Summary
It is well established that perception is largely multisensory [1]; often served by modalities such as touch, vision, and hearing that detect stimuli emanating from a common point in space [2, 3]; and processed by brain tissue maps that are spatially aligned [4]. However, the neural interactions among modalities that share no spatial stimulus domain yet are essential for robust perception within noisy environments remain uncharacterized. Drosophila melanogaster makes its living navigating food odor plumes. Odor acts to increase the strength of gaze-stabilizing optomotor reflexes [5] to keep the animal aligned within an invisible plume, facilitating odor localization in free flight [6–8]. Here, we investigate the cellular mechanism for cross-modal behavioral interactions. We characterize a wide-field motion-selective interneuron of the lobula plate that shares anatomical and physiological similarities with the “Hx” neuron identified in larger flies [9, 10]. Drosophila Hx exhibits cross-modal enhancement of visual responses by paired odor, and presynaptic inputs to the lobula plate are required for behavioral odor tracking but are not themselves the target of odor modulation, nor is the neighboring wide-field “HSE” neuron [11]. Octopaminergic neurons mediating increased visual responses upon flight initiation [12] also show odor-evoked calcium modulations and form connections with Hx dendrites. Finally, restoring synaptic vesicle trafficking within the octopaminergic neurons of animals carrying a null mutation for all aminergic signaling [13] is sufficient to restore odor-tracking behavior. These results are the first to demonstrate cellular mechanisms underlying visual-olfactory integration required for odor localization in fruit flies, which may be representative of adaptive multisensory interactions across taxa.
Highlights
-
•
Small-field motion detection neurons are required for odor-tracking behavior
-
•
Responses of a directional wide-field interneuron (Hx) increase with paired odor
-
•
Odor activates octopaminergic (OA) neurons that innervate the visual system
-
•
OA cells contact Hx; OA vesicle trafficking is required for odor-tracking behavior
Wasserman et al. report that a directionally selective wide-field motion-detecting neuron (Hx) in the fly increases response gain with paired odor. This multimodal interaction is dependent upon vesicle trafficking from octopaminergic neurons, which are themselves responsive to odor and make cell-cell contact with Hx.
Results and Discussion
In addition to feedback from head movements [14–18], a fly in flight stabilizes its gaze by optomotor steering movements of the wings that turn the whole body [19]. The strength of steering optomotor responses increases when flies experience an appetitive odor [5]. Here we tethered a fly rigidly within a flight simulator composed of a wrap-around electronic display and equipped with an odor port (Figure 1A) to measure the optomotor impulse response to a rapid rotation of the visual panorama [21]. Pairing an appetitive food odor (vinegar) with the visual stimulus results in a roughly 40% increase in the optomotor response (OMR), which is assessed by measuring the mean difference in wing beat amplitude across the two wings (ΔWBA) elicited by an impulse in yaw velocity (Figure 1B), consistent with prior measurements [5].
Optomotor responses in Drosophila can be elicited by optogenetic activation of tangential wide-field collating neurons HSE and HSN housed in the third optic ganglion, the lobula plate [22]. To examine whether motion integrating circuitry of the lobula plate is involved in odor-enhanced OMRs, we genetically hyperpolarized the small-field columnar neurons T4 and T5, which supply retinotopic motion signals to the lobula plate [23]. Using the same magnetic-tether flight simulator (Figure 1C) applied to demonstrate the dependence of self-generated visual motion signals for active plume tracking [24], we measured the animals’ ability to locate and stabilize their heading within a vinegar plume. We divided plume-tracking behavior into three components: (1) initial detection, defined by the proportion of flies that oriented themselves within ±10° of the odor nozzle—flies that did not do so were not included in the subsequent analysis; (2) acquisition, defined by time spent within the plume over the first 10 s of the trial; and (3) continuous tracking, defined by how much of the final 10 s of the trial the fly spent oriented within the plume (Figure 1D). We found no significant difference between the proportions of T4T5-blocked versus control flies that detected the plume (chi-square test, p > 0.05). Similarly, blocking T4T5 did not significantly alter the mean time spent in the plume during the acquisition phase, but T4T5-blocked flies were unable to sustain plume tracking for the duration of the trial, in comparison to controls (Figures 1D and 1E). This shows that whereas the lack of motion signals carried by T4T5 to the lobula plate does not compromise the animals’ ability to detect or initially localize an odor plume, local motion signals are required to stabilize flight heading within the plume. This is consistent with the finding that switching the high-contrast grating displayed in the flight arena to an equiluminant grayscale, thereby reducing optic flow generated by the fly’s own movements, eliminates its ability to remain within the plume [24].
A lobula plate tangential cell (LPTC) was recently identified anatomically in Drosophila, along with a number of neurons within higher-order olfactory regions of the mushroom bodies, by its shared expression of the Odd-skipped transcription factor [9]. The tangential dendritic arbor of this LPTC spans the dorsal projection of the lobula plate (Figure 2Bi), tightly restricted to layer 2 (Figures 2Bii and 2Biii), which is the layer receiving back-to-front directional motion input from the columnar T4T5 terminals [23]. The axon projects heterolateral to the cell body and dendrites [9]. To characterize its motion-coding properties, we expressed a genetically encoded calcium indicator, GCaMP6m [25], under the Odd-Gal4 driver [9] and recorded cellular activity under a two-photon excitation imaging system equipped with an LED display [26] (Figure 2A). Imaging from dendritic regions of interest (ROIs) (see Experimental Procedures) in response to a narrow vertical bar, we demonstrate that this cell is excited by back-to-front motion across the ipsilateral eye within a 50° receptive field positioned just ipsilateral to the visual midline (Figure 2C) and is more excited by progressively wider randomly textured bars (Figure 2D). We found no systematic response differences within small ROIs spanning the tangential dendritic arbor (data not shown) and therefore focused subsequent imaging analysis on a primary dendritic branch that was identifiable in each preparation (Figure 2B, white box). To further explore wide-field response properties, we varied the orientation of a full-field grating, demonstrating that this cell is strongly tuned to front-to-back motion oriented along the horizontal body axis (Figure 2E) and, like other wide-field Drosophila LPTCs [11, 23], exhibits a 1 Hz temporal frequency optimum (Figure 2F). The matched directional preferences and layer specificity strongly suggest that Hx receives local motion signals from the T4T5 system but do not preclude other potential inputs. The neuronal morphology and receptive-field properties of this cell are strongly reminiscent of the Hx neuron characterized in blowflies [10], and we refer to it thusly hereafter.
Motivated by the transcription-factor spatial profile shared with higher-order olfactory projection neurons, we sought to determine whether Hx was cross-modally activated by odor. The two-photon recording preparation and LED display was equipped with a laminar flow olfactometer (Figure 2A). We presented a regime of five repeated 10-s epochs of back-to-front wide-field motion interspersed with rest periods. The second motion epoch was accompanied by a 10-s odor pulse (delivered bilaterally). There was a subtle yet significant increase in the motion-elicited excitatory response of Hx during paired odor presentation (epoch 2, Figure 3A), observed within each individual fly preparation tested (Figure 3B) but absent in water vapor controls (Figure 3C). To determine whether the primary site of visual-olfactory integration resided with Hx or the local motion detectors presynaptic to the lobula plate, we performed the same experiment and recorded the intracellular activity of T4T5 cells. The T4T5-Gal4 driver labels cell processes within the medulla, lobula, and lobula plate ([22, 23] and Figure 3D), and we found no differences between responses from ROIs imaged within the processes of these neuropils (chi-square test, p > 0.05), nor did we observe any changes in the excitatory motion responses of T4T5 ROIs found within the lobula plate upon paired odor presentation (Figures 3E and 3F). These results reject the possibility that odor-enhanced responses in Hx represent general arousal phenomena and confirm that the site of cross-modal interaction resides within wide-field-integrating lobula plate neurons rather than presynaptic local motion detectors. To assess whether odor activates all LPTCs, we examined the activity of HSE, a neighboring neuron to Hx that is selective for horizontal motion (HSE [11]; Figure 3G), but we did not observe odor-evoked changes in the visual responses of this cell (Figures 3H and 3I).
Visual responses by LPTCs are modulated by the onset of locomotion [27, 28], and this increase in response gain is mediated by octopaminergic innervation [29–31]. We reasoned that octopamine release might also be triggered by olfactory signaling within the visual system to modulate Hx responses. We first determined that the octopaminergic terminals innervating the lobula plate show increased GCaMP fluorescence in response to an odor pulse (Figure 4A), which was demonstrated in each fly tested (Figure 4B). To examine whether these octopaminergic interneurons make synapses with Hx, we made use of a genetic construct that recombines GFP between two cells in close contact (GFP reconstitution across synaptic partners [GRASP] [32]). Expressing one inactive half of the split-GFP within the Tdc2 octopaminergic neurons and the other half within Hx resulted in GFP puncta distributed within the lobula plate (Figure 4C) in a pattern similar to the dendritic profile of Hx (Figure 4C, inset), indicating synapses or other close cell-cell connections such as gap junctions between Tdc2 and Hx. In addition to implicating Tdc2 in the olfactory modulation of Hx, our GRASP data also support prior findings demonstrating that octopaminergic signaling in the brain is necessary for locomotion-induced gain in LPTCs [12].
Likely owing to the role of Odd-skipped in development, driving neuronal inactivators with Hx-Gal4 is lethal and nevertheless would have been impossible to evaluate for visual-olfactory integration due to its expression in both visual and olfactory centers [25]. Therefore, we reasoned that if octopaminergic modulation of visual circuitry is important for odor-tracking behavior, then the absence of octopaminergic signaling throughout the brain should strongly perturb odor-tracking behavior. To test this hypothesis, we used a fly strain carrying a null (loss-of-function) mutation in the Drosophila vesicular monoamine transporter (dVMAT) [13]. Rescue with a DVMAT transgene in octopaminergic neurons, but not with dopaminergic or serotonergic neurons, is sufficient to restore plume-tracking behavior (Figure 4D). As a negative control, we tested animals rescued with a DVMAT trafficking mutant (Tdc2-Gal4/Δ3VMAT [13]); these animals were unable to maintain their heading within the odor plume of the olfactory flight simulator (Figure 4D). These three lines of evidence—odor activation of Tdc2 cells, GFP puncta (GRASP) between Hx and Tdc2 neurons, and the rescue of olfactory tracking when synaptic release by octopaminergic cells is restored—provide a parsimonious interpretation that odor-driven octopamine release modulates the gain of visual circuitry.
Octopamine mediates locomotion-induced modulation of another LPTC, the HSE neuron [12], which is not activated by odor (Figure 3H). This provides an exciting experimental platform for broader investigation into how aminergic signaling differentially modulates postsynaptic targets within the same neuropil. It is possible that, like Hx, HSE is also modulated by odor, but that the effect is observable only when superposed with a flight-activated increase in visual response gain [33]. Additionally, like norepinephrine, octopamine acts through multiple receptor-signaling pathways having wide-ranging influences over cellular physiology. One receptor class (OCTα-R) increases calcium entry, while another (OCTβ-R) elevates intracellular cAMP levels [34] to act as either an agonist or an antagonist on synaptic and behavioral plasticity in an octopamine receptor-dependent fashion [34]. Differential receptor expression could in turn mediate differential octopaminergic neuromodulation of visual circuitry.
In summary, we have revealed a novel cellular cross-modal interaction that could support behavioral findings whereby food odor detection increases visual stability in an odor plume. Future work could elaborate additional neuronal pathways supporting related cross-modal behaviors such as enhanced salience of visual objects by odor [35]. These cross-modal interactions provide a mechanism to dynamically enhance sensory perception in a contextually appropriate manner.
Experimental Procedures
Animals
For behavior experiments, we used wild-type D. melanogaster, 3- to 6-day-old posteclosion females. Other lines used for behavior and imaging experiments included T4T5-Gal4 (Bloomington ID 40034), Tdc2-Gal4 (Bloomington ID 9313), UAS-Kir2.1-EGFP (Bloomington ID 6596), UAS-mCD8::GFP (Bloomington ID 5137), HSE-Gal4 (Bloomington ID 49211), UAS-GCaMP6m (Bloomington ID 42748), UAS-GCaMP6s (Bloomington ID 42749), and Odd-Skipped-Gal4 [9]. GRASP constructs were generated using the transgenes Odd-Gal4 [9], Tdc2-LexA [36], and UAS-CD4::spGFP1-10; LexAop-CD4::spGFP11 [37]. Random individuals were selected from a population for each experimental group according to genotype. No experimenter blinding was done.
Behavior
Closed-Loop Magnetic-Tether Flight Simulator
The magnetic-tether flight arena allows a fly to steer freely in the yaw plane, allowing assessment of odor plume-tracking capability, and has been described in detail previously [6, 24, 38].
Rigid-Tether Flight Simulator
The rigid-tether arena records a fixed fly’s wing kinematic responses to visual stimuli, closing a feedback loop to allow the animal to control the velocity of image motion on the display or allow the assessment of visual response gain under open-loop feedback conditions, and has been described in detail previously [5, 20]. Odor was delivered through a narrow nozzle as reported previously [5].
In order to quantify the response of the fly to panoramic yaw motion, we use a white-noise method for estimating the yaw impulse response for each individual animal. The impulse response, g(t), of the fly’s steering plant is measured by cross-correlating a time-varying and spectrally broad sequence of velocity impulses, x(t), with the time-varying output signal produced by difference in wing beat amplitudes (ΔWBA), y(t). The kernel function [5] represents the steering response to an impulsive step in the pattern display position by one pixel (3.75°). Impulse responses to water control and odor [5] were fit and parameters were calculated as described previously [39].
Calcium Imaging
Adult female D. melanogaster expressing the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCamp6m [40] under one of the four Gal4 drivers were anesthetized under cold sedation. Imaging was performed with a two-photon excitation scanning microscope (Intelligent Imaging Innovations). We used a 20×/NA 1.0 water-immersion objective lens (Carl Zeiss). Laser power was regulated to 10–20 mW measured at the focus of the objective lens. Images were collected at 8–11 Hz and 300–500 nm/pixel. Temporal registration with input stimuli was achieved by recording a voltage pulse at the completion of each frame that was output to our data acquisition device (National Instruments). Visual stimulus was produced by a 12–20 panel arena that was oriented orthogonal to the anterior-posterior axis of the head, subtending 216° of visual azimuth and 120° of elevation on the retina (IO Rodeo) using open-source MATLAB packages (https://bitbucket.org/mreiser/panels/src). Stimulus and data acquisition were controlled by custom-written software in MATLAB (The Mathworks).
Visual Stimuli for Sensory Integration Experiments
One of eight randomly textured display patterns was selected at random for each trial and held static for 10-s periods of rest or 10-s periods of motion stimulation. For odor recordings from Tdc2-Gal4 neurons, the visual pattern was on but stationary. In all visual motion experiments, the pattern moved with a velocity of 22°/s for 10 s. Preparations that showed too much movement artifact or fluorescent bleaching, or that did not demonstrate ΔF/F responses over background levels for at least two presentations of the full stimulus set, were excluded from analysis.
Odor Delivery
Odor was injected at 50 ml/min (Sensirion mass flow controller) into a 200 ml/min constant air stream (Sable Systems intelligent mass flow control unit) and removed via vacuum. A miniature photoionization detector (miniPID, Aurora Scientific) was used to confirm presence and absence of odor during and after odor pulse.
Author Contributions
S.M.W., J.W.A., and M.A.F. designed experiments. A.G., D.E.K., and C.L. provided reagents and advised on genetic procedures. S.M.W., J.W.A., P.L., J.N., A.L.W., M.F.K., and C.L. collected and/or analyzed data. S.M.W. and M.A.F. wrote the manuscript.
Acknowledgments
Support for this work was provided by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH076900 to D.E.K.), the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, and the Joanne and George Miller and Family Endowed Chair in Depression Research at the UCLA Brain Research Institute (D.E.K.); the Wellcome Trust (WT085026MA to C.L.); and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute and US Air Force Office of Scientific Research (FA9550-12-1-0034) (M.A.F.).
Footnotes
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Supplemental Information
References
- 1.Shams L., Kim R. Crossmodal influences on visual perception. Phys. Life Rev. 2010;7:269–284. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Amedi A., Malach R., Hendler T., Peled S., Zohary E. Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:324–330. doi: 10.1038/85201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Watkins S., Shams L., Josephs O., Rees G. Activity in human V1 follows multisensory perception. Neuroimage. 2007;37:572–578. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Meredith M.A., Stein B.E. Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J. Neurophysiol. 1986;56:640–662. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.3.640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chow D.M., Theobald J.C., Frye M.A. An olfactory circuit increases the fidelity of visual behavior. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:15035–15047. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1736-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Duistermars B.J., Frye M.A. Crossmodal visual input for odor tracking during fly flight. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:270–275. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Frye M.A., Tarsitano M., Dickinson M.H. Odor localization requires visual feedback during free flight in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Exp. Biol. 2003;206:843–855. doi: 10.1242/jeb.00175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Stewart F.J., Baker D.A., Webb B. A model of visual-olfactory integration for odour localisation in free-flying fruit flies. J. Exp. Biol. 2010;213:1886–1900. doi: 10.1242/jeb.026526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Levy P., Larsen C. Odd-skipped labels a group of distinct neurons associated with the mushroom body and optic lobe in the adult Drosophila brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 2013;521:3716–3740. doi: 10.1002/cne.23375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Krapp H.G., Hengstenberg B., Hengstenberg R. Dendritic structure and receptive-field organization of optic flow processing interneurons in the fly. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;79:1902–1917. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.4.1902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Schnell B., Joesch M., Forstner F., Raghu S.V., Otsuna H., Ito K., Borst A., Reiff D.F. Processing of horizontal optic flow in three visual interneurons of the Drosophila brain. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:1646–1657. doi: 10.1152/jn.00950.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Suver M.P., Mamiya A., Dickinson M.H. Octopamine neurons mediate flight-induced modulation of visual processing in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:2294–2302. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Grygoruk A., Chen A., Martin C.A., Lawal H.O., Fei H., Gutierrez G., Biedermann T., Najibi R., Hadi R., Chouhan A.K. The redistribution of Drosophila vesicular monoamine transporter mutants from synaptic vesicles to large dense-core vesicles impairs amine-dependent behaviors. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:6924–6937. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0694-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Land M.F. Head movement of flies during visually guided flight. Nature. 1973;243:299–300. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schilstra C., van Hateren J.H. Blowfly flight and optic flow. I. Thorax kinematics and flight dynamics. J. Exp. Biol. 1999;202:1481–1490. doi: 10.1242/jeb.202.11.1481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Schilstra C., van Hateren J.H. Stabilizing gaze in flying blowflies. Nature. 1998;395:654. doi: 10.1038/27114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hengstenberg R. Gaze control in the blowfly Calliphora: a multisensory, two-stage integration process. Semin. Neurosci. 1991;3:19–29. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Duistermars B.J., Care R.A., Frye M.A. Binocular interactions underlying the classic optomotor responses of flying flies. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2012;6:6. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Götz K.G. Flight control in Drosophila by visual perception of motion. Kybernetik. 1968;4:199–208. doi: 10.1007/BF00272517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Reiser M.B., Dickinson M.H. A modular display system for insect behavioral neuroscience. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2008;167:127–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Theobald J.C., Ringach D.L., Frye M.A. Dynamics of optomotor responses in Drosophila to perturbations in optic flow. J. Exp. Biol. 2010;213:1366–1375. doi: 10.1242/jeb.037945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Haikala V., Joesch M., Borst A., Mauss A.S. Optogenetic control of fly optomotor responses. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:13927–13934. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0340-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Maisak M.S., Haag J., Ammer G., Serbe E., Meier M., Leonhardt A., Schilling T., Bahl A., Rubin G.M., Nern A. A directional tuning map of Drosophila elementary motion detectors. Nature. 2013;500:212–216. doi: 10.1038/nature12320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Krishnan P., Duistermars B.J., Frye M.A. Odor identity influences tracking of temporally patterned plumes in Drosophila. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12:62. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-12-62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Chen T.-W., Wardill T.J., Sun Y., Pulver S.R., Renninger S.L., Baohan A., Schreiter E.R., Kerr R.A., Orger M.B., Jayaraman V. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Tuthill J.C., Chiappe M.E., Reiser M.B. Neural correlates of illusory motion perception in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:9685–9690. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100062108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chiappe M.E., Seelig J.D., Reiser M.B., Jayaraman V. Walking modulates speed sensitivity in Drosophila motion vision. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1470–1475. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Jung S.N., Borst A., Haag J. Flight activity alters velocity tuning of fly motion-sensitive neurons. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:9231–9237. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1138-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.van Breugel F., Suver M.P., Dickinson M.H. Octopaminergic modulation of the visual flight speed regulator of Drosophila. J. Exp. Biol. 2014;217:1737–1744. doi: 10.1242/jeb.098665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Longden K.D., Krapp H.G. Octopaminergic modulation of temporal frequency coding in an identified optic flow-processing interneuron. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2010;4:153. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.de Haan R., Lee Y.-J., Nordström K. Octopaminergic modulation of contrast sensitivity. Front Integr Neurosci. 2012;6:55. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Feinberg E.H., Vanhoven M.K., Bendesky A., Wang G., Fetter R.D., Shen K., Bargmann C.I. GFP Reconstitution Across Synaptic Partners (GRASP) defines cell contacts and synapses in living nervous systems. Neuron. 2008;57:353–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Maimon G., Straw A.D., Dickinson M.H. Active flight increases the gain of visual motion processing in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:393–399. doi: 10.1038/nn.2492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Farooqui T. Review of octopamine in insect nervous systems. Open Access Insect Physiol. 2012;4:1–17. [Google Scholar]
- 35.van Breugel F., Dickinson M.H. Plume-tracking behavior of flying Drosophila emerges from a set of distinct sensory-motor reflexes. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:274–286. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Burke C.J., Huetteroth W., Owald D., Perisse E., Krashes M.J., Das G., Gohl D., Silies M., Certel S., Waddell S. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492:433–437. doi: 10.1038/nature11614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gordon M.D., Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron. 2009;61:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Maimon G., Straw A.D., Dickinson M.H. A simple vision-based algorithm for decision making in flying Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:464–470. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fox J.L., Aptekar J.W., Zolotova N.M., Shoemaker P.A., Frye M.A. Figure-ground discrimination behavior in Drosophila. I. Spatial organization of wing-steering responses. J. Exp. Biol. 2014;217:558–569. doi: 10.1242/jeb.097220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Akerboom J., Chen T.-W., Wardill T.J., Tian L., Marvin J.S., Mutlu S., Calderón N.C., Esposti F., Borghuis B.G., Sun X.R. Optimization of a GCaMP calcium indicator for neural activity imaging. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:13819–13840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2601-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.