research Program), No. 2010-0019349 (Pioneer Research Center Program)) and a grant of the Korea Health Technology R&D Project through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute (KHIDI), funded by the Ministry of Health & Welfare (grant number: HI13C14600000). The authors thank J.H. Yi and H.G. Kim for their valuable comments. This research presented here was submitted by the first author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a doctorate degree in pharmaceutical science at Kyung Hee University.
References
- van Os J. & Kapur S. Schizophrenia. Lancet 374, 635–645 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross C. A. et al. Neurobiology of schizophrenia. Neuron 52, 139–153 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keshavan M. S. & Hogarty G. E. Brain maturational processes and delayed onset in schizophrenia. Dev Psychopathol 11, 525–543 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennand K. J. et al. Modelling schizophrenia using human induced pluripotent stem cells. Nature 473, 221–225 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altamura A. C., Pozzoli S., Fiorentini A. & Dell'osso, B. Neurodevelopment and inflammatory patterns in schizophrenia in relation to pathophysiology. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 42, 63–70 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer U., Schwarz M. J. & Muller N. Inflammatory processes in schizophrenia: a promising neuroimmunological target for the treatment of negative/cognitive symptoms and beyond. Pharmacol Ther 132, 96–110 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Potvin S. et al. Inflammatory cytokine alterations in schizophrenia: a systematic quantitative review. Biol Psychiatry 63, 801–808 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connor C. M. et al. Maternal immune activation alters behavior in adult offspring, with subtle changes in the cortical transcriptome and epigenome. Schizophr Res 140, 175–184 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibney S. M. & Drexhage H. A. Evidence for a dysregulated immune system in the etiology of psychiatric disorders. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol 8, 900–920 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson K. V. Toll signaling pathways in the innate immune response. Curr Opin Immunol 12, 13–19 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawai T., Akira S. The role of pattern-recognition receptors in innate immunity: update on Toll-like receptors. Nat Immunol 11, 373–384 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun E. et al. Toll-like receptor 3 inhibits memory retention and constrains adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 15625–15630 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun E., Griffioen K. J. & Mattson M. P. Toll-like receptor signaling in neural plasticity and disease. Trends Neurosci 34, 269–281 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls A. et al. Toll-like receptors modulate adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Nat Cell Biol 9, 1081–1088 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun E. et al. Evidence for a Developmental Role for TLR4 in Learning and Memory. PLoS One 7, e47522 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fazel S. et al. Schizophrenia and violence: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med 6, e1000120 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freyberg Z., Ferrando S. J. & Javitch J. A. Roles of the Akt/GSK-3 and Wnt signaling pathways in schizophrenia and antipsychotic drug action. Am J Psychiatry 167, 388–396 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emamian E. S. et al. Convergent evidence for impaired AKT1-GSK3β signaling in schizophrenia. Nat Genet 36, 131–137 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porsolt R. D., Moser P. C. & Castagne V. Behavioral indices in antipsychotic drug discovery. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 333, 632–638 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellenbroek B. A. & Cools A. R. Animal models for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Behav Pharmacol 11, 223–233 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell S. B., Weber M. & Geyer M. A. Genetic models of sensorimotor gating: relevance to neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 12, 251–318 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frantseva M. V. et al. Evidence for impaired long-term potentiation in schizophrenia and its relationship to motor skill learning. Cereb Cortex 18, 990–996 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyden E. S. et al. Selective engagement of plasticity mechanisms for motor memory storage. Neuron 51, 823–834 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vita A., De Peri L., Silenzi C. & Dieci M. Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of quantitative magnetic resonance imaging studies. Schizophr Res 82, 75–88 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos-Sierra S. et al. Mal connects TLR2 to PI3Kinase activation and phagocyte polarization. EMBO J 28, 2018–2027 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oranje B. et al. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on the prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in patients with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol 22, 359–365 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapfhamer D. et al. Protein Phosphatase 2α and glycogen synthase kinase 3 signaling modulate prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response by altering cortical M-Type potassium channel activity. J Neurosci 30, 8830–8840 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janeway C. A. Jr. & Medzhitov R. Innate immune recognition. Annu Rev Immunol 20, 197–216 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akira S., Uematsu S. & Takeuchi O. Pathogen recognition and innate immunity. Cell 124, 783–801 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanke M. L. & Kielian T. Toll-like receptors in health and disease in the brain: mechanisms and therapeutic potential. Clin Sci (Lond) 121, 367–387 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lathia J. D. et al. Toll-like receptor 3 is a negative regulator of embryonic neural progenitor cell proliferation. J Neurosci 28, 13978–13984 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun E. et al. TLR2 activation inhibits embryonic neural progenitor cell proliferation. J Neurochem 114, 462–474 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Consortium SWGotPG. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 511, 421–427 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnone D. et al. Magnetic resonance imaging studies in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry 195, 194–201 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller N. et al. Impaired monocyte activation in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 198, 341–346 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lien Y. J. et al. The multidimensionality of schizotypy in nonpsychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia and its applications in ordered subsets linkage analysis of schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 153B, 1–9 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang W. S. et al. Association between genetic polymorphisms of Toll-like receptor 2 (TLR2) and schizophrenia in the Korean population. Gene 526, 182–186 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones C. A., Watson D. J. & Fone K. C. Animal models of schizophrenia. Br J Pharmacol 164, 1162–1194 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyakawa T. et al. Conditional calcineurin knockout mice exhibit multiple abnormal behaviors related to schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100, 8987–8992 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell C. M. & Miyakawa T. Schizophrenia-relevant behavioral testing in rodent models: a uniquely human disorder? Biol Psychiatry 59, 1198–1207 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckley P. F., Miller B. J., Lehrer D. S. & Castle D. J. Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 35, 383–402 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Tuathaigh C. M., Kirby B. P., Moran P. M. & Waddington J. L. Mutant mouse models: genotype-phenotype relationships to negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 36, 271–288 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff D. L. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: a window on the brain in schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 4, 349–371 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braff D. L., Geyer M. A. & Swerdlow N. R. Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 156, 234–258 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perry W., Minassian A., Feifel D. & Braff D. L. Sensorimotor gating deficits in bipolar disorder patients with acute psychotic mania. Biol Psychiatry 50, 418–424 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerdlow N. R. et al. Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 199, 331–388 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kent J. S. et al. Motor deficits in schizophrenia quantified by nonlinear analysis of postural sway. PLoS One 7, e41808 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Halloran C. J., Kinsella G. J. & Storey E. The cerebellum and neuropsychological functioning: a critical review. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 34, 35–56 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gulsuner S. et al. Spatial and temporal mapping of de novo mutations in schizophrenia to a fetal prefrontal cortical network. Cell 154, 518–529 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo N. et al. A sensitive period for GABAergic interneurons in the dentate gyrus in modulating sensorimotor gating. J Neurosci 33, 6691–6704 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hattiangady B. & Shetty A. K. Neural stem cell grafting counteracts hippocampal injury-mediated impairments in mood, memory, and neurogenesis. Stem Cells Transl Med 1, 696–708 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim J. E. et al. The effects of MyD88 deficiency on exploratory activity, anxiety, motor coordination, and spatial learning in C57BL/6 and APPswe/PS1dE9 mice. Behav Brain Res 227, 36–42 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arbibe L. et al. Toll-like receptor 2-mediated NF-kappa B activation requires a Rac1-dependent pathway. Nat Immunol 1, 533–540 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaidanovich-Beilin O. & Woodgett J. R. GSK-3: Functional Insights from Cell Biology and Animal Models. Front Mol Neurosci 4, 40 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svenningsson P. et al. Diverse psychotomimetics act through a common signaling pathway. Science 302, 1412–1415 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi O. et al. Differential roles of TLR2 and TLR4 in recognition of gram-negative and gram-positive bacterial cell wall components. Immunity 11, 443–451 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosienko V. et al. Exaggerated aggression and decreased anxiety in mice deficient in brain serotonin. Transl Psychiatry 2, e122 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naert A., Callaerts-Vegh Z. & D'Hooge R. Nocturnal hyperactivity, increased social novelty preference and delayed extinction of fear responses in post-weaning socially isolated mice. Brain Res Bull 85, 354–362 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxe M. D. et al. Ablation of hippocampal neurogenesis impairs contextual fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103, 17501–17506 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X. et al. The ameliorating effects of 5,7-dihydroxy-6-methoxy-2(4-phenoxyphenyl)-4H-chromene-4-one, an oroxylin A derivative, against memory impairment and sensorimotor gating deficit in mice. Arch Pharm Res 36, 854–863 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee H. E. et al. Ethanolic extract of the seed of zizyphus jujuba var. spinosa ameliorates cognitive impairment induced by cholinergic blockade in mice. Biomol & Ther 21, 299–306 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seo J. et al. Regulation of hippocampal long-term potentiation and long-term depression by diacylglycerol kinase zeta. Hippocampus 22, 1018–1026 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chae H. G. et al. Transient receptor potential canonical channels regulate the induction of cerebellar long-term depression. J Neurosci 32, 12909–12914 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullmann J. F. et al. An MRI atlas of the mouse basal ganglia. Brain Struct Funct 219, 1343–1353 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.