Skip to main content
Taylor & Francis Open Select logoLink to Taylor & Francis Open Select
. 2015 Jan 14;15(1):1–8. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2015.992732

Paternal Depression in the Postnatal Period and Early Father–Infant Interactions

Vaheshta Sethna b, Lynne Murray c, Elena Netsi d, Lamprini Psychogiou e, Paul G Ramchandani a,*
PMCID: PMC4337747  PMID: 25745364

SYNOPSIS

Objective. Paternal depressive disorder is associated with adverse effects on child development. One possible mechanism for this is through the effects of the disorder on parenting capacities. The link between paternal depression and father–infant interactions was investigated at three-months postpartum. Design. Major depressive disorder was assessed in N = 192 fathers using a structured clinical interview (SCID). Altogether, 54 fathers met criteria for depression, and 99 fathers were categorized as non-depressed. Observational assessments of face-to-face father–infant interactions were conducted in an infant-seat setting and a floor-mat setting. Associations between paternal depression and father–infant interactions were analyzed. Results. Paternal depression is associated with more withdrawn parental behavior in interactions on the floor-mat. There were few other differences in observed interaction between depressed and non-depressed fathers. Conclusions. Fathers with depression may be more withdrawn, displaying less verbal and behavioral stimulation during interactions with their young infants. They may initiate a pattern of parenting that remains compromised, potentially affecting their children’s development.

INTRODUCTION

Depression frequently affects both parents in the postnatal period. Most research has focused on maternal postnatal depression and its potential impact on children’s development (Field, 2010). However, meta-analytic evidence suggests that depression is also common in fathers, with a prevalence of approximately 5% being diagnosed with a major depressive disorder (Paulson & Bazemore, 2010). Children whose fathers are depressed in the postnatal period are at increased risk of adverse psychosocial development (Carro, Grant, Gotlib, & Compass, 1993; Paulson, Keefe, & Leiferman, 2009; Ramchandani et al., 2008). While a number of potential pathways exist by which parental depression may affect children’s development, evidence suggests that parenting capacities, such as sensitivity and responsiveness, are important and also modifiable (Lawrence, Davies, & Ramchandani, 2012).

Previous empirical research supports an association between paternal depression and parenting impairment (for a review see, Wilson & Durbin, 2010). However, very few studies have examined father–infant interactions in the early months after birth, when key, and potentially enduring, patterns of interaction can become established. McElwain and Volling found that depressed fathers were less intrusive (uninvolved) during free-play with their 12-month-olds (McElwain & Volling, 1999). Similarly, depressed fathers (compared to non-depressed fathers) are less likely to engage in reading to their 1-year-old infants (Davis, Davis, Freed, & Clark, 2011) and demonstrate decreased tactile and vocal stimulation with their 2.5- to 4-month-old infants (Zaslow, Pedersen, Cain, Suwalsky, & Kramer, 1985). Although these studies suggest that paternal depression reduces the amount of engagement with infants, others have failed to confirm this association (Field, Hossain, & Malphurs, 1999; Lundy, 2002). Many of these studies (Davis et al., 2011; Edhborg, Matthiesen, Lundh, & Widstrom, 2005; Paulson, Dauber, & Leiferman, 2006) are subject to important methodological limitations, most notably the use of self-report measures of both depressive symptoms and parenting, leading to the potential for “shared-method inflation.”

The present study aimed to investigate the link between paternal major depressive disorder and the quality of father–infant interactions at three-months postpartum, using an interview assessment of depression and an observed measure of parenting in two interactive settings. The focus of this study on early infancy is critical given that this is a period of rapid development and high susceptibility (Knudsen, 1999) when the infant’s primary source of interaction and communication is with their parents.

METHOD

Participant Recruitment and Screening

The current report comprises data from a prospective cohort study of fathers and their families. Fathers were recruited from postnatal wards of two hospitals in the United Kingdom and met the following eligibility criteria: fathers were aged 18 years or older at the time of the child’s birth and spoke sufficient English to complete all assessments; infants had been born at no less than 37 weeks, had a birth weight of at least 2.5 kg, and did not have any severe illness or abnormality. All parents who consented were sent the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox, Holden, & Sagovsky, 1987) 7 weeks after the birth of their child—1,562 out of 4,107 (38.0%) were returned. An attempt was made to contact all fathers scoring 10 or above on the EPDS, and a 1 in 4 random sample of fathers scoring less than 10. Those agreeing to participate were visited at home; N = 192 fathers agreed to participate.

Fathers had a mean age of 35.04 years (SD = 5.9), and the mean age of infants was 14.37 weeks (SD = 2.28). All but one set of parents were either married and/or living together as a couple. The majority of fathers (92%) were European. There was some evidence that fathers in the depressed group were slightly older than those in the non-depressed group. See Table 1 for background characteristics of the sample.

TABLE 1 .

Paternal and Infant Demographic Characteristics

  Totala Depressedb Non-Depressedb    
  n = 192 n = 54 n = 99    
Demographic Details M SD M SD M SD t p
Father’s age (years) 34.99 5.9 36.20 5.34 34.47 6.13 −1.75 .083
Infant’s age (weeks) 14.51 3.04 14.76 2.78 14.17 1.96 −1.36 .179
  % % % χ2 p
Father’s education level       4.71 .194
 GCSC’s/A levels 20.8 22.6 24.2    
 Diploma 15.6 41.5 15.8    
 Degree 32.3 13.2 25.3    
 Postgraduate degree 28.1 22.6 34.7    
Marital status       .55 .46
 Single .6 1    
 Married/living together 99.4 98.1 97    
Father’s ethnicity       2.04 .154
 White 92 97.3 87.9    
 Non-White 5.6 1.9 7.1    
Infant gender       2.66 .103
 Female 52.6 40.7 54.5    
 Male 47.4 59.3 45.5    
Infant’s birth order (%)       5.27 .153
 First born 59.4 48.1 64.6    
 Second born 31.8 40.7 26.3    
 Third born 7.3 7.4 8.1    
 Fourth born 1.6 3.7 1.0    

aSample used for the analysis of depression scores on the EPDS.

bSample for group analyses using the SCID.

Procedure

A home-based assessment took place when the child was three-months old. Fathers were interviewed with the SCID for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders (First, Gibbon, Spitzer, & Williams, 2002) to assess whether they met criteria for major depressive disorder. Altogether, 54 fathers met criteria for depression, and 99 fathers were categorized as non-depressed. A further 39 fathers who had high scores on the EPDS, but did not meet diagnostic criteria for depression, were not included in these group comparisons.

Observations of father–infant interactions were videotaped during two three-minute sessions of face-to-face play; one with the standard interaction with the infant placed in an infant-seat, and the other with the infant on a floor-mat the father sitting facing the infant. The latter setting was introduced after pilot work suggested this setting would help capture the more physical and stimulating aspects of interactions that fathers are more likely to engage in, and also provide a context in which fathers might “behave in accord with depressive symptomatology, namely to withdraw from interaction with the infant” (McElwain & Volling, 1999, p. 79). Fathers were instructed to play with and talk to their infant as they would normally, but without using any toys or objects. Data on both exposure and outcome were available for a maximum of 183 fathers in the infant-seat setting and 178 fathers in the floor-mat setting.

Paternal Depression

The EPDS (Cox et al., 1987) is a 10-item self-report scale, assessing the symptoms of depression. The EPDS has been validated in men (Matthey, Barnett, Kavanagh, & Howie, 2001).

The SCID (First et al., 2002) for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders was used to make the DSM-IV diagnosis for major depressive disorder. The SCID has high reliability and validity (Basco et al., 2000).

Observations of Father–Infant Interactions

The global rating scales (GRS; Murray, Fiori-Cowley, Hooper, & Cooper, 1996) were used to code interactions. It has well-established psychometric properties with good discriminant validity (Gunning, Murray, & Lawson, 2002). Interactions were rated by two trained coders blind to paternal diagnostic status. The coding scheme comprised rating behaviors on a series of 5-point scales (1–5) with lower scores indicating inadequate interactions. The following dimensions of paternal interaction were measured: (1) sensitivity: Paternal response to the infant’s communication cues in a way that is appropriate to the infant’s needs and experiences, including attitude and feelings toward the infant; (2) intrusiveness: over-stimulating vocal and physical activity around the infant, cutting across his/her communication, using body games, tactile stimulation, and unpredictable, arousing vocalizations; (3) remoteness: withdrawal and disengagement manifested verbally, psychologically, and physically; and (4) depressive affect: affective state and level of enjoyment in interacting with the infant, including anxious, vocal, and physical activity. For clarity of interpretation the direction of scores on this scale were reversed—higher scores indicating increased depressive affect.

A randomly selected 20% of interactions were independently coded by pairs of coders for inter-rater agreement. Inter-rater intra-class correlations (ICC; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) ranged from .74 to .88. Discrepancies between raters were discussed, and final ratings were determined in collaboration with members of the Winnicott Research Unit who were involved in the development of the scale.

Covariates

Potential covariates were assessed including father’s age, education, and maternal depressive symptoms (EPDS scores). However, none of these was significantly associated with the outcome measures and so were not included in subsequent analyses.

Analytic Strategy

Analyses were conducted separately for the infant-seat and floor-mat settings. In the study, (1) a continuous measure of current depressive symptoms in fathers (EPDS) and (2) diagnosed depressed (n = 54) and non-depressed (n = 99) fathers (SCID) were analyzed. Use of a continuous measure of current depressive symptoms is supported by evidence suggesting that sub-threshold levels of depression are often of clinical importance (Karsten et al., 2011).

Bivariate associations were examined using Pearson correlations (with continuous EPDS scores), and mean differences and independent t-tests (for SCID diagnostic groups). As none of the covariates was associated with the outcome measure, the multiple linear regression analyses was not conducted.

RESULTS

Description of Exposure and Outcome Variables

Fifty-four fathers met the SCID criteria for depression (19 with current depression, and 35 with a history of depression). Fathers with depression scored significantly higher on the EPDS (M = 14.79, SD = 3.41) than non-depressed fathers (M = 6.64, SD = 4.40), U = 258.00, p < .001. Descriptive analyses for the whole sample on the dimensions in the floor-mat interaction setting are presented in Table 2 together with comparative analyses between the depressed and non-depressed groups. In both father–infant interaction settings, increased depressive affect was associated with more insensitive (infant-seat: r (182) = −.30, p < .01; floor-mat: r (177) = −.32, p < .01) and remote (infant-seat: r (182) = −.55, p < .01; floor-mat: r (177) = −.56, p < .01) interactions. More sensitive fathers were less intrusive in both interaction settings (infant-seat: r (182) = .33, p < .01; floor-mat: r (177) = .41, p < .01). However, more remote fathers were only less intrusive, (r (177) = .26, p < .01) in the floor-mat session.

TABLE 2 .

Father–Infant Interactions: Whole Group Descriptive Statistics and Comparisons Between Depressed and Non-Depressed Groupsa

  Whole Sample
       
Interaction Dimensions (Floor-mat) M (SD) Median Depressed n = 54* Mb (SD) Non-Depressed n = 99* Mb (SD) t p
Sensitivity 3.70 (.55) 3.8 3.67 (.54) 3.68 (.54) .06 .955
Intrusivenessc 3.71 (.80) 4.0 3.84 (.65) 3.58 (.90) −1.98 .050
Remoteness 4.59 (.80) 5.0 4.50 (.93) 4.60 (.75) .70 .459
Depressive affect 4.02 (.53) 4.0 4.00 (.57) 4.03 (.53) .44 .658

aMajor depressive disorder assessed using the SCID.

bGRS scored on a scale of 1–5.

cHigher scores indicate less stimulating interactions.

*There was missing parent–child interaction data for four depressed and six non-depressed fathers.

Association Between Depressive Symptoms (EPDS scores) and Father–Infant Interaction

In the infant-seat setting, paternal depressive scores were not associated with any of the father–infant interaction dimensions.

In the floor-mat setting, fathers with increased depressive symptoms (high EPDS scores) were less intrusive (higher scores), r (176) = .15, p = .043. EPDS scores were positively associated with the expression of depressive affect, albeit weakly, r (176) = .15, p = .053, implying that high EPDS scorers displayed increased negative affect in their interactions. There was no evidence of an association between EPDS scores and paternal remoteness and sensitivity.

Association Between Major Depressive Disorder (SCID) and Father–Infant Interaction

In the infant-seat setting, diagnosed depression was not associated with any of the father–infant interaction dimensions and hence, detailed results are not reported.

In the floor-mat setting, there was no significant difference between the depressed and non-depressed groups on paternal sensitivity, remoteness, or depressive affect (see Table 2). There was, however, a marginally significant difference between the groups on paternal intrusiveness, t (141) = −1.98, p = .050, with depressed fathers showing less intrusion, that is using less vocal and physical stimulation than non-depressed fathers. Because none of the covariates was associated with intrusiveness, multiple linear regression analysis is not reported.

DISCUSSION

This study is, to the authors’ knowledge, the first to examine the association between diagnosed paternal depression in the postnatal period and specific dimensions of observed father–infant interaction. Although no group differences on paternal sensitivity were found, some evidence emerged for more withdrawn behavior from depressed fathers during interaction with their infants.

There are a number of strengths and limitations of the current study to consider. A major strength is the inclusion of blind-coded, direct observations of fathers, rather than relying on self-report or mothers as the source of information about father’s behavior. This is the first study of father–infant interaction to include a clinical interview assessment of major depressive disorder. A number of limitations must also be considered. First, a relatively modest number of fathers with current depression were recruited, despite significant efforts at recruitment. Second, although the sample was recruited from universally provided clinical services, the ethnic and socioeconomic composition of the participants may limit generalizability of the findings to other groups. Third, like much previous research, a measure of parent-child interaction that had been developed for mothers was used (Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, London, & Cabrera, 2002). This choice can limit what is assessed in fathers, potentially missing the importance of stimulating interactions that are more characteristic of fathers (Lamb, 1977).

Previous research on the association between paternal depression and father–infant interaction has been relatively inconsistent (McElwain & Volling, 1999; Paulson et al., 2006; Zaslow et al., 1985), and the findings of the present study were also mixed. No differences were found on measures of parental sensitivity, whereas it was found that fathers with depression were less intrusive, or more withdrawn, in their interactions but only in the floor-mat setting. Such an association between depressed mood and less involved parenting is consistent with previous research and emphasizes the importance of assessing parenting in different contexts (McElwain & Volling, 1999; Paulson et al., 2006; Zaslow et al., 1985). It is likely that the free-play session allowed depressive behaviors to emerge, leading to parental withdrawal, whereas the infant-seat setting required greater involvement from the parent; which is of concern, since young children in particular need the active involvement of a caring adult for their cognitive and socioemotional developmental needs to be met. However, it should be noted that the magnitude of the differences between depressed and non-depressed fathers was not large.

The results extend previous research in two key ways. First, they suggest that father–infant interactions may in some cases be a potential pathway by which parental depression affects children’s development from a very young age. Father-child interactions are potentially amenable to intervention (Lawrence et al., 2012), and, therefore, represent an important preventive opportunity. Second, the findings highlight the importance of considering father–infant interactions, both in their similarities to mother–infant interactions, but also how they may differ (Tamis-LeMonda, 2004). An understanding of these differences may enable researchers to better understand the complementary roles of parents within the family system and progress toward a more comprehensive theory of parenting.

IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, APPLICATION, AND POLICY

The findings of the present study suggest that depression in fathers during the postnatal period may be associated with withdrawn father–infant interactions. They highlight the importance of the early postpartum months for the development of the father–infant relationship, and indicate the need, not only for interventions designed to alleviate depression in new fathers, but also to help improve the quality of interpersonal communication between fathers and their infants. An understanding of the nature of difficulties in father–infant interactions in the context of postnatal depression is important because of their possible implications for the longer-term development of the child. Therefore, it is important that clinicians are aware of the risk of depression in new fathers and the possible risks to the developing infant.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors would like to thank Olivia Edmondson, Jane Iles, Haido Vlachos, Annemarie Lodder, Nicola Harvey, Nicole Hickey, the fathers, and their families for their participation.

FUNDING

Dr. Ramchandani was supported by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Fellowship 078434, and Dr. Sethna was supported by a Medical Research Council studentship.

Funding Statement

Dr. Ramchandani was supported by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Fellowship 078434, and Dr. Sethna was supported by a Medical Research Council studentship.

REFERENCES

  1. Basco M. R., Bostic J. Q., Davies D., Rush J., Witte B., Hendrickse W. Barnett V. Methods to improve diagnostic accuracy in a community mental health setting. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;(10):1599–1609. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Carro M. G., Grant K. E., Gotlib I. H. Compass B. E. Postpartum depression and child development: An investigation of mothers and fathers as sources of risk and resilience. Development and Psychopathology. 1993;(4):567–579. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006167. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cox J. L., Holden J. M. Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1987:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Davis R. N., Davis M. M., Freed G. L. Clark S. J. Fathers’ depression related to positive and negative parenting behaviours with 1-year old children. Pediatrics. 2011:612–618. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Edhborg M., Matthiesen A.-S., Lundh W. Widstrom A.-M. Some early indicators for depressive symptoms and bonding 2 months postpartum—A study of new mothers and fathers. Archives of Women’s Mental Health. 2005;(4):221–231. doi: 10.1007/s00737-005-0097-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Field T. M. Postpartum depression effects on early interactions, parenting, and safety practices: A review. Infant Behavior and Development. 2010:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Field T. M., Hossain Z. Malphurs J. Depressed fathers interactions with their infants. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1999;(3):322–332. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199923)20:3&#x0003c;322::AID-IMHJ8&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-T. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. First M. B., Gibbon M., Spitzer R. L. Williams J. B. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders. New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  9. Gunning M., Murray L. Lawson C. Global rating scheme. Paper presented at the Eight Congress of the World Association for Infant Mental Health; Amsterdam: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  10. Karsten J., Hartman C., Smit J., Zitman F., Beekman A., Cuijpers P. Penninx B. W. J. H. Psychiatric history and subthreshold symptoms as predictors of the occurrence of depressive or anxiety disorder within 2 years. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;(3):206–212. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Knudsen E. I. Early experience and critical periods. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  12. Lamb M. E. Father–infant and mother–infant interaction in the first year of life. Child Development. 1977;(1):167–181. doi: 10.2307/1128896. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Lawrence P. J., Davies B. Ramchandani P. G. Using video feedback to improve early father–infant interaction: A pilot study. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;(1):61–71. doi: 10.1177/1359104512437210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lundy B. L. Paternal socio-psychological factors and infant attachment: The mediating role of synchrony in father–infant interactions. Infant Behavior & Development. 2002:221–236. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00123-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Matthey S., Barnett B., Kavanagh D. J. Howie P. Validation of the Edinburgh postnatal depression scale for men, and comparison of item endorsement with their partners. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2001:175–184. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00236-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. McElwain N. L. Volling B. L. Depressed mood and marital conflict: Relations to maternal and paternal intrusiveness with one-year-old infants. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1999;(1):63–83. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(99)80004-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Murray L., Fiori-Cowley A., Hooper R. Cooper P. The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother–infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Development. 1996;(5):2512–2526. doi: 10.2307/1131637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Paulson J. F. Bazemore S. D. Prenatal and postpartum depression in fathers and its association with maternal depression. [Review] Journal of the American Medical Association. 2010;(19):1961–1969. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Paulson J. F., Dauber S. Leiferman J. A. Individual and combined effects of postpartum depression in mothers and fathers on parenting behavior. Pediatrics. 2006;(2):659–668. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Paulson J. F., Keefe H. A. Leiferman J. A. Early parental depression and child language development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2009;(3):254–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01973.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ramchandani P. G., Stein A., O’Connor T. G., Heron J., Murray L. Evans J. Depression in men in the postnatal period and later child psychopathology: A population cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;(4):390–398. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816429c2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Shannon J. D., Tamis-LeMonda C. S., London K. Cabrera N. Beyond rough and tumble: Low-income fathers’ interactions and children’s cognitive development at 24 months. Parenting Science and Practice. 2002;(2):77–104. doi: 10.1207/S15327922PAR0202_01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Shrout P. E. Fleiss J. L. Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;(2):420–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Tamis-LeMonda C. S. Conceptualizing fathers’ roles: Playmates and more. Human Development. 2004;(4):220–227. doi: 10.1159/000078724. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Wilson S. Durbin C. E. Effects of paternal depression on fathers’ parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;(2):167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Zaslow M. J., Pedersen F. A., Cain R. L., Suwalsky J. T. Kramer E. L. Depressed mood in new fathers: Associations with parent–infant interaction. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 1985;(2):135–150. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Parenting, Science and Practice are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES