Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2015 Feb 17;10(2):e0117339. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117339

Enhanced Responsiveness to Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors during Lactation

Nicholas J Jury 1,*,¤, Betsy A McCormick 4, Nelson D Horseman 1,3, Stephen C Benoit 5, Karen A Gregerson 1,2,3
Editor: Jodi Pawluski6
PMCID: PMC4331562  PMID: 25689282

Abstract

The physiology of mood regulation in the postpartum is poorly understood despite the fact that postpartum depression (PPD) is a common pathology. Serotonergic mechanisms and their dysfunction are widely presumed to be involved, which has led us to investigate whether lactation induces changes in central or peripheral serotonin (5-HT) systems and related affective behaviors. Brain sections from lactating (day 10 postpartum) and age-matched nulliparous (non-pregnant) C57BL/6J mice were processed for 5-HT immunohistochemistry. The total number of 5-HT immunostained cells and optical density were measured. Lactating mice exhibited lower immunoreactive 5-HT and intensity in the dorsal raphe nucleus when compared with nulliparous controls. Serum 5-HT was quantified from lactating and nulliparous mice using radioimmunoassay. Serum 5-HT concentrations were higher in lactating mice than in nulliparous controls. Affective behavior was assessed in lactating and non-lactating females ten days postpartum, as well as in nulliparous controls using the forced swim test (FST) and marble burying task (MBT). Animals were treated for the preceding five days with a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI, citalopram, 5mg/kg/day) or vehicle. Lactating mice exhibited a lower baseline immobility time during the FST and buried fewer marbles during the MBT as compared to nulliparous controls. Citalopram treatment changed these behaviors in lactating mice with further reductions in immobility during the FST and decreased marble burying. In contrast, the same regimen of citalopram treatment had no effect on these behaviors in either non-lactating postpartum or nulliparous females. Our findings demonstrate changes in both central and peripheral 5-HT systems associated with lactation, independent of pregnancy. They also demonstrate a significant interaction of lactation and responsiveness to SSRI treatment, which has important implications in the treatment of PPD. Although recent evidence has cast doubt on the effectiveness of SSRIs, these results support their therapeutic use in the treatment of PPD.

Introduction

Mood alterations during the postpartum and postpartum depression (PPD) adversely affect not only the mother, but also disrupt bonding and the health of the child [1]. The relationship between untreated maternal depression and negative infant outcomes, even through adolescence, are well established [2,3,4]. PPD (defined in the psychiatric nomenclature as a major depressive disorder with a specifier of onset during pregnancy and/or following childbirth) affects 10–20% of women who give birth [5,6,7,8].

From a biological perspective, it is an evolutionary imperative that female mammals cope with the physiological stresses of pregnancy, child birth, and lactation without suffering the debilitations inherent with PPD. From this biological perspective, attention naturally focuses on PPD as a disorder, and several studies have suggested specific mechanisms of PPD [9;10,11].

The control of mood and the etiology of depressive disorders in particular, are not completely understood. However, substantial evidence has accrued that serotonergic systems play a central role [1,12,13,14]. Genetic variants in components of the serotonergic system have been correlated with depression [15]. Altered function of the serotonin transporter (SERT) or tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) has been found in PPD subjects [1,14,15]. Levels of serotonin (5-HT) and its major metabolite, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), are significantly lower in the cerebrospinal fluid of depressed patients and in brain tissue of suicide victims [16,17,18]. Reduced availability of the 5-HT precursor, tryptophan, has also been found in depressed patients [19]. Moreover, SSRIs are the first line of pharmacotherapy in PPD and relieve depressive symptoms in most of these patients [4,20].

Although there is evidence that SSRIs are effective in treating PPD [4,21,22], there is still much debate about the effectiveness of SSRIs in treating depressive disorders. Two independent research consortiums conducted meta-analyses on clinical trials submitted to the Food and Drug Administration and determined that the therapeutic effect of the SSRIs were relatively small when compared to placebo in severely depressed patients [23,24]. In contrast, two other independent research teams conducted meta-analyses and concluded that SSRIs were effective in treating depressive symptoms when compared to placebo regardless of the severity of the depressive symptoms [25,26].

In 2004 a novel serotonergic biosynthetic system in the mammary gland was identified and found to be highly upregulated during late pregnancy and lactation [27]. This discovery provides a new context in which to consider whether serotonergic systems are altered in the postpartum, and ultimately whether the central and peripheral serotonergic systems influence one another during this time. This study presents our initial examination of these serotonin systems in the context of the lactating animal, using a selective SSRI (citalopram) with which to probe the behavioral responsiveness of the central serotonin system. Here we investigated the biochemical changes in central (dorsal raphe nucleus) and peripheral (serum) 5-HT systems in lactating mice using immunohistochemistry and radioimmunoassay, respectively. We also examined the effect(s) of sub-chronic SSRI treatment on affective state, as measured by depression-related (forced swim test, FST) and anxiety-related behaviors (marble burying task, MBT) in the postpartum. The present studies compared these behaviors among normal lactating and non-lactating dams without experimentally induced depression. This experimental design was chosen in lieu of a model of maternal depression in order to examine the effects of lactation on the serotonergic systems and affective behavior during a period of peak lactation, day 10 postpartum [27,28,29]. Herein the data demonstrate an enhanced responsiveness to SSRI treatment in lactating dams compared with non-lactating females. We also report changes in both central and peripheral 5-HT systems during lactation. The culmination of these results have clinical implications in the treatment of PPD.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

C57BL/6J mice (age 12–20 weeks) were used in these studies. The initial analysis of immunoreactive 5-HT was performed on brains from lactating dams (day 10 postpartum; Fig. 1A, top panel, see below) and age-matched virgin females. Virgin and lactating (day 10 postpartum) mice were also used for the initial analysis of serum 5-HT (Fig. 1A, bottom panel). For subsequent studies involving behavioral analyses, all females were housed with a stud male for two weeks, checked for vaginal plugs, then separated and housed individually [30,31] on a 12h:12h light:dark cycle with water and standard lab chow available ad libitum. Mated mice that did not produce a litter were assigned to the “nulliparous”, non-pregnant groups. The breeding pairs yielded a mating success rate of 47%, which is consistent with other literature reporting the mating behavior of the C57BL/6J inbred mouse line [32]. Mice that became pregnant and delivered pups had their litters culled to six pups on day 1 postpartum (day of parturition = day 0) to normalize for suckling stimulus, and these constituted the “lactating” groups. In behavioral experiment 1(see Fig. 1B), a third group of dams was included (“postpartum-nonlactating”), which were mice that had their entire litters removed immediately after delivery (postpartum day 0). All procedures were reviewed and approved by the University of Cincinnati’s Institution for Animal Care and Use Committee.

Fig 1. Schemas for experiments assessing behavior after multiple days of citalopram treatment.

Fig 1

A: Preliminary Experiment (Virgin vs. Lactating): 1) Top panel: virgin and lactating (day 10 postpartum) mice were perfused and brain tissue was collected for immunohistochemical analysis of 5-HT. 2) Bottom panel: virgin and lactating (day 10 postpartum) mice were terminated and trunk blood was collected for determination of serum 5-HT via radioimmunoassay. B: Behavioral Experiment 1 in which three groups were studied: 1) nulliparous females; 2) lactating mice; and 3) postpartum-nonlactating mice. Only the FST was administered in this study. C: Behavioral Experiment 2 examined only two groups: 1) nulliparous females and 2) lactating mice. This study included “control” behavioral tests (home cage activity and motor performance task) and an alternate test of mood-related behavioral assessment—the marble burying test (MBT)

Experimental Designs

Preliminary Experiment: Dorsal Raphe 5-HT Immunohistochemistry Virgin vs. Lactating Mice. Female C57BL/6J mice ∼12–16 weeks in age (n = 3 virgin, n = 4 lactating) were used for the initial immunohistochemical analyses for 5-HT. Animals were deeply anesthetized with an i.p. injection of ketamine-xylazine (90mg/kg and10mg/kg, respectively) 10 minutes prior to the perfusion procedure (Fig. 1A, top panel). Animals underwent transcardial perfusion with 10mL of 0.01M phosphate buffered saline (PBS) followed by 20mL of 4% paraformaldehyde. Brains were removed (Fig. 1A, top panel), post-fixed overnight in 4% PFA, and then stored in 30% sucrose at 4°C until they sank. Brains were sectioned on a freezing microtome at 30 μm coronal slices between -4.84 Bregma and -5.02 Bregma, according to a mouse brain stereotaxic atlas [33].

Brain slices were immunostained for 5-HT as free-floating sections, after which they were mounted on slides. Briefly, sections were rinsed with ice-cold 0.01 M PBS (5 min, 3x), then incubated for 60 minutes in blocking buffer (0.01 M PBS containing 3% rabbit serum, 2% BSA, and 0.4% Triton X-100), followed by additional washes (0.01M PBS; 5 min, 3x). Sections were then incubated for 16 hours (room temp) with rat monoclonal anti-5-HT (1:200 in 0.01 M PBS + 0.4% Triton X-100). Following rinses in ice-cold 0.01 M PBS (5 min, 3x), sections were incubated for 60 min (room temp) with biotinylated rabbit anti-rat IgG (1:5000 in 0.01 M PBS + 0.4% Triton X-100). Sections were then processed according to manufacturer’s instructions, using the Vectastain Elite ABC immunoperoxidase system and Ag/Ab complexes were visualized with Ni2+-DAB enzyme substrate. Brain slices from virgin and lactating mice were exposed to the Ni2+-DAB solution for identical periods of time (5 min; in the same solution). Negative controls omitted the primary antibody from the 16-hr incubation.

Stained brain sections were analyzed using the open source NIH Image software on a Macintosh computer. The border of the dorsal raphe was defined [33] and background determined on neighboring tissue. In the thresholding mode (background subtracted) the number of stained cell bodies within the dorsal raphe and their optical densities were documented for each section. Rat polyclonal anti-5-HT and biotinylated rabbit anti-rat antibodies were purchased from Chemicon International (Billerica, MA) and Vectastain Elite ABC immunoperoxidase system from Vector Laboratories Ltd (Burlingame, CA).

Preliminary Experiment: Serum 5-HT Blood Collection and Radioimmunoassay Virgin vs. Lactating Mice. Trunk blood samples were collected from virgin (n = 30) and lactating (n = 24, day 10 postpartum) mice ∼12–20 weeks of age (Fig. 1A, bottom panel). Blood samples were allowed to clot overnight at 4°C to release 5-HT from platelets. Serum was separated by centrifugation at 12,000 rpm for 15 minutes at 4°C, and stored at -80°C until assayed. 5-HT levels were determined in duplicate using a commercial RIA kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions. The sensitivity of the RIA kit for serum 5-HT was 6.7 ng/mL and the interassay coefficient of variation was less than 5.0%. 5-HT RIA Fast Track kits were purchased from Rocky Mountain Diagnostics (Colorado Springs, CO). Citalopram was purchased from Tocris Biosciences (Ellisville, MO). Fluoxetine and all other chemicals, unless otherwise noted, were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO).

Forced Swim Test Pilot Experiment. Citalopram dose and duration used in the experiments described below were based on published literature, which has used male mice almost exclusively [34,35]. To confirm the reproducibility of the published results and the FST technique by our laboratory, a preliminary study was conducted to confirm the efficacy of this sub-chronic SSRI treatment (S1A Fig.). Male mice (age ∼12–16 weeks, n = 15 per group) received daily i.p. injections of citalopram, (5mg/kg/day) or vehicle (0.9% saline) between 08:00h and 09:30h for 5 days. Each animal was subjected to the FST thirty minutes after the final injection.

Behavioral Experiment 1: Subchronic Citalopram and Forced Swim Test. Using the FST as an assay of depressive-like behavior, three groups of female mice were tested: Group 1, nulliparous (non-pregnant, age ∼12–18 weeks; n = 30 vehicle, n = 17 citalopram); Group 2, lactating (age ∼12–18 weeks; n = 20 vehicle, n = 18 citalopram); and Group 3, postpartum-nonlactating (age ∼12–18 weeks; n = 7 vehicle; n = 7 citalopram; see Fig. 1B). On day 6 postpartum, dams of Groups 2 and 3, and age-matched nulliparous females (Group 1) began receiving daily i.p. injections of SSRI (citalopram, 5mg/kg/day) or vehicle (0.9% saline). Injections were administered between 08:00h and 09:30h for 5 days. Thirty minutes after the fifth injection on day 10 postpartum, a period of peak lactation [27,28,29], each animal was subjected to the FST.

Behavioral Experiment 2: Subchronic Citalopram and Marble Burying Task. The effect of SSRI treatment was tested again, using the marble burying task (MBT), an assay of anxiety-like behavior. In addition, two other behaviors (home cage activity and motor performance) were analyzed to determine if differences were specific to affective behavior. Only lactating dams (age ∼12–16 weeks; n = 8 per group) and age-matched nulliparous females (n = 10 per group) were used in this study (see Fig. 1C), and all animals received daily i.p. injections of citalopram (5mg/kg/day) or vehicle for five days. As in behavioral experiment 1 (Fig. 1B), injections in the lactating dams began on day 6 postpartum. Between the fourth and fifth injection, home cage activity was monitored for 24 hours (see Fig. 1C). Thirty minutes after the last injection, each animal was assessed with the MBT and then tested for motor performance using a rotarod (see Fig. 1C).

Behavioral Experiment 3: Increased Citalopram Dosage and Longer Administration. Nulliparous female mice did not exhibit any changes in FST behavior following a five day treatment with citalopram (5mg/kg/day). In light of these results we decided to increase the dose of citalopram from 5 mg/kg to 30 mg/kg, and from five days of treatment to ten days in nulliparous mice (age ∼15 weeks; n = 6 per group) prior to the FST (see S1B Fig.).

Behavioral Experiment 4: Acute Fluoxetine and Forced Swim Test. In order to rule out a citalopram-specific effect in the nulliparous mice, we decided to treat with fluoxetine, another SSRI with different pharmacokinetics [50]. Lactating mice (day 10 postpartum, age ∼16 weeks; n = 14 vehicle, n = 10 (10mg/kg), n = 9 (40mg/kg)) were treated with a single dose of either fluoxetine (10mg/kg or 40mg/kg), or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST (see S1C Fig., left panel). The higher dose (40mg/kg) was found to be effective in reducing immobility during the FST in lactating mice (see S3A Fig.). Nulliparous mice (age ∼ 12–14 weeks; n = 11 per group) were given a single injection of fluoxetine (40mg/kg) or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST (see S1C Fig., right panel).

Behavioral Tests

Mice were removed from the housing area and taken to a separate room for all behavioral testing. Mice were allowed to acclimate to the testing room for approximately one hour.

Forced Swim Test. The FST was always administered between 09:00h and 11:30h. Mice were placed in a swim tank (height = 30cm, diameter = 20cm) containing water (25°C ± 1°C) at a height of 20cm for a total of six minutes. All sessions were recorded on video and scored by two independent observers who were blinded to the treatments. The total time of immobility was recorded. The first two minutes were not used in the determination of the total immobility time as per Porsolt et al. [36]. A mouse was deemed to be immobile when it was floating and making only minor movements to keep its head above water. Tanks were emptied and rinsed clean after each animal.

Home Cage Activity. In behavioral experiment 2 (see Fig. 1C), locomotor activity within the home cage was monitored for 24 hours beginning 30 minutes after the penultimate injection of vehicle or citalopram. The home cage was placed in a SmartFrame stainless steel rack (Hamilton-Kinder Scientific Company, Poway, CA) and infrared photobeam interruption recorded both vertical and horizontal movements. Data were collected and analyzed using HMM100 MotorMonitor software and the total numbers of basic movements were broken into 60-minute time intervals.

Marble Burying Task. For the marble burying task (MBT) in behavioral experiment 2 (see Fig. 1C), each test mouse was placed into a larger, novel cage with a 5cm layer of sawdust on top of the bedding. In the lactating group, each dam was transferred to the novel cage without their pups (the home cages containing the pups were placed into a separate room so the ultrasonic vocalizations of the pups did not influence the behavioral testing). Mice were allowed to acclimate in the novel cage for 30 minutes after which 20 clean, transparent glass marbles (1.5 cm diameter) were placed on top of the sawdust, in five rows of four marbles each, with the marbles spaced equally apart. At the end of 20 minutes, the number of buried marbles (at least two-thirds covered with sawdust) was recorded [37,38]. This design was chosen following a preliminary experiment in which the effects of novel cage versus separation from pups on marble burying behavior in lactating dams was assessed (see S4 Fig.). In that experiment, marbles were either introduced into the home cage (pups remaining with some dams and removed from others), or in a novel cage setting (some dams having been transferred with their pups and others without their pups).

Motor Performance Task. Motor performance was determined using a rotarod (see Fig. 1C). Mice were placed on a stationary drum (∼62 mm diameter) that then began to revolve at a constant speed. The total amount of time the mouse remained on the rotarod was recorded during each of three trials administered 30 minutes apart. The first (acclimation) trial used a speed of 16 rpm for 120 seconds, or until the mouse failed to remain on the revolving drum. The second and third trials used a speed of 20 rpm for up to 300 seconds [39].

Blood Collection and Assays

Serum 5-HT: Measurement following FST and MBT. 20 minutes after the FST (Behavioral Experiment 1; Fig. 1B) or rotarod test (Behavioral Experiment 2; Fig. 1C), animals were terminated and trunk blood collected. The blood samples were allowed to clot overnight at 4°C to release serotonin (5-HT) from platelets. Serum was separated by centrifugation at 12,000 rpm for 15 minutes at 4°C, and stored at -80°C until assayed. 5-HT levels were determined in duplicate using a commercial RIA kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions. The sensitivity of the RIA kit for serum 5-HT was 6.7 ng/mL and the interassay coefficient of variation was less than 5.0%. 5-HT RIA Fast Track kits were purchased from Rocky Mountain Diagnostics (Colorado Springs, CO). Citalopram was purchased from Tocris Biosciences (Ellisville, MO). Fluoxetine and all other chemicals, unless otherwise noted, were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO).

Data Analysis

Data obtained in each experiment were analyzed using either one-way or two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Bonferonni post hoc test or Dunn’s post hoc test for comparison of the means. In some experiments when only two means were compared, a two-tailed Student’s t-test (unpaired) was used. All data shown are presented as the mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). All experiments were designed to achieve a statistical power of 80 percent. Means were considered to be significantly different when p<0.05. All statistical tests were performed using Graph Pad Prism statistical software.

Results

Preliminary Experiment: Dorsal Raphe 5-HT Immunohistochemistry Virgin vs. Lactating Mice

In all sections of the dorsal raphe, immunoreactive 5-HT was noticeably less in the lactating mice as compared to virgin mice, examples of which are presented in Fig. 2, A and B. The differences in 5-HT immunoreactivity were particularly evident in the lateral “wings” of the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN)—the ventrolateral aspect of the periaqueductal gray (VLPAG). Image analysis of the staining indicated that both the number of stained cells and the intensity (over background) of staining were significantly reduced in the DRN of lactating mice (Fig. 2, C and D [t = 12.48, df = 6, p<0.01]).

Fig 2. Serotonin immunostaining of the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) immediately caudal to oculomotor nucleus.

Fig 2

(Bregma -4.48 mm). Representative examples of sections processed for 5-HT immunostaining and visualized with Ni2+-DAB (blue-black stain) from virgin female mice (A; n = 3) and lactating mice (B; n = 4). C: Average optical density of cell bodies (background-subtracted; threshold detection) in the field as demarcated by the dashed lines in A and B. D: Average number of detectable stained cells in the field. Aq, Aqueduct of Sylvius; DRD, dorsal raphe-dorsal part; DRV, dorsal raphe-ventral part; VLPAG, ventrolateral aspect of the periaqueductal gray; mlf, medial longitudinal fasciculus. Student’s t-test comparison of the means: *p<0.05, **p<0.01 compared to virgin controls.

Preliminary Experiment: Serum 5-HT Virgin vs. Lactating Mice

Serum 5-HT was significantly elevated in lactating mice compared with virgin female mice (Fig. 3 [t = 2.220, df = 52, p<0.05]). Nearly all circulating 5-HT is sequestered in the dense granules of the platelets and is released into the serum upon clotting [40].

Fig 3. Serum levels of serotonin in virgin female and lactating C57Bl/6J mice.

Fig 3

Trunk blood was allowed to clot overnight, releasing platelet 5-HT stores. Lactating mice had significantly higher levels of serum 5-HT than virgin females. Student’s t-test comparison of the means: *p<0.05 compared to virgin controls (n = 24–30 per group).

Forced Swim Test Pilot Experiment

Male mice treated with citalopram via injection for five days (5mg/kg/day), showed a significant [(t = 2.550, df = 28, p<0.05)] reduction in immobility time in the FST as compared to vehicle-treated controls (S2A Fig.), as has been widely reported in the literature [34,35].

Behavioral Experiment 1: Subchronic Citalopram and Forced Swim Test

In female mice subjected to the FST there was a significant main effect of lactation state (Fig. 4), such that the lactating females overall had lower immobility times than either of the non-lactating groups as determined by two-way ANOVA [F(2,93) = 11.23, p<0.001]. There was also a main effect of drug treatment [F(1,93) = 5.033, p = 0.0272] on immobility during the FST, but there was no interaction between lactation and drug treatment. In response to citalopram the lactating females showed a significant response to the SSRI as demonstrated by a decrease in immobility time [(t = 2.820, df = 36, p<0.05, compared with vehicle-treated)]. However, neither of the non-lactating groups (nulliparous or pup-deprived) responded to citalopram (Fig. 4).

Fig 4. In the FST, lactating mice respond to regimen of citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating female mice.

Fig 4

Mice received daily injections of vehicle or citalopram (5mg/kg/day, i.p.) for 5 days. Total immobility time (excluding the first two minutes of the test). Bonferonni post hoc test comparison of the means: *p<0.05 compared to vehicle-treated lactating mice; ***p<0.001 overall effect of lactation determined by 2-way ANOVA (n = 7–30 per group).

Behavioral Experiment 2: Subchronic Citalopram and Marble Burying Task

To verify the differential responsiveness of lactating and non-lactating mice to SSRIs, and also to determine if these responses are specific to affective behavior, multiple behaviors were assessed following five days of citalopram treatment (see Fig. 1C).

Marble Burying Task

Affective behavior was assessed using the MBT, a measurement of anxiety-like behavior. Separation of dams from their pups during the MBT caused elevated anxiety-like behavior (more marbles buried) [41,42,43], which was independent of whether they remained in the home cage, or were moved to a novel cage (S4 Fig.). Lactating mice kept with their pups buried so few marbles (1.2 ± 0.7, with several dams burying no marbles at all) that an effect of SSRI treatment would be impossible to measure. Thus, we chose to test citalopram on dams moved to a novel cage without their pups (Fig. 5). Similar to results in the FST, non-lactating females exhibited no behavioral changes in the MBT following citalopram treatment (five days, 5mg/kg/day). Two-way ANOVA revealed an interaction between lactation and drug treatment on marble burying behavior [F(1,32) = 13.11, p = 0.0010, Fig. 5]. Lactating females experienced a significant reduction in anxiety-like behavior in response to citalopram (Fig. 5 [t = 4.857, df = 16, p<0.0001]). Notably, the marble-burying activity of citalopram-treated lactating females in the absence of their pups was similar to that in mothers with their pups present (Fig. 5).

Fig 5. In the marble-burying test, lactating mice respond to citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous mice.

Fig 5

All mice were moved to a novel cage for testing. Lactating mice were tested while separated from their pups. Student’s t-test comparisons of the mean: *p<0.05 compared to vehicle-treated nulliparous mice; ***p<0.001 compared to vehicle-treated lactating group (n = 5–10 per group).

Home Cage Activity

SSRI treatment did not significantly alter locomotor activity measured during the 24 hour period between the 4th and 5th injection of citalopram in either lactating or nulliparous mice. Not surprisingly, lactating dams had significantly less overall home-cage activity than nulliparous mice (Fig. 6, inset [F(1,64) = 64.4; p<0.0001]). Since rodents are nocturnal animals, the home cage activity was divided into light and dark phases and reanalyzed. SSRI treatment did not alter locomotor activity in either lactating or nulliparous controls during the light or dark phases of the cycle (Fig. 6). As expected, nulliparous animals exhibited an increase in locomotor activity during the dark phase. However, lactating dams (day 9–10 postpartum) did not exhibit any significant changes in locomotor activity between the dark and light phases (Fig. 6).

Fig 6. Citalopram has no effect on home cage activity of either nulliparous or lactating mice.

Fig 6

Activity was monitored over a 24-h period and data is shown for light and dark periods separately or for entire 24-h period (Inset). Citalopram treatment did not affect home cage activity in any of the groups. Note significant increase in activity of nulliparous animals during the dark period. Bonferonni post hoc test for comparison of the means: *p<0.05, **p<0.01 activity in dark vs. corresponding light period. 2-way ANOVA***p<0.001 lactating vs. corresponding nulliparous group (n = 8–10 per group).

Motor Performance Task

SSRI treatment also had no effect on motor performance as assessed by the rotarod task in either lactating mice (day 10 postpartum) or nulliparous mice (Fig. 7). The data obtained from the rotarod task were analyzed using the Friedman test followed by a Dunn’s post-hoc analysis, which indicated that both lactating and nulliparous mice spent significantly more time on the rotarod during Trial 3 than Trial 1 for both vehicle and citalopram (5mg/kg/day) treatment groups (Lactating: q = 4.884, df = 68, p<0.0001; Nulliparous: q = 5.885, df = 68, p<0.0001, Fig. 7).

Fig 7. Citalopram has no effect on motor performance in either nulliparous or lactating mice.

Fig 7

The time spend on the rotarod in each of 3 trials is shown. All animals demonstrated improved ability to stay on the rotarod with each subsequent trial. Inset: Means of the slopes for each animal’s performance (time over trial number, n = 8–10 per group).

Serum 5-HT: Measurement following FST and MBT

Circulating 5-HT levels were also analyzed in the mice from the behavioral experiments. Two-way ANOVA (drug treatment X lactation state) of these data determined there were significant main effects of both drug treatment [F(1,88) = 10.75; p<0.01] and lactation state [F(2,88) = 6.079; p<0.01], but the interaction between the two factors was not quite significant [F(2,88) = 2.507; p = 0.08]. As found in the mice used for the preliminary experiment (Fig. 3), lactating mice (vehicle-treated controls) again had significantly elevated levels of serum 5-HT (Fig. 8, [t = 2.698, df = 34, p<0.05]). Serum 5-HT was not significantly different between nulliparous and postpartum-nonlactating female mice. In addition, citalopram treatment significantly reduced serum 5-HT concentration in lactating mice (Fig. 8, [t = 3.618, df = 28, p<0.01]), but had no effect on serum 5-HT in either the nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating groups.

Fig 8. Serum 5-HT from female mice at various reproductive states, following five days of treatment with vehicle or SSRI (citalopram, 5mg/kg/day).

Fig 8

All mice were age-matched and mated. Blood was collected from postpartum-nonlactating and lactating animals on day 10 postpartum. Trunk blood was allowed to clot overnight, releasing platelet 5-HT stores. Bonferonni post hoc test for comparison of means: *p<0.05 compared to vehicle-treated nulliparous group; **p<0.01 compared to vehicle-treated lactating group (n = 12–20 per group).

Behavioral Experiment 3: Increased Citalopram Dosage and Longer Administration

Nulliparous female mice tested at a higher dose and longer duration of citalopram (30mg/kg/day, 10 days) also showed no response to citalopram in the FST (S2B Fig.).

Behavioral Experiment 4: Acute Fluoxetine and Forced Swim Test

Similarly, non-lactating females were insensitive to an acute dose of a second SSRI, fluoxetine (S3B Fig.), while lactating females responded to fluoxetine with a significant reduction in immobility during the FST (S3A Fig. [t = 3.210, df = 21, p<0.01]).

Discussion

The present study produced several novel findings. First, and perhaps most significantly, lactating mice, day 10 postpartum, a time of peak lactation [27,28,29] exhibit behavioral responsiveness to SSRI treatments that have no measurable effect on non-lactating female mice. Second, lactating mice have significantly elevated serum 5-HT stores when compared to non-lactating females. Moreover, SSRI treatment significantly reduced serum 5-HT stores in lactating dams, while having no effect on those of non-lactating females. Third, immunoreactive 5-HT is significantly decreased in the dorsal raphe of lactating mice compared with nulliparous females.

Lactating mice exhibited baseline affective behavior consistent with a more positive mood compared with nonlactating females. There is literature that has reported an increase in depressive-like behavior during the postpartum period [44]. Furthermore, additional studies reported an increase in anhedonia during the postpartum period [45,46]. We did not observe an increase in depressive-like behaviors during our experiments. Our experiments were conducted in C57BL/6J mice, whereas, the aforementioned studies were conducted in Sprague-Dawley Rats, which could account for the different results. It should be noted that the majority (80–90%) of women who give birth do not experience PPD [47]. In the clinical literature the women who do decide to breastfeed often report elevations in mood [47,48]. We did not utilize a model of induced maternal depression; and our results seem to support those reported in the clinical literature. Our lactating mice also exhibited an anxiolytic phenotype (buried fewer marbles in a home cage setting) when compared to age-matched nulliparous controls. Another research group also reported a decrease in anxiety-related behaviors during the postpartum, but in contrast to our results, these anxiolytic effects were independent of lactation [49].

Lactating mice exhibited an increased responsiveness to SSRI treatments and this was true with both the sub-chronic administration of citalopram (5mg/kg/day for 5 days) and the acute administration of fluoxetine (single injection of 40mg/kg). While both block 5-HT uptake via SERT, the two SSRIs differ in structure, metabolism, and pharmacokinetics [50]. Neither SSRI altered affective behavior in nulliparous female mice. Even longer treatment with a much higher dose of citalopram (30mg/kg/day for 10 days) did not change this lack of response in the nulliparous females.

Also not responsive to SSRI treatment were female mice that had gone through a full pregnancy but had their pups removed and were not lactating. These data indicate that it is the state of lactation and/or the presence of the pups, but not parity per se, that underlies the change in responsiveness to SSRIs. This does not necessarily mean that pregnancy is not required to prepare the central circuitry for the alterations observed in the lactating female. The brain undergoes dramatic changes that often begin during pregnancy in preparation for parturition and lactation. The supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei, sites of oxytocin cell bodies, undergo extensive neuro-glia remodeling [51]. Also during lactation, the tuberoinfundibular dopaminergic (TIDA) neurons that normally inhibit prolactin release begin to express enkephalin, which stimulates prolactin [52]. If changes were initiated by pregnancy, the enhanced responsiveness to SSRI clearly requires the active state of lactation.

As with the measures of affective behavior, only lactating dams had a significant decrease in serum 5-HT, reflecting platelet stores, in response to SSRI treatment. In the clinical literature, various parameters of platelet 5-HT have been examined as a potential peripheral marker of SSRI efficacy. This has arisen from the search for a biochemical marker that would have greater discriminatory power than the subjective rating scales used for measuring depression. The concentration of platelet 5-HT has consistently appeared useful as a marker in data from multiple laboratories. There is a single gene for SERT and the transporter expressed in platelets is the same as that expressed in neurons [53], so SSRI treatment blocks uptake of 5-HT into platelets as well. Because platelets do not contain 5-HT biosynthetic enzymes, blocking SERT results in a gradual loss of platelet serotonin stores. It has been demonstrated by independent laboratories that the magnitude of SSRI effect on platelet 5-HT directly correlates with their efficacy in alleviating depressive symptoms as determined by standard psychometric tools such as the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale [54,55]. Correlation of SSRI-induced decrease in platelet 5-HT levels with effects on affective behavior was also seen in the present mouse experiments. Citalopram treatment significantly reduced serum 5-HT concentration only in lactating dams (Fig. 8), and the lactating mice was the only group in which citalopram (or fluoxetine) significantly altered behaviors relevant to mood as assessed by the FST and MBT (Figs. 4 and 5).

The elevated serum 5-HT storage pools in lactating dams is striking, being up to 40% greater than that in nulliparous mice. The majority of serotonin in the body is produced by the enterochromaffin cells in the mucosa of the gut [56] where it regulates peristaltic and secretory reflexes [57,58]. Serotonin produced in the gut enters the bloodstream where it is rapidly transported into platelets via SERT [53] and stored in dense-core granules [40]. 5-HT is released upon platelet activation, and functions in vasoconstriction and thrombosis. High circulating 5-HT is found in certain pathologies, such as carcinoid tumors, but lactation appears to be a unique physiological state in which circulating 5-HT levels are elevated. Platelet 5-HT levels do not differ between sexes or among healthy people of varying age (with the exception of newborns) [59]. Also, there is no circadian rhythm in platelet 5-HT content despite a small circadian rhythm in plasma tryptophan [60]. It is conceivable that the elevated 5-HT during lactation is traceable to 5-HT synthesis in the mammary glands [27], but it is also the case that the intestine of female mammals undergoes marked mucosal hyperplasia during lactation [61]. Thus, the elevated serum (platelet stores) 5-HT during lactation may be supplied by multiple sources.

The vast majority of cell bodies of the central serotonergic system are located within the brainstem and caudal midbrain. Of particular interest are the cell bodies contained within the DRN, as these are the origins of the major ascending serotonergic pathways to the forebrain, including numerous limbic structures. The markedly lower 5-HT immunoreactivity in the dorsal raphe nuclei (DRN) of lactating dams as compared with nulliparous controls may indicate a change in central 5-HT activity, resulting in decreased storage pools of 5-HT. However, the quantification of 5-HT staining in the DRN must be interpreted in light of other evidence. For example, the predicted effects on affective behaviors are very different in the case of reduced 5-HT synthesis or increased 5-HT transport and release. The reduced 5-HT staining in the DRN of lactating mice was particularly striking in the lateral aspects of the DRN and VLPAG. Serotonergic neurons arising from these regions project to a distributed system involved in physiological and behavioral responses associated with stress and anxiety [62]. Retrograde labeling and double-staining experiments [96] have demonstrated that the serotonergic neurons projecting to the amygdala [63] and lateral septum [64] are concentrated at the level shown in Fig. 2, A and B (Bregma -4.84). Both the amygdala and the lateral septum are key components in the brain’s emotional circuitry that modulates affective behavior and stress responses [65,66]. Increased serotonergic neurotransmission to these regions could be expected to reduce depressive-like or anxiety-related behavior.

We do not propose that 5-HT acts alone in regulating affective behavior and mood during lactation. Numerous other neurohormones and transmitters alter affective states, many of which undergo dramatic changes during pregnancy and lactation. Obvious candidates during lactation include prolactin and oxytocin, the endocrine secretions of which are required for milk production and milk let-down in the mammary gland. Receptors for both hormones are present in a number of brain regions, including those associated with affective behavior, such as the amygdala and lateral septum [67,68]. Both hormones regulate behaviors associated with reproduction, such as pair-bonding and parenting [69,70]. In addition, oxytocin has been reported to have antidepressant and anxiolytic activities [71,72]. One study found anxiolytic action of oxytocin only in pregnant and lactating rats, and not in virgin females [73]. One possible explanation for the differential responsiveness to SSRIs between non-lactating and lactating mice could be the fact that there is a higher release of oxytocin from the paraventricular (PVN) and supraotic (SON) nuclei during lactation [74]. Oxytocin has been shown to be important in pair bonding behavior [69]. Furthermore, oxytocin-labeled fibers have been found within limbic structures that could modulate mood-like behaviors [75,76]. SERT labeled fibers have been identified within the PVN and SON of non-human primates [77]. There is some evidence that SSRIs can increase the release of oxytocin in humans [78]. It could be hypothesized that the SSRIs are acting through these SERT-containing fibers, and increasing the release of oxytocin, often considered a “feel-good” modulator, in the lactating mice. This might explain the difference in responsiveness to SSRIs between non-lactating and lactating female mice.

Intracerebroventricular administration of antisense oligonucleotides against the prolactin receptor was found to increase anxiety-related behavior in lactating rats as well as impair maternal behavior [79]. 5-HT serves as a mediator in the neuroendocrine reflexes that result in the secretion of oxytocin and prolactin, including suckling-induced and stress-induced release [80,81,82]. Thus, either or both of these two hormones may be involved in the differential behavioral responsiveness to SSRIs in lactating and nonlactating mice that we report here.

In addition to being exclusive to lactating dams, the effect of SSRI treatment was specific for measures of affective and mood-related behaviors. The FST is considered a measure of behavioral coping in animals presented with an inescapable stress and has predictive validity in detecting the effects of antidepressants [36]. Treatments or psychological states that elevate mood result in decreased time of immobility. The MBT is considered a measure of anxiety-related behavior and treatments that reduce anxiety in humans result in a decrease in the number of marbles buried. Both of these behavioral tests are commonly used in the pharmaceutical industry for screening antidepressants and anxiolytics [37,38,83,84]. One potential caveat to our experiments is that we administered the SSRI via an i.p. injection instead of via oral gavage. The altered metabolism that often occurs during pregnancy and lactation could dramatically decrease the concentration of the SSRI being administered orally. There is a possibility that our route of administration may have circumvented the increase in metabolism [85]. The clinical implications of our results should be interpreted based on the administration route. Future animal studies examining the interaction between lactation and SSRI efficacy should address this difference in administration route.

5-HT is involved in numerous central functions including motor skills, learning and memory, sleep-wakefulness cycles, and others. SSRI treatment had no effect on motor peformance in the rotorod test. This test also involves some learning over the three trials and SSRI treatment did not alter the rate at which motor performance improved in any of the animals (Fig. 7). Spontaneous locomotor activity in the home cage also was unaffected by SSRI in both the nulliparous and lactating mice. The absence of a circadian rhythm in home cage activity in lactating mice was both surprising and novel (Fig. 6). Of the few studies in which circadian rhythms were assessed in lactating animals, even fewer monitored spontaneous activity. Both nocturnal rats and hamsters [86,87] have a significant reduction in the difference between the levels of spontaneous activity during the light and dark periods in lactating animals as compared with virgin or pregnant animals. Yet a significant rhythm is still present. The diurnal Nile grass rat also exhibits a circadian rhythm in activity during lactation which is even greater than that of virgin females but less than that of pregnant females [88]. Although we expected a dampening of the difference in cage activity between the light and dark periods in lactating mice, due to time spent nursing in the nest, we were surprised at the total absence of a rhythm. By day 10 of lactation, pups are more than 70% of their weight at weaning [27,28,29] and should well tolerate the dam spending time outside the nest. Whether this arhythmicity in cage activity of lactating C57BL/6J mice is characteristic of other non-maternal behaviors and/or vegetative functions (e.g. hormone rhythms) would require further investigation.

Any potential relationship between breastfeeding and maternal mood has not been definitively established. Although most studies have found a positive correlation between breastfeeding and alleviation of PPD symptoms [48,89,90], a few studies have found a higher incidence of PPD among breastfeeding women [91,92]. A recent qualitative systematic review of the literature found that most of these latter studies have methodological limits and weaknesses that equivocate conclusions about how breastfeeding influences maternal depressive symptomatology [93]. However, the authors did conclude that there is unequivocal evidence that PPD negatively influences infant-feeding outcomes.

To our knowledge, only one other study has investigated the interaction of lactation and efficacy of SSRIs [94]. Pawluski and colleagues used a model of maternal adversity to examine the effects of stress during gestation and the effects of fluoxetine on behaviors during the postpartum. In most animal models, studies on the efficacy of antidepressant treatment have been done primarily in males and, to a much lesser extent, in virgin females. The few studies that have used lactating animals have not compared their responses to those of non-lactating females. The clinical literature also has not addressed whether breastfeeding can alter responsiveness to SSRIs. Those studies that have examined SSRI use in lactating mothers have been concerned with risk assessment for negative outcomes in babies exposed to the medications during breastfeeding. Current research focuses on two aspects of antidepressant use during breastfeeding: 1) how much of the medication passes into breast milk, and 2) does the medication affect the infant?

Conclusions

The clinical implications of these findings are significant. It is clear that untreated maternal depression has a highly negative impact on the development of the child [95], while breastfeeding is recognized as having a highly positive effect on child health and development. Yet, mothers with PPD often believe that they must make a choice between treatment and breastfeeding [97]. It may be that combined treatment and breastfeeding has the greatest benefit for both mother and child. Moreover, if the state of lactation does increase sensitivity to the antidepressant actions of SSRIs, then it is possible that lower doses of drug may be effective in treating PPD in many patients. What remains clear is that clinical studies on the potential interactions of lactation and SSRI efficacy are very much needed.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Schemas for experiments assessing behavior after treatment with citalopram or fluoxetine.

A: FST Pilot Experiment: the effectiveness of subchronic treatment (5 days) with citalopram (5mg/kg/day) and determined replicability of previous published results. B: Behavioral Experiment 3: the effects of an increased dose (30mg/kg/day) and duration (10 days) of citalopram in nulliparous mice during the FST. C: Behavioral Experiment 4: 1) Left panel: lactating mice (day 10 postpartum) were treated with a single dose of fluoxetine (10mg/kg or 40mg/kg), or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST. 2) Right panel: nulliparous mice were given a single injection of fluoxetine (40mg/kg) or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST.

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. Citalopram treatment reduces total immobility during FST in male C57Bl6 mice, but not in nulliparous female mice.

A. Male mice respond to regimen of citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating female mice (5 days @ 5mg/kg/day, i.p.; see Fig. 4; n = 15 per group). B. Nulliparous female mice do not respond to increased dose and duration of citalopram. Mice received daily injections of vehicle or citalopram (30mg/kg/day, i.p.) for 10 days (n = 6 per group). Student’s t-test for comparison of the means: **p<0.05 compared with vehicle-treated controls.

(TIFF)

S3 Fig. Lactating mice, but not nulliparous female mice, respond to acute fluoxetine treatment in the FST.

All mice received a single injection of vehicle or fluoxetine (Flx; 10 or 40 mg/kg) and subjected to FST 90 minutes later. A. Acute SSRI reduced total immobility time in the FST (n = 9–14 per group). B. Acute SSRI treatment (fluoxetine, 40mg/kg) had no effect on total immobility time nulliparous female C57/Bl6 mice (n = 11 per group). **p<0.01 compared vehicle-treated controls.

(TIFF)

S4 Fig. Separation from pups increases marble-burying activity of lactating female mice.

The number of marbles buried by lactating mice with pups or separated from their pups was measured either in their home cage or a novel cage. Bonferonni post hoc for comparison of means: ***p<0.001 vs. respective “with pups” group (n = 5–8 per group).

(TIFF)

Acknowledgments

The authors are deeply indebted to Dr. Heather Christensen and Michael Murawsky for analysis of the FST videos, and to Dr. Courtney Pinard and Dr. Hadley Bergstrom for critical review of the manuscript.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

KAG received National Institutes of Health grant DK-54966. NDH received United States Department of Agriculture NRI grant #2007-35206-17898. NJJ received a predoctoral fellowship on National Institutes of Health training grant DK-059803. The funders had a significant role in study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, and preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Doornbos B, Fekkes D, Marit AC, de Jonge P, Korf J (2008) Sequential serotonin and noradrenalin associated processes involved in postpartum blues. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 32: 1320–1325. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Halligan SL, Murray L, Martins C, Cooper PJ (2007) Maternal depression and psychiatric outcomes in adolescent offspring: a 13-year longitudinal study. J Affect Dis 97: 145–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Hay DF, Pawlby S, Waters CS, Sharp D (2008) Antepartum and postpartum exposure to maternal depression: different effects on different adolescent outcomes. J Child Psychol Psyc 49: 1070–1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Pearlstein T, Howard M, Salisbury A, Zlotnick C (2009) Postpartum depression. Am J Obstet Gynecol 200: 357–364. 10.1016/j.ajog.2008.11.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. O’Hara MW, Swain AM (1996) Rates and risk of postpartum depression—a meta-analysis. Int Rev Psychiatr 8: 37–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Yonkers KA, Ramin SM, Rush AJ, Navarrete CA, Carmody T, et al. (2001) Onset and persistence of postpartum depression in an inner-city maternal health clinic system. Am J Psychiat 158: 1856–1863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Dietz PM, Williams SB, Callaghan WM, Bachman DJ, Whitlock EP, et al. (2007) Clinically identified maternal depression before, during, and after pregnancies ending in live births. Am J Psychiat 164: 1515–1520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5th edition American Psychiatric Publishing; Arlington, VA, USA: 10.1002/cpp.1875 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Maguire J, Mody I (2008) GABAAR plasticity during pregnancy: relevance to postpartum depression. Neuron 59: 207–213. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Pawluski JL, Lieblich SE, Galea LA (2009) Offspring-exposure reduces depressive-like behaviour in the parturient female rat. Behav Brain Res 197: 55–61. 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Brummelte S, Galea LA (2010) Chronic corticosterone during pregnancy and postpartum affects maternal care, cell proliferation and depressive-like behavior in the dam. Horm Behav 58: 769–779. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Delgado PL, Price LH, Miller HL, Salomon RM, Licinio J, et al. (1991) Rapid serotonin depletion as aprovocative challenge test for patients with major depression: relevance to antidepressant action and the neurobiology of depression. Psychopharmacol Bull 27: 321–330. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Owens MJ, Nemeroff CB (1994) Role of serotonin in the pathophysiology of depression: Focus on the serotonin transporter. Clin Chem 41:288–295. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Newport DJ, Owens MJ, Knight DL, Ragan K, Morgan N, et al. (2004) Alterations in platelet serotonin transporter binding in women with postpartum onset major depression. J Psychiat Res 38: 467–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Lohoff FW (2010) Overview of the genetics of major depressive disorder. Cur Psychiat Rep 12: 539–546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Hannah P, Adams D, Lee A, Glover V, Sandler M (1992) Links between early post-partum mood and post-natal depression. Brit J Psychiat 160: 777–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. van Praag HM (1984) Depression, suicide, and serotonin metabolism in the brain In: Post RM and Ballenger JC (eds). Neurobiology of Mood Disorders. Williams & Wilkins: Baltimore, MD: pp 601–618. [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Nordstrom P, Asberg M (1992) Suicide risk and serotonin. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 6: 6–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Cowen PJ, Parry-Billings M, Newsholme EA (1989) Decreased plasma tryptophan levels in major depression. J Affect Dis 16: 27–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Yonkers KA (2003) Special issue related to the treatment of depression in women. J Clin Psychiat 64: 8–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Epperson N, Czarkowski KA, Ward-O’Brien D, Weiss E, Gueorguieva R, et al. (2001) Maternal sertraline treatment and serotonin transport in breast-feeding mother-infant pairs. Am J Psychiat 158: 1631–1637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ng R C, Hirata CK, Yeung W, Haller E, Finley PR (2010) Pharmacologic treatment for postpartum depression: A systematic review. Pharmacotherapy 30:928–941. 10.1592/phco.30.9.928 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Kirsch I, Deacon BJ, Huedo-Medina TB, Scoboria A, Moore TJ, et al. (2008) Initial Severity and Antidepressant Benefits: A Meta-Analysis of Data Submitted to the Food and Drug Administration. PLOS Med 5: e45 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Fournier JC, DeRubeis RJ, Hollon SD, Dimidjian S, Amsterdam JD, et al. (2010) Antidepressant drug effects and depression severity. J Am Med Assoc 303: 47–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Horder J, Matthews P, Waldmann R (2011) Placebo, prozac, and PLOS: significant lessons from psychopharmacology. J Psychopharmacol 25: 1277–1288. 10.1177/0269881110372544 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Gibbons RD, Hur K, Brown CH, Davis JM, Mann JJ (2012) Benefits from antidepressants: synthesis of 6-week patient-level outcomes from double blind placebo-controlled randomized trials of fluoxetine and venlafaxine. Arch Gen Psychiat 69: 572–579. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Matsuda M, Imaoka T, Vomachka AJ, Gudelsky GA, Hou Z, et al. (2004) Serotonin regulates mammary gland development via an autocrine-paracrine loop. Dev Cell 6: 193–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Johnson MS, Thomson SC, Speakman JR (2001) Limits to sustained energy intake: I. Lactation in the laboratory mouse mus musculus . J Exp Biol 204: 1925–1935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Christensen HR, Murawsky MK, Horseman ND, Wilson TA, Gregerson KA (2013) Completely humanized prolactin rescues infertility in prolactin knockout mice and leads to human prolactin expression in extrapituitary mouse tissues. Endocrinology 154: 4777–4789. 10.1210/en.2013-1476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Brown RE, Mathieson WB, Stapleton J, Neumann PE (1999) Maternal behavior in female C57BL/6J and DBA/2J inbred mice. Physiol and Beh 67:599–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Foldi CJ, Eyles DW, McGrath JJ, Burne THJ (2011) The effects of breeding protocol on in C57BL/6J mice on adult offspring behaviour. PLOS ONE 6: e18152 10.1371/journal.pone.0018152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Lambert R (2007) Breeding strategies for maintaining colonies of laboratory mice: A Jackson Laboratory resource manual Jackson Laboratories; www.jax.org/jaxmice 10.1093/jxb/erm028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ (2001) The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates 2nd ed. Elsevier Science Publishers; San Diego, CA: 71–73. 10.1023/A:1014088919546 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Cervo L, Canetta A, Calcagno E, Burbassi S, Sachetti G, et al. (2005) Genotype-dependent activity of tryptophan hydroxylase-2 determines response to citalopram in a mouse model of depression. J Neuroscience 25: 8165–8172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Chenu F, Guiard BP, Bourin M, Gardier AM (2006) Antidepressant-like activity of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors combined with NK1 receptor antagonist in the mouse forced swimming test. Beh Brain Res 172: 256–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Porsolt RD, Bertin A, Jalfre M (1977) Behavioral despair in mice: a primary screening test for antidepressants. Arch Int Pharmacod Ther 229: 327–336. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Njung'e K, Handley SL (1991) Effects of 5-HT uptake inhibitors, agonists and antagonists on the burying of harmless objects by mice; a putative test for anxiolytic agents. Brit J Pharmacol 104: 105–112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Njung'e K, Handley SL (1991) Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Beh 38:63–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Dawson PA, Steane SE, Markovich D (2004) Behavioral abnormalities of the hyposulphataemic Nas1 knock-out mouse. Beh Brain Res 154: 457–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. da Prada M, Picotti GB (1979) Content and subcellular localization of catecholamines and 5-hydroxytryptamine in human and animal blood platelets: monoamine distribution between platelets and plasma. Brit J Pharmacol 65: 653–662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Huot RL, Gonzalez ME, Ladd CO, Thrivikraman KV, Plotsky PM (2004) Foster litters prevent hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis sensitization mediated by neonatal maternal separation. Psychoneuroendocrino 29: 279–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Michaels CC, Holtzman SG (2006) Neonatal stress and litter composition alter sucrose intake in both rat dam and offspring. Physiol Beh 89: 735–741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Kosten TA, Kehoe P (2010) Immediate and enduring effects of neonatal isolation on maternal behavior in rats. Int J Dev Neuroscience 28: 53–61. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2009.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Craft RM, Kostick ML, Rogers JA, White CL, Tsutsui KT (2010) Forced swim test behavior in postpartum rats. Physiol Beh 96: 402–412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Green AD, Barr AM, Galea LAM (2009) Role of estradiol withdrawal in ‘anhedonic’ sucrose consumption: A model of postpartum depression. Physiol Beh 97: 259–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Navarre BM, Laggart JD, Craft RM (2010) Anhedonia in postpartum rats. Physiol Beh 99: 59–66. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Kendell RE, McGuire RJ, Connor Y, Cox JL (1981) Mood changes in the first three weeks after childbirth. J Affect Dis 3: 317–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. McCoy SJB, Beal JM, Shipman SBM, Peyton ME, Watson GH (2006) Risk factors for postpartum depression: a retrospective investigation at 4-weeks postnatal and a review of the literature. J Am Osteopath Assoc 106: 193–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Lonstein JS (2005) Reduced anxiety in postpartum rats requires recent physical interactions with pups, but is independent of suckling or peripheral sources of hormones. Horm Beh 47: 241–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Baumann P, Rochat B (1995) Comparative pharmacokinetics of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: a look behind the mirror. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 10: 15–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Theodosis DT (2002) Oxytocin-secreting neurons: A physiological model of morphological neuronal and glial plasticity in the adult hypothalamus. Front Neuroendocrin 23: 101–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Merchenthaler I (1993) Induction of enkephalin in tuberoinfundibular dopaminergic neurons during lactation. Endocrinology 133: 2645–2651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Lesch K-P, Wolozin BL, Murphy DL, Riederer P (1993) Primary structure of the human platelet serotonin uptake site: Identity with the brain serotonin transporter. J Neurochem 60: 2319–2322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Axelson DA, Perel JM, Birmaher B, Rudolph G, Nuss S, et al. (2005) Platelet serotonin reuptake inhibition and response to SSRIs in depressed adolescents. Am J Psychiat 162: 802–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Maurer-Spurej E, Pittendreigh C, Misri S (2007) Platelet serotonin levels support depression scores for women with postpartum depression. J Psychiat Neuroscience 32:23–29. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Barter R, Pearse AG (1955) Mammalian enterochromaffin cells as the source of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine). J Patho Bacteriol 69: 25–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Grider JR, Kuemmerle JF, Jin JG (1996) 5-HT released by mucosal stimuli initiates peristalsis by activating 5-HT4/5-HT1p receptors on sensory CGRP neurons. Am J Physiol 270: G778–G782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Cooke HJ (2000) Neurotransmitters in neuronal reflexes regulating intestinal secretion. An NY Acad Sci 915: 77–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Flachaire E, Beney C, Berthier A, Salandre J, Quincy C, et al. (1990) Determination of reference values for serotonin concentration in platelets of healthy newborns, children, adults, and elderly subjects by HPLC with electrochemical detection. Clin Chem 36: 2117–2120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Eynard N, Flachaire E, Lestra C, Broyer M, Zaidan R, et al. (1993) Platelet serotonin content and free and total plasma tryptophan in healthy volunteers during 24 hours. Clin Chem 39: 2337–2340. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Hammond KA (1997) Adaptation of the maternal intestine during lactation. J Mamm Gland Biol Neoplas 2: 243–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Lowry CA, Hale MW, Evans AK, Heerkens J, Staub DR, et al. (2008) Serotonergic systems, anxiety, and affective disorder. Focus on the dorsomedial part of the dorsal raphe nucleus. Stress, Neurotransmitters, and Hormones: Ann NY Acad Sci 1148: 86–94. 10.1196/annals.1410.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Kiyasova V, Fernandez SP, Laine J, Stankovski L, Muzerelle A, et al. (2011) A genetically defined morphologically and functionally unique subset of 5-HT neurons in the mouse raphe nuclei. J Neuroscience 31: 2756–2768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Köhler C, Chan-Palay V, Steinbusch H (1982) The distribution and origin of serotonin-containing fibers in the septal area: a combined immunohistochemical and fluorescent retrograde tracing study in the rat. J Com Neurol 209: 91–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Aggleton JP (1993) The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trend Neurosciences 16: 328–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Singewald GM, Rjabokon A, Singewald N, Ebner K (2011) The modulatory role of the lateral septum on neuroendocrine and behavioral stress responses. Neuropsychopharm 36: 793–804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Bale TL, Davis AM, Auger AP, Dorsa DM, McCarthy MM (2001) CNS region-specific oxytocin receptor expression: importance in regulation of anxiety and sex behavior. J Neuroscience 21: 2546–2552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Bakowska JC, Morrell JI (1997) Atlas of the neurons that express mRNA for the long form of the prolactin receptor in the forebrain of the female rat. J Comp Neurol 386: 161–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Insel TR, Hulihan TJ (1995) A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: oxytocin and partner preference formation in monogamous voles. Beh Neuroscience 109: 782–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Bridges RS, Numan M, Ronsheim PM, Mann PE, Lupini CE (1990) Central prolactin infusions stimulate maternal behavior in steroid-treated nulliparous female rats. Proc Nat Acad Sci 87: 8003–8007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Arletti R, Bertolini A (1987) Oxytocin acts as an antidepressant in two animal models of depression. Life Sci 41: 1725–1530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. McCarthy MM, McDonald CH, Brooks PJ, Goldman D (1996) An anxiolytic action of oxytocin is enhanced by estrogen in the mouse. Physiol Behav 60: 1209–1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Neumann ID, Torner L, Wigger A (2000) Brain oxytocin: differential inhibition of neuroendocrine stress responses and anxiety-related behaviour in virgin, pregnant and lactating rats. Neuroscience 95: 567–575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Leng G, Caquineau C, Sabatier N (2005) Regulation of oxytocin secretion. Vit Horm 71: 27–58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Sofroniew MV (1980) Projections from vasopressin, oxytocin, and neurophysin neurons to neural targets in the rat and human. J Histochem Cytochem 28: 475–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Fliers E, Guldenaar SE, van de Wal N, Swaab DF (1986) Extrahypothalamic vasopressin and oxytocin in the human brain; presence of vasopressin cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Brain Res 375: 363–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Emiliano ABF, Cruz T, Pannoni V, Fudge JL (2007) The interface of oxytocin labeled cells and serotonin transporter-containing fibers in the primate hypothalamus: A Substrate for SSRIs therapeutic effects? Neuropsychopharmacol 32: 977–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Marazziti D, Baroni S, Giannaccini G, Betti L, Massimetti G, et al. (2012) A link between oxytocin and serotonin: supporting evidence from peripheral markers. Eur J Neuropsychopharmacol 22: 578–583. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.12.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Torner L, Toschi N, Nava G, Clapp C, Neumann ID (2002) Increased hypothalamic expression of prolactin in lactation: involvement in behavioural and neuroendocrine stress response. Eur J Neuroscience 15: 1381–1389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Kordon C, Blake CA, Terkel J, Sawyer CH (1973) Participation of serotonin-containing neurons in the suckling-induced rise in plasma prolactin levels in lactating rats. Neuroendocrinol 13: 213–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Moos F, Richard P (1983) Serotonergic control of oxytocin release during suckling in the rat: opposite effects in conscious and anesthetized rats. Neuroendocrinol 36: 300–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Jørgensen H, Knigge U, Warberg J (1992) Effect of serotonin 5-HT1, 5-HT2, and 5-HT3 receptor antagonists on the prolactin response to restraint and ether stress. Neuroendocrinol 56: 371–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Cryan JF, Valentino RJ, Lucki I (2005) Assessing substrates underling the behavioral effects of antidepressants using the modified rat forced swimming test. Neuroscience Biobehav Rev 29: 547–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Nicolas LB, Kolb Y, Prinssen EPM (2006) A combined marble burying—locomotor activity test in mice: A practical screening test with sensitivity to different classes of anxiolytics and antidepressants. Eur J Pharmacol 547: 106–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Hebert MF (2013) Impact of pregnancy on pharmacokinetics of medications. J Pop Therap Clin Pharmacol 20: e350–e357. [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Kittrell EMW, Satinoff E (1988) Diurnal rhythms of body temperature, drinking and activity over reproductive cycles. Physiol Beh 42: 477–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Scribner SJ, Wynne-Edwards KE (1994) Disruption of body temperature and behavior rhythms during reproduction in Dwarf hamsters (Phodopus) . Physiology and Behavior 55: 361–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Schrader JA, Walaszczyk EJ, Smale L (2009) Changing patterns of daily rhythmicity across reproductive states in diurnal female Nile grass rats (Arvicanthis niloticus). Physiol Beh 98: 547–556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Misri S, Sinclair D, Kuan A (1997) Breast-feeding and postpartum depression: is there a relationship? Can J Psychiat 42: 1061–1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Fergerson SS, Jamieson DJ, Lindsay M (2002) Diagnosing postpartum depression: can we do better? Am J Obstet Gynecol 186: 899–902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Alder EM, Cox JL (1983) Breast feeding and postnatal depression. J Psychosom Res 27: 139–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Alder E, Bancroft J (1988) The relationship between breast feeding persistence, sexuality and mood in postpartum women. Psycholog Med 18: 389–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Dennis CL, McQueen K (2009) The relationship between infant-feeding outcomes and postpartum depression: a qualitative systematic review. Pediatrics 123: e736–e751. 10.1542/peds.2008-1629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Pawluski JL, Charlier TD, Fillet M, Houbart V, Crispin HT, et al. (2012) Chronic fluoxetine treatment and maternal adversity differentially alter neurobehavioral outcomes in the rat dam. Beh Brain Res 228: 159–168. 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Dossett EC (2008) Perinatal Depression. Obstet Gynecol Clinics N Am 35: 419–434. 10.1016/j.ogc.2008.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Li YQ, Takada M, Matsuzaki S, Shinonaga Y, Mizuno N (1993) Identification of periaqueductal gray and dorsal raphe nucleus neurons projecting to both the trigeminal sensory complex and forebrain structures: a fluorescent retrograde double-labeling study in the rat. Brain Res 623:267–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Kendall-Tackett K, Hale TS (2010) The use of antidepressants in pregnant and breastfeeding women: A review of recent studies. J Hum Lactation 26:187–195. 10.1177/0890334409342071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Fig. Schemas for experiments assessing behavior after treatment with citalopram or fluoxetine.

A: FST Pilot Experiment: the effectiveness of subchronic treatment (5 days) with citalopram (5mg/kg/day) and determined replicability of previous published results. B: Behavioral Experiment 3: the effects of an increased dose (30mg/kg/day) and duration (10 days) of citalopram in nulliparous mice during the FST. C: Behavioral Experiment 4: 1) Left panel: lactating mice (day 10 postpartum) were treated with a single dose of fluoxetine (10mg/kg or 40mg/kg), or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST. 2) Right panel: nulliparous mice were given a single injection of fluoxetine (40mg/kg) or vehicle 30 minutes prior to the FST.

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. Citalopram treatment reduces total immobility during FST in male C57Bl6 mice, but not in nulliparous female mice.

A. Male mice respond to regimen of citalopram treatment that has no effect on nulliparous or postpartum-nonlactating female mice (5 days @ 5mg/kg/day, i.p.; see Fig. 4; n = 15 per group). B. Nulliparous female mice do not respond to increased dose and duration of citalopram. Mice received daily injections of vehicle or citalopram (30mg/kg/day, i.p.) for 10 days (n = 6 per group). Student’s t-test for comparison of the means: **p<0.05 compared with vehicle-treated controls.

(TIFF)

S3 Fig. Lactating mice, but not nulliparous female mice, respond to acute fluoxetine treatment in the FST.

All mice received a single injection of vehicle or fluoxetine (Flx; 10 or 40 mg/kg) and subjected to FST 90 minutes later. A. Acute SSRI reduced total immobility time in the FST (n = 9–14 per group). B. Acute SSRI treatment (fluoxetine, 40mg/kg) had no effect on total immobility time nulliparous female C57/Bl6 mice (n = 11 per group). **p<0.01 compared vehicle-treated controls.

(TIFF)

S4 Fig. Separation from pups increases marble-burying activity of lactating female mice.

The number of marbles buried by lactating mice with pups or separated from their pups was measured either in their home cage or a novel cage. Bonferonni post hoc for comparison of means: ***p<0.001 vs. respective “with pups” group (n = 5–8 per group).

(TIFF)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES