Skip to main content
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy logoLink to Alzheimer's Research & Therapy
. 2015 Jan 15;7(1):2. doi: 10.1186/s13195-014-0079-9

Fitting the epidemiology and neuropathology of the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease to prevent dementia

Javier Mar 1,2,, Myriam Soto-Gordoa 2,3, Arantzazu Arrospide 2,3, Fermín Moreno-Izco 4, Pablo Martínez-Lage 5
PMCID: PMC4338563  PMID: 25713598

Abstract

Introduction

Recent research on biomarkers has made possible the diagnosis of pre-dementia and even preclinical Alzheimer’s disease (AD), thus providing the ideal context for prevention. The aim of this study was to investigate the epidemiology of the early stages of AD by fitting neuropathologic and epidemiological data to assess the feasibility of prevention programs.

Methods

The study addressed primarily the construction of a discrete event simulation model of the stages of dementia. Age was included in the mathematical functions to combine the two competitive risks that determine the epidemiology of AD, that is, time to onset of dementia and time until death by other causes. Subsequently, this model was calibrated to reproduce the prevalence of pathological findings associated with AD. The beginning of the preclinical stage was taken to coincide with Thal phase 1 deposition of amyloid-beta. The duration of the prodromal stage, marked by mild cognitive impairment, was based on a 10% annual conversion rate from this level of impairment to dementia. The validation of prevalence figures also permitted estimation of the incidence and duration of preclinical and prodromal stages.

Results

In Spain, half of the nearly 10 million people aged more than 60 years are in the early stages of AD; 35.9% are in a preclinical stage, and up to 14.2% are in a prodromal stage. However, dementia will develop in only 38% of this population. The weighted mean time to dementia was 22.0 years from the start of Thal phase 1 and 9.0 years from the start of phase 2. Results of simulation models showed a lack of correlation between clinical and pathological classifications.

Conclusions

These findings raise questions about the feasibility of drug-based prevention strategies. Currently, screening programs with biomarkers in the early stages of AD cannot be applied to the half of the general population older than 60 years. Hence, intensive research is needed regarding risk factors, so that more affordable strategies may be planned. More efficient criteria are also needed to select those subjects with mild cognitive impairment who have an increased probability of positive screening for biomarkers (prodromal stage).

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s13195-014-0079-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Introduction

The definition of Alzheimer’s pathology and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) has been the subject of profound conceptualization [1]. The research diagnostic criteria proposed by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association Work Group in 1984 characterized AD as a type of dementia in which the clinical diagnosis could be determined on an exclusionary basis and confirmed only post mortem [2]. For more than 25 years, this approach was generally embraced, until advances in biomarker research reached the clinical setting and promoted a new paradigm [3]. The International Work Group [4] and the National Institute of Aging–Alzheimer’s Association task force [5,6] proposed new diagnostic criteria that cover all possible clinical manifestations of the disease and allow a premortem biological diagnosis to be made on the basis of positive biomarkers. Moreover, the concept of a preclinical stage of AD, with no cognitive or behavioral symptoms, has been defined as the finding of positive biomarkers of amyloid deposition with or without neurodegenerative changes [7]. Accordingly, AD is defined as a long, degenerative process that starts with the development of specific neuropathological changes in the brain without clinical manifestations (preclinical stage) until progression to a prodromal stage of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and finally to dementia [1]. Available empirical information about the early preclinical stage comes mainly from brain registries [8], although in vivo information is now being gathered with biomarker studies [9].

The fact that AD may now be detected in its earliest symptomatic, or even in its asymptomatic, preclinical stage has opened new appealing lines of research to investigate potential prevention strategies at the secondary or tertiary level. Prevention strategies could reduce the population burden of AD through the introduction of disease-modifying drug treatments or intervention programs for risk-factor modification [10,11]. However, the targets and time periods of both observational and interventional studies are too limited to estimate the long-term impact of prevention at the general population level, and many questions regarding feasibility remain. The reproduction of the natural history of AD with mathematical models has been used to predict its evolution through simulation and to evaluate the health impact and cost-effectiveness of intervention programs [12,13]. In addition, such models may help in calculating epidemiological parameters of prevalence, incidence and duration of disease stage [14].

The objective of this study was to estimate the epidemiology of early stages of AD by fitting the incidence and prevalence with neuropathological findings of AD in autopsies in the general population to assess the feasibility of prevention programs.

Methods

The study primarily addressed the construction of a validated model of the dementia stages on the basis of the results of two meta-analyses that estimated the incidence and prevalence of AD dementia in European populations [15,16]. Subsequently, this model was calibrated to reproduce the prevalence of pathological findings associated with AD in the population by age group, according to the study by Braak and colleagues [8]. The epidemiology of the early stages of AD was described by calculating the incidence, prevalence and duration of the preclinical and prodromal stages. As there is currently no empirical data to estimate these parameters in any population, a discrete event simulation (DES) model was used to represent the relation of AD to the Spanish population more than 40 years old and to calculate them [14]. The Technical Appendix (Additional file 1) contains the complete description of the model and its parameters. Discrete event simulation is a flexible modeling method characterized by the ability to represent complex behavior within, and interactions between, individuals, populations and their environments [17]. This study was based on a literature review and a computing model without patient involvement, so no ethical approval or consent was needed.

Conceptual model

The natural history model divides AD into three clinical stages: preclinical, prodromal and dementia [4-6]. Because the clinical classification of AD does not correlate with the presence and density of deposits of amyloid beta (Aβ) and tau proteins in the brain [18], the discrete event simulation model was populated directly with findings associated with progress of Aβ deposition [8]. Clinicopathological studies indicate that the presence of Aβ is more specific than tau deposits for the etiology of AD [18,19], so we reproduced in the model the evolution of neuropathological findings by Thal phases [8,20]. Braak and colleagues analyzed 2,332 brains for findings associated with AD and estimated the presence of Aβ deposits in terms of the Thal phases by age group [8]. From these results, we identified the beginning of the preclinical stage of AD by Thal phase 1, which is characterized by the presence of plaque-like Aβ deposits in the temporal neocortex. The next stages showed deposits also in the allocortex and associated areas of the neocortex (Thal phases 2 and 3) or in virtually all cerebrocortical regions (Thal phase 4) [8,20]. In the representation of the conceptual model of AD, the relevant input was only the time from the beginning of the Thal 1 phase. The progression of pathological findings according to Thal phases was calculated to compare the results with estimates based on clinical classification.

Beyond Aβ deposition, the duration of the preclinical stage depends on different factors that still remain to be discovered. The combination of these conditions and the competitive risk of death from other causes determines which individuals reach the clinical stage. As the National Institutes of Health State-of-the-Science Conference noted, there is currently no evidence about modifiable factors causally associated with AD [21]. However, Barnes and Yaffe identified factors consistent with evidence of AD, such as diabetes mellitus, present smoking, depression, cognitive impairment, physical activity and poor diet (high saturated fat and low vegetable intake) [11]. Following their approach in our study, we used the term risk factors instead of risk markers to account not only for the current scenario but also for potential future findings; however, we stress that this use does not indicate any demonstrated causal relationship between epidemiological condition or behavior and AD.

Given the lack of correlation of prodromal stage with Thal phases, the duration was estimated from studies measuring the conversion of MCI to dementia with rates that ranged from 5 to 20% per year [18,22]. This variability derives from the heterogeneity of samples from patients with MCI, which includes both clinical studies (high rates) and population studies (low rates), so we applied a 10% rate that resulted in a duration of 10 years by application of the exponential function [23].

Discrete event simulation model

The model was preloaded with the entire Spanish population older than 40 years of age in 2009 to measure the stage of AD in individuals in 2010. On entry into the simulation model, individuals were characterized by the relevant attributes of age, sex, duration until death and duration until dementia (Figure 1). The stochastic nature of the process meant that the individual’s behavior was randomly determined in the model, depending on which risk (either the development of dementia or death by other causes) finally occurred [14,17]. Table 1 presents the main parameters of the model.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The natural history of Alzheimer’s disease by clinical and pathological classifications. Aβ, amyloid beta.

Table 1.

Parameters of the model

Source Function Parameters Male Female
Time until death due to other causes INE Gompertz ln(α) −10.022 −11.922
β 0.090 0.108
Time until dementia [15] Gompertz ln(α) −17.825 −16.772
β 0.164 0.157
Dementia survival [21] Polynomic b0 30.835 42.767
b1 −0.447 −0.709
b2 0.002 0.003
Prodromal stage length [17,18] Exponential λ 10 10

INE, Spanish National Institute of Statistics.

We calculated the parameters for the Gompertz function that reproduces life expectancy by age and sex from mortality rates in the Spanish population in 2010, as obtained from the National Institute of Statistics (INE) [24]. It was assumed that during the prodromal and preclinical stages the mortality rate was the same as that in the general population. The same procedure, but with the incidence rates of AD dementia, allowed us to calculate the parameters for the Gompertz function that set the age of onset of dementia [15,24]. The calculated incidence [15] matched the model used by Brookmeyer and colleagues and the results of Ferri and colleagues [12,25]. Dementia incidence was not a model input, but the result of the interaction between two competing risks (death or development of dementia) in cohorts of individuals entering the preclinical stage in the model and was used as a main result of external validation. The prevalence of dementia was the result of the combination of incidence and disease duration, and this was also measured to validate the model. The figures used to parameterize dementia risk function derive from studies that may not be representative enough for ages older than 90 years. Our model could overestimate the weight of such a population, as risk grows over time and, with use of the Gompertz function, reaches high levels for older people. However, this limitation would not apply to the early stages of AD, which are the main concern of this work. Survival after a diagnosis of dementia was determined from the study by Dodge and colleagues [26].

Once we determined the age of onset of dementia, it was possible to identify the starting points of different Thal phases by calibration. For each phase, we established the time from the beginning of Aβ deposition to dementia. The results were obtained in 2010 and grouped by age and sex. Calibration is the process of determining the values of unobservable parameters by constraining model output to replicate observed data [27]. The calibration step consisted of estimating the duration of the preclinical stage by adjusting the model until it matched the observed pattern for Thal phase 1 in the study by Braak and colleagues [8]. Different values were tested by calibration [27], as Thal phase 1 prevalence is a directly unobservable parameter. Epidemiological parameters for Thal phases 2 and 3 were also calculated by applying the same method.

Results

The model was validated by comparing the calculated life expectancy, incidence and prevalence of the dementia stage in 2010 with the values of Dodge and colleagues, of Fratiglioni and colleagues and of Lobo and colleagues (see Additional file 1) [15,16,26]. The number of individuals with dementia due to AD obtained with the model was 370,300 in 2010, which appears to correlate with the 400,000 people estimated in a review of Spanish surveys by de Pedro-Cuesta and colleagues [28].

The duration to dementia from the start of the Thal phases was variable, with a weighted mean of 22.0 years for phase 1 and 9.0 years for phase 2 (Table 2). Table 3 presents the disaggregated incidence by age group in the Spanish population according to clinical and pathological classifications. The disaggregated prevalence figures by epidemiological and pathological classifications (Table 4) highlight the huge range of early stages of AD in the population. From the population in Spain of nearly 10 million people older than 60 years of age, 35.9% were shown to be in a preclinical stage and 14.2% in a prodromal stage.

Table 2.

Time from the start of each Thal phase to the onset of dementia

Time until dementia from
Age Thal1 Thal2
40 to 44 45.6 22.0
45 to 49 35.3 22.0
50 to 54 31.8 21.9
55 to 59 29.8 20.5
60 to 64 28.1 18.5
65 to 69 26.6 16.7
70 to 74 23.1 13.0
75 to 79 19.0 8.7
80 to 84 14.9 4.3
85 to 89 10.8 2.1
90 to 94 6.7 1.0
95 to 99 2.9 0.9
>100 2.1 1.0
Weighted mean 22.0 9.0

Table 3.

Incidence of new cases of Alzheimer’s disease by 1,000 person-years and by clinical and pathological classifications

Incidence/1,000 person-years
Clinical classification Pathological classification
Age group Preclinical Prodromal Dementia Thal1 Thal2 Thal3
40 to 44 24.01 0.13 0.00 23.07 1.68 1.11
45 to 49 5.61 0.31 0.00 5.61 2.58 0.68
50 to 54 7.53 0.64 0.16 7.53 4.98 1.54
55 to 59 18.51 1.45 0.46 18.51 10.00 2.44
60 to 64 27.60 3.49 0.70 27.60 16.98 3.32
65 to 69 32.76 7.65 1.87 32.76 18.13 7.08
70 to 74 10.43 16.24 4.22 10.43 28.14 12.14
75 to 79 10.34 32.65 7.50 10.34 28.72 14.21
80 to 84 10.10 57.29 15.27 10.10 10.51 20.27
85 to 89 13.80 16.98 35.33 13.80 11.22 26.84
90 to 94 12.09 22.32 61.83 12.09 12.09 36.26
>95 33.85 23.44 109.38 33.85 49.48 104.17

Table 4.

Prevalence by age group and type of classification (clinical and pathological) in the Spanish population

Clinical classification a Pathological classification b
Age group Population Preclinical Prodromal Dementia Thal1 Thal2 Thal3
Number
40 to 44 2,981,900 252,700 800 0 253,500 16,900 9,400
45 to 49 3,530,900 388,400 4,900 0 393,300 47,200 19,800
50 to 54 3,109,400 431,800 9,400 1,100 442,300 93,600 31,600
55 to 59 2,620,100 517,600 19,400 3,200 540,200 172,400 49,900
60 to 64 2,438,200 739,200 43,500 9,200 791,900 336,900 82,400
65 to 69 2,091,000 929,800 79,800 17,800 1,027,400 461,000 120,900
70 to 74 1,705,800 817,900 148,900 31,300 998,100 583,200 178,600
75 to 79 1,653,700 699,100 291,800 58,200 1,049,100 814,400 266,400
80 to 84 1,218,300 329,500 418,600 90,500 838,600 709,500 277,300
85 to 89 659,500 67,200 330,800 90,400 488,400 416,600 195,600
90 to 94 215,100 14,100 97,300 52,200 163,600 136,000 74,700
>95 38,400 2,700 9,100 16,400 28,200 22,800 14,100
Total >40 22,262,300 5,190,000 1,454,300 370,300 7,014,600 3,810,500 1,320,700
Total >60 10,020,000 3,599,500 1,419,800 366,000 5,385,300 3,480,400 1,210,000
Percentage
40 to 44 100.0 8.5 0.0 0.0 8.5 0.6 0.3
45 to 49 100.0 11.0 0.1 0.0 11.1 1.3 0.6
50 to 54 100.0 13.9 0.3 0.0 14.2 3.0 1.0
55 to 59 100.0 19.8 0.7 0.1 20.6 6.6 1.9
60 to 64 100.0 30.3 1.8 0.4 32.5 13.8 3.4
65 to 69 100.0 44.5 3.8 0.9 49.1 22.0 5.8
70 to 74 100.0 47.9 8.7 1.8 58.5 34.2 10.5
75 to 79 100.0 42.3 17.6 3.5 63.4 49.2 16.1
80 to 84 100.0 27.0 34.4 7.4 68.8 58.2 22.8
85 to 89 100.0 10.2 50.2 13.7 74.1 63.2 29.7
90 to 94 100.0 6.6 45.2 24.3 76.1 63.2 34.7
>95 100.0 7.0 23.7 42.7 73.4 59.4 36.7
Total >40 100.0 23.3 6.5 1.7 31.5 17.1 5.9
Total >60 100.0 35.9 14.2 3.7 53.7 34.7 12.1

aData for clinical stages are not aggregated. bData for pathological classification prevalence are aggregated.

Figure 2 shows the percentage of individuals in each age group according to clinical and pathological classifications. The same data, but incorporating the figures of Braak et al. for Thal phases, appear in the Technical Annex to display the goodness of fit for results (see Additional file 1).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Disaggregated prevalence by age group and by clinical and pathological classification.

Discussion

One of the main findings of this research is that one-half of the population older than 60 years of age in a developed country has some degree of Aβ deposition in the brain. This result agrees with those from population-based autopsy studies, such as the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Aging Study that showed 47% of nondemented subjects older than 70 years had mild, moderate or severe densities of neuritic plaques [29]. The enormous size of such a group of affected individuals is highlighted by the fact that it would encompass 15.2% of the total Spanish population (46,148,700 persons). Since the lifetime risk of AD at age 60 years is 19%, an intervention program targeting the 50% of subjects with Aβ deposits would include treating 68% of that population who would never reach the dementia stage [30]. With use of both neuropathological and clinicoepidemiological data, discrete event simulation modeling has provided an estimate of the time lapse between the earliest preclinical changes and the onset of dementia. This interval is usually considered to range from 10 to 20 years [5], but according to the model may be more than 30 years. The findings of both high prevalence and prolonged asymptomatic periods have important public health implications in the study of dementia, particularly in the prevention of AD. Salomon and colleagues have discussed the relevance for prevention plans based on two different concepts of AD onset [30]; that is, whether the starting point relies on pathological changes only [5-7] or on specific clinical manifestations [4]. Consistent with the definition of AD onset, the same preventive intervention can be labeled as primary when dealing with clinical symptoms only or as secondary if prior pathological changes are considered. In relation to our use of Aβ deposition in the model as the marker of the preclinical stage, we understand that primary prevention interventions should be applied before the onset of pathological changes by promoting initiation and maintenance of good health and eliminating or modifying potential risk conditions, if these are ever ascertained [30,31]. We believe secondary prevention would target subjects at the preclinical phase to avoid progression to overt clinical AD and this objective could be hypothetically achieved by risk-factor intervention or disease-modifying therapies. In turn, interventions in the prodromal stage would qualify as tertiary prevention to target the delay or avoidance of dementia onset in the presence of symptoms. In keeping with these definitions and our results, the size of the population targets would include the whole nondemented population for primary prevention, 23.3% of that population older than 40 years for secondary prevention, and 14.2% older than 60 years for tertiary programs. The size of these target populations constrains the viability of an ideal public health perspective to reduce AD prevalence based on a scalable approach, with interventions first targeting risk factors and then pharmacologic treatment in a highly selected group of individuals [9].

Much information has been gathered regarding the role of vascular risk factors and certain lifestyle conditions, such as low education, diet or sedentary behavior, and their association with dementia in general and AD in particular [32]. However, at present, no causal relationship has been demonstrated, and there is no evidence that the reduction of risk factors directly results in decreased incidence of either biomarker positivity or clinical AD [11]. Prevention strategies with anti-amyloid therapies have been proposed for asymptomatic individuals with the genetic and sporadic forms of AD [33]. However, our findings suggest that pharmacologic interventions in subjects with preclinical sporadic AD are hardly feasible. First, screening programs to detect amyloid based on positron emission tomography are so far unaffordable, and those based on lumbar puncture are invasive and need further standardization. Second, even if a more accessible biomarker becomes available and a therapy shows sufficient efficacy and safety, its application would target the 50% of subjects older than 60 years who have amyloid deposits, 62% of whom would never reach the dementia stage [34]. Overdiagnosis is a concept borrowed from cancer-screening evaluation that can also be applied to AD research; in this situation, overdiagnosis describes those individuals found to have AD by the presence of positive biomarkers but who will never have symptoms [35]. The risk of falling into the big prostate mistake has already been highlighted [36].

Tertiary programs might hypothetically treat symptomatic subjects with MCI due to AD to delay or prevent progression to the dementia stage. According to our data, it is still difficult to take a different approach for people with MCI. Subjects with MCI are at a significantly increased risk for the development of dementia associated with AD [37], and therefore represent a target population for preventive strategies. In our model, if the intervention in 2010 had been limited to subjects older than 60 years of age in the prodromal stage (MCI with positive AD biomarkers), the size of the target population would have been 14.2% (1,419,000 individuals). However, to make the diagnosis in this population, subjects with general MCI or MCI of any type should be screened with the appropriate AD markers, but they are not yet widely available. The size of the population with general MCI limits the generalized use of biomarkers or therapies and shows the need for more specificity in identification of the target population. Identifying those subjects with MCI who have a high probability of biomarker positivity would help to select those at high risk of rapid conversion to dementia. Improved predictive values at early diagnosis would result in fewer treated individuals who otherwise would never have had dementia. It is true that this approach can be only speculative before a disease-modifying treatment is available, but subjects with MCI and a positive biomarker could well be targeted in clinical trials with the already approved, cheaper cholinesterase inhibitors in an attempt to delay onset of dementia [38]. The definition of MCI from clinical studies has determined the super-selection of patient groups at high risk of dementia [39], but direct application of such data to population-based programs may not be completely appropriate. On one hand, the clinical course of MCI is not always progressive. On the other, not all subjects in whom dementia develops have previously fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for MCI [39].

One of the strengths of our work is the combination of the two competitive risks that determine the epidemiology of AD: time to onset of dementia and time until death by other causes. The inclusion of age in the mathematical functions and the thorough validation process (see Additional file 1) shows that our AD model has not ignored the ‘elephant in the room’ analogy described by Brayne in her seminal paper [40]. Our approach to AD is a view extending over the patients’ lifetime and incorporating the long preclinical stage that can only be characterized by progressive neuropathological changes. We also acknowledge some limitations in our approach. First, modeling findings should always be interpreted with caution, because they are based on a mathematical representation of the disease and not directly on empirical data. However, our representation of the natural history of AD is robust because it relies on an integrative framework that includes clinical, neuropathological, demographic and epidemiological elements. Second, we did not address the current multifactorial expression of the course of AD in late life in terms of cognitive impairment and dementia [31]. Defining mathematical functions to explore the role of vascular factors or other comorbidities in the development of the early stages of AD should be the object of further analysis. As an initial approach, we have reproduced a pure AD model based on Aβ deposition. However, we acknowledge that understanding the role of vascular and lifestyle-related factors is a key issue in the research agenda for dementia prevention and including them will be an advance in the mathematical representation of the natural history of AD. Third, estimating the prevalence of the preclinical and clinical stages from a model grounded in neuropathological data confronts the problem of the well-established lack of correlation between amyloid neuropathological findings and clinical manifestations [18,19]. However, our model is intended to provide epidemiological indicators to characterize the time from the onset of AD-related changes in the brain until the onset of clinical milestones, not to support or question the pathogenic or bystander role of amyloid. Simulation models can integrate existing partial knowledge of preclinical and prodromal phases of AD in joint representation of its natural history and assessment of the impact of possible interventions in terms of health benefit and economic burden. As has been noted in other studies with different approaches, results of simulation models show the lack of correlation between clinical and pathological classifications [18,19]. Aβ (Thal phase 1) is present in a significant percentage of the population 65 to 70 years of age; it seems to increase thereafter and plateau in the population aged 85 years or older. These differences support the hypothesis that Aβ accumulation is necessary, but insufficient alone, to cause dementia; other factors including vascular conditions, inflammation, brain reserve and cognitive reserve are also involved.

Conclusions

These findings raise questions about the feasibility of drug-based prevention strategies. Currently, screening programs with biomarkers in the early stages of AD cannot be applied to the one-half of the general population older than 60 years of age. Intensive research regarding risk factors is therefore needed, so that more affordable strategies may be planned. More efficient criteria are also needed to select those subjects with MCI who have an increased probability of positive screening for biomarkers (prodromal stage).

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the editorial assistance provided by Ideas Need Communicating Language Services and Sally Ebeling.

All authors were funded by their institutions. This work was also supported by grant 2010111096 from the Department of Health, Basque Government, grant DFG11/003 from Diputación de Gipuzkoa and grant PI11/02599 from the Carlos III Health Institute, Madrid.

Abbreviations

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

amyloid beta

MCI

mild cognitive impairment

Additional file

Additional file 1: (434.1KB, pdf)

Presents the Technical Appendix for the AD model.

Footnotes

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JM conceived the study, and planned, designed and wrote the initial draft with the assistance of MS-G. AA and MS-G wrote the methodological and results sections, and carried out the model construction and validation to obtain the results. PM-L and FM-I participated in the design, reviewed all the clinical and epidemiological data, and drafted the introduction and conclusions. All authors made substantial contributions to the conception and design, or acquisition of data, or analysis and interpretation of data; have been involved in drafting the manuscript or revising it critically for important intellectual content; have given final approval of the version to be published; and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Contributor Information

Javier Mar, Email: javier.marmedina@osakidetza.net.

Myriam Soto-Gordoa, Email: myriam.sotoruizdegordoa@osakidetza.net.

Arantzazu Arrospide, Email: arantzazu.arrospideelgarresta@osakidetza.net.

Fermín Moreno-Izco, Email: fermin.morenoizco@osakidetza.net.

Pablo Martínez-Lage, Email: pmlage@cita-alzheimer.org.

References

  • 1.Cummings J. Alzheimer’s disease diagnostic criteria: practical applications. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2012;4:35. doi: 10.1186/alzrt138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–44. doi: 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Aisen PS, Weiner MW, et al. Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:119–28. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70299-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Cummings JL, Dekosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, et al. Revising the definition of Alzheimer’s disease: a new lexicon. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:1118–27. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70223-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft S, Fagan AM, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:280–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Jack CR, Jr, Albert MS, Knopman DS, McKhann GM, Sperling RA, Carrillo MC, et al. Introduction to the recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:257–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Weigand SD, Wiste HJ, Vemuri P, Lowe V, et al. An operational approach to National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association criteria for preclinical Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. 2012;71:765–75. doi: 10.1002/ana.22628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E, Del Tredici K. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2011;70:960–9. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vos SJ, Xiong C, Visser PJ, Jasielec MS, Hassenstab J, Grant EA, et al. Preclinical Alzheimer’s disease and its outcome: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:957–65. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70194-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bateman RJ, Klunk WE. Measuring target effect of proposed disease modifying therapies in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurotherapeutics. 2008;5:381–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10:819–28. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Brookmeyer R, Gray S, Kawas C. Projections of Alzheimer’s disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset. Am J Public Health. 1998;88:1337–42. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.88.9.1337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Budd D, Burns LC, Guo Z, L’Italien G, Lapuerta P. Impact of early intervention and disease modification in patients with predementia Alzheimer’s disease: a Markov model simulation. Clinicoecon Outcomes Res. 2011;3:189–95. doi: 10.2147/CEOR.S22265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mar J, Arrospide A, Comas M. Budget impact analysis of thrombolysis for stroke in Spain: a discrete event simulation model. Value Health. 2010;13:69–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1524-4733.2009.00655.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fratiglioni L, Launer LJ, Andersen K, Breteler MM, Copeland JR, Dartigues JF, et al. Incidence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: a collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurology. 2000;54:S10–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lobo A, Launer LJ, Fratiglioni L, Andersen K, Di Carlo A, Breteler MM, et al. Prevalence of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: a collaborative study of population-based cohorts: Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research group. Neurology. 2000;54:S4–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Karnon J, Stahl J, Brennan A, Caro JJ, Mar J, Möller J, et al. Modeling using discrete event simulation: a report of the ISPOR-SMDM Modeling Good Research Practices Task Force–4. Value Health. 2012;15:821–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jval.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jicha GA, Carr SA. Conceptual evolution in Alzheimer’s disease: implications for understanding the clinical phenotype of progressive neurodegenerative disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;19:253–72. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-1237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nelson PT, Alafuzoff I, Bigio EH, Bouras C, Braak H, Cairns NJ, et al. Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: a review of the literature. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2012;71:362–81. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e31825018f7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Thal DR, Rüb U, Orantes M, Braak H. Phases of A beta-deposition in the human brain and its relevance for the development of AD. Neurology. 2002;58:1791–800. doi: 10.1212/WNL.58.12.1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Daviglus ML, Bell CC, Berrettini W, Bowen PE, Connolly ES, Jr, Cox NJ, et al. National Institutes of Health State-of-the-Science Conference statement: preventing Alzheimer disease and cognitive decline. Ann Internal Med. 2010;153:176–81. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-153-3-201008030-00260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Geda YE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol. 2009;66:1447–55. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ward A, Tardiff S, Dye C, Arrighi HM. Rate of Conversion from Prodromal Alzheimer’s Disease to Alzheimer’s Dementia: a Systematic Review of the Literature. Dement Ger Cogn Disord Extra. 2013;3:320–32. doi: 10.1159/000354370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Roman R, Comas M, Hoffmeister L, Castells X. Determining the lifetime density function using a continuous approach. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61:923–5. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.052639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, Ganguli M, et al. Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet. 2005;366:2112–7. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67889-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dodge HH, Shen C, Pandav R, DeKosky ST, Ganguli M. Functional transitions and active life expectancy associated with Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:253–9. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stout NK, Knudsen AB, Kong CY, McMahon PM, Gazelle GS. Calibration methods used in cancer simulation models and suggested reporting guidelines. Pharmacoeconomics. 2009;27:533–45. doi: 10.2165/11314830-000000000-00000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.de Pedro-Cuesta J, Virués-Ortega J, Vega S, Seijo-Martínez M, Saz P, Rodríguez F, et al. Prevalence of dementia and major dementia subtypes in Spanish populations: a reanalysis of dementia prevalence surveys, 1990–2008. BMC Neurol. 2009;9:55. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-9-55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Neuropathology Group of the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Aging Study Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia in a multicenter, community based population in England and Wales. Lancet. 2001;357:169–75. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03589-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Solomon A, Mangialasche F, Richard E, Andrieu S, Bennett DA, Breteler M, et al. Advances in the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia. J Intern Med. 2014;275:229–50. doi: 10.1111/joim.12178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kivipelto M, Solomon A. Preventive neurology: on the way from knowledge to action. Neurology. 2009;73:168–9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ad1df4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Qiu C, Xu W, Fratiglioni L. Vascular and psychosocial factors in Alzheimer’s disease: epidemiological evidence toward intervention. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20:689–97. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mills SM, Mallmann J, Santacruz AM, Fuqua A, Carril M, Aisen PS, et al. Preclinical trials in autosomal dominant AD: implementation of the DIAN-TU trial. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2013;169:737–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2013.07.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Seshadri S, Wolf PA. Lifetime risk of stroke and dementia: current concepts, and estimates from the Framingham Study. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:1106–14. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70291-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Puliti D, Duffy SW, Miccinesi G, EUROSCREEN Working Group. de Koning H, Lynge E, et al. Overdiagnosis in mammographic screening for breast cancer in Europe: a literature review. J Med Screen. 2012;19:42–56. doi: 10.1258/jms.2012.012082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Brayne C, Davis D. Making Alzheimer’s and dementia research fit for populations. Lancet. 2012;380:1441–3. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61803-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mattsson N, Zetterberg H, Hansson O, Andreasen N, Parnetti L, Jonsson M, et al. CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. JAMA. 2009;302:385–93. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Petersen RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, Bennett D, Doody R, Ferris S, et al. Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. 2005;352:2379–88. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa050151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Matthews FE, Stephan BC, McKeith IG, Bond J, Brayne C, Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study Two-year progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia: to what extent do different definitions agree? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1424–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01820.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Brayne C. The elephant in the room - healthy brains in later life, epidemiology and public health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:233–9. doi: 10.1038/nrn2091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Alzheimer's Research & Therapy are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES