Abstract
Extant literature provides support for emotion dysregulation as a transdiagnostic construct with relevance to the pathogenesis and treatment of numerous psychiatric difficulties and maladaptive behaviors, including risky, self-destructive, and health-compromising behaviors (e.g., substance use, risky sexual behavior). The aim of the present review is to synthesize theory and empirical research on the relationship between emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors. In addition, we highlight cutting-edge approaches for investigating the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior, including examination of the role of positive emotional experiences and inclusion of context-dependent and physiological assessments. Finally, we note the relevance of the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior relation to intervention efforts aimed at reducing risky behaviors.
Keywords: emotion dysregulation, risky behaviors, difficulties regulating positive emotions, context-dependent assessment, physiological assessment
Introduction
Over the past decade, researchers have become increasingly interested in identifying factors that underlie or maintain risky behaviors (i.e., any behavior that puts one at risk for a negative outcome, be it physical, emotional, social, or financial), with a particular focus on such behaviors that heighten risk for negative health or safety outcomes, such as substance use, risky sexual behavior, deliberate self-harm, aggressive behavior, and disordered eating. These risky, self-destructive, and health-compromising behaviors have been linked to a wide range of deleterious societal costs, including disease, injury, and healthcare usage; crime and criminal justice system costs; educational attainment; and worker productivity [1,2].
A growing body of research suggests that risky behaviors may be driven by deficits in emotion regulation. Emotion regulation is a foundational skill considered to be integral to normative development and adaptive functioning across multiple domains [3,4]. Developmental researchers have defined emotion regulation as the intrinsic and extrinsic processes involved in monitoring, evaluating, and modulating emotional reactions to accomplish one's goals [5]. Drawing upon this definition, KL Gratz and L Roemer [6] proposed an integrative conceptualization of emotion regulation in adulthood as a multidimensional construct involving the awareness, understanding, and acceptance of emotions; ability to control impulsive behaviors and engage in goal-directed behaviors when experiencing negative emotions; and flexible use of situationally-appropriate strategies to modulate the intensity and duration of emotional responses, rather than to eliminate emotions entirely. Conversely, deficits in any of these areas are considered indicative of emotion dysregulation. A substantial body of research provides support for the role of emotion dysregulation in various forms of psychopathology and maladaptive behaviors [7], including behaviors that were traditionally thought to be driven primarily by impulsivity, disinhibition, or risk-taking propensity, such as risky sexual behavior [8,9].
The aim of the present review is to synthesize research on the relationship between emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors to inform future research and clinical practice in this area. First, we provide a theoretical account relating emotion dysregulation to risky behaviors. We then describe empirical support for the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors, with an emphasis on novel approaches to the study of the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior relation. Finally, we discuss the relevance of these findings to intervention efforts aimed at reducing risky behaviors.
Theory Linking Emotion Dysregulation and Risky Behaviors
Extant research suggests that risky behaviors are most likely to occur in the context of intense emotion states [10,11], suggesting that engagement in risky behaviors is context-dependent. A growing body of literature indicates that heightened emotion dysregulation may increase the likelihood of engaging in risky behaviors in the presence of intense emotions. For example, it has been theorized that individuals who exhibit greater emotion dysregulation may be more likely to engage in risky behaviors in an attempt to alleviate or distract themselves from emotional states perceived as aversive [12]. Alternatively, the short-term pleasure that may be associated with certain risky behaviors may function to counter or distract from unpleasant emotional states that an individual is unwilling to approach, tolerate, or accept. Notably, however, although risky behaviors may result in an immediate (short-term) reduction in emotional distress and/or an increase in pleasurable emotion states, they are likely to have paradoxical consequences in the long-term. For example, studies show that engagement in emotionally-avoidant regulation strategies can result in the exacerbation of distress [13]. Moreover, the negative consequences associated with risky behaviors may contribute to the experience of more negative emotions (e.g., guilt, shame) that further drive emotionally-avoidant regulation strategies. Further, because risky behaviors provide immediate reinforcement, individuals may be more likely to use these behaviors to escape emotional distress in the future, reducing experiences in which adaptive modulation of emotions is reinforced [14].
It is also possible that heightened levels of emotion dysregulation may reduce one's capacity to control risky behaviors in the context of urges or cues (e.g., drugs, food) for such behaviors. For example, according to the ego-depletion model [15], the capacity for self-regulation is a limited resource. Thus, situations that require self-regulation will deplete this resource, temporarily limiting one's capacity in this regard [15,16]. Low emotional clarity or the nonacceptance of emotions (i.e., secondary emotional responding) may increase emotional intensity and/or reactivity, depleting self-regulation resources. These forms of emotion dysregulation may also increase the likelihood that an individual attempts to avoid their emotions, an emotion regulation strategy that requires effort and may further deplete self- regulation resources [16]. As a result, an individual may have fewer self-regulation resources available to adequately control impulses to engage in rash or risky behaviors when faced with cues for these behaviors.
Empirical Investigations of Emotion Dysregulation and Risky Behaviors
A burgeoning body of empirical research highlights the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors. Emotion dysregulation has been found to be positively associated with overall risky behaviors [17], as well as specific risky behaviors, including substance use [18,19], risky sexual behavior [8,9], deliberate self-harm [20,21], aggressive behavior [22,23], and disordered eating [24,25]. Importantly, many of these aforementioned studies highlight the unique role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors above and beyond other risk factors for these behaviors [9,21,25]. For example, in one study of substance dependent inpatients, emotion dysregulation significantly predicted risky sexual behavior above and beyond demographics, depression, sensation seeking, traumatic exposure, and substance use severity [9]. While much of the extant research in this area is cross-sectional, initial prospective investigations also underscore the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors. For example, NH Weiss, et al. [26**] found that improvements in emotion regulation fully accounted for reductions in risky behaviors one month post-manipulation among African American women with sexual assault-related posttraumatic stress disorder. This preliminary research has provided a foundation for more complex investigations of the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior relation. The following section will highlight cutting-edge approaches for broadening our understanding of the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors. Specifically, we have chosen to focus on (1) positive emotional experiences; (2) context-dependent assessment; and (3) physiological markers.
Difficulties regulating positive emotions and risky behaviors
Research on the regulation of emotions has overwhelmingly focused on negative (versus positive) emotional experiences (with the exception of mania) [27]. Yet, dysregulation can occur across both positive and negative emotional systems. For example, individuals have been found to regulate positive emotional experiences [28], and the use of putatively maladaptive strategies to regulate positive emotions increases sympathetic nervous system activation [29], suggesting that the regulation of positive emotions is cognitively taxing [30]. Further, difficulties in specific domains of positive emotion dysregulation (i.e., nonacceptance and/or avoidance of positive emotional states) have also been observed among individuals with psychiatric disorders, including borderline personality disorder, major depressive disorder, panic disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder [31-34]. Regarding the role of positive emotions in risky behaviors, positive emotional states in particular have been found to increase distractibility [35] and lead to less discriminative use of information [36], which may result in disadvantageous decision-making focused on immediate (vs. long-term) goals [37]. Indeed, research indicates that positive urgency (i.e., behavioral dyscontrol in the context of positive emotions) is associated with a range of risky behaviors, including substance use, gambling, and risky sexual behavior [10].
Notably, however, a dearth of research has explicated the role of other dimensions of positive emotion dysregulation outside of urgency in risky behaviors. In one such study, KL Dixon-Gordon, et al. [38**] utilized ecological momentary assessment to identify the roles of negative and positive emotion differentiation (i.e., the ability to distinguish between distinct emotions of similar valence) in the relation between borderline personality pathology and urges for risky behaviors. Participants high and low in borderline personality pathology responded to questions regarding emotions and impulses to engage in risky behaviors eight times over one day using handheld computers. The relationship between borderline personality pathology group (high versus low) and urges for risky behaviors was found to increase in magnitude as positive (but not negative) emotion differentiation moved from high to low. These findings suggest that difficulties distinguishing among positive emotions may confer risk for risky behaviors among individuals high in borderline personality pathology.
Although recent advances have further elucidated the role of positive emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors, several important questions remain. Likely contributing to the relative lack of research in this area has been the absence of a comprehensive measure assessing positive emotion dysregulation. To address this limitation, NH Weiss, et al. [39*] recently developed and validated a measure of positive emotion dysregulation (the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale – Positive [DERS-Positive]). Results of this study revealed the presence of three dimensions of positive emotion dysregulation, including (1) nonacceptance of positive emotions, (2) difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior when experiencing positive emotions, and (3) difficulties controlling behaviors when experiencing positive emotions. Consistent with the multidimensional conceptualization of emotion dysregulation on which this measure is based [6], the DERS-Positive subscales evidenced differential associations with relevant emotional and behavioral constructs. These findings highlight the importance of assessing responses to positive emotions beyond simply the ability to inhibit risky behaviors in their presence. Given that difficulties regulating positive emotions are often overlooked in clinical settings, it is critical that future research explicate the role of these additional dimensions of positive emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors.
Finally, it warrants mention that research on positive emotion dysregulation has focused almost exclusively on heightened positive emotion states, despite theoretical evidence to suggest that emotion dysregulation may also occur in response to low positive emotion states. Individuals who report low levels of positive affect have been found to exhibit greater difficulties with goal-directed behavior (40). Further, nonacceptance of low levels of positive affect has been found to result in paradoxical effects, such that telling individuals to increase their levels of positive affect results in reductions in positive affect (41,42). Finally, difficulties identifying (43), describing (44), and modulating (45) both low and high positive emotion states have been reported in the literature. Importantly, initial evidence suggests that emotion dysregulation stemming from low levels of positive affect may also contribute to risky behaviors. For example, the four-function model (FFM) of self-harm purports that self-harm may function to generate positive emotion states or stimulation (46). Likewise, positive reinforcement motives for alcohol use have been described by the motivational model of alcohol use (47). Consistent with this theory, research provides support for the positive emotion-regulating function of risky behaviors, such that low levels of positive affect have been cited as an antecedent to risky behaviors (48,49), and engagement in risky behaviors has been found to result in an increase in positive affect (48-50). This aforementioned literature highlights the need for future research on the regulation of both low and high intensity positive affective states. For example, empirical literature has explored positive emotion intensity as an antecedent and consequence of risky behaviors; however, the role of individual's responses to diminished positive affective states in risky behaviors is unclear. Further, in a recent review, Bentley, Nock, and Barlow (51) suggested the need for laboratory-based and experience sampling methodologies to determine whether positive and negative reinforcement processes underlying risky behaviors are distinct.
Context-dependent assessment of emotion dysregulation and risk-taking in the lab
The vast majority of research on the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior relation has relied on correlational designs and self-report data, thus assessing dispositional tendencies towards emotion dysregulation (i.e., average or typical experiences) and retrospective reports of risky behaviors. Importantly, however, literature suggests that emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors are context-dependent. Indeed, levels of emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors may vary in response to external [52*,53] and internal [12,54] events. For example, emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors may be related to the presence of specific emotional experiences (e.g., shame) or intensities, or occur only when confronted with specific stressors (e.g., interpersonal).
Although limited, what research has been done supports the context-dependent nature of both emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors. For example, MA Cyders, et al. [55**] explored the role of dispositional tendencies towards positive urgency in both risk-taking propensity (using the Balloon Analog Risk Task [BART]) [56] and alcohol consumption following both neutral and positive mood inductions among college students. They found that positive urgency predicted greater risk-taking propensity following a positive (but not neutral) mood induction. Similarly, higher positive urgency was associated with greater alcohol consumption only after a positive mood induction. In a second study, JM Lavender, et al. (unpublished) assessed the relation between state levels of emotion dysregulation (assessed using the State – Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale) following a laboratory stressor and retrospective reports of alcohol and drug problems in a sample of community women. State emotion dysregulation (overall and many of the specific dimensions) was found to be significantly positively associated with alcohol and drug problems. Finally, empirical evidence suggests that negative affect and trauma cue exposures are associated with an increase in risky behavior-related outcomes in the laboratory (e.g., attentional bias to drug cues and cravings) [57-59]. Taken together, these findings suggest that levels of emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors following emotionally-evocative tasks are important correlates of self-reported risky behaviors and emotion dysregulation, respectively. Notably, however, we are not aware of any investigations that have utilized state-dependent measures of both emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors to assess their relation to one another. Elucidating the specific external and internal states related to risk-taking is a critical step in this body of research, as such findings might highlight the utility of targeted interventions focused on teaching strategies for reducing risky behaviors in particular contexts.
Physiological markers of emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors in the lab
In addition to focusing primarily on the regulation of negative affective states and dispositional tendencies towards emotion dysregulation, extant research is also limited through its reliance on subjective assessments of in vivo emotional experiences. Notably, however, a growing body of literature highlights physiological processes underlying the ability to regulate emotions. For example, reduced autonomic nervous system flexibility, and heart rate variability (HRV) in particular, is considered a central physiological index of emotion regulation capacity [60]. HRV provides an index of cardiac vagal tone, or parasympathetic nervous system influences on the heart, such that higher HRV is related to flexible and adaptive responding to environmental demands [60,61], whereas lower HRV is a marker of worse emotion regulation ability [60,62]. Additionally, pre-ejection period (PEP), an index of central sympathetic nervous system activation, has been linked to reward sensitivity [63]. Specifically, shortened PEP has been associated with greater sympathetic nervous system activation, and this attenuated sympathetic nervous system activation has been linked to reward insensitivity [63]. Other research highlights the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis as an important index of emotion dysregulation [5,64,65]. Specifically, use of putatively maladaptive strategies following exposure to stress- inducing stimuli has been found to be associated with increases in cortisol reactivity and delayed recovery (65).
Consistent with findings from studies that have utilized subjective reports of emotion dysregulation, evidence suggests that physiological markers of emotion dysregulation are related to involvement in risky behaviors. In general, this research has found risky behaviors to be positively associated with HRV [66, 67**,68], PEP [63], and cortisol reactivity [66,69*]. However, because this research is still in its infancy, several important questions remain. First, given evidence to suggest that the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems and the HPA axis can have antagonistic, synergistic, or independent effects [70,71], it is essential that research assess the role of multiple physiological indices of emotion dysregulation (and their interactions) in risky behaviors. Indeed, recent research suggests that concurrently low or high autonomic nervous system and HPA axis activation is associated with the highest levels of involvement in risky behaviors [72*,73]. It may be particularly important to explicate the role of physiological markers of emotion dysregulation in response to specific emotions (versus aggregating negatively and positively valenced emotions), as autonomic nervous system and HPA axis reactivity have been shown to be uniquely associated with anger and sadness, respectively [74]. Second, much of the research to date has focused on resting and/or peak levels of emotional responding and reactivity in risky behaviors; however, AK Santucci, et al. [75*] found that longer duration of autonomic nervous system activation following a frustrating task, but not autonomic nervous system reactivity, was associated with emotion dysregulation. This finding suggests that (a) the inability to down-regulate autonomic nervous system activity may be a better marker of emotion dysregulation than simply the level of autonomic arousal, and (b) the need for investigations of the role of autonomic nervous system recovery in risky behaviors. Finally, as previously noted, it will be important for future research to identify the context-dependent role of physiological processes related to emotion dysregulation and their relation to risky behaviors. Indeed, initial evidence suggests that autonomic nervous system reactivity differs as a function of the stressor (cognitive versus interpersonal) [76*].
Implications for Treatment
While more nuanced investigations of the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior relation are needed, including those that elucidate the role of positive emotional experiences and include context-dependent and physiological assessments of emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors, existing research highlights the potential utility of targeting emotion dysregulation in treatments aimed at reducing risky behaviors. Consistent with the aforementioned basic research findings, results of clinical outcome studies suggest that treatments targeting emotion dysregulation result in reductions in risky behaviors. For instance, Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) [77] incorporates a wide range of skills (e.g., emotion regulation, distress tolerance, mindfulness) that target emotion dysregulation and has been shown to result in reductions in risky behaviors, including deliberate self-harm [78], disordered eating [79], and substance use [80]. Likewise, KL Gratz and MT Tull [81] have found that a brief, adjunctive emotion regulation group therapy (ERGT) for women with borderline personality disorder can reduce deliberate self-harm, as well as various forms of risky behaviors (e.g., binge eating, substance use, risky sex), by focusing specifically on improving emotion regulation [82]. Future research would benefit from exploring the effect of treatments targeting emotion dysregulation, such as DBT and ERGT, in reducing risky behaviors among more diverse populations.
Conclusions
Theoretical and empirical literature provide growing support for the underlying role of emotion dysregulation in a wide range of risky behaviors. While advancing our understanding of the emotion dysregulation-risky behavior relation, findings from extant investigations have largely been limited through the use of correlational designs and self-report data to identify the role of difficulties regulating negative emotions in risky behaviors. However, emerging research has begun to address these limitations. First, while research on the regulation of emotions has overwhelmingly focused on negative emotional experiences, recent investigations have begun to provide support for the role of specific domains of difficulties regulating positive emotions (i.e., positive urgency and emotion differentiation) in risky behaviors, suggesting the utility of targeting difficulties regulating positive emotions in interventions aimed at reducing risky behaviors. Second, research to date has generally assessed dispositional tendencies towards emotion dysregulation and retrospective reports of risky behaviors; however, there is evidence for the context-dependent nature of both emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors. Finally, growing research highlights the role of physiological processes underlying the ability to regulate emotions, such as HRV and cortisol reactivity, in risky behaviors. These advancements can improve our understanding of the ways in which emotion dysregulation may increase motivations to engage in risky behaviors, as well as inform the development and refinement of more efficacious interventions for reducing risky behaviors.
Highlights.
Emotion dysregulation has been implicated in the pathogenesis of risky behaviors
Difficulties regulating negative and positive emotions are related to risky behaviors
Emotion dysregulation and risky behaviors are context-dependent
Physiological markers of emotion dysregulation are related to risky behaviors
Treatments targeting emotion dysregulation result in a reduction in risky behaviors
Acknowledgments
The research described here was supported, in part, by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (T32DA019426).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Nicole H. Weiss, Yale University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry 389 Whitney Avenue, New Haven, CT, 06511
Tami P. Sullivan, Yale University School of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry 389 Whitney Avenue, New Haven, CT, 06511 tami.sullivan@yale.edu
Matthew T. Tull, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry 2500 North State Street, Jackson, MS, 39216 mtull@umc.edu
References
- 1.Zohrabian A, Philipson TJ. External costs of risky health behaviors associated with leading actual causes of death in the US: A review of the evidence and implications for future research. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010;7:2460–2472. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7062460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cawley J, Ruhm C. The economics of risky health behaviors. In: Pauly MV, McGuire TG, Barros PP, editors. Handbook of Health Economics. Elsevier; New York, NY: 2011. pp. 95–199. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Calkins SD. Origins and outcomes of individual differences in emotion regulation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59:53–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cole PM, Michel MK, Teti LO. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59:73–102. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59:25–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26:41–54. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gratz KL, Tull MT. Emotion regulation as a mechanism of change in acceptance-and mindfulness-based treatments. In: Baer RA, editor. Assessing Mindfulness and Acceptance: Illuminating the Theory and Practice of Change. New Harbinger Publications; 2010. pp. 105–133. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Messman-Moore TL, Walsh KL, DiLillo D. Emotion dysregulation and risky sexual behavior in revictimization. Child Abuse Negl. 2010;34:967–976. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tull MT, Weiss NH, Adams CE, Gratz KL. The contribution of emotion regulation difficulties to risky sexual behavior within a sample of patients in residential substance abuse treatment. Addict Behav. 2012;37:1084–1092. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cyders MA, Smith GT. Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: Positive and negative urgency. Psychol Bull. 2008;134:807–828. doi: 10.1037/a0013341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Leith KP, Baumeister RF. Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71:1250–1267. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.6.1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Crepaz N, Marks G. Are negative affective states associated with HIV sexual risk behaviors? A meta-analytic review. Health Psychol. 2001;20:291–299. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Levitt JT, Brown TA, Orsillo SM, Barlow DH. The effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behav Ther. 2004;35:747–766. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fischer S, Smith GT, Spillane N, Cyders MA. Urgency: Individual differences in reaction to mood and implications for addictive behaviors. In: Clark AV, editor. The Psychology of Mood. Nova Science Publishers; 2005. pp. 85–107. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:1252. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Muraven M, Tice DM, Baumeister RF. Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:774. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.3.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Weiss NH, Tull MT, Viana AG, Anestis MD, Gratz KL. Impulsive behaviors as an emotion regulation strategy: Examining associations between PTSD, emotion dysregulation, and impulsive behaviors among substance dependent inpatients. J Anxiety Disord. 2012;26:453–458. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bonn-Miller MO, Vujanovic AA, Zvolensky MJ. Emotional dysregulation: Association with coping-oriented marijuana use motives among current marijuana users. Subst Use Misuse. 2008;43:1653–1665. doi: 10.1080/10826080802241292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vilhena-Churchill N, Goldstein AL. Child maltreatment and marijuana problems in young adults: Examining the role of motives and emotion dysregulation. Child Abuse Negl. 2014;38:962–972. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gratz KL, Roemer L. The relationship between emotion dysregulation and deliberate self-harm among female undergraduate students at an urban commuter university. Cogn Behav Ther. 2008;37:14–25. doi: 10.1080/16506070701819524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gratz KL, Tull MT. The relationship between emotion dysregulation and deliberate self-harm among inpatients with substance use disorders. Cognit Ther Res. 2010;34:544–553. doi: 10.1007/s10608-009-9268-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gratz KL, Paulson A, Jakupcak M, Tull MT. Exploring the relationship between childhood maltreatment and intimate partner abuse: Gender differences in the mediating role of emotion dysregulation. Violence Vict. 2009;24:68–82. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.24.1.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cohn AM, Jakupcak M, Seibert LA, Hildebrandt TB, Zeichner A. The role of emotion dysregulation in the association between men's restrictive emotionality and use of physical aggression. Psychol Men Masc. 2010;11:53–64. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lavender JM, Anderson DA. Contribution of emotion regulation difficulties to disordered eating and body dissatisfaction in college men. Int J Eat Disord. 2010;43:352–357. doi: 10.1002/eat.20705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Whiteside U, Chen E, Neighbors C, Hunter D, Lo T, Larimer M. Difficulties regulating emotions: Do binge eaters have fewer strategies to modulate and tolerate negative affect? Eat Behav. 2007;8:162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26**.Weiss NH, Tull MT, Gratz KL. A preliminary experimental examination of the effect of emotion dysregulation and impulsivity on risky behaviors among women with sexual assault-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Modif. doi: 10.1177/0145445514547957. in press. [This prospective experimental study examined the effect of emotion dysregulation and impulsivity in risky behaviors across time in a sample of African American women with sexual assault-related PTSD. Women were randomly assigned to receive emotion modulation, impulsivity reduction, or healthy living (comparison condition) skills trainings. Participants completed self-report measures of emotion dysregulation, impulsivity, and risky behaviors, as well as a behavioral measure of risk-taking propensity (the BART) pre-manipulation and one month post-manipulation. At post-manipulation, women in the emotion modulation condition evidenced the greatest reduction in emotion dysregulation, whereas the largest reductions in impulsivity were detected among women in the impulsivity reduction condition. Women in the emotion modulation and impulsivity reduction conditions evidenced significant decreases over time in both self-reported risky behaviors and risk-taking propensity. Reductions in emotion dysregulation were found to account for changes in risky behaviors across time. The findings of this study provide preliminary experimental support for the role of emotion dysregulation in risky behaviors.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gruber J. A review and synthesis of positive emotion and reward disturbance in bipolar disorder. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2011;18:356–365. doi: 10.1002/cpp.776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gross JJ, Richards JM, John OP. Emotion regulation in everyday life. In: Snyder DK, Simpson A, Hughes N, editors. Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 2006. pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:95–103. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85:348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Beblo T, Fernando S, Kamper P, Griepenstroh J, Aschenbrenner S, Pastuszak A, Schlosser N, Driessen M. Increased attempts to suppress negative and positive emotions in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2013;210:505–509. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kissen M. Characterological aspects of depression in borderline patients. Current Issues in Psychoanalytic Practice. 1986;2:45–63. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Roemer L, Litz BT, Orsillo SM, Wagner AW. A preliminary investigation of the role of strategic withholding of emotions in PTSD. J Trauma Stress. 2001;14:149–156. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tull MT, Roemer L. Emotion regulation difficulties associated with the experience of uncued panic attacks: Evidence of experiential avoidance, emotional nonacceptance, and decreased emotional clarity. Behav Ther. 2007;38:378–391. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dreisbach G, Goschke T. How positive affect modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2004;30:343–353. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Forgas JP. Mood and the perception of unusual people: Affective asymmetry in memory and social judgments. Eur J Soc Psychol. 1992;22:531–547. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004;24:311–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38**.Dixon-Gordon KL, Chapman AL, Weiss NH, Rosenthal MZ. A preliminary examination of the role of emotion differentiation in the relationship between borderline personality and urges for maladaptive behaviors. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. doi: 10.1007/s10862-014-9423-4. in press. [This ecological momentary study assessed the roles of negative and positive emotion differentiation in risky behaviors among participants high and low in borderline personality (BP) pathology. Participants responded to questions regarding emotions and impulses to engage in risky behaviors using handheld computers, and emotion differentiation was calculated by averaging the intraclass correlations with absolute agreement across ratings of negative and positive affect adjectives. Participants high in BP pathology reported greater emotion-related impulsivity as well as daily urges to engage in risky behaviors. Positive (but not negative) emotion differentiation moderated the relation between BP pathology and both emotion-related impulsivity and urges for risky behaviors, such that emotion-related impulsivity and urges to engage in risky behaviors were strongest among individuals with high BP pathology who had low levels of positive emotion differentiation. Among individuals high in BP pathology, the ability to differentiate between positive emotions may be an important target for reducing risky behaviors.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39*.Weiss NH, Lavender J, Gratz KL. Factor structure and initial validation of a multidimensional measure of difficulties in the regulation of positive emotions: The DERS-Positive. Behav Modif. doi: 10.1177/0145445514566504. in press. [The aim of this study was to develop and validate a measure of clinically-relevant difficulties in the regulation of positive emotions (DERS-Positive). Findings revealed the presence of three separate (albeit related) dimensions of positive emotion regulation difficulties, including (1) nonacceptance of positive emotions, (2) difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior when experiencing positive emotions, and (3) difficulties controlling behaviors when experiencing positive emotions. The DERS-Positive subscales evidenced differential associations with relevant emotional and behavioral constructs. Greater overall difficulties in regulating positive emotions and the specific dimensions of difficulties controlling behaviors and engaging in goal-directed behavior when experiencing positive emotions were associated with greater difficulties regulating negative emotions, greater intensity/reactivity of positive emotions, and greater use of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (i.e., experiential avoidance and dissociation). Further, overall difficulties in regulating positive emotions and difficulties controlling behavior when experiencing positive emotions were negatively correlated with emotional expressivity, and difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior when experiencing positive emotions was positively associated with the expression of negative emotions. Finally, greater nonacceptance of positive emotions was associated with greater difficulties regulating negative emotions, greater emotional neglect, and lower levels of general emotional expressivity. Taken together, findings provide evidence for the validity of the DERS-Positive total and subscales scores.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Germans MK, Kring AM. Hedonic deficit in anhedonia: Support for the role of approach motivation. Pers Individ Dif. 2000;28:659–672. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schooler JW, Ariely D, Loewenstein G. The pursuit and assessment of happiness may be self-defeating. In: Carrillo J, Brocas I, editors. The psychology of economic decisions. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2003. pp. 41–70. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mauss IB, Tamir M, Anderson CL, Savino NS. Can seeking happiness make people unhappy? Paradoxical effects of valuing happiness. Emotion. 2011;11:807–815. doi: 10.1037/a0022010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Abbott G, Byrne LK. Schizotypal traits are associated with poorer identification of emotions from dynamic stimuli. Psychiatry Res. 2013;207:40–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Muller AM. Alexithymic features and depressive symptoms: Differences in personality characteristics and in emotional expression among college students. Diss Abstr Int. 2004;65:2639. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Feldman GC, Joormann J, Johnson SL. Responses to positive affect: A self-report measure of rumination and dampening. Cognit Ther Res. 2008;32:507–525. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9083-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Nock MK, Prinstein MJ. A functional approach to the assessment of self-mutilative behavior. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72:885–890. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cox WM, Klinger E. A motivational model of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol. 1988;97:168–180. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.97.2.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Muehlenkamp JJ, Engel SG, Wadeson A, Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, Simonich H, Mitchell JE. Emotional states preceding and following acts of non-suicidal self-injury in bulimia nervosa patients. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47:83–87. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Smyth JM, Wonderlich SA, Heron KE, Sliwinski MJ, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, Engel SG. Daily and momentary mood and stress are associated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in the natural environment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75:629–638. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Franklin JC, Lee KM, Hanna EK, Prinstein MJ. Feeling worse to feel better: Pain-offset relief simultaneously stimulates positive affect and reduces negative affect. Psychol Sci. 2013;24:521–529. doi: 10.1177/0956797612458805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bentley KH, Nock MK, Barlow DH. The four-function model of nonsuicidal self-injury: Key directions for future research. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2:638–656. [Google Scholar]
- 52*.Dixon-Gordon KL, Aldao A, De Los Reyes A. Repertoires of emotion regulation: A person-centered approach to assessing emotion regulation strategies and links to psychopathology. Cogn Emot. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.983046. in press. [This laboratory-based study aimed to explicate repertoires of emotion regulation strategies following social-oriented and achievement-related stressors, and their relation to psychopathology. Participants recalled six situations under which they experienced moderate and high levels of anxiety, sadness, and anger for both achievement-related and social-oriented stressors, following which they reported their use of seven emotion regulation strategies (i.e., acceptance, cognitive reappraisal, problem solving, experiential avoidance, expressive suppression, self-criticism, and worry/rumination), as well as their levels of psychopathology symptoms (i.e., mood, anxiety, borderline personality disorder, social anxiety, and disordered eating symptoms). Using latent class analysis, a seven-class solution provided the best fit for social-related emotion regulation (i.e., Low Regulators, High Regulators, Adaptive Regulators, Worriers, Self-Criticizers, Avoiders, and Acceptors), whereas a four-class solutions provided the best fit for achievement-related emotion regulation (i.e., Adaptive Regulators, Low Regulators, Maladaptive Regulators, and High Regulators). For social-related classes, Avoiders reported lower overall psychopathology compared with all other groups, and Low Regulators reported lower psychopathology compared with High Regulators and Self-Criticizers. For achievement-related classes, Adaptive Regulators reported lower overall psychopathology compared with High Regulators, whereas both Adaptive and Low Regulators reported lower psychopathology than the Maladaptive Regulators. Findings of this study provide support for the context-dependent role of repertoires of emotion regulations strategies.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mohr CD, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G, Hromi A. Daily interpersonal experiences, context, and alcohol consumption: Crying in your beer and toasting good times. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80:489–500. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Egloff B, Schmukle SC, Burns LR, Schwerdtfeger A. Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: Associations with negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion. 2006;6:356–366. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55**.Cyders MA, Zapolski TCB, Combs JL, Settles RF, Fillmore MT, Smith GT. Experimental effect of positive urgency on negative outcomes from risk taking and on increased alcohol consumption. Psychol Addict Behav. 2010;24:367–375. doi: 10.1037/a0019494. [This laboratory-based study explored the role of dispositional tendencies towards positive urgency in both risk-taking propensity (using the Balloon Analog Risk Task) and alcohol consumption following neutral and positive mood inductions among college students. In Study 1, participants completed a measure of positive urgency and the BART (risk-taking propensity during a neutral mood state); underwent a combined positive mood induction procedure; and were again administered the BART (risk-taking propensity during a positive mood state). Positive urgency was found to predict greater risk-taking propensity on the BART following a positive (but not a neutral) mood induction. In Study 2, participants underwent two experimental sessions (one positive mood induction and one neutral mood induction) on separate days, following which they had 90 minutes to drink an unlimited amount of beer. Consistent with findings for Study 1, higher positive urgency predicted greater alcohol consumption following a positive (but not neutral) mood induction. Findings highlight the context-dependent nature of risk-taking propensity and alcohol consumption, such that risk-taking propensity and alcohol consumption among participants was greatest during a positive mood state.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, Strong DR, Brown RA. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol App. 2002;8:75–84. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.8.2.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Tull MT, McDermott MJ, Gratz KL, Coffey SF, Lejuez CW. Cocaine-related attentional bias following trauma cue exposure among cocaine dependent in-patients with and without post-traumatic stress disorder. Addiction. 2011;106:1810–1818. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03508.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Heckman BW, Kovacs MA, Marquinez NS, Meltzer LR, Tsambarlis ME, Drobes DJ, Brandon TH. Influence of affective manipulations on cigarette craving: A meta-analysis. Addiction. 2013;108:2068–2078. doi: 10.1111/add.12284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Coffey SF, Saladin ME, Drobes DJ, Brady KT, Dansky BS, Kilpatrick DG. Trauma and substance cue reactivity in individuals with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and cocaine or alcohol dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;65:115–127. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00157-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. 2000;61:201–216. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Porges SW. The polyvagal perspective. Biol Psychol. 200774:116–143. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Appelhans BM, Luecken LJ. Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Rev Gen Psychol. 2006;10:229–240. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Beauchaine TP. Physiological markers of emotion and behavior dysregulation in externalizing psychopathology. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2012;77:79–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00665.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Stansbury K, Gunnar MR. Adrenocortical activity and emotion regulation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59:108–134. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zoccola PM, Dickerson SS. Assessing the relationship between rumination and cortisol: A review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2012;73:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Scarpa A, Fikretoglu D, Luscher K. Community violence exposure in a young adult sample: II. Psychophysiology and aggressive behavior. J Community Psychol. 2000;28:417–425. [Google Scholar]
- 67**.Garland EL, Franken IHA, Howard MO. Cue-elicited heart rate variability and attentional bias predict alcohol relapse following treatment. Psychopharmacology. 2012;222:17–26. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2618-4. [This laboratory-based, prospective study evaluated cue-elicited high frequency heart rate variability (HFHRV) and alcohol attentional bias as predictors of relapse following residential treatment for alcohol dependence. Participants were inpatients with an alcohol use disorder who were randomly assigned to participate in 10 weeks of either a mindfulness-oriented therapy or an addiction support group. Following treatment, attentional bias for alcohol cues and affect-modulated cue-reactivity (i.e., induced using IAPS alcohol-related pictures) were assessed while heart rate was recorded. Relapse (yes/no) was assessed 6-months post-treatment. HFHRV cue-reactivity and alcohol attentional bias were found to predict the occurrence and timing of relapse. HFHRV reactivity to alcohol cues was significantly higher among inpatients who relapsed compared with inpatients who did not relapse. Findings of this study highlight the role of HFHVR and attentional bias to alcohol cues in alcohol use following treatment for alcohol dependence.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Quintana DS, Guastella AJ, McGregor IS, Hickie IB, Kemp AH. Moderate alcohol intake is related to increased heart rate variability in young adults: Implications for health and well-being. Psychophysiology. 2013;50:1202–1208. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69*.Kobak R, Zajac K, Levine S. Cortisol and antisocial behavior in early adolescence: The role of gender in an economically disadvantaged sample. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21:579–591. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000315. [This laboratory-based study explored the role of cortisol reactivity in antisocial behaviors among adolescents from economically disadvantaged families. Adolescents and their caregivers participated in two 5-minute conflict discussions. Salivary samples were taken before the adolescent-caregiver interaction and 20 minutes post-conflict discussion. Antisocial behaviors were associated with lower pre-task cortisol levels and greater cortisol response to the conflict discussion. Gender moderated the relationship between antisocial behaviors and cortisol reacitivty, such that girls who reported more antisocial behaviors showed an elevated cortisol response. Results of this study provide support for the role of cortisol reactivity in antisocial behaviors, particularly among girls.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Adrian M, Zeman J, Veits G. Methodological implications of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011;110:171–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Larsen RJ, Prizmic-Larsen Z. Measuring emotions: Implications of a multimethod perspective. In: Eid M, Diener E, editors. Handbook of Multimethod Measurement in Psychology. American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 337–351. [Google Scholar]
- 72*.El-Sheikh M, Erath SA, Buckhalt JA, Granger DA, Mize J. Cortisol and children's adjustment: The moderating role of sympathetic nervous system activity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36:601–611. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9204-6. [This laboratory-based study examined the main and interactive effects of cortisol and markers of sympathetic nervous system activity (i.e., salivary alpha-amylase and skin conductance) in children's internalizing and externalizing problems. Children listened to an interadult argument and completed a star-tracing task. Salivary cortisol and alpha amylase were collected 20 minutes after arrival to the lab and two hours later, following the interadult argument. Skin conductance was assessed before and during the interadult argument and star-tracing task. Parents completed self-report measures assessing children's internalizing and externalizing problems. Sympathetic nervous system activity was found to moderate the relation between children's internalizing and externalizing problems and their cortisol levels. Specifically, higher cortisol levels were associated with higher internalizing and externalizing problems among children with higher (versus lower) sympathetic nervous system activity. Results underscore the importance of accounting for the interaction between HPA axis and sympathetic nervous system activity in internalizing and externalizing behaviors.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Gordis EB, Granger DA, Susman EJ, Trickett PK. Asymmetry between salivary cortisol and< i> α</i>-amylase reactivity to stress: Relation to aggressive behavior in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31:976–987. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Lewis M, Ramsay DS, Sullivan MW. The relation of ANS and HPA activation to infant anger and sadness response to goal blockage. Dev Psychobiol. 2006;48:397–405. doi: 10.1002/dev.20151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75*.Santucci AK, Silk JS, Shaw DS, Gentzler A, Fox NA, Kovacs M. Vagal tone and temperament as predictors of emotion regulation strategies in young children. Dev Psychobiol. 2008;50:205–216. doi: 10.1002/dev.20283. [This laboratory-based study investigated the roles of vagal tone, effortful control, and negative affectivity in children's use of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies. Childrens use of emotion regulation strategies were observed during a frustrating task (i.e., delay of gratification). Children also completed an emotional challenge. Vagal tone was assessed pre-, during-, and post-emotional challenge. Mother's rated children's levels of effortful control and negative affectivity. Post-challenge vagal tone and negative affectivity were associated with children's maladaptive strategies during the frustrating task. Results of this study provide support for the role of emotional recovery, but not reactivity, in maldaptive emotion regulation strategies.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76*.Obradović J, Bush NR, Boyce WT. The interactive effect of marital conflict and stress reactivity on externalizing and internalizing symptoms: The role of laboratory stressors. Dev and Psychopathol. 2011;23:101–114. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000672. [This laboratory-based study investigated whether the interaction effect of marital conflict and autonomic nervous system reactivity on externalizing and internalizing symptoms differs as a function of the nature of the laboratory challenge task among children. Sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system responsivity were measured using preejection period and respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity to interpersonal (i.e., film clip depicting a boy being bullied on the playground) and cognitive (i.e., digit span recitation) challenges. The interaction between autonomic nervous system reactivity and marital conflict significantly predicted children's externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Marital conflict was associated with externalizing symptoms for children who exhibited higher respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity to the cognitive stressor, whereas marital conflict was associated with externalizing symptoms for children who demonstrated lower respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity to the interpersonal stressor. Marital symptoms were negatively associated with internalizing symptoms for children who exhibited lower reejection period reactivity to the cognitive task, whereas this interaction was non-significant for the interpersonal task. Results of this study highlight the impact of a cognitive versus interpersonal on the relations among marital conflict, autonomic nervous system reactivity, and internalizing and externalizing symptoms.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Linehan MM. Cognitive behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Linehan MM, Armstrong HE, Suarez A, Allmon D. Heard HL: Cognitive-behavioral treatment of chronically parasuicidal borderline patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:1060–1064. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810360024003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Telch CF, Agras WS, Linehan MM. Dialectical behavior therapy for binge eating disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69:1061–1065. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.6.1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Linehan MM, Schmidt H, Dimeff LA, Craft JC, Kanter J, Comtois KA. Dialectical Behavior Therapy for patients with borderline personality disorder and drug-dependence. Am J Addict. 1999;8:279–292. doi: 10.1080/105504999305686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Gratz KL, Tull MT. Extending research on the utility of an adjunctive emotion regulation group therapy for deliberate self-harm among women with borderline personality pathology. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2011;2:316–326. doi: 10.1037/a0022144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Gratz KL, Levy R, Tull MT. Emotion regulation as a mechanism of change in an acceptance-based emotion regulation group therapy for deliberate self-harm among women with borderline personality pathology. J Cogn Psychother. 2012;26:365–380. [Google Scholar]