Approximately 200 scientific reports have been published at the intersection of sleep, cognition, and aging.1 These papers range from population-based cross-sectional studies that subjectively measured both sleep quality and cognitive vitality with large sample sizes (N > 100,000 in Sternberg et al.2) to small-sample experimental studies that manipulated PSG-defined sleep and objectively measured cognitive performance.3,4 What is missing from this extensive literature is a longitudinal, population-based PSG and cognition study in aging adults with substantial statistical power. In this issue of SLEEP, Song et al.5 addressed this gap in the literature by capitalizing on the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men (MrOS) Study. They obtained one night of at-home PSG recordings in a large group of older adults who did not have a dementia diagnosis (N = 2,601) and evaluated cognitive performance using the Trail Making Test Part B and the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination at the baseline sleep visit and again 3–4 years later. Their most impressive findings were that after adjusting for 15 potentially confounding variables (e.g., age, depression, hypertension), men with the lowest quartile of REM sleep (< 15%) and the highest quartile of N1 sleep (≥ 8.52%) showed an accelerated rate of cognitive decline relative to those in the upper quartile of REM sleep (≥ 23.7%) and lowest quartile of N1 sleep (< 3.98%), respectively.
The findings of Song et al. are bolstered by a long history of related studies that also implicated diminished REM sleep in cognitive aging. Nearly a half-century ago, Feinberg and colleagues6 conducted the first PSG and cognition correlational study in healthy older adults, and one of their primary findings was that low REM activity in healthy older adults was associated with lower performance on psychometric tests. Furthermore, from 1976 to 1990, Spiegel and colleagues evaluated cognitive performance (e.g., mini-mental state examination) in a group of 57 older adults who had baseline PSG data.7,8 They found that low REM density predicted speedier decline on some cognitive tests. More recently, REM sleep in older adults has been associated in cross-sectional studies with a variety of cognitive measures including episodic memory.9–11 Similar findings between cognitive function and REM sleep have emerged in aged rodent studies.12,13 The above findings, however, belie the inconsistent associations involving PSG in the literature,1 which perhaps reflect small sample sizes (Ns < 20) that do not generalize to the population and increase risk for type I and type II errors.14 Song et al.'s report,5 which included the largest and most generalizable sample for a longitudinal PSG and cognitive aging study, provides the most convincing evidence to-date for low REM sleep (and high N1 sleep) being a predictor of declining cognitive functions with aging.
The null findings in Song et al.5 are perhaps equally interesting. Much of the current work on sleep, aging, and cognition—often inspired by results emanating from memory consolidation research15,16—has focused on the role of slow wave sleep (SWS) and whether the changes observed in slow wave activity across the lifespan17,18 presage declining cognition.19–23 Yet, most PSG and cognitive aging studies indicate that SWS measures are not associated with cognitive measures in healthy older adults. A few studies have reported positive cognitive correlations with SWS or EEG sleep delta activity in older adults, but there may be nearly as many negative associations in the literature.1 The finding of Song et al.5 that duration and percent of SWS at baseline did not predict cognitive decline over 3–4 years is consistent with this negative literature. An important caveat is that the MrOS study lacked a measure of episodic memory, which might be more likely to correlate with SWS (or delta band power22 or spindle density10).
The longitudinal design used by Song et al.5 makes it tempting to conclude that low REM and high N1 in advanced age cause cognitive function to decline. The reverse explanation—that lower cognitive function or effort leads to decreased REM sleep or sleep continuity—seems unintuitive but requires careful consideration. First, two longitudinal studies in humans found that lower cognitive performance at baseline was a predictor of subsequently lesser REM sleep duration24 and lower sleep efficiency25 years later. Similar findings for impaired memory predicting shorter duration of paradoxical sleep bouts have been observed in rats as well.26 Second, in MrOS's “sister study” (the Study of Osteoporotic Fractures), women whose mini-mental state examination scores declined over the preceding 15 years were more likely than non-decliners to show lower actigraphy-defined sleep efficiency.27 Third, experimental evidence also converges with the account that cognitive function, use, and/or effort impacts sleep in older adults. Interventional “cognitive training” studies have noted that after nearly two months of repeated training on cognitive tasks, subjective and actigraphy-defined measures of sleep improved.28–30 Such findings might be subsumed under synaptic downscaling theory that predicts that the amount of learning that occurs during the day directly impacts sleep quality that night31,32; however, synaptic downscaling theory has tended to focus more on the impact on SWS rather than REM sleep or sleep continuity.
Sleep decline and cognitive decline could also be epiphenomenal in older age, and be better explained by diminishing general health. The study of Song et al.5 discourages this explanation because they controlled for many health variables. However, the health variables were often self-reported, thereby possibly lacking in precision. A possible explanation for correlations between REM sleep and cognitive function in old age is central cholinergic dysfunction. Cholinergic innervation is critical to eliciting REM sleep33; cholinergic dysfunction has consistently been associated with cognitive deficits34; and reduced cholinergic innervation is observed in many older adults, particularly those with the early stages of Alzheimer disease.35 It is therefore noteworthy that when researchers manipulated REM sleep in older adults via REM deprivation, REM rebound, and with an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, that only the cholinergic medication had an effect on cognitive functioning.36
The well-designed study of Song et al.5 is a valuable reminder that REM sleep has historically been more consistently connected with cognitive measures than has SWS in older adults. Whether low REM sleep causes cognitive function to decline is a hypothesis that requires further investigation, with particular attention paid to the possibility that a common substrate (e.g., decreased cholinergic neurotransmission) may explain declines in both cognitive function and REM activity.
CITATION
Scullin MK, Bliwise DL. Is cognitive aging associated with levels of rem sleep or slow wave sleep? SLEEP 2015;38(3):335–336.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Scullin MK, Bliwise DL. Sleep, cognition, and aging: integrating a half-century of multidisciplinary research. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10:97–137. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sternberg DA, Ballard K, Hardy JL, Katz B, Doraiswamy PM, Scanlon M. The largest human cognitive performance dataset reveals insights into the effects of lifestyle factors and aging. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:292. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Campbell SS, Stanchina MD, Schlang JR, Murphy PJ. Effects of a month-long napping regimen in older individuals. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:224–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03264.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mathias S, Zihl J, Steiger A, Lancel M. Effect of repeated gaboxadol administration on night sleep and next-day performance in healthy elderly subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:833–41. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Song Y, Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Redline S, Stone KL Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Study Group. Relationships between sleep stages and changes in cognitive function in older men: the MrOS Sleep Study. Sleep. 2015;38:411–21. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Feinberg I, Koresko RL, Heller N. EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and pathological aging in man. J Psychiatr Res. 1967;5:107–44. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(67)90027-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Spiegel R. Sleep and sleeplessness in advanced age. New York: Spectrum Publications; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Spiegel R, Herzog A, Köberle S. Polygraphic sleep criteria as predictors of successful aging: an exploratory longitudinal study. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:435–42. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00042-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kim SJ, Lee JH, Lee DY, Jhoo JH, Woo JI. Neurocognitive dysfunction associated with sleep quality and sleep apnea in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19:374–81. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181e9b976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lafortune M, Gagnon JF, Martin N, et al. Sleep spindles and rapid eye movement sleep as predictors of next morning cognitive performance in healthy middle-aged and older participants. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:159–67. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Scullin MK, Fairley JA, Trotti LM, Goldstein FC, Factor SA, Bliwise DL. Sleep correlates of trait executive function and memory in Parkinson's disease. J Parkinsons Dis. 2015 Jan 13; doi: 10.3233/JPD-140475. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Markowska AL, Stone WS, Ingram DK, et al. Individual differences in aging: behavioral and neurobiological correlates. Neurobiol Aging. 1989;10:31–43. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(89)80008-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stone WS, Altman HJ, Berman RF, Caldwell DF, Kilbey MM. Association of sleep parameters and memory in intact old rats and young rats with lesions in the nucleus basalis magnocellularis. Behav Neurosci. 1989;103:755–64. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.4.755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Yarkoni T. Big correlations in little studies: inflated fMRI correlations reflect low statistical power—Commentary on Vul et al. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2009;4:294–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rasch B, Born J. About sleep's role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93:681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pan SC, Rickard TC. Sleep and motor learning: is there room for consolidation? Psychol Bull. 2015 doi: 10.1037/bul0000009. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bliwise DL. Sleep in normal aging and dementia. Sleep. 1993;16:40–81. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Carrier J, Viens I, Poirier G, et al. Sleep slow wave changes during the middle years of life. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:758–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Anderson C, Horne JA. Prefrontal cortex: links between low frequency delta EEG in sleep and neuropsychological performance in healthy, older people. Psychophysiology. 2003;40:349–57. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.00038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Backhaus J, Born J, Hoeckesfeld R, Fokuhl S, Hohagen F, Junghanns K. Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep. Learn Mem. 2007;14:336–41. doi: 10.1101/lm.470507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Van Der Werf YD, Altena E, Schoonheim MM, et al. Sleep benefits subsequent hippocampal functioning. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:122–3. doi: 10.1038/nn.2253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mander BA, Rao V, Lu B, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:357–64. doi: 10.1038/nn.3324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Scullin MK. Sleep, memory, and aging: the link between slow-wave sleep and episodic memory changes from younger to older adults. Psychol Aging. 2013;28:105–14. doi: 10.1037/a0028830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Prinz PN. Sleep patterns in the healthy aged: relationship with intellectual function. J Gerontol. 1977;32:179–86. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hoch CC, Dew MA, Reynolds CF, Monk TH. A longitudinal study of laboratory-and diary-based sleep measures in healthy “old old” and “young old” volunteers. Sleep. 1994;17:489–96. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.6.489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Stone WS, Rudd RJ, Parsons MW, Gold PE. Memory scores in middle-aged rats predict later deficits in memory, paradoxical sleep, and blood glucose regulation in old age. Exp Aging Res. 1997;23:287–300. doi: 10.1080/03610739708254285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yaffe K, Blackwell T, Barnes DE, Ancoli-Israel S, Stone KL. Preclinical cognitive decline and subsequent sleep disturbance in older women. Neurology. 2007;69:237–42. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000265814.69163.da. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Diamond K, Mowszowski L, Cockayne N, et al. Randomized controlled trial of a healthy brain aging cognitive training program: effects on memory, mood, and sleep. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014 Nov 18; doi: 10.3233/JAD-142061. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Haimov I, Shatil E. Cognitive training improves sleep quality and cognitive function among older adults with insomnia. PLoS One. 2013;8:e61390. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Conte F, Carobbi G, Errico BM, Ficca G. The effects of pre-sleep learning on sleep continuity, stability, and organization in elderly individuals. Front Neurol. 2012;3:109. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cirelli C. Brain plasticity, sleep and aging. Gerontology. 2012;58:441–5. doi: 10.1159/000336149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res Bull. 2003;62:143–50. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Van Dort CJ, Zachs DP, Kenny JD, et al. Optogenetic activation of cholinergic neurons in the PPT or LDT induces REM sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112:584–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423136112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Schliebs R, Arendt T. The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behav Brain Res. 2011;221:555–63. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sun X, Salat D, Upchurch K, Deason R, Kowall N, Budson A. Destruction of white matter integrity in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. J Investig Med. 2014;62:927–33. doi: 10.1097/JIM.0000000000000102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hornung OP, Regen F, Danker-Hopfe H, Schredl M, Heuser I. The relationship between REM sleep and memory consolidation in old age and effects of cholinergic medication. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61:750–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]