Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Feb 17.
Published in final edited form as: Fertil Steril. 2014 Apr;101(4):898–904. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2014.02.025

Effects of Hormone Therapy on Cognition and Mood

Barbara Fischer 1, Carey Gleason 1,2,3, Sanjay Asthana 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC4330961  NIHMSID: NIHMS658989  PMID: 24680649

Abstract

Objective

Results of the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) and Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) suggested that hormone therapy (HT) may be detrimental to cognitive health. This article reviews clinical studies which address issues relevant to those results.

Design

A search of PubMed and Web of Science was conducted using the search terms HT and cognition, HT and mood. Clinical and observational studies were selected if they were published after the year 2000. Theories of HT mechanisms of action, pharmacology, biology, and observational and clinical trials are discussed.

Results

While observational and clinical trials show conflicting findings, methodological considerations must be acknowledged. HT formulation and dose, route of administration, timing of initiation, length of treatment, and health of participants all contribute to inconsistencies in results. Transdermal estradiol and micronized progesterone administered at time of menopause are generally associated with cognitive and affective benefit.

Conclusions

At the present time, results from existing studies are equivocal regarding the benefits of HT on cognition and affect. Future studies, such as the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) should address methodological inconsistencies in order to provide clearer answers to this important question.

Keywords: Hormone therapy, cognition, mood

Introduction

A woman born in the United States in the second half of the twentieth century is expected to live between 71 and 83 years (1), such that twenty to thirty years may be spent in post-menopause. Precipitated by a substantial decline in circulating endogenous estrogen, post-menopause is by no means a disease state. However, relative to pre-menopause, it represents a distinctly different physiological stage, for which there may be systemic consequences (e.g. osteoporosis). Indeed, estrogen, a complex gonadal hormone with receptor sites throughout the brain and periphery (2), appears to facilitate numerous functions, including autonomic regulation, mood and cognition. As a result, HT has been widely prescribed to partially replace naturally occurring declines in hormones (both estrogen and progesterone) at menopause.

The general term estrogen refers to three different hormones, estradiol (E2), estrone (E1), and estriol (E3). Estradiol, the most potent form of the hormone, is predominant prior to menopause, while estrone is the primary form postmenopausally. During pregnancy, estriol is produced by the placenta and becomes predominant (3). Prior to menopause, E2 and E1 are produced by the ovaries and circulate freely throughout the body, dynamically interacting with each other, such that E1 may metabolize into E2 and vice versa (4). After menopause, however, estradiol is produced from aromatization of estrone and androstenedione in adipose tissue, and the volume of circulating estradiol drops to approximately 10% of prior levels (5). By contrast, E1 declines to a much lesser degree, and continues to be synthesized by fatty tissue (6). Thus the ratio of estradiol to estrone changes markedly at menopause.

Neurobiology of estrogen’s effects on cognition and mood

Estrogen works synergistically with many biological systems to promote physical, cognitive and affective function. Basic science reveals that administration of estrogen (estradiol alone and estradiol plus progesterone) results in increased levels of antioxidants, reduces free radicals and substantially lowers oxidative damage to mitochondrial DNA (7). Critical to neuronal health, estrogen also regulates glucose and oxidative metabolism, mitochondrial function, and promotes ATP (8). Indeed, declines in these processes are characteristic of neurodegenerative diseases such as AD (9).

Estrogen promotes neural plasticity both directly and indirectly. Normal aging is associated with a decline in dendritic spines and synapses in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and hippocampus (see (10) for review, (11)). In animal studies, impaired memory function is correlated with reductions in hippocampal dendritic spines (12). Estrogen treatment (ER α and β receptors) promotes the growth of long thin spines in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (13)(14)(15). Higher levels of pre-synaptic estrogen alpha receptors are associated with stronger memory performance in ovariectomized animals treated with exogenous estrogen (15). Neurotrophins such as nerve growth factor (NGF) and brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) represent another mechanism of neural plasticity in the brain. These substances facilitate the growth of dendritic spines in the hippocampus (see (13) for review). In vitro investigations support a favorable effect of estrogens on the activity of neurotrophins, (16)(17). By increasing hippocampal BDNF levels, estrogen transiently increases dendritic spines, thereby potentiating opportunity for increased connectivity and plasticity (14).

Estradiol has been shown to favorably influence several factors potentially altering the health of the neurovascular unit, including anti-oxidative protection from by-products of mitochondrial energy metabolism (reactive oxygen species (ROS) and peroxides) (18), and reduced thrombolic (19) and endothelial inflammatory activity (20) (21). Additionally, estrogens exert a number of more general neuroprotective actions to maintain the health of the cerebrovasculature. For example, estradiol reportedly prevents glutamate-induced excitotoxicity (22) (23), protects neurons from ischemic insults (24), and offers antioxidant protection against several sources of cell damage and death (25) (reviewed in (26)). By facilitating anti-apoptotic actions of Bcl proteins, estrogens may also protect neurons from toxin-induced and ischemic brain injury (27) (28). Like other actions of the estrogens, characteristic of the substrate to which the hormones are applied, age appears to influence their efficacy. Preliminary evidence suggests that anti-inflammatory and neuroprotective effects of the estrogens may be suppressed or abolished in aged animals (29) (30).

Estrogen also interacts with a number of neurotransmitters affecting cognition and mood. Animal research reveals that long-term use of estrogen prevented declines in cholinergic nerve cell fiber length and density at menopause (31). In humans, the negative effect of cholinergic challenge on cognitive performance and neural activation patterns was counteracted by prior exposure to estradiol (32)(33). Of note, naturally occurring higher levels of estrone were associated with poorer cognition, specifically working memory performance (33).

Estrogen modulates mood via the serotonergic system at the cellular and synaptic levels (34) (35). Moreover, although tryptophan deletion reduced neural activation during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a verbal working memory task, women who received pre-treatment with transdermal estradiol displayed less signal change/activation disruption (36). Estrogen contributes to upregulation of serotonin 5-HT1 receptors and to downregulation of 5-HT-2 receptors and monoamine oxidase activity (37) (38). A serotonin agonist, estrogen responds via multiple mechanisms in brain regions supporting mood regulation (35) (37).

Implications of the WHIMS Study

Prior to publications of the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) and Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) (39)(40) (41)(42), hormone therapy (HT) was thought to be neuroprotective and a large body of basic science and generally supportive epidemiological evidence suggested that HT reduced the risk of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) by approximately 50% (see reviews (43) (44)). Approach to hormone therapy (HT) changed radically however, following the results of the WHI and WHIMS. The largest clinical trials of HT ever conducted, these studies revealed an increased risk of cancer, dementia and cognitive decline with prolonged administration of conjugated equine estrogen (CEE), and the studies were discontinued. Additionally, the Heart and Estrogen/Progestin Replacement Study (HERS), an investigation of long-term effects of HT on cognition and physical health, produced consistent results, indicating that long term use of CEE was associated with poorer cognitive performance (45). In the wake of the WHI and HERS publications, there was an abrupt reversal of opinion, leading many to dismiss the previous epidemiological evidence as spurious.

While WHIMS and HERS were pivotal studies, a closer evaluation suggested methodological concerns that might at least partially explain their results. Both studies utilized orally administered opposed CEE (Prempro), which exerts different effects on the CNS than unopposed, transdermal estrogen. Additionally, participants in both studies were older women over the age of sixty five. Various theories were offered to help assimilate the conflicting findings. We review several of these below.

Critical window theory

Differences in cognitive response to HT have been identified depending on women’s age or timing of HT initiation. These discrepancies support the notion of a critical window during which HT may be beneficially initiated. The theory posits that HT administered at or around the time of menopause may elicit improved cognition, while HT initiated five years or more after menopause may engender no cognitive benefit or even cognitive detriment. Indeed a number of studies have revealed a positive association between early initiation of HT and cognition (for reviews, see (2) (46)). For example, exposure to HT at peri-menopause resulted in improved memory and hippocampal function later in life (47). The REMEMBER pilot observational study revealed that early HT initiators demonstrated stronger global cognition and executive function than later initiators (48). Additionally, shorter time between menopause and initiation of HT was associated with larger hippocampal volume, bilaterally (49). Support for the critical window theory has not been uniformly positive, however. A recent study found no evidence of influence of time since menopause on cognitive outcomes (50). Moreover, a report on pre-randomization data from the Early versus Late Intervention Trial (ELITE) specifically investigating the effect of length of time in menopause on cognition, found no influence of time since menopause on the relationship between verbal memory and endogenous concentrations of free estradiol (51). In light of these equivocal findings, additional research is needed to investigate the influence of time since menopause on HT efficacy.

Healthy Cell Bias Theory

A related framework with which to understand conflicting data of prior clinical studies is the “healthy cell bias theory,” proposed by Brinton and colleagues. This theory purports that women who use HT during the menopausal transition may be systematically healthier than non-users, and it is these fundamental differences that result in HT’s salutary effects (52) (9). Specifically, evidence is emerging that the health of the neuronal substrate may influence the effect of estrogen exposure such that the same group of compounds, the estrogens, could be both beneficial and harmful, depending upon when during the life time the estrogens are initiated. In healthy neurons, estrogen appears to provide multiple neuroprotective benefits, described above, including increased mitochondrial respiration and ATP generation, while also protecting cells by improving the cell’s tolerance for calcium influx, and triggering antioxidant actions (8). These actions may sustain cardiovascular and cognitive health (8) (9). However, in diseased cells, calcium homeostasis is disrupted, and estrogen-induced calcium influx now becomes deleterious to the neuron incapable of maintaining calcium homeostasis(9). Thus, while women may benefit from HT before cells are exposed to neuropathological changes associated with disease (53), once disease processes affect cells, HT may not confer cognitive benefit. Indeed, as neurological health deteriorates, estrogenic effects may facilitate disease pathogenesis (9).

Differences in HT formulation-Potential Reasons for Discrepant Results of Prior Studies

As noted above, differences in hormone formulation may result in divergent cognitive and affective outcomes. Estrogen preparations are available in a variety of forms. Transdermal estrogen is composed of E2, while oral CEE, a formulation of at least 10 different hormones, is comprised mainly of E1 (54). Although both forms alleviate menopausal symptoms, HT utilizing E2 more effectively recalibrates the estradiol/estrone ratio to approximate pre-menopausal levels. Moreover, oral CEE is metabolized by the liver which alters the estrone: estradiol ratio, and introduces risk of venous thromboembolic complications (55). Oral HT also increases binding glycoproteins that lowers levels of biologically active free estradiol (56). By contrast, transdermal estradiol bypasses hepatic metabolism, avoiding these potential complications.

Differences in Progesterone Preparations

Progesterone, synthesized by the ovaries and adrenal glands, also has widely distributed receptors. Among its multiple roles is to function in an antagonistic manner to estrogen, modulating estrogen’s trophic effects (57). In women who still have a uterus, HT is prescribed in combination form (estrogen together with progesterone) in order to counteract the potential for endometrial cancer which estrogen would otherwise facilitate. As with estrogen, progesterone may be taken in different forms. Medroxyprogesterone acetate (MPA), commonly prescribed with CEE in the HT Prempro, is a synthetic progestin, which has been found to ameliorate the potential for cognitive benefit engendered in other hormones such as E2 (see (58) for a review). Prempro has been associated with numerous side effects, including heart disease, adverse changes in lipid metabolism, and thrombo-embolic processes (59). By contrast, micronized progestin (MP), bioidentical to the progesterone produced in the ovaries, has not been associated with such side effects. Taken together, evidence suggests that the type of hormone preparation utilized may significantly influence outcomes.

Hormone Therapy and Cognition

Observational Trials

Despite findings from earlier epidemiological studies failing to document cognitive enhancing effects of estrogen and reduced risk of AD (60), a number of studies have reported salutary effects of HT on cognition. Early results of the Cache County study found that use of HT for at least 10 years was associated with a 2.5 times lower incidence of AD compared to men (61). More recent results from the same study revealed that any HT initiated within five years of menopause resulted in a 30% reduced risk of AD, particularly if it was used for 10 years or more. Additionally, opposed HT initiated after the age of 70 increased the risk of AD. HT use has also been shown to affect neural activation patterns (62). HT initiated at perimenopause and utilized continuously was associated with increased activation in left hippocampus and decreased bilateral activation in parahippocampal gyrus, relative to never users. These patterns were associated with improved memory performance on a task performed in the fMRI scanner (47).

Randomized Controlled Trials

While favorable results have been obtained from randomized controlled trials investigating the effects of HT on cognition, the evidence has been mixed. Combination HT (either norethisterone and estradiol or tibolone) was associated with improved semantic memory relative to placebo (63). Similarly, in a cross-over study of early menopausal women taking CEE or placebo, participants on CEE exhibited better oral reading and semantic memory relative to control (64). Consistent with these results, compared to placebo, 12 weeks of estradiol was associated with improved verbal memory in healthy post-menopausal women (65). Improvements in verbal memory (and very mild declines in auditory attention) were also observed among postmenopausal women randomized to six months of estradiol valerate plus norethisterone versus placebo (66). Sherwin and Grigorova (67) found that while CEE plus MPA was unassociated with any cognitive change after a three month trial, CEE plus MP resulted in a mild (non-clinically significant) decline in verbal memory and a significant improvement in working memory. Conversely, forty eight weeks of E2 valerate plus placebo was associated with increased immediate verbal memory but decline in simple attention relative to E2 plus testosterone (68). HT has also been shown to be beneficial among women with AD. Short-term high-dose estradiol was associated with significantly improved verbal memory, visual memory and attention in older post-menopausal women with AD (69). Furthermore, three months of HT (both opposed and unopposed) was associated with improvements in visual memory among postmenopausal women with moderate AD (70).

Findings from two randomized controlled trials of estradiol versus placebo produced equivocal results. In one cross-over study, transdermal estradiol for 12 weeks resulted in cognitive benefit only in faster reaction time, while performance on measures of verbal and visual memory, attention and working memory did not differ by group (71). Another study found that estradiol was associated with greater improvement on immediate face recall, whereas the placebo group demonstrated stronger verbal memory and fluency (72). Two other studies did not display any group differences (estradiol, estradiol plus micronized progesterone relative to placebo; testosterone or oral E2 relative to placebo) on cognitive measures (73) (50). Finally, the Cognitive Complaints in Early Menopause Trial (COGENT) study revealed no cognitive benefit of Prempro compared to placebo; indeed there was a close to significant mild decline in verbal memory in the HT group (74). The potential reasons for divergent findings reflecting methodological shortcomings are discussed in the previous sections.

Hormone Therapy and Mood/Emotion

The higher prevalence of depressive disorders in women than men has been well established (35), and sex hormones contribute to this differential disease burden. Although not all women experience mood changes, for some women, substantial changes in estrogen during physiological transitions such as pregnancy and menopause are particularly associated with worsening of depressive symptoms. While the precipitous drop in estrogen levels during the menopausal transition is associated with higher depression rates, the evidence for treatment of depressive symptoms with exogenous estrogen remains unclear (37).

Observational Studies

Although results of observational studies have been mixed, recent investigations have not found positive associations between HT and improved mood. Early cross-sectional studies demonstrated correlations between estrogen use and reductions in depressive symptoms (75), but later research has not supported these findings. Use of HT in the previous month was associated with worse depression and anxiety among 6000 peri- and postmenopausal women. No differences in mood were found based on type of estrogen preparation (76). Amore et al. (77) found that relative to peri-menopause, postmenopausal women experienced higher levels of depression and sexual symptoms, and HT (type not specified) was not associated with any change in mood. Neither HT (type not specified) nor menopausal status was associated with additional mood improvement in women taking anti-depressant medication (78). Moreover, postmenopausal women taking HT for three months (either estrogen alone or estrogen plus progesterone) did not exhibit any change in mood when evaluated cross-sectionally. However, a separate group of 18 women taking HT (type not specified) who were followed prospectively for three months did demonstrate improvements in mood (79). Importantly, since hormone preparation was often not indicated, it is difficult to evaluate the degree to which type of estrogen or route of administration may have contributed to results.

Randomized Controlled Trials

By contrast, findings from clinical trials support a positive effect of estrogen on mood. Despite negative findings from one small, single-blind study in which 14 women randomized to two forms of HT revealed no effect of either drug on mood after six months (63), five recent randomized, placebo-controlled trials demonstrated significant beneficial associations between HT and mood. Eighty percent of women randomized to estradiol reported significantly decreased mood symptoms after three or six weeks, compared to only 22% of women on placebo (80). Similarly, estradiol resulted in improved mood in 68% of peri-menopausal women with depressive disorders, whereas only 20% of women on placebo experienced similar benefit (81). Onalan et al., (82) prospectively examined the effects of three preparations of HT (CEE plus 2.5 mg MPA, CEE plus 5 mg MPA, or 2.5 mg of tibolone) on mood over a twelve month period. Results showed that all three preparations improved depressive symptoms in menopausal women relative to placebo. In a randomized, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial, transdermal estradiol was associated with significantly improved psychological well-being compared to placebo (71). Moreover, administration of transdermal estradiol with or without norethisterone over three months was associated with significantly improved scores on anxiety and depression screening measures (83). Taken together, the data suggest that for the subset of women who experience depression associated with menopause, HT may be beneficial in addressing mood symptoms.

Shortcomings of the clinical trials to date

Dose, timing of HT initiation, duration of HT, and type of HT preparation are all important factors to consider in interpreting study results. Much of the research demonstrating no benefit of HT on cognition and mood has been marred by methodological flaws or inconsistencies. Many studies were small, initiated hormone treatment more than five years after the menopausal transition, utilized a less potent form of estrogen (e.g. CEE), a small dose, or a brief duration of treatment. As noted earlier, the effects of progesterone (especially MPA) are generally antagonistic to those of estrogen, and studies of cognition and affect using opposed HT display weaker results. Route of administration also affects outcomes. Moreover, there has been little attempt to mimic the pre-menopausal hormonal milieu, in which a complex array of hormones interact temporally and synergistically to promote cognitive, affective and physical function. Steady-state HT may produce different effects from a cyclic pattern (46). While inconsistencies in findings remain, trials which address these methodological considerations remain promising.

Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS)

One of the studies that will address several methodological short-comings of prior trials is the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) and its ancillary study, the KEEPS Cognitive and Affective Study (KEEPS-Cog). All women enrolled in the clinical trial are non-hysterectomized, within three years of their last menstrual period, and are at low-risk for cardiovascular disease. The KEEPS-Cog will evaluate the efficacy of 4 years of therapy with oral CEE and transdermal estradiol on mood and cognition. Micronized progesterone or matching placebo is administered cyclically, with dosing occurring during the first 12 days of the 30-day cycle. The parent KEEPS examines surrogate markers of cardiovascular disease, osteoporosis, lipid profile, and coagulation cascade in this group of healthy recently menopausal women. The findings of this study will test many of the theoretical explanations offered since the WHI study. For example, the long-term follow-up (anticipated for 20–25 years) of the KEEPS cohort will provide the opportunity to evaluate whether HT initiated at menopause may reduce the incidence of neurodegenerative diseases such as AD at later ages (4) (54).

Conclusions

The relationship between estrogen, cognition and mood is complex. HT has not yet succeeded in achieving a postmenopausal hormonal milieu that approaches the intricate and dynamic premenopausal interactions between reproductive hormones and their multiple target systems. Despite negative findings which are marred by methodological concerns however, evidence continues to mount regarding the beneficial effect of HT, especially transdermal estradiol and micronized progestin, on cognitive and affective function. Like the KEEPS, future trials should systematically address inconsistencies in methodology in order to accurately characterize HT’s effects. The potential for benefit is large. In addition to the possibility of increasing postmenopausal cognitive and affective function, HT may eventually represent a means to delay or even prevent neurodegenerative diseases.

References

  • 1.United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs PD. Population and Vital Statistics Report. Table 3. 2007 Downloaded on February 3, 2014 from http://unstats.un.org/UNSD/demographic/sconcerns/mortality/mort2.htm.
  • 2.Genazzani AR, Pluchino N, Luisi S, Luisi M. Estrogen, cognition and female ageing. Hum Reprod Update. 2007 Mar-Apr;13(2):175–87. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dml042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Coelingh Bennink HJ. Are all estrogens the same? Maturitas. 2004 Apr 15;47(4):269–75. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2003.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gleason CE, Carlsson CM, Johnson S, Atwood C, Asthana S. Clinical pharmacology and differential cognitive efficacy of estrogen preparations. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2005 Jun;1052:93–115. doi: 10.1196/annals.1347.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Asthana S. Estrogen and cognition: a true relationship? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004 Feb;52(2):316–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52078.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rannevik G, Jeppsson S, Johnell O, Bjerre B, Laurell-Borulf Y, Svanberg L. A longitudinal study of the perimenopausal transition: altered profiles of steroid and pituitary hormones, SHBG and bone mineral density. Maturitas. 1995 Feb;21(2):103–13. doi: 10.1016/0378-5122(94)00869-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Irwin RW, Yao J, Hamilton RT, Cadenas E, Brinton RD, Nilsen J. Progesterone and estrogen regulate oxidative metabolism in brain mitochondria. Endocrinology. 2008 Jun;149(6):3167–75. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Brinton RD. Estrogen regulation of glucose metabolism and mitochondrial function: therapeutic implications for prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Adv Drug Deliv Rev. 2008 Oct-Nov;60(13–14):1504–11. doi: 10.1016/j.addr.2008.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Brinton RD. The healthy cell bias of estrogen action: mitochondrial bioenergetics and neurological implications. Trends Neurosci. 2008 Oct;31(10):529–37. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bailey ME, Wang AC, Hao J, Janssen WG, Hara Y, Dumitriu D, et al. Interactive effects of age and estrogen on cortical neurons: implications for cognitive aging. Neuroscience. 2011 Sep 15;191:148–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.05.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hao J, Rapp PR, Janssen WG, Lou W, Lasley BL, Hof PR, Morrison JH. Interactive effects of age and estrogen on cognition and pyramidal neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007 Jul 3;104(27):11465–70. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704757104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Luine VN, Wallace ME, Frankfurt M. Age-related deficits in spatial memory and hippocampal spines in virgin, female Fischer 344 rats. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2011;2011:316386. doi: 10.1155/2011/316386. Available from. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Luine V, Frankfurt M. Interactions between estradiol, BDNF and dendritic spines in promoting memory. Neuroscience. 2013 Jun 3;239:34–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Srivastava DP. Two-step wiring plasticity--a mechanism for estrogen-induced rewiring of cortical circuits. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2012 Aug;131(1–2):17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jsbmb.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang AC, Hara Y, Janssen WG, Rapp PR, Morrison JH. Synaptic estrogen receptor-alpha levels in prefrontal cortex in female rhesus monkeys and their correlation with cognitive performance. J Neurosci. 2010 Sep 22;30(38):12770–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3192-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Green PS, Simpkins JW. Neuroprotective effects of estrogens: potential mechanisms of action. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2000 Jul-Aug;18(4–5):347–58. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(00)00017-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Solum DT, Handa RJ. Estrogen regulates the development of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA and protein in the rat hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2002 Apr 1;22(7):2650–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02650.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stirone C, Duckles SP, Krause DN, Procaccio V. Estrogen increases mitochondrial efficiency and reduces oxidative stress in cerebral blood vessels. Mol Pharmacol. 2005 Oct;68(4):959–65. doi: 10.1124/mol.105.014662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ono H, Sasaki Y, Bamba E, Seki J, Giddings JC, Yamamoto J. Cerebral thrombosis and microcirculation of the rat during the oestrus cycle and after ovariectomy. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol. 2002 Jan-Feb;29(1–2):73–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1681.2002.03600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sunday L, Osuna C, Krause DN, Duckles SP. Age alters cerebrovascular inflammation and effects of estrogen. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2007 May;292(5):H2333–40. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.01057.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Johnson AB, Bake S, Lewis DK, Sohrabji F. Temporal expression of IL-1beta protein and mRNA in the brain after systemic LPS injection is affected by age and estrogen. J Neuroimmunol. 2006 May;174(1–2):82–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2006.01.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sribnick EA, Del Re AM, Ray SK, Woodward JJ, Banik NL. Estrogen attenuates glutamate-induced cell death by inhibiting Ca2+ influx through L-type voltage-gated Ca2+ channels. Brain Res. 2009 Jun 18;1276:159–70. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.04.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nilsen J, Diaz Brinton R. Mechanism of estrogen-mediated neuroprotection: regulation of mitochondrial calcium and Bcl-2 expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003 Mar 4;100(5):2842–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0438041100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bagetta G, Chiappetta O, Amantea D, Iannone M, Rotiroti D, Costa A, et al. Estradiol reduces cytochrome c translocation and minimizes hippocampal damage caused by transient global ischemia in rat. Neurosci Lett. 2004 Sep 16;368(1):87–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.06.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Prediger ME, Gamaro GD, Crema LM, Fontella FU, Dalmaz C. Estradiol protects against oxidative stress induced by chronic variate stress. Neurochem Res. 2004 Oct;29:1923–30. doi: 10.1023/b:nere.0000042219.98446.e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chiueh C, Lee S, Andoh T, Murphy D. Induction of antioxidative and antiapoptotic thioredoxin supports neuroprotective hypothesis of estrogen. Endocrine. 2003 Jun;21(1):27–31. doi: 10.1385/endo:21:1:27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Dubal DB, Shughrue PJ, Wilson ME, Merchenthaler I, Wise PM. Estradiol modulates bcl-2 in cerebral ischemia: a potential role for estrogen receptors. J Neurosci. 1999 Aug 1;19(15):6385–93. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06385.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Garcia-Segura LM, Cardona-Gomez P, Naftolin F, Chowen JA. Estradiol upregulates Bcl-2 expression in adult brain neurons. Neuroreport. 1998 Mar 9;9(4):593–7. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199803090-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bake S, Ma L, Sohrabji F. Estrogen receptor-alpha overexpression suppresses 17beta-estradiol-mediated vascular endothelial growth factor expression and activation of survival kinases. Endocrinology. 2008 Aug;149(8):3881–9. doi: 10.1210/en.2008-0288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nordell VL, Scarborough MM, Buchanan AK, Sohrabji F. Differential effects of estrogen in the injured forebrain of young adult and reproductive senescent animals. Neurobiol Aging. 2003 Sep;24(5):733–43. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00193-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tinkler GP, Tobin JR, Voytko ML. Effects of two years of estrogen loss or replacement on nucleus basalis cholinergic neurons and cholinergic fibers to the dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex of monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 2004 Feb 16;469(4):507–21. doi: 10.1002/cne.11028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dumas J, Hancur-Bucci C, Naylor M, Sites C, Newhouse P. Estrogen treatment effects on anticholinergic-induced cognitive dysfunction in normal postmenopausal women. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006 Sep;31(9):2065–78. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dumas JA, Kutz AM, Naylor MR, Johnson JV, Newhouse PA. Estradiol treatment altered anticholinergic-related brain activation during working memory in postmenopausal women. Neuroimage. 2012 Apr 2;60(2):1394–403. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Henderson JA, Bethea CL. Differential effects of ovarian steroids and raloxifene on serotonin 1A and 2C receptor protein expression in macaques. Endocrine. 2008 Jun;33(3):285–93. doi: 10.1007/s12020-008-9087-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Halbreich U, Kahn LS. Role of estrogen in the aetiology and treatment of mood disorders. CNS Drugs. 2001;15(10):797–817. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200115100-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Epperson CN, Amin Z, Ruparel K, Gur R, Loughead J. Interactive effects of estrogen and serotonin on brain activation during working memory and affective processing in menopausal women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012 Mar;37(3):372–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wharton W, Gleason CE, Olson SR, Carlsson CM, Asthana S. Neurobiological Underpinnings of the Estrogen - Mood Relationship. Curr Psychiatry Rev. 2012 Aug 1;8(3):247–256. doi: 10.2174/157340012800792957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chakravorty SG, Halbreich U. The influence of estrogen on monoamine oxidase activity. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1997;33(2):229–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Rapp SR, Thal L, Wallace RB, Ockene JK, et al. Estrogen plus progestin and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2003 May 28;289(20):2651–62. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Kuller L, Rapp SR, Thal L, Lane DS, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. 2004 Jun 23;291(24):2947–58. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Rapp SR, Espeland MA, Shumaker SA, Henderson VW, Brunner RL, Manson JE, et al. Effect of estrogen plus progestin on global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. May 28;289(20):2663–72. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.20.2663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Espeland MA, Rapp SR, Shumaker SA, Brunner R, Manson JE, Sherwin BB, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and global cognitive function in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. JAMA. 2004 Jun 23;291(24):2959–68. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Simpkins JW, Perez E, Wang X, Yang S, Wen Y, Singh M. The potential for estrogens in preventing Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 2009 Jan;2(1):31–49. doi: 10.1177/1756285608100427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hogervorst E, Williams J, Budge M, Riedel W, Jolles J. The nature of the effect of female gonadal hormone replacement therapy on cognitive function in post-menopausal women: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience. 2000;101(3):485–512. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00410-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Grady D, Yaffe K, Kristof M, Lin F, Richards C, Barrett-Connor E. Effect of postmenopausal hormone therapy on cognitive function: the Heart and Estrogen/progestin Replacement Study. Am J Med. 2002 Nov;113(7):543–8. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9343(02)01270-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Maki PM. Critical window hypothesis of hormone therapy and cognition: a scientific update on clinical studies. Menopause. 2013 Jun;20(6):695–709. doi: 10.1097/GME.0b013e3182960cf8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Maki PM, Dennerstein L, Clark M, Guthrie J, LaMontagne P, Fornelli D, et al. Perimenopausal use of hormone therapy is associated with enhanced memory and hippocampal function later in life. Brain Res. 2011 Mar 16;1379:232–43. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.MacLennan AH, Henderson VW, Paine BJ, Mathias J, Ramsay EN, Ryan P, et al. Hormone therapy, timing of initiation, and cognition in women aged older than 60 years: the REMEMBER pilot study. Menopause. 2006 Jan-Feb;13(1):28–36. doi: 10.1097/01.gme.0000191204.38664.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Kramer AF. A cross-sectional study of hormone treatment and hippocampal volume in postmenopausal women: evidence for a limited window of opportunity. Neuropsychology. 2010 Jan;24(1):68–76. doi: 10.1037/a0017292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Kocoska-Maras L, Zethraeus N, Rådestad AF, Ellingsen T, von Schoultz B, Johannesson M, et al. A randomized trial of the effect of testosterone and estrogen on verbal fluency, verbal memory, and spatial ability in healthy postmenopausal women. Fertil Steril. 2011 Jan;95(1):152–7. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2010.05.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Henderson VW, St John JA, Hodis HN, McCleary CA, Stanczyk FZ, Karim R, et al. Cognition, mood, and physiological concentrations of sex hormones in the early and late postmenopause. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013 Dec 10;110(50):20290–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312353110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Gleason CE, Dowling NM, Friedman E, Wharton W, Asthana S. Using predictors of hormone therapy use to model the healthy user bias: how does healthy user status influence cognitive effects of hormone therapy? Menopause. 2012 May 19;19(5):524–33. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e318238ff2c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hogervorst E. Effects Of Gonadal Hormones On Cognitive Behavior In Elderly Men And Women. J Neuroendocrinol. 2013 Jul 29;(5):1182–95. doi: 10.1111/jne.12080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wharton W, Gleason CE, Miller VM, Asthana S. Rationale and design of the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) and the KEEPS cognitive and affective sub study (KEEPS Cog) Brain Res. 2013 Jun 13;1514:12–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hogervorst E, Yaffe K, Richards M, Huppert FA. Hormone replacement therapy to maintain cognitive function in women with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009 Jan 21;(1) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003799.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Weissberger AJ, Ho KK, Lazarus L. Contrasting effects of oral and transdermal routes of estrogen replacement therapy on 24-hour growth hormone (GH) secretion, insulin-like growth factor I, and GH-binding protein in postmenopausal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1991 Feb;72(2):374–81. doi: 10.1210/jcem-72-2-374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Brinton RD, Thompson RF, Foy MR, Baudry M, Wang J, Finch CE, et al. Progesterone receptors: form and function in brain. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008 May;29(2):313–39. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Walf AA, Paris JJ, Rhodes ME, Simpkins JW, Frye CA. Divergent mechanisms for trophic actions of estrogens in the brain and peripheral tissues. Brain Res. 2011 Mar 16;1379:119–36. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.de Lignières B. Oral micronized progesterone. Clin Ther. 1999;21(1):41–60. doi: 10.1016/S0149-2918(00)88267-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Buckwalter JG, Crooks VC, Robins SB, Petitti DB. Advanced Age. 2004:182–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52053.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Zandi PP, Carlson MC, Plassman BL, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Mayer LS, Steffens DC, et al. Hormone replacement therapy and incidence of Alzheimer disease in older women: the Cache County Study. JAMA. 2002 Nov 6;288(17):2123–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.17.2123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Shao H, Breitner JCS, Whitmer RA, Wang J, Hayden K, Wengreen H, et al. Hormone therapy and Alzheimer disease dementia: new findings from the Cache County Study. Neurology. 2012 Oct 30;79(18):1846–52. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318271f823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Albertazzi P, Natale V, Barbolini C, Teglio L, Di Micco R. The effect of tibolone versus continuous combined norethisterone acetate and oestradiol on memory, libido and mood of postmenopausal women: a pilot study. Maturitas. 2000 Oct 31;36(3):223–9. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5122(00)00147-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Shaywitz SE, Naftolin F, Zelterman D, Marchione KE, Holahan JM, Palter SF, et al. Better oral reading and short-term memory in midlife, postmenopausal women taking estrogen. Menopause. 2003 Sep-Oct;10(5):420–6. doi: 10.1097/01.GME.0000060241.02837.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Joffe H, Hall JE, Gruber S, Sarmiento Ia, Cohen LS, Yurgelun-Todd D, et al. Estrogen therapy selectively enhances prefrontal cognitive processes: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study with functional magnetic resonance imaging in perimenopausal and recently postmenopausal women. Menopause. 2006 May-Jun;13(3):411–22. doi: 10.1097/01.gme.0000189618.48774.7b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Alhola P, Tuomisto H, Saarinen R, Portin R, Kalleinen N, Polo-Kantola P. Estrogen + progestin therapy and cognition: a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind study. J Obstet Gynaecol Res. 2010 Aug;36(4):796–802. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0756.2010.01214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sherwin BB, Grigorova M. Differential effects of estrogen and micronized progesterone or medroxyprogesterone acetate on cognition in postmenopausal women. Fertil Steril. 2011 Aug;96(2):399–403. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2011.05.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Möller MC, Bartfai AB, Rådestad AF. Effects of testosterone and estrogen replacement on memory function. Menopause. 2010 Sep-Oct;17(5):983–9. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181dc2e40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Asthana S, Baker LD, Craft S, Stanczyk FZ, Veith RC, Raskind MA, et al. High-dose estradiol improves cognition for women with AD: results of a randomized study. Neurology. 2001 Aug 28;57(4):605–12. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.4.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Wharton W, Baker LD, Gleason CE, Dowling M, Barnet JH, Johnson S, et al. Short-term hormone therapy with transdermal estradiol improves cognition for postmenopausal women with Alzheimer’s disease: results of a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011;26(3):495–505. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Schiff R, Bulpitt CJ, Wesnes KA, Rajkumar C. Short-term transdermal estradiol therapy, cognition and depressive symptoms in healthy older women. A randomised placebo controlled pilot cross-over study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005 May;30(4):309–15. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Almeida OP, Lautenschlager NT, Vasikaran S, Leedman P, Gelavis A, Flicker L. A 20-week randomized controlled trial of estradiol replacement therapy for women aged 70 years and older: effect on mood, cognition and quality of life. Neurobiol Aging. 2006 Jan;27(1):141–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Wolf OT, Heinrich AB, Hanstein B, Kirschbaum C. Estradiol or estradiol/progesterone treatment in older women: no strong effects on cognition. Neurolbiol Aging. 2005 Jul;26:1029–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Maki PM, Gast MJ, Vieweg aJ, Burriss SW, Yaffe K. Hormone therapy in menopausal women with cognitive complaints: a randomized, double-blind trial. Neurology. 2007 Sep 25;69(13):1322–30. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000277275.42504.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Whooley MA, Grady D, Cauley JA. Postmenopausal estrogen therapy and depressive symptoms in older women. J Gen Intern Med. 2000 Aug;15(8):535–41. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2000.04029.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Toffol E, Heikinheimo O, Partonen T. Associations between psychological well-being, mental health, and hormone therapy in perimenopausal and postmenopausal women: results of two population-based studies. Menopause. 2013 Jun;20:1–10. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e318278eec1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Amore M, Di Donato P, Berti A, Palareti A, Chirico C, Papalini A, et al. Sexual and psychological symptoms in the climacteric years. Maturitas. 2007 Mar 20;56(3):303–11. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2006.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Kornstein SG, Toups M, Rush aJ, Wisniewski SR, Thase ME, Luther J, et al. Do menopausal status and use of hormone therapy affect antidepressant treatment response? Findings from the Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) study. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 2013 Feb;22(2):121–31. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2012.3479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Stephens C, Bristow V, Pachana NA. HRT and everyday memory at menopause_: a comparison of two samples of mid-aged women. 2006;43(1):37–57. doi: 10.1300/J013v43n01_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Schmidt PJ, Nieman L, Danaceau MA, Tobin MB, Roca CA, Murphy JH, et al. Estrogen replacement in perimenopause-related depression: a preliminary report. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2000 Aug;183(2):414–20. doi: 10.1067/mob.2000.106004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Soares CN, Almeida OP, Joffe H, Cohen LS. Efficacy of estradiol for the treatment of depressive disorders in perimenopausal women: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001 Jun;58(6):529–34. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Onalan G, Onalan R, Selam B, Akar M, Gunenc Z, Topcuoglu A. Mood scores in relation to hormone replacement therapies during menopause: a prospective randomized trial. Tohoku J Exp Med. 2005 Nov;207(3):223–31. doi: 10.1620/tjem.207.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Karsidag C, Karsidag A, Buyukbayrak E, kars B, Pirimoglu M, Unal O, Turan C. Comparison of effects of two different hormone therapies on mood and symptomatic postmenopausal women. Arch Neuropsychiatry. 2012;49:39–43. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES