Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Environ Health Rep. 2014 Dec;1(4):287–301. doi: 10.1007/s40572-014-0027-7

Environmental Health Factors and Sexually Dimorphic Differences in Behavioral Disruptions

Cheryl S Rosenfeld 1,2,3, Brian C Trainor 4,5
PMCID: PMC4334464  NIHMSID: NIHMS661584  PMID: 25705580

Abstract

Mounting evidence suggests that environmental factors—in particular, those that we are exposed to during perinatal life—can dramatically shape the organism’s risk for later diseases, including neurobehavioral disorders. However, depending on the environmental insult, one sex may demonstrate greater vulnerability than the other sex. Herein, we focus on two well-defined extrinsic environmental factors that lead to sexually dimorphic behavioral differences in animal models and linkage in human epidemiological studies. These include maternal or psychosocial stress (such as social stress) and exposure to endocrine-disrupting compounds (such as one of the most prevalent, bisphenol A [BPA]). In general, the evidence suggests that early environmental exposures, such as BPA and stress, lead to more pronounced behavioral deficits in males than in females, whereas female neurobehavioral patterns are more vulnerable to later in life stress. These findings highlight the importance of considering sex differences and developmental timing when examining the effects of environmental factors on later neurobehavioral outcomes.

Keywords: Developmental Origins of Health and Disease, In Utero Environment, Stress, Endocrine Disruptors, Bisphenol A, Cognition, Anxiety, Sexual Selection, Social and Reproductive Behaviors, Glucocorticoids, Anhedonia, Sex Differences, Gonadal Hormones

Introduction

The notion that many adult diseases originate from early environmental changes has gained currency in the past decade [13, 4•]. Several systems—in particular, the reproductive and central nervous systems—are programmed by developmental exposure to steroid hormones [512] and genes carried on the Y chromosome [13•]. Sexually dimorphic responses to environmental changes might originate from variable placental responses to environmental exposures, which may buffer the fetus of one sex more than the other [1416]. For the purpose of this review, a sexually dimorphic response will be considered a phenotypic effect that either differs in absolute occurrence between the sexes (with a response present in one sex but absent in the other) or a significant difference in the magnitude of the intensity. For instance, early reports indicated that certain toxicants and food additives lead to kidney tumors in male but not female rats or mice [17, 18]. From these original reports, extrinsic factors have been reported to lead to varying degrees of sexually dimorphic responses—including, most recently, in neurobehavioral endpoints, which will be the primary focus of this review. With the National Institutes of Health (NIH) requesting a stronger emphasis on reporting health outcomes in both sexes, it is likely that research in the coming decades will further elucidate how various environmental cues result in dramatic sexually dimorphic differences.

Sexually selected cognitive traits might be especially vulnerable to extrinsic environmental changes [19]. As originally defined by Darwin [20], such traits promote intrasexual competition and intersexual choice of mating partners [20, 21]. Female mating preferences can drive the evolution of anatomical or behavioral traits in males and, in some cases, male mating preferences have been found to exert selection on female traits [22]. Expression of sexually selected behaviors in mature animals is programmed by developmental (fetal and neonatal or perinatal) exposure of the brain to steroid hormones (an organizational effect) and is maintained later in life by these same hormones—in particular, testosterone (an activational effect) [19, 23].

Environmental factors may abolish or augment the normal sex-specific brain programming or organizational effects necessary for sexually selected and other sexually dimorphic behaviors, leading to later behavioral deficits [1, 19, 24••, 25]. These changes could have a long-lasting impact on how endogenous hormones act in mature individuals.

The cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and hypothalamus (including the arcuate, ventromedial, and paraventricular nuclei) are key brain regions that demonstrate pronounced sexually dimorphic differences in neural programming [26]. While androgen receptors (ARs) are expressed in these regions, many testosterone-mediated neurobehavioral effects are due to aromatization of testosterone to estrogen [2730]. Hippocampal development is dependent upon this steroid conversion [27, 28]. Being lipophilic, these circulating steroid hormones, along with many endocrine-disrupting compounds (EDCs), can easily cross the blood–brain barrier. Endogenous and exogenous steroids bind to their cognate receptors, including ARs, estrogen receptor (ESR)1, and ESR2, which are widely expressed in these brain regions [3137]. There are also sex differences in the expression of these steroid hormone receptors in the brain. For example, in the medial preoptic area (mPOA) of males, the Esr1 promoter is hypermethylated and Esr1 expression is reduced, relative to that in females [38]. In contrast, neonatal female pups treated with estradiol exhibit a masculinized DNA methylation pattern for Esr1. It is also clear that EDCs, such as bisphenol A (BPA), can alter, in a sex-dependent manner, the neural expression of Esr1 and Esr2 [33, 39, 40], which may be one mechanism by which these extrinsic factors can either ablate or heighten sex-specific behavioral responses.

While various environmental factors have been linked to sex-specific disruptions, this review will focus on two well-characterized factors: perinatal exposure to EDCs, with the prototype being BPA, and psychosocial stress. We will consider how such environmental factors affect sexually selected behavioral traits in adults, including cognition and reproductive and emotional behaviors.

Sexually Dimorphic Behavioral Differences Associated with Endocrine-Disrupting Compounds

Bisphenol A (BPA)

Most EDCs are manufactured chemicals [41], and, of these, BPA is one of the most mass produced, with production exceeding 8 billion pounds per year [42, 43]. One known action of BPA is binding and activation of ESRs [43], leading to disrupted development of hormone-dependent systems. The pervasiveness of this chemical predicts widespread and continued exposure of animals and humans [42, 43]. BPA is almost ubiquitously found in people; it is detectable in the urine of 93 % of the US population [44], as well as in fetal plasma, placental tissue [45], and breast milk [46]. Results from animal models and human studies indicate that exposure to BPA induces sex-specific effects in three general categories: (1) cognitive function, (2) emotional function (e.g., anxiety), and (3) sociosexual behaviors (Table 1).

Table 1.

Example animal model and human epidemiological studies linking bisphenol A (BPA) exposure and sex-specific behavioral alterations

Publication(s) Animal model/
human cohort
population
EDC(s) tested or
correlation
analysis
performed
Dosing regimen / method to measure BPA concentrations Major findings
Jasarevic et al.,
 2011; Jasarevic
 et al. 2013
 [24, 49]
Deer mice
 (Peromyscus
maniculatus
bairdii)
BPA and Ethinyl
 estradiol (EE)
50 μg BPA/ kg feed weight (FW), 5 mg BPA/kg FW, 50 mg BPA/kg
 FW, and 0.1 ppb EE administered two weeks prior to conception
 through lactation in the maternal diet.
Both sexes were exposed and examined.
  • Developmental exposure of males through the maternal to two doses (5 mg/kg feed weight and 50 mg/kg feed weight) or EE (0.1 ppb) led to cognitive deficits, as evidenced by prolonged latency and increased error rate compared to control males in the Barnes dry land spatial navigational maze and inability to convert to the direct search strategy.

  • BPA- and EE-exposed males were also more anxious, as evidenced by increased time in the closed arms and less time spent in the open arms of the Elevated Plus Maze (EPM) relative to control males.

  • In contrast, 5 mg BPA/kg feed weight and EE exposure led to masculinized spatial navigational ability in exposed females relative to control females.

  • No effect on anxiety behaviors was observed in BPA- or EE-exposed females.

  • In mate choice experiments, control and BPA-exposed females preferred control over BPA-exposed males.

Cox et al., 2010
 [58]
C57Bl6J mice BPA 50 mg/kg FW one week prior to breeding to parturition
 in the maternal diet.
Both sexes were exposed and examined.
Juvenile and adult BPA-exposed males were more anxious, as evidenced by EPM
 testing.
No effect in females.
Xu et al., 2012 [59] ICR mice BPA 0.4 and 4 mg/kg/d from gestational days 7 through 20
 to the mother.
Both sexes were exposed and examined.
Both BPA-exposed males and females exhibited anxiety- and depressive-like
 behaviors. However, the effect in the females was more pronounced and
 consistent across anxiety-testing behavioral tests (open field, dark–light transition
 task, mirrored maze, and EPM).
Xu et al., 2013 [50] ICR mice BPA 0.4, 4, and 40 mg/kg day for 12 weeks during adulthood.
 Both sexes were treated and examined.
Adult exposure to BPA increased number of times males reared in the Open field
 maze but reduced this behavior in females. Additionally, exposed males
 demonstrated impaired spatial and passive avoidance memory, as evidenced
 when tested in the Morris water maze and footshocks. Similar effects were not
 observed in BPA-exposed females.
Gioiosa et al., 2013
 [60]
CD1 mice BPA 10 μg/kg bw/day beginning last week of pregnancy through
the first week after pregnancy to the mother.
Both sexes were exposed and examined.
Females exposed to BPA pre- or post-natally showed increased anxiety behaviors
 and were less likely to explore novel environments relative to control females.
 Such effects were not observed in exposed males.
Jones et al., 2012
 [62]
Long Evans rats BPA 5, 50, 500 and 5,000 μg/kg body weight/day from gestational
 day 7 through post-natal day 14 to the mother.
Both sexes were exposed and examined.
Perinatal exposure to BPA eliminated sex differences that existed between control
 males and females in EPM and Forced Swim Test (FST) where control females
 generally travel greater distance in the EPM, spending more time being mobile in
 the EPM and FST, and showed greater rotations than control males.
Kundakovic et al.
 2013 [39]
BALB/c mice BPA 2, 20, and 200 μg/kg body weight/day from gestational day 0
 through 19 to the mother.
 Both sexes were exposed and examined.
Prenatal BPA exposure disrupted normal sexually dimorphic differences in play
 behaviors, such as chasing. Exposed males were hyperactive; whereas exposed
 females were hypoactive in the open field-test. Similarly, BPA-exposed males
 demonstrated decreased anxiety-like behavior. In contrast, BPA exposed females
 were more anxious.
Williams et al.,
 2013 [55]
California mice
 (P.
californicus)
BPA 50 mg BPA/kg FW two weeks prior to conception through lactation
 in the maternal diet.
Both sexes were exposed and examined.
Developmental exposure to BPA abolished sex differences for exploratory behavior
 where control females are more exploratory than control males. BPA-exposed
 males exhibited decreased territorial marking when a control male was present in
 the testing arena.
Braun et al., 2009
 [63]
Two-year-old
 children
BPA Median maternal urinary concentrations: 1.3-1.8 μg/L, as measured
 at 16 and 26 weeks gestation and at birth.
Both sexes were examined.
Linkage of prenatal BPA
 concentrations (as determined by maternal urinary concentrations during pregnancy) and externalizing behaviors for girls but not
 boys.
Perera et al., 2012
 [64]
Three- to five-
 year old
 children
BPA Median maternal urinary concentrations at 34 weeks gestation:
 1.8 μg/L Median child urinary concentrations at 3 to 4 years
 of age: 3.5 μg/L.
Both sexes were examined.
Linkage of prenatal BPA concentrations (as determined by maternal urinary
 concentrations during pregnancy) and increased emotionally reactive and
 aggressive behaviors in boys but opposite effect in girls with increased BPA
 exposure correlated with decreased anxiety and aggressive behaviors.
Harley et al., 2013
 [65]
Seven- and nine-
 year-old
 children
BPA Median maternal urinary concentrations: 1.1 μg/L
Median child urinary concentrations at 5 years of age: 2.3 μg/L.
Both sexes were examined
Linkage of prenatal BPA concentrations (as determined by maternal urinary
 concentrations during pregnancy) and increased internalizing behaviors in boys,
 including anxiety and depression at seven years of age. Childhood urinary BPA
 concentrations positively correlated with increased externalizing behaviors, e.g.
 conduct problems, in girls at age 7 and increased internalizing, inattention, and
 hyperactive behaviors in boys and girls at this age.

Sexually mature, polygamous male deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii) exhibit enhanced spatial navigational learning and memory, allowing them to locate potential female partners, which are widely dispersed throughout the environment [47]. Consequently, this behavior might be considered a sexually selected cognitive trait, which requires prenatal exposure to testosterone and photoperiod-dependent increases in this same hormone [47, 48]. Developmental exposure to environmentally relevant concentrations of BPA or ethinyl estradiol (EE) through the maternal diet compromises this behavior in males, as determined when both sexes are tested at adulthood in a dry-land spatial navigation maze (a Barnes maze) [24••, 49]. Under the notion that BPA may act through its weak binding of ESR1/ESR2 [43], the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has mandated that any study that is to be considered in influencing policy decisions must include EE as a positive control.

Male imprinting control region (ICR) mice exposed to BPA as adults also exhibit impaired spatial and passive avoidance memory, as evidenced by their performance in the Morris water maze and footshock testing, whereas these deficiencies are not observed in exposed females [50]. These differences may be mediated by early exposure to estrogens or testosterone, which affects development of the hippocampus and cerebral cortex [48, 5153].

In control, non-treated female deer mice, spatial navigational ability is not enhanced when they become sexually mature, as increased exploration and expansion of the home range may be disadvantageous, with an increased predation risk. However, females that are developmentally exposed to a low dose of BPA or EE at doses comparable to human exposure exhibit masculinized or increased spatial abilities and exploratory behaviors [24••, 49].

Male California mice (Peromyscus californicus) generally pair-bond with a single female, and thus they do not possess enhanced spatial navigational ability relative to females [54]. Consistently, developmental exposure to BPA or EE has not altered spatial navigation in male or female California mice [55]. However, BPA exposure abolished typical sex differences in the exploratory behaviors of this species, with exposed females spending less time in the open arms of the maze (30 cm platforms lacking barriers on each side, comparable to the animal being placed on a diving board 100 cm above the ground)—a positive index of exploration—than control females and an equivalent amount of time to that of control males.

Although BPA disruptions in exploratory, anxiety-like, and depressive-like behaviors have been found to differ across contrasting species of rodents, a common thread is that BPA exposure reduces sex differences in behaviors. In the deer mice studies described above, BPA or EE exposure during development increased anxiety-like behavior in elevated-plus-maze males but not in females [56, 57]. Similar sexually dimorphic responses were seen in C57BL6J mice, as developmental BPA exposure increased anxiety-like behavior in males but not in females [58]. In both of these species, control females showed higher levels of anxiety-like behavior than males. In contrast, studies with ICR, BALB-c, and CD1 mice reported that developmental BPA exposure increased anxiety-like behavior in females but not in males [5961]. Similar disruptions of typical sexually dimorphic differences in exploratory behaviors have been reported in Long–Evans rat females exposed to BPA during the perinatal period [62]. In all four strains, control males showed higher levels of anxiety like behavior than females.

In humans, the primary neurodevelopmental types of outcomes associated with increased maternal or childhood urinary BPA concentrations have been emotional behaviors. One study reported that elevated maternal urinary BPA concentrations between 16 weeks of gestation and birth were linked to increased externalizing behaviors (e.g., hyperactivity and aggression) in girls but not in boys at 2 years of age [63]. The absence of an effect in boys might be related to the developmental stage that was examined. Another epidemiological study focusing on older children (3–5 years of age) reported that higher maternal BPA concentrations in urine at 34 weeks of gestation and higher child urinary BPA concentrations were associated with increased externalizing behaviors in boys, whereas the opposite correlation was reported in girls [64]. Examination of children at even later ages (7- to 9-year-olds) also demonstrated trends for an interaction between prenatal BPA exposure and externalizing problems, with higher BPA concentrations being linked to increased externalizing problems in boys but not in girls [65]. Externalizing problems in boys become more prevalent at 5–7 years of age, which may partially explain why the effects of BPA on externalizing problems were observed only in studies with older children [66]. Other environmental factors are also likely to be important, as there was variability across the studies in socioeconomic status. All of the studies reviewed above considered internalizing problems as well, but there was little consistency in how these problems were associated with BPA across the studies. It is not clear whether these differences are due to intrinsic sex-specific behavioral vulnerabilities to environmental factors or whether BPA is accentuating already existing sexually dimorphic behavioral differences.

BPA exposure disrupts several sociosexual behaviors in various rodent models. In mate choice experiments, both BPA and control female deer mice preferred control males over males that had been developmentally exposed to BPA [24••]. Male California mice must defend their territory and their mate from male intruders by engaging in territorial marking. However, BPA-exposed California male mice exhibited suppressed territorial marking when control males were present in the test arena [55]. Prenatal exposure of BALB-c mice disrupted normal sexually dimorphic differences in play behavior [39]. It remains to be determined whether these behavioral changes equate to altered male reproductive success. It is also not clear how well the various sex-specific behavioral differences will translate to humans.

Another concern relates to potential differences in metabolism of BPA between rodents and humans. Both adult rodents and primates primarily metabolize BPA through glucuronidation of BPA via UDP-glucuronosyltransferases (UGTs). However, clearance of BPA in primates is generally through urinary excretion, whereas the biliary–fecal route is the primary route of excretion in rodents [67, 68]. Other potential pharmacokinetic differences across species include metabolism of BPA through sulfonation, which is generally minor in adult animals [6971], and enterohepatic recirculation of BPA, which occurs to a minor extent in rodents and even less in primates [72, 73]. Importantly, a side-by-side analysis of serum BPA concentrations in mice and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), who were both given a comparable oral bolus, demonstrated that the clearance of BPA was comparable in the two species [73]. Thus, even though there are minor differences in the pharmacokinetics of BPA metabolism between rodents and primates, findings in rodents are almost assuredly applicable to primates, including humans [7375].

While BPA weakly binds to ESR1 and ESR2, it is not clear if all of the above sex-specific behavioral differences are due to engaging these steroid receptors. This chemical can also bind to other steroid and non-steroid receptors, such as thyroxine receptor (TR), AR, glucocorticoid receptor (GR), peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor (PPAR ), and pregnane X receptor [43, 7679].

Other Endocrine-Disrupting Compounds

It is beyond the scope of this review to cover all of the EDCs that are proposed to disrupt sexually dimorphic behaviors in various animal models. Table 2 provides a comprehensive list of other EDCs and their impact on the above behavioral categories. Three types of examples are (1) the estrogenic compounds EE, estrogen present in birth control pills, and diethylstilbestrol (DES), which was administered from the 1950s to the 1970s under the misconception that it prevented miscarriages; (2) the androgenic compounds testosterone, 17α methyldihydrotestosterone (MDHT), and levonorgestrel, and the antiandrogenic class, exemplified by flutamide; and (3) a broad range of other EDCs, including insecticides, such as methoxychlor (MXC) and dichlorodiphenyltrichloroethane (DDT) and its active metabolite, dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene (p,p-DDE), fungicides (especially vinclozolin, which has antiandrogenic properties), plasticizers [e.g., di(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate (DEHP)], and coolants/insulating fluids (e.g., polychlorinated biphenyls [PCBs]). Even some commonly used antimicrobials, such as triclosan and triclobarban, may exert endocrine disrupting properties. The two illustrative chemicals, EE and vinclozolin, where across taxa sex-specific disruptions have been reported, will be further discussed.

Table 2.

Example animal model linking other EDC exposure and sex-specific behavioral alterations

Publication Animal model EDC(s) tested or correlation
analysis performed
Dosing regimen Major findings
Bertolasio et al.,
 2011 [163]
Sprague–Dawley rats MXC and E2 2.0 mg MXC and 10 mg E2 on post-natal days 1, 3, and 5.
Only females treated.
Perinatal exposure to MXC and E2 combined led to adult
 females demonstrating suppressed proceptive behaviors
 and increased rejection of males.
Carbone et al.,
 2013 [164]
Wistar rats Di-2-ethylhexyl phthalate
 (DEHP)
30 mg/kg/bw/day from postnatal 1 to weaning.
Both males and females were treated.
Perinatal DEHP exposure increased anxiety-like behaviors
 (decreased open arm entries and time spent in the open
 arms of the EPM and increased time
 spent in the closed arms); whereas, no effect was observed
 in exposed females.
Hotchkiss et al.,
 2003 [87]
Sprague–Dawley rats Vinclozolin, flutamide, and
 testosterone proprionate
Vinclozolin (50 mg/kg/day) or testosterone proprionate
 (TP, 250 μg/kg/day) were administered at postnatal
 days 2 and 3.
Both sexes were treated.
Perinatal exposure to vinclozolin or flutamide decreased
 social play behaviors in juvenile males to approach
 control female levels. When neonatal testosterone-treated
 females were tested as juveniles, they demonstrated
 increase (masculinized response)
 in these same play behaviors.
Baatrup et al.,
 2001 [85]
Guppy fish (Poecilia
reticulata)
Vinclozolin, p,p-DDE, and
 flutamide
Adults were treated for 30 days with these three chemicals in the
 fodder at concentrations between 0.1 and 100 μg/g/fodder.
Only males were treated.
Adult exposure of males to all three of the chemicals resulted
 in discoloration with reduced sexually attractive orange-
 yellow areas and severe reduction
 in courtship behaviors.
Schultz et al.,
 2012 [165]
Fathead minnow
 (Pimephales
promelas)
Triclosan (TCS) and
 triclobarban (TCC)
Newly hatched and adult fathead minnows were exposed for 12 and
 21 days, respectively, to TCC (1.6 μg/L) or a mixture (560 ng/l TCS +
 179 ng/L TCC and 1.6 μg/L TCS + 450 ng/L TCC).
Both sexes were treated.
Adult exposure of males to triclosan or the combination
 of triclosan and triclocarban led to decreased aggression.
Knapp et al.,
 2011 [166]
Mosquitofish
 (Gambusia affinis)
Cortisol Experiment 1: cortisol treatments were 0.5, 1.0 or 2.0 mg/L,
Experiment 2 cortisol treatments were 0.05, 0.1 or 0.5 mg/L
Only females were tested.
Developmental exposure of females to cortisol resulted in
 adult male-typical anatomical structures (elongated anal
 fins with distal tip features) and demonstrated
 masculinized male reproductive behaviors, including
 attempting to copulate with control females.
Saaristo et al.,
 2010 [80]
Sand gobies
 (Pomatoschistus
minutus)
EE Chronic adult exposure (1 to 4 weeks) to EE (11 ng/L).
Only males were treated.
Adult EE exposed males took longer to build a nest, had
 decreased courtship and leading behaviors, and were less
 aggressive than control males.
Saaristo et al.,
 2010 [81]
Sand gobies
 (Pomatoschistus
minutus)
EE Adult exposure for 10 to 31 days to EE (41 ng/L) Adult exposure to EE led to altered parental care by males, as
 evidenced by increased fanning behavior of the eggs
 relative to controls.
Partridge et al.,
 2010 [82]
Gulf pipefish
 (Syngnathus scovelli)
EE Adult exposure to 1 ng EE/L or 100 ng EE/L for 10 days.
Only males were treated.
This species is sex-role reversed with sexual selection acting
 more strongly on females than males and females
 competed for the limited number of males.
 However, adult EE exposure results in males exhibiting
 female-like secondary sexual traits. Even though these
 exposed males are capable of reproduction, females
 discriminate against them in mate choice trials.
Hoffmann et al.,
 2012 [83]
African clawed frogs
 (Xenopus laevis)
EE Adults exposed to EE (0.296 ng/L, 2.96 ng/L, 29.64 ng/L,
 2.96 μg/L and 296.4 μg/L) for 96 hours.
Only males were tested.
Adult exposure of males to EE lowered male sexual arousal,
 as evidenced by decreased proportion and temportal/
 spectral parameters of advertisement calls and increased
 proportion of rasping calls (indicative of an unaroused
 male). Mate choice experiments demonstrated that
 females found exposed males less sexually attractive.
Hoffmann et al.,
 2010 [86]
African clawed frogs
 (Xenopus laevis)
Vinclozolin Adults exposed to Vinclozolin at concentrations ranging
 from 10−6 to 10−10 M for 96 hours.
Only males were treated.
Adult exposure of males to vinclozolin (10−6 M) decreased
 advertisement calls and chirping and increased proportion
 of rasping calls.
Frederick et al.,
 2011 [167]
American white ibises
 (Eudocimus albus)
Methylmercury (MeHg) Three year treatment of adults to MeHg (0.05 to 0.3 ppm wet weight).
Both males and females were treated.
Long term exposure (3 years) resulted in increased
 proportion of males pair-bonding with males and
 decreased male courtship behavior to females and affected
 female sexual preference with courting females
 approaching exposed males less than
 control males. Exposed females demonstrated decreased
 egg productivity.
Halldin et al.,
 1999 [84]
Japanese quail (Coturnix
japonica)
EE and diethylstilbestrol
 (DES)
In ovo treatment to EE (6 ng/g) or DES (19 ng/g).
Both sexes were treated but only males were further studied.
In ovo exposure to EE and DES resulted in depressed male
 sexual behavior.

Effects of EE on sex-specific behaviors have been documented in a wide range of species, including rodents, fish, pipefish, frogs, and birds, as detailed above (for Peromyscus species, where BPA was tested alongside EE, as required by current FDA guidelines for BPA studies) and below. Sand goby (Pomatoschistus minutus) males exposed to EE as adults become demasculinized, as evidenced by their protracted time to engage in nest-building, compromised courtship behaviors, diminished aggressive behaviors, and altered parental care relative to control males [80, 81]. Gulf pipefish (Syngnathus scovelli) possess sex-role reversal in that females must compete for a limited number of males to carry fertilized eggs. However, EE-treated males exhibited female-like secondary sexual traits, and mate choice trials have revealed that females, which are typically not the choosier sex in this species, selectively reject these males [82]. African clawed male frogs exposed as adults to EE display reductions in the number of and temporal/spectral qualities in advertisement calls and demonstrate elevated number of rasping calls [83]. Moreover, females selectively reject these males in mate choice trials. In ovo exposure of Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) to EE leads to later depression of male sexual behaviors [84]. Taken together, the cross-species findings provide robust evidence that developmental and adult exposure to EE compromises later male sexual behaviors and attractiveness to females across taxa.

The antiandrogenic compound vinclozolin has been reported to disrupt sexually dimorphic behaviors in fish, frogs, and rodents. In guppy fish (Poecilia reticulata), males exposed as adults to this chemical possessed less sexually attractive orange-yellow coloration and suppression of male-typical courtship behaviors [85]. Male African clawed frogs (Xenopus laevis) exposed as adults to vinclozolin demonstrated a decreased proportion of advertisement calls and chirping (indicative of an aroused state) and an increased proportion of rasping calls (indicative of a non-aroused state) [86]. Perinatal exposure to vinclozolin or flutamide eliminates male-typical social play behaviors in Sprague Dawley juvenile male rats [87].

Sexually Dimorphic Behavioral Differences in Psychosocial Stress

Just as the effects of EDC depend on developmental timing, the effects of psychosocial stress on the brain and behavior also depend on the stage of development. Intriguingly, when the mother is exposed to psychosocial stress during pregnancy, stronger effects are observed in male offspring, whereas the effects of psychosocial stress during adolescence or after sexual maturity are usually stronger in females.

Early Life Stress

Early life stress induces long-lasting changes in a diverse set of metabolic and neurobiological systems [88]. The effects of psychosocial stress early during gestation usually have more long-lasting consequences than the effects of stress later in gestation. For example, when pregnant female mice were exposed to chronic variable stress, male but not female off-spring spent more time floating in the forced swim test [89]. Floating in this test is considered to be a measure of behavioral despair because drugs with antidepressant properties reduce floating and increase swimming [90]. Although many physiological systems are affected by psychosocial stress, the impact of elevated glucocorticoids has been best studied. When rat dams were injected with corticosterone during pregnancy, male but not female adolescent offspring showed increased anxiety-like behavior [91]. Corticosterone injections during pregnancy also increased floating behavior in the forced swim test in both male and female adolescent offspring. On average, it appears that males are more sensitive to glucocorticoids during prenatal development, which is consistent with the findings from clinical studies reporting that maternal depression during or after pregnancy has stronger effects on anxiety in boys than in girls [92]

Increased corticosterone levels in the mother can also impact postnatal development. Dams injected with corticosterone have increased corticosterone levels in breast milk, as well as increased brain levels of corticosterone in their pups [93]. Males but not females had suppressed neurogenesis in the hippocampus [94], which is thought to be an important mechanism conferring resilience to the long-term effects of stress [95]. In this study, postnatal corticosterone exposure had no effects on behavior in the forced swim test, which is a short-term behavioral response. Future studies could also consider the impact of postnatal corticosterone exposure on longer-lasting stressors, such as chronic variable stress or social defeat.

Adolescence appears to be an important transition with regard to sex differences in sensitivity to psychosocial stress [96]. In adolescent rats, a combination of social and physical stressors increased depression-like behaviors in females but not in males [97•]. Adolescent stress also increases behavioral sensitivity to drugs of abuse, and this effect is stronger in female rats than in males [98]. These results suggest that females have increased physiological responses to psychosocial stress during adolescence. These findings are also consistent with epidemiological work supporting the hypothesis that girls are more likely than boys to carry risk factors for stress-induced mental disorders (such as depression), and that the challenges of adolescence engage these risk factors to impact mental health [99, 100].

Chronic Mild Stress

The chronic mild stress (CMS) paradigm has become a widely used approach to induce behavioral phenotypes associated with depression. CMS consists of a combination of physical and low-level social stressors administered in an unpredictable order [101]. Examples of stressors include changes of cage mates, exposure to cold temperatures, and disruption of light cycles. One of the most common responses to CMS is anhedonia, or reduced interest in rewards, such as sucrose, high-fat food, or sex. Reduced motivation to obtain reward is a key component of depression [102]. Consumption of preferred food items, such as sucrose, generates hedonic behavioral responses, which are evolutionarily conserved [103]. There have been concerns that reduced sucrose intake could be induced by stress-induced weight loss rather than by anhedonia per se [104]. While stress-induced weight loss can be included as a covariate statistically [105], some laboratories have been unable to replicate the effects of CMS on sucrose intake [106, 107]. An intriguing alternative approach is examination of more ethological measures of behavior. For example, several lab groups have observed that CMS reduces male sexual behavior in rats [108110]. Although more study is needed, it will be interesting to see if the effects of CMS on sexual behavior are more robust than the effects on food/taste preferences.

The overwhelming majority of CMS studies have focused on males, but a few have examined females [111]. In general, CMS reduced sucrose consumption in both males and females. While stress-induced anhedonia is usually stronger in females [112114], some studies have reported that males and females are equally affected [115, 116]. This inconsistency suggests that the use of more ethological approaches, such as sexual behavior, could be useful. The effect of social isolation on anhedonia has also been reported to induce stronger responses in females [117]. However when male and female rats were challenged with a novel stressor (the forced swim test), females exposed to CMS engaged in more floating behavior (considered to be an index of behavioral despair), whereas males engaged in less floating behavior [112]. To our knowledge, no study has examined whether CMS impairs female sexual behavior. Mixed results have been reported on the effects on CMS on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis. The most common result is that CMS increases baseline corticosterone levels, which has been reported in both males [107, 112, 118, 119] and females [114]. However, other reports have noted no effects of CMS on corticosterone levels [116, 120122]. Divergent results may be a result of genetic variability across genetic lines. There is substantial variation in coping responses between the different lines of rats [123]. Some lines respond to stress with reactive coping strategies, consisting of behavioral responses such as immobility. Other lines respond with more proactive coping strategies, such as escape. Individuals using more reactive coping strategies are more likely to have higher stress-induced glucocorticoid levels than individuals using more proactive strategies [124].

Social Stress

A powerful source of stress in many species comes from competitive interactions and aggression. Interestingly, many of the behavioral and neurobiological phenotypes observed in individuals that lose aggressive encounters are evolutionarily conserved [125, 126]. One of the most widely reported phenotypes induced by defeat stress is withdrawal from social contexts, even non-threatening contexts [127, 128]. Social withdrawal has special relevance for mental health, because patients diagnosed with depression show stronger avoidance responses to social cues [129, 130]. Social avoidance further reduces social support and helps to maintain depression [131]. Like CMS, defeat stress induces reduced intake of sucrose [132, 133]. Scent marking can also be used to estimate sexual motivation, and this behavior is strongly inhibited by defeat stress [134]. Defeat stress has proven to be a robust approach, as many behavioral phenotypes have been replicated in different lab groups and species [125]. One drawback, however, is that it has been very difficult to study sex differences, because female rats and mice are generally not aggressive toward other females. However, creative use of different species or manipulations of context have allowed several groups to study defeat stress in females.

Under certain conditions, lactating female rats will engage in aggression toward other females. Variability between different rat strains may be an important factor influencing the intensity of defeat. Sprague Dawley dams appear to have lower aggression levels, and females exposed to defeat by a dam actually had decreased levels of floating behavior in the forced swim test, compared with controls [135]. In contrast, Wistar dams appear to have higher aggression levels, and females exposed to defeat showed increased floating in the forced swim test and sucrose anhedonia [136]. Long–Evans dams also appear to be more aggressive, and females exposed to defeat showed increases in depression-like behaviors [137•] and increased self-administration of cocaine [138]. This approach represents a significant advance because it allows for the study of defeat stress in a species in which there is a strong literature in males. However, given the relatively low aggression levels of dams toward other females, it is harder to directly compare males and females. Other rodent species such as Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) and California mice (Peromyscus californicus) allow for more direct comparisons of males and females because their intrafemale aggression levels are higher and females will engage in aggression in the absence of pups. Intriguingly, female Syrian hamsters appear to be more resistant to the behavioral effects of defeat stress than males [139], and this resistance appears to be mediated in part by circulating estradiol [140]. In California mice, defeat stress has very different long-term effects on behavior and the HPA axis in males and females.

The California mouse is a monogamous species, in which both males and females defend a territory. Female California mice are aggressive in resident–intruder tests [141], which facilitates the use of social defeat in both males and females. Acute responses of the HPA axis are more sensitive to social conflict in females than in males. Females show a significant increase in corticosterone following a resident–intruder test, and this increase is observed in both residents (winners [142]) and intruders (losers [143]). In contrast, neither male residents nor intruders show an increase in corticosterone. This sex difference in corticosterone responsiveness is mediated by gonadal hormones (Fig. 1a). Castration sensitizes male corticosterone responses to defeat stress, whereas ovariectomy blocks defeat-induced increases in corticosterone in females [143]. Notably, the long-term effects of defeat stress on baseline corticosterone are quite different. Defeat stress increases male baseline corticosterone during both the active phase and the inactive phase, but has no effect on females [144••], which is similar to findings in female rats [135]. Elevated corticosterone during the inactive phase is often observed in patients diagnosed with depression, and some recent reports have suggested that this symptom is more likely to occur in men [145, 146]. While defeat stress has a stronger effect on baseline corticosterone levels in males, defeat stress has a stronger effect on social interaction behavior in female California mice.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Effects of social defeat on corticosterone (a) and social interaction (b) behavior in California mice. Social stress increases corticosterone levels in female California mice, and ovariectomy blocks this effect. In contrast, castrated males have increased corticosterone levels following stress. The effect of defeat stress on social interaction is stronger in females than in males, but the effect is weaker in females raised on corncob bedding. * p<0.05 versus control, *** p<0.001 versus control, † p<0.05 male versus female. cccorncob, cfcardboard bedding, Gdx gonadectomy, sc woodchip bedding

Male and female California mice are highly motivated to approach unfamiliar individuals, like many other strains of Mus [147]. However, social defeat reliably reduces social approach in female California mice, whereas this effect is weaker or absent in male California mice [144••, 148]. This result mimics reports in humans that social withdrawal occurs to a greater degree in women with depression than in men [149]. Unlike the effects of defeat stress on corticosterone, there is no effect of gonadectomy on social interaction behavior [143]. Similarly, social withdrawal is observed across different stages of the estrous cycle [144••]. These results suggest a diminished role for gonadal hormones in mediating sex differences in social withdrawal behavior in adults. However, there is evidence that hormones may have more important effects early in life. This evidence comes from an unexpected finding that cage bedding has important effects on behavioral responses to stress (Fig. 1b). Corncob bedding contains tetrahydrofuran (THF) diols, which have estrogenic properties but do not bind directly to ESRs [150]. California mice raised on corncob have significantly elevated levels of THF diols [151], most likely through consumption of bedding. When female California mice were raised on corncob bedding, the effect of defeat stress on social interaction behavior was blunted, compared with females raised on aspen wood chips or cardboard-based bedding [143]. This finding suggests that females raised on corncob could be masculinized during development. Interestingly, THF diols stimulate cyclooxygenase (COX)-2 activity [152]. In male rats, estradiol increases COX2 activity to increase production of prostaglandins in the preoptic area (POA) of the hypothalamus [153]. Prostaglandins induce the formation of dendritic spines in the POA, which are more abundant in males than in females and are thought to play an important role in controlling male sexual behavior [154].

Conclusions

Developmental/adult exposure to EDCs—in particular, BPA—is associated with sex-specific effects in various animal models and human epidemiological studies. Across animal models, male sexually selected traits that affect intrasexual competition and intersexual choice appear to be especially vulnerable to the EDCs that have been tested to date. EDCs also seem to exert sex-specific effects on anxiety/depressive-like behaviors in animal models and human epidemiological studies [24••, 39, 49, 5860, 6365]. While only a handful of studies have directly assessed linkages between BPA exposure and sex-specific behavioral differences in humans, a somewhat consistent finding is that prenatal BPA exposure is linked to increased externalizing behavioral problems, particularly in older boys [6365]. However, future studies are needed to clarify whether early BPA exposure alters neurobehavioral programming in animal models and humans, and whether the observed differences persist with maturity.

EDCs likely exert sex-specific behavioral effects by disrupting normal steroid programming of the brain, which could occur through direct binding of neural steroid receptors [155157], modulation of steroid hormone synthesis or metabolism [156158], and/or epigenetic alterations of steroid-dependent genes [159]. These changes likely shape how an individual reacts to challenges faced later in life.

The effects of psychosocial stress are also dependent on the developmental stage, with males exhibiting increased vulnerability during the prenatal stage and females exhibiting increased sensitivity in adolescence. In adults, the effects of psychosocial stress are sex dependent. For example, defeat stress induces social withdrawal in females and disrupts baseline corticosterone levels in males.

Future studies should thus consider the combined impact of early exposure to an EDC, such as BPA, and later life stress (the “two-hit” model) on sex-specific behaviors. It is clear that BPA can induce anxiogenic effects, which vary according to the sex. However, the interaction between BPA exposure and psychosocial stress is still unclear. It will be essential to disentangle direct effects of BPA on brain development from indirect effects of BPA that may be mediated by altered parental behavior. BPA reduces hypothalamic GR expression [160], which might exaggerate glucocorticoid responses to stress. It has also been suggested that BPA can directly bind and activate GR [77], which would be another avenue by which BPA can affect later social and environmental stressors [161].

In conclusion, behaviors such as sexually selected traits might serve as a barometer of exposure to EDC or psychosocial stress. An important implication of sex-specific disruptions in behavior is the long-term impact on susceptibility to stress later in life. There are important sex differences in behavioral and neurobiological responses to stress [125, 162], and at least some of these differences originate from early life experience. Thus exposure to EDC early in life is likely to alter sex-specific responses to stress in adults. However, to our knowledge, there has been no study to date aimed at testing this novel hypothesis that developmental exposure to EDCs followed by later social/environmental stressors can lead to harmful biomolecular and behavioral changes that are sex dependent. Therefore, future studies are needed to determine the potential combined epigenetic and gene expression effects that these extrinsic factors exert on the various brain regions governing adult cognitive and sociosexual behaviors and whether the neurobehavioral responses vary according to sex.

Acknowledgments

Drs. Sandra M. Losa-Ward, Heather B. Patisaul, and Joseph M. Braun are acknowledged for identifying some of the articles that report sex differences in response to BPA.

Cheryl S. Rosenfeld’s support includes grant nos. RC1 1026 ES018195 and U01 ES020929, a Mizzou Advantage Grant, and a University of Missouri College of Veterinary Medicine Faculty Award. Brian C. Trainor is supported by grant nos. R01 MH085069 and R01 MH097714.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Cheryl S. Rosenfeld and Brian C. Trainor declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.

References

Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been highlighted as:

• Of importance

•• Of major importance

  • 1.Rosenfeld CS. Effects of maternal diet and exposure to bisphenol A on sexually dimorphic responses in conceptuses and offspring. Reprod Domest Anim. 2012;47(Suppl 4):23–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0531.2012.02051.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hanson M, Gluckman P. Developmental origins of noncommunicable disease: population and public health implications. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011;94:1754S–8. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.110.001206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barker DJ. Maternal nutrition, fetal nutrition, and disease in later life. Nutrition. 1997;13:807–13. doi: 10.1016/s0899-9007(97)00193-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4•.Barouki R, Gluckman PD, Grandjean P, Hanson M, Heindel JJ. Developmental origins of non-communicable disease: implications for research and public health. Environ Health. 2012;11:42. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-11-42. This article synthesizes the current understanding of developmental origins of health and disease for a range of systems, implications for public health policy, and evidence that prevention strategies to prevent later adverse health outcomes should focus on the perinatal period. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Arnold AP, Breedlove SM. Organizational and activational effects of sex steroids on brain and behavior: a reanalysis. Horm Behav. 1985;19:469–98. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(85)90042-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Morris JA, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. Sexual differentiation of the vertebrate nervous system. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1034–9. doi: 10.1038/nn1325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McCarthy MM. Estradiol and the developing brain. Physiol Rev. 2008;88:91–124. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00010.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Phoenix CH, Goy RW, Gerall AA, Young WC. Organizing action of prenatally administered testosterone propionate on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the female guinea pig. Endocrinology. 1959;65:369–82. doi: 10.1210/endo-65-3-369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Robinson J. Prenatal programming of the female reproductive neuroendocrine system by androgens. Reproduction. 2006;132:539–47. doi: 10.1530/rep.1.00064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Scott HM, Mason JI, Sharpe RM. Steroidogenesis in the fetal testis and its susceptibility to disruption by exogenous compounds. Endocr Rev. 2009;30:883–925. doi: 10.1210/er.2009-0016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nugent BM, Tobet SA, Lara HE, Lucion AB, Wilson ME, Recabarren SE, et al. Hormonal programming across the lifespan. Horm Metab Res. 2012;44:577–86. doi: 10.1055/s-0032-1312593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.O’Donnell K, O’Connor TG, Glover V. Prenatal stress and neurodevelopment of the child: focus on the HPA axis and role of the placenta. Dev Neurosci. 2009;31:285–92. doi: 10.1159/000216539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13•.McCarthy MM, Arnold AP. Reframing sexual differentiation of the brain. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:677–83. doi: 10.1038/nn.2834. This article summarizes the current understanding of how environmental factors, gonadal steroid hormones, and molecular signals and pathways interact to govern sex differences in brain development. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mao J, Zhang X, Sieli PT, Falduto MT, Torres KE, Rosenfeld CS. Contrasting effects of different maternal diets on sexually dimorphic gene expression in the murine placenta. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:5557–62. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000440107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mueller BR, Bale TL. Sex-specific programming of offspring emotionality after stress early in pregnancy. J Neurosci. 2008;28:9055–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1424-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gabory A, Ferry L, Fajardy I, Jouneau L, Gothie JD, Vige A, et al. Maternal diets trigger sex-specific divergent trajectories of gene expression and epigenetic systems in mouse placenta. PLoS One. 2012;7:e47986. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hard GC, Whysner J. Risk assessment of d-limonene: an example of male rat-specific renal tumorigens. Crit Rev Toxicol. 1994;24:231–54. doi: 10.3109/10408449409021607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Blumbach K, Pahler A, Deger HM, Dekant W. Biotransformation and male rat-specific renal toxicity of diethyl ethyl- and dimethyl methylphosphonate. Toxicol Sci. 2000;53:24–32. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/53.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jasarevic E, Geary D, Rosenfeld C. Sexually selected traits: a fundamental framework for studies on behavioral epigenetics. ILAR J. 2012;53:253–69. doi: 10.1093/ilar.53.3-4.253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Darwin C. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. John Murray; London: 1871. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Trivers RL. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B, editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971. Aldine; Chicago: 1972. pp. 136–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Edward DA, Chapman T. The evolution and significance of male mate choice. Trends Ecol Evol. 2011;26:647–54. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Trainor BC, Marler CA. Testosterone, paternal behavior, and aggression in the monogamous California mouse (Peromyscus californicus) Horm Behav. 2001;40:32–42. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24••.Jasarevic E, Sieli PT, Twellman EE, Welsh TH, Jr, Schachtman TR, Roberts RM, et al. Disruption of adult expression of sexually selected traits by developmental exposure to bisphenol A. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:11715–20. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107958108. This article was one of the first to establish that sexually selected traits are vulnerable to developmental exposure to the endocrine-disrupting compounds bisphenol A and ethinyl estradiol, and that females will discriminate against males that have been developmentally exposed to BPA. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.León-Olea M, Martyniuk CJ, Orlando EF, Ottinger MA, Rosenfeld CS, Wolstenholme JT, et al. Current concepts in neuroendocrine disruption. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2014;203C:158–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2014.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Jasarevic E, Geary DC, Rosenfeld CS. Sexually selected traits: a fundamental framework for studies on behavioral epigenetics. ILAR J. 2012;53:253–69. doi: 10.1093/ilar.53.3-4.253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bowers JM, Waddell J, McCarthy MM. A developmental sex difference in hippocampal neurogenesis is mediated by endogenous oestradiol. Biol Sex Differ. 2010;1:8. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-1-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Konkle AT, McCarthy MM. Developmental time course of estradiol, testosterone, and dihydrotestosterone levels in discrete regions of male and female rat brain. Endocrinology. 2011;152:223–35. doi: 10.1210/en.2010-0607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Watson J, Adkins-Regan E. Activation of sexual behavior by implantation of testosterone propionate and estradiol benzoate into the preoptic area of the male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) Horm Behav. 1989;23:251–68. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(89)90065-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Watson J, Adkins-Regan E. Testosterone implanted in the preoptic area of male Japanese quail must be aromatized to activate copulation. Horm Behav. 1989;23:432–47. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(89)90055-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mitterling KL, Spencer JL, Dziedzic N, Shenoy S, McCarthy K, Waters EM, et al. Cellular and subcellular localization of estrogen and progestin receptor immunoreactivities in the mouse hippocampus. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:2729–43. doi: 10.1002/cne.22361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zuloaga DG, Zuloaga KL, Hinds LR, Carbone DL, Handa RJ. Estrogen receptor beta expression in the mouse forebrain: age and sex differences. J Comp Neurol. 2014;522:358–71. doi: 10.1002/cne.23400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cao J, Patisaul HB. Sexually dimorphic expression of hypothalamic estrogen receptors alpha and beta and Kiss1 in neonatal male and female rats. J Comp Neurol. 2011;519:2954–77. doi: 10.1002/cne.22648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ikeda Y, Nagai A, Ikeda MA, Hayashi S. Sexually dimorphic and estrogen-dependent expression of estrogen receptor beta in the ventromedial hypothalamus during rat postnatal development. Endocrinology. 2003;144:5098–104. doi: 10.1210/en.2003-0267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.McEwen BS. Invited review: estrogens effects on the brain. Multiple sites and molecular mechanisms. J Appl Physiol. 2001;91:2785–2801. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.91.6.2785. 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Walker DM, Kirson D, Perez LF, Gore AC. Molecular profiling of postnatal development of the hypothalamus in female and male rats. Biol Reprod. 2012;87:129. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod.112.102798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kumar RC, Thakur MK. Androgen receptor mRNA is inversely regulated by testosterone and estradiol in adult mouse brain. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:925–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kurian JR, Olesen KM, Auger AP. Sex differences in epigenetic regulation of the estrogen receptor-alpha promoter within the developing preoptic area. Endocrinology. 2010;151:2297–305. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-0649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kundakovic M, Gudsnuk K, Franks B, Madrid J, Miller RL, Perera FP, et al. Sex-specific epigenetic disruption and behavioral changes following low-dose in utero bisphenol A exposure. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(24):9956–61. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214056110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cao J, Rebuli ME, Rogers J, Todd KL, Leyrer SM, Ferguson SA, et al. Prenatal bisphenol A exposure alters sex-specific estrogen receptor expression in the neonatal rat hypothalamus and amygdala. Toxicol Sci. 2013;133:157–73. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kft035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Diamanti-Kandarakis E, Bourguignon JP, Giudice LC, Hauser R, Prins GS, Soto AM, et al. Endocrine-disrupting chemicals: an Endocrine Society scientific statement. Endocr Rev. 2009;30:293–342. doi: 10.1210/er.2009-0002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Environment Canada, Health Canada Screening assessment for the challenge: phenol, 4,4′-(1-methylethylidene)bis-(bisphenol A) 2008 Chemical Abstracts Service Registry Number 80-05-7. http://www.ec.gc.ca/ese-ees/3C756383-BEB3-45D5-B8D3-E8C800F35243/batch2_80-05-7_en.pdf. Accessed 30 Aug 2014.
  • 43.Vandenberg LN, Maffini MV, Sonnenschein C, Rubin BS, Soto AM. Bisphenol-A and the great divide: a review of controversies in the field of endocrine disruption. Endocr Rev. 2009;30:75–95. doi: 10.1210/er.2008-0021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Calafat AM, Ye X, Wong LY, Reidy JA, Needham LL. Exposure of the US population to bisphenol A and 4-tertiary-octylphenol: 2003–2004. Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116:39–44. doi: 10.1289/ehp.10753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.vom Saal FS, Akingbemi BT, Belcher SM, Birnbaum LS, Crain DA, Eriksen M, et al. Chapel Hill Bisphenol A Expert Panel consensus statement: integration of mechanisms, effects in animals and potential to impact human health at current levels of exposure. Reprod Toxicol. 2007;24:131–8. doi: 10.1016/j.reprotox.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Vandenberg LN, Hauser R, Marcus M, Olea N, Welshons WV. Human exposure to bisphenol A (BPA) Reprod Toxicol. 2007;24:139–77. doi: 10.1016/j.reprotox.2007.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Galea LA, Kavaliers M, Ossenkopp KP. Sexually dimorphic spatial learning in meadow voles Microtus pennsylvanicus and deer mice Peromyscus maniculatus. J Exp Biol. 1996;199:195–200. doi: 10.1242/jeb.199.1.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Pyter LM, Trainor BC, Nelson RJ. Testosterone and photoperiod interact to affect spatial learning and memory in adult male white-footed mice (Peromyscus leucopus) Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:3056–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04821.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Jasarevic E, Williams SA, Vandas GM, Ellersieck MR, Liao C, Kannan K, et al. Sex and dose-dependent effects of developmental exposure to bisphenol A on anxiety and spatial learning in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii) offspring. Horm Behav. 2013;63:180–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Xu X, Liu X, Zhang Q, Zhang G, Lu Y, Ruan Q, et al. Sex-specific effects of bisphenol-A on memory and synaptic structural modification in hippocampus of adult mice. Horm Behav. 2013;63:766–75. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Shughrue PJ, Merchenthaler I. Estrogen is more than just a “sex hormone”: novel sites for estrogen action in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2000;21:95–101. doi: 10.1006/frne.1999.0190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Pan M, Zhang C. Stimulatory effect of gonadal hormones on fetal rat hippocampal neural proliferation requires neurotrophin receptor activation in vitro. Neurosci Lett. 2013;546:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.04.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Galea LA, Uban KA, Epp JR, Brummelte S, Barha CK, Wilson WL, et al. Endocrine regulation of cognition and neuroplasticity: our pursuit to unveil the complex interaction between hormones, the brain, and behaviour. Can J Exp Psychol. 2008;62:247–60. doi: 10.1037/a0014501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Jasarevic E, Williams SA, Roberts RM, Geary DC, Rosenfeld CS. Spatial navigation strategies in Peromyscus: a comparative study. Anim Behav. 2012;84:1141–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Williams SA, Jasarevic E, Vandas GM, Warzak DA, Geary DC, Ellersieck MR, et al. Effects of developmental bisphenol A exposure on reproductive-related behaviors in California mice (Peromyscus californicus): a monogamous animal model. PLoS One. 2013;8:e55698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ryan BC, Hotchkiss AK, Crofton KM, Gray LE., Jr In utero and lactational exposure to bisphenol A, in contrast to ethinyl estradiol, does not alter sexually dimorphic behavior, puberty, fertility, and anatomy of female LE rats. Toxicol Sci. 2010;114:133–48. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfp266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ryan BC, Vandenbergh JG. Developmental exposure to environmental estrogens alters anxiety and spatial memory in female mice. Horm Behav. 2006;50:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cox KH, Gatewood JD, Howeth C, Rissman EF. Gestational exposure to bisphenol A and cross-fostering affect behaviors in juvenile mice. Horm Behav. 2010;58:754–61. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Xu X, Hong X, Xie L, Li T, Yang Y, Zhang Q, et al. Gestational and lactational exposure to bisphenol-A affects anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in mice. Horm Behav. 2012;62:480–90. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Gioiosa L, Parmigiani S, Vom Saal FS, Palanza P. The effects of bisphenol A on emotional behavior depend upon the timing of exposure, age and gender in mice. Horm Behav. 2013;63:598–605. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kundakovic M, Gudsnuk K, Franks B, Madrid J, Miller RL, Perera FP, et al. Sex-specific epigenetic disruption and behavioral changes following low-dose in utero bisphenol A exposure. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:9956–61. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214056110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jones BA, Watson NV. Perinatal BPA exposure demasculinizes males in measures of affect but has no effect on water maze learning in adulthood. Horm Behav. 2012;61:605–10. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Braun JM, Yolton K, Dietrich KN, Hornung R, Ye X, Calafat AM, et al. Prenatal bisphenol A exposure and early childhood behavior. Environ Health Perspect. 2009;117:1945–52. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0900979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Perera F, Vishnevetsky J, Herbstman JB, Calafat AM, Xiong W, Rauh V, et al. Prenatal bisphenol A exposure and child behavior in an inner-city cohort. Environ Health Perspect. 2012;120:1190–4. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1104492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Harley KG, Gunier RB, Kogut K, Johnson C, Bradman A, Calafat AM, et al. Prenatal and early childhood bisphenol A concentrations and behavior in school-aged children. Environ Res. 2013;126:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2013.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Egger HL, Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:313–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Inoue H, Tsuruta A, Kudo S, Ishii T, Fukushima Y, Iwano H, et al. Bisphenol a glucuronidation and excretion in liver of pregnant and nonpregnant female rats. Drug Metab Dispos. 2005;33:55–9. doi: 10.1124/dmd.104.001537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sakamoto T, Eguchi H, Omoto Y, Ayabe T, Mori H, Hayashi S. Estrogen receptor-mediated effects of tamoxifen on human endometrial cancer cells. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2002;192:93–104. doi: 10.1016/s0303-7207(02)00086-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Balakrishnan B, Henare K, Thorstensen EB, Ponnampalam AP, Mitchell MD. Transfer of bisphenol A across the human placenta. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2010;202:393. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2010.01.025. e391–397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Kurebayashi H, Betsui H, Ohno Y. Disposition of a low dose of 14C-bisphenol A in male rats and its main biliary excretion as BPA glucuronide. Toxicol Sci. 2003;73:17–25. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfg040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Pottenger LH, Domoradzki JY, Markham DA, Hansen SC, Cagen SZ, Waechter JM. The relative bioavailability and metabolism of bisphenol A in rats is dependent upon the route of administration. Toxicol Sci. 2000;54:3–18. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/54.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Doerge DR, Twaddle NC, Vanlandingham M, Fisher JW. Pharmacokinetics of bisphenol A in neonatal and adult Sprague–Dawley rats. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol. 2010;247:158–65. doi: 10.1016/j.taap.2010.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Taylor JA, Vom Saal FS, Welshons WV, Drury B, Rottinghaus G, Hunt PA, et al. Similarity of bisphenol A pharmacokinetics in rhesus monkeys and mice: relevance for human exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:422–30. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1002514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gies A, Heinzow B, Dieter HH, Heindel J. Bisphenol A Workshop of the German Federal Environment Agency—March 30–31, 2009 Work Group report: public health issues of bisphenol A. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2009;212:693–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2009.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Sieli PT, Jasarevic E, Warzak DA, Mao J, Ellersieck MR, Liao C, et al. Comparison of serum bisphenol A concentrations in mice exposed to bisphenol A through the diet versus oral bolus exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:1260–5. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1003385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Alonso-Magdalena P, Ropero AB, Soriano S, Garcia-Arevalo M, Ripoll C, Fuentes E, et al. Bisphenol-A acts as a potent estrogen via non-classical estrogen triggered pathways. Mol Cell Endocrinol. 2012;355:201–7. doi: 10.1016/j.mce.2011.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Prasanth GK, Divya LM, Sadasivan C. Bisphenol-A can bind to human glucocorticoid receptor as an agonist: an in silico study. J Appl Toxicol. 2010;30:769–74. doi: 10.1002/jat.1570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Riu A, Grimaldi M, le Maire A, Bey G, Phillips K, Boulahtouf A, et al. Peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma is a target for halogenated analogs of bisphenol A. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:1227–32. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1003328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Sui Y, Ai N, Park SH, Rios-Pilier J, Perkins JT, Welsh WJ, et al. Bisphenol A and its analogues activate human pregnane X receptor. Environ Health Perspect. 2012;120:399–405. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1104426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Saaristo M, Craft JA, Lehtonen KK, Lindstrom K. Exposure to 17alpha-ethinyl estradiol impairs courtship and aggressive behaviour of male sand gobies (Pomatoschistus minutus) Chemosphere. 2010;79:541–6. doi: 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2010.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Saaristo M, Craft JA, Lehtonen KK, Lindstrom K. An endocrine disrupting chemical changes courtship and parental care in the sand goby. Aquat Toxicol. 2010;97:285–92. doi: 10.1016/j.aquatox.2009.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Partridge C, Boettcher A, Jones AG. Short-term exposure to a synthetic estrogen disrupts mating dynamics in a pipefish. Horm Behav. 2010;58:800–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Hoffmann F, Kloas W. Estrogens can disrupt amphibian mating behavior. PLoS One. 2012;7:e32097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Halldin K, Berg C, Brandt I, Brunstrom B. Sexual behavior in Japanese quail as a test end point for endocrine disruption: effects of in ovo exposure to ethinylestradiol and diethylstilbestrol. Environ Health Perspect. 1999;107:861–6. doi: 10.1289/ehp.99107861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Baatrup E, Junge M. Antiandrogenic pesticides disrupt sexual characteristics in the adult male guppy Poecilia reticulata. Environ Health Perspect. 2001;109:1063–70. doi: 10.1289/ehp.011091063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Hoffmann F, Kloas W. An environmentally relevant endocrine-disrupting antiandrogen, vinclozolin, affects calling behavior of male Xenopus laevis. Horm Behav. 2010;58:653–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Hotchkiss AK, Ostby JS, Vandenbergh JG, Gray LE., Jr An environmental antiandrogen, vinclozolin, alters the organization of play behavior. Physiol Behav. 2003;79:151–6. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00093-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Charil A, Laplante DP, Vaillancourt C, King S. Prenatal stress and brain development. Brain Res Rev. 2010;65:56–79. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Mueller BR, Bale TL. Sex-specific programming of offspring emotionality after stress early in pregnancy. J Neurosci. 2008;28:9055–65. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1424-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Lucki I. The forced swimming test as a model for core and component behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. Behav Pharmacol. 1997;8:523–32. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199711000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Brummelte S, Lieblich SE, Galea LA. Gestational and postpartum corticosterone exposure to the dam affects behavioral and endocrine outcome of the offspring in a sexually-dimorphic manner. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:406–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Garardin P, Wendland J, Bodeau N, Galin A, Bialobos S, Tordjman S, et al. Depression during pregnancy: is the developmental impact earlier in boys? A prospective case-control study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:378–87. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05724blu. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Brummelte S, Schmidt KL, Taves MD, Soma KK, Galea LA. Elevated corticosterone levels in stomach milk, serum, and brain of male and female offspring after maternal corticosterone treatment in the rat. Dev Neurobiol. 2010;70:714–25. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Brummelte S, Pawluski JL, Galea LA. High post-partum levels of corticosterone given to dams influence postnatal hippocampal cell proliferation and behavior of offspring: a model of post-partum stress and possible depression. Horm Behav. 2006;50:370–82. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Schloesser RJ, Lehmann ML, Martinowich K, Manji H, Herkenham M. Environmental enrichment requires adult neurogenesis to facilitate the recovery from psychosocial stress. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:1152–63. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Cooke BM, Weathington JM. Human and animal research into sex-specific effects of child abuse. Horm Behav. 2014;65(4):416–26. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97•.Bourke CH, Neigh GN. Behavioral effects of chronic adolescent stress are sustained and sexually dimorphic. Horm Behav. 2011;60:112–20. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.03.011. This study showed that female sensitivity to psychosocial stress, relative to males, increases during adolescence. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.McCormick CM, Robarts D, Kopeikina K, Kelsey JE. Long-lasting, sex- and age-specific effects of social stressors on corticosterone responses to restraint and on locomotor responses to psychostimulants in rats. Horm Behav. 2005;48:64–74. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Swartz M, Blazer DG, Nelson CB. Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. I: lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence. J Affect Disord. 1993;29:85–96. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(93)90026-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psych Bull. 1994;115:424–43. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Wilner P. Chronic mild stress (CMS) revisited: consistency and behavioural–neurobiological concordance in the effects of CMS. Neuropsychobiology. 2005;52:90–110. doi: 10.1159/000087097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Hasler G, Drevets WC, Manji H, Charney DS. Discovering endophenotypes for major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1765–81. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Steiner JE, Glaser D, Hawilo ME, Berridge KC. Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:53–74. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Matthews K, Forbes N, Reid IC. Sucrose consumption as an hedonic measure following chronic mild stress. Physiol Behav. 1995;57:241–8. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00286-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Gronli J, Murison R, Bjorvatn B, Sorensen E, Portas CM, Ursin R. Chronic mild stress affects sucrose intake and sleep in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2004;150:139–47. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00252-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Nielsen CK, Arnt J, Sanchez C. Intracranial self-stimulation and scurose intake differ as hedonic measures following chronic mild stress: interstrain and inter-individual differences. Behav Brain Res. 2000;107:21–33. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00110-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Bielajew C, Konkle ATM, Merali Z. The effects of chronic mild stress on male Sprague-Dawley and Long Evans rats: I. Biochemical and physiological analyses. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:583–92. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00222-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Gronli J, Murison R, Fiske E, Bjorvatn B, Sorensen E, Portas CM, et al. Effects of chronic mild stress on sexual behavior, locomotor activity and consumption of sucrose and saccharine solutions. Physiol Behav. 2005;84:571–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.D’Aquila PS, Brain P, Willner P. Effects of chronic mild stress on performance in behavioural tests relevant to anxiety and depression. Physiol Behav. 1994;56:861–7. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90316-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Brotto LA, Gorzalka BB, LaMarre AK. Melatonin protects against the effects of chronic stress on sexual behaviour in male rats. Neuroreport. 2001;12:3465–9. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200111160-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Hill MN, Hellemans KGC, Verma P, Gorzalka BB, Weinberg J. Neurobiology of chronic mild stress: parallels to major depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:2085–117. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Dalla C, Antoniou K, Drossopoulou G, Xagoraris M, Kokras N, Sfikakis A, et al. Chronic mild stress impact: are females more vulnerable? Neuroscience. 2005;135:703–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Dalla C, Antoniou K, Kokras N, Drossopoulou G, Papathanasiou G, Bekris S, et al. Sex differences in the effects of two stress paradigms on dopaminergic neurotransmission. Physiol Behav. 2008;93:595–605. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Konkle ATM, Baker SL, Kentner AC, Barbagallo LSM, Merali Z, Bielajew C. Evaluation of the effects of chronic mild stressors on hedonic and physiological responses: sex and strain compared. Brain Res. 2003;992:227–38. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2003.08.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Grippo AJ, Sullivan NR, Damjanoska KJ, Crane JW, Carrasco GA, Shi J, et al. Chronic mild stress induces behavioral and physiological changes, and may alter serotonin 1A receptor function, in male and cycling female rats. Psychopharmacology. 2005;179:769–80. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2103-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Duncko R, Kiss A, Skulteyova I, Rusnak M, Jezova D. Corticotropin-releasing hormone mRNA levels in response to chronic mild stress rise in male but not in female rats while tyrosine hydroxylase mRNA levels decrease in both sexes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26:77–89. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00040-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Grippo AJ, Wu KD, Hassan I, Carter CS. Social isolation in prairie voles induces behaviors relevant to negative affect: toward the development of a rodent model focused on co-occurring depression and anxiety. Depression Anxiety. 2008;25:E17–26. doi: 10.1002/da.20375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Ayensu WK, Pucilowski O, Mason GA, Overstreet DH, Rezvani AH, Janowsky DS. Effects of chronic mild stress on serum complement activity, saccharin preferences and corticosterone levels in Flinders lines of rats. Physiol Behav. 1995;57:165–9. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00204-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Garcia LS, Comim CM, Valvassori SS, Reus GZ, Stertz L, Kapczinski F, et al. Ketamine treatment reverses behavioral and physiological alterations induced by chronic mild stress in rats. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:450–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Reich CG, Taylor ME, McCarthy MM. Differential effects of chronic unpredictable stress on hippocampal CB1 receptors in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2009;203:264–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Harris RBS, Zhou J, Youngblood BD, Smagin GN, Ryan DH. Failure to change exploration or saccharin preference in rats exposed to chronic mild stress. Physiol Behav. 1997;63:91–100. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00425-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.De Jong TR, Harris BN, Perea-Rodriguez JP, Saltzman W. Physiological and neuroendocrine responses to chronic variable stress in male California mice (Peromyscus californicus): influence of social environment and paternal state. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:2023–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, Van Der Vegt BJ, Van Reenen CG, Hopster H, et al. Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23:925–35. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.De Boer SF, Slangen JL, Van der Gugten J. Plasma catecholamine and corticosterone levels during active and passive shock-prod avoidance behavior in rats: effects of chlordiazepoxide. Physiol Behav. 1990;47:1089–98. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90357-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Trainor BC. Stress responses and the mesolimbic dopamine system: social contexts and sex differences. Horm Behav. 2011;60:457–69. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Miczek KA, Yap JJ, Covington HE. Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol Ther. 2008;120:102–28. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Russo SJ, Nestler EJ. The brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:609–25. doi: 10.1038/nrn3381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Huhman KL. Social conflict models: can they inform us about human psychopathology? Horm Behav. 2006;50:640–6. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Derntl B, Seidel E-M, Eickhoff SB, Kellermann T, Gur RC, Schneider F, et al. Neural correlates of social approach and withdrawal in patients with major depression. Soc Neurosci. 2011;6:482–501. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.579800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Moser JS, Huppert JD, Foa EB, Simons RF. Interpretation of ambiguous social scenarios in social phobia and depression: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biol Psychol. 2012;89:387–97. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Lara ME, Leader J, Klein DN. The association between social support and course of depression: is it confounded with personality? J Abnorm Psych. 1997;106:478–82. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.3.478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Krishnan V, Han M-H, Graham DL, Berton O, Renthal W, Russo SJ, et al. Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell. 2007;131:391–404. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Jiang B, Wang WW, Wang F, Hu Z-L, Xiao J-L, Yang S, et al. The stability of NRsb in the nucleus accumbens controls behavioral and synaptic adaptations to chronic stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:145–55. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Lehmann ML, Geddes CE, Lee JL, Herkenham M. Urine scent marking (USM): a novel test for depressive-like behavior and a predictor of stress resiliency in mice. PLoS One. 2013;8:e69822. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Ver Hoeve E, Kelly G, Luz S, Ghanshani S, Bhatnagar S. Short-term and long-term effects of repeated social defeat during adolescence or adulthood in female rats. Neuroscience. 2013;249:63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Bourke CH, Neigh GN. Exposure to repeated maternal aggression induces depressive-like behavior and increases startle in adult female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;227:270–5. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137•.Shimamoto A, DeBold JF, Holly EN, Miczek KA. Blunted accumbal dopamine response to cocaine following chronic social stress in female rats: exploring a link between depression and drug abuse. Psychopharmacology. 2011;218:271–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2364-7. This study was one of the first to develop a social defeat stress protocol for females, allowing the application of this important approach to females as well as males. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Holly EN, Shimamoto A, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Sex differences in behavioral and neural cross-sensitization and escalated cocaine taking as a result of episodic social defeat stress in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2012;224:179–88. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2846-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Huhman KL, Solomon MB, Janicki M, Harmon AC, Lin SM, Israel JE, et al. Conditioned defeat in male and female Syrian hamsters. Horm Behav. 2003;44:293–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Farruzzi AN, Solomon MB, Demas GE, Huhman KL. Gonadal hormones modulate the display of submissive behavior in socially defeated female Syrian hamsters. Horm Behav. 2005;47:569–75. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Silva AL, Fry WH, Sweeney C, Trainor BC. Effects of photoperiod and experience on aggressive behavior in female California mice. Behav Brain Res. 2010;208:528–34. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Trainor BC, Takahashi EY, Silva AL, Crean KK, Hostetler C. Sex differences in hormonal responses to social conflict in the monogamous California mouse. Horm Behav. 2010;58:506–12. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Trainor BC, Takahashi EY, Campi KL, Florez SA, Greenberg GD, Laman-Maharg A, et al. Sex differences in stress-induced social withdrawal: independence from adult gonadal hormones and inhibition of female phenotype by corncob bedding. Horm Behav. 2013;63:543–50. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144••.Trainor BC, Pride MC, Villalon Landeros R, Knoblauch NW, Takahashi EY, Silva AL, et al. Sex differences in social interaction behavior following social defeat stress in the monogamous California mouse (Peromyscus californicus) PLoS One. 2011;6:e17405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017405. In this study, female California mice raised on corncob bedding showed a more male-typical phenotype when exposed to defeat stress later in life, suggesting that exposure to estrogen-like compounds during development affects responses to stress later in life. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Hinkelmann K, Botzenhardt J, Muhtz C, Agorastos A, Wiedemann K, Kellner M, et al. Sex differences of salivary cortisol secretion in patients with major depression. Stress. 2012;15:105–9. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.582200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Bremmer MA, Deeg DJ, Beekman AT, Penninx BW, Lips P, Hoogendijk WJ. Major depression in late life is associated with both hypo- and hypercortisolemia. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:479–86. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Young NB, Nonneman RJ, Segall SK, Andrade GM, et al. Social approach and repetitive behavior in eleven inbred mouse strains. Behav Brain Res. 2008;191:118–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Greenberg GD, Laman-Maharg A, Campi KL, Voigt H, Orr VN, Simion C, et al. Sex differences in stress-induced social withdrawal: role of brain derived neurotrophic factor in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;7:223. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Seidel E-M, Habel U, Finkelmeyer A, Schneider F, Gur RC, Derntl B. Implicit and explicit behavioral tendencies in male and female depression. Psychiatry Res. 2010;177:124–30. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Markaverich BM, Alejandro MA, Markaverich D, Zitzow L, Casajuna N, Camarao N, et al. Identification of an endocrine disrupting agent from corn with mitogenic activity. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2002;291:692–700. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.2002.6499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Villalon Landeros R, Yoo HJ, Morisseau C, Fu S, Hammock BD, Trainor BC. Corncob bedding reverses the effects of estrogens on aggressive behavior and reduces estrogen receptor alpha expression in the brain. Endocrinology. 2012;153:949–53. doi: 10.1210/en.2011-1745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Markaverich BM, Crowley J, Rodriguez M, Shoulars K, Thompson T. Tetrahydrofurandiol stimulation of phospholipase A2, lipoxygenase, and cyclooxygenase gene expression and MCF-7 human breast cancer cell proliferation. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115:1727–31. doi: 10.1289/ehp.10659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Amateau SK, McCarthy MM. Induction of PGE(2) by estradiol mediates developmental masculinization of sex behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:643–50. doi: 10.1038/nn1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.McCarthy MM, Wright CL, Schwarz JM. New tricks by an old dogma: mechanisms of the organizational/activational hypothesis of steroid-mediated sexual differentiation of brain and behavior. Horm Behav. 2009;55:655–65. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.D’Ursi P, Salvi E, Fossa P, Milanesi L, Rovida E. Modelling the interaction of steroid receptors with endocrine disrupting chemicals. BMC Bioinformatics. 2005;6(Suppl 4):S10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-6-S4-S10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Li Y, Luh CJ, Burns KA, Arao Y, Jiang Z, Teng CT, et al. Endocrine-disrupting chemicals (EDCs): in vitro mechanism of estrogenic activation and differential effects on ER target genes. Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121:459–66. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1205951. 466e1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Teng C, Goodwin B, Shockley K, Xia M, Huang R, Norris J, et al. Bisphenol A affects androgen receptor function via multiple mechanisms. Chem Biol Interact. 2013;203:556–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cbi.2013.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Clairardin SG, Paitz RT, Bowden RM. In ovo inhibition of steroid metabolism by bisphenol-A as a potential mechanism of endocrine disruption. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280:20131773. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.1773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Yaoi T, Itoh K, Nakamura K, Ogi H, Fujiwara Y, Fushiki S. Genome-wide analysis of epigenomic alterations in fetal mouse forebrain after exposure to low doses of bisphenol A. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2008;376:563–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2008.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Panagiotidou E, Zerva S, Mitsiou DJ, Alexis MN, Kitraki E. Perinatal exposure to low-dose bisphenol A affects the neuroendocrine stress response in rats. J Endocrinol. 2014;220:207–18. doi: 10.1530/JOE-13-0416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Palanza P, Howdeshell KL, Parmigiani S, vom Saal FS. Exposure to a low dose of bisphenol A during fetal life or in adulthood alters maternal behavior in mice. Environ Health Perspect. 2002;110:415–22. doi: 10.1289/ehp.02110s3415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Goel N, Bale TL. Examining the intersection of sex and stress in modelling neuropsychiatric disorders. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21:415–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01843.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Bertolasio J, Fyfe S, Snyder BW, Davis AM. Neonatal injections of methoxychlor decrease adult rat female reproductive behavior. Neurotoxicology. 2011;32:809–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2011.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Carbone S, Ponzo OJ, Gobetto N, Samaniego YA, Reynoso R, Scacchi P, et al. Antiandrogenic effect of perinatal exposure to the endocrine disruptor di-(2-ethylhexyl) phthalate increases anxiety-like behavior in male rats during sexual maturation. Horm Behav. 2013;63:692–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Schultz MM, Bartell SE, Schoenfuss HL. Effects of triclosan and triclocarban, two ubiquitous environmental contaminants, on anatomy, physiology, and behavior of the fathead minnow (Pimephales promelas) Arch Environ Contam Toxicol. 2012;63:114–24. doi: 10.1007/s00244-011-9748-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Knapp R, Marsh-Matthews E, Vo L, Rosencrans S. Stress hormone masculinizes female morphology and behaviour. Biol Lett. 2011;7:150–2. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Frederick P, Jayasena N. Altered pairing behaviour and reproductive success in white ibises exposed to environmentally relevant concentrations of methylmercury. Proc Biol Sci. 2011;278:1851–7. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES