Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2015 Jan 9;162(0):189–195. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.005

Perceived Emotional Intelligence is Impaired and Associated with Poor Community Functioning in Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder

Naomi T Tabak a,b, Michael F Green c,b, Jonathan K Wynn b,c, Greg H Proudfit d, Lori Altshuler c, William P Horan b,c
PMCID: PMC4339495  NIHMSID: NIHMS649344  PMID: 25579055

Abstract

Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder have been associated with shared and distinct emotion processing abnormalities. Initial findings indicate that these disorders differ with respect to the domain of emotional intelligence (EI). Individuals with schizophrenia display deficits on performance measures of EI, whereas those with bipolar disorder do not. However, no research has examined patients’ subjective beliefs about their own EI (referred to as “perceived EI”). This study examined perceived EI, assessed with the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS), and its clinical and functional correlates in outpatients with schizophrenia (n = 35) or bipolar disorder I (n = 38) and matched healthy controls (n = 35). The TMMS includes three subscales that assess beliefs about one’s ability to attend to (Attention to Feelings), understand (Clarity of Feelings), and repair emotions (Mood Repair). Participants in the clinical groups also completed community functioning and symptom assessments. Both clinical groups reported significantly lower perceived EI than controls, but did not differ from each other. Higher total TMMS correlated with higher levels of independent living in the schizophrenia group (r = .36) and better social functioning in the bipolar group (r = .61). In addition, although higher Attention to Feelings scores correlated with greater psychiatric symptoms in the schizophrenia group, higher scores across all subscales correlated with less manic symptoms in the bipolar group. The findings suggest that perceived EI is impaired and related to community functioning in both disorders.

Keywords: schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, emotion, emotional intelligence

1. Introduction

Research from an affective science perspective has begun to specify differences and similarities in the emotion processing abnormalities associated with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. On one hand, these disorders are associated with different patterns of in-the-moment responses to emotionally evocative stimuli. While schizophrenia patients demonstrate normal emotional responses to evocative stimuli (Kring and Elis, 2013), bipolar patients demonstrate prolonged positive emotion during emotion-eliciting laboratory tasks (Gruber, 2011). However, both clinical groups show deficits in emotion regulation, including difficulty modulating neural responses to unpleasant stimuli through cognitive reappraisal in schizophrenia (Horan et al., 2013) and, in bipolar disorder, using more emotion regulation strategies, but with less success, than healthy people (Gruber et al., 2012). Examining other aspects of emotion processing can further illuminate differences and similarities across these disorders. For example, an aspect of emotion processing that has received research attention in major mental illness is emotional intelligence (EI). Mayer and Salovey (2008) define EI as the capacity to process one’s own and others’ emotions (i.e. perceive, access, generate, and reflectively regulate emotions) to guide thinking about behavior. Accordingly, EI is viewed as a unique set of abilities that plays a critical role in adaptive socio-emotional functioning.

To date, nearly all research on EI in mental illness has focused on performance-based measures, particularly the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT; Mayer et al., 2002). The MSCEIT assesses four “branches” of EI: emotion perception, using emotions, understanding, and managing emotions. Patients with schizophrenia demonstrate impairments on this measure across all four domains, and lower MSCEIT scores are related to greater positive and negative symptoms, low functional capacity, and poor functional outcome (Eack et al., 2010; Kee et al., 2009; Lin et al., 2012). We are aware of only two studies that used the MSCEIT in bipolar disorder and both found that performance was normal (Burdick et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2013).

In addition to performance-based measures, affective scientists have assessed self-reported or “perceived EI.” While the MSCEIT measures one’s ability to identify and understand emotions in oneself and others, perceived EI measures the meta-experience of emotion, or one’s subjective beliefs about his or her emotional abilities. The most commonly-used measure of perceived EI is the Trait-Meta Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey et al., 1995). It contains three subscales: (1) Attention to Feelings (Attention): the tendency to notice and value emotions; (2) Clarity of Feelings (Clarity): the tendency to experience and name feelings clearly; and (3) Mood Repair (Repair): one’s belief in his or her ability to repair negative emotions by fostering positive feelings. In healthy samples, TMMS scores are not strongly related to performance-based measures, such as the MSCEIT (Mayer et al., 2004), yet both are associated with mental and physical health and better social functioning (Schutte et al., 2007). Studies have demonstrated incremental validity for TMMS for outcomes; in healthy individuals, the TMMS predicts subjective well-being and adaptive functioning above and beyond factors such as current mood state, personality traits, and general intelligence (Extremera and Fernández-Berrocal, 2005; Fernandez Berrocal and Extremera, 2008; Palmer et al., 2002).

Higher scores on Clarity and Repair are consistently associated with positive outcomes, such as greater life satisfaction, interpersonal satisfaction, and less social anxiety and depression (Palmer et al., 2002; Salguero et al., 2012; Salovey et al., 2002). Higher scores on the third subscale, Attention, are sometimes associated with positive outcomes, such as empathy, self-esteem, and adaptive physiological responses to acute stress (Salovey et al., 2002), but have also been associated with greater anxiety and depression (Salguero et al., 2012; Salovey et al., 1995).

We are unaware of any studies of perceived EI in schizophrenia or bipolar disorder using the TMMS. Research has been conducted on concepts related to perceived EI, such as mindfulness and alexithymia (related to the Attention and Clarity subscales) and emotion regulation (related to Repair). Schizophrenia patients display low levels of dispositional mindfulness (Chadwick et al., 2008), while bipolar patients and healthy controls do not differ on mindfulness (Perich et al., 2011). Also, schizophrenia patients report increased alexithymia (van’t Wout et al., 2007), which has been associated with increased positive (Serper and Berenbaum, 2008) and negative symptoms (van’t Wout et al., 2007). Individuals with schizophrenia additionally report abnormal emotion regulation styles and show impairment on performance-based and neurophysiological tasks (Horan et al., 2013; Kee et al., 2009; Kimhy et al., 2012). Although individuals with bipolar disorder also report abnormal emotion regulation styles (Gruber et al., 2012; Wolkenstein et al., 2014), they do not consistently show impairments on performance-based assessments (Burdick et al., 2011; Gruber et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2013).

There were three objectives for the current study. The first was to compare levels of perceived EI across schizophrenia, bipolar, and control participants. Based on prior research, we expected schizophrenia patients to report lower scores on all TMMS subscales, but we did not have clear directional predictions for the bipolar group. The second objective was to examine the correlations among perceived EI, characteristic symptoms for the two disorders (positive and negative symptoms, mania, and depression), and community functioning within each clinical group. The third objective was to determine whether perceived EI accounted for unique variance in functional outcome for each clinical group, above and beyond any contribution of symptoms.

2. Methods

2.1 Participants

Participants were 73 outpatients with schizophrenia (n=35) or bipolar disorder (n=38) and 35 healthy control subjects. Patients were recruited from outpatient clinics at University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), the Veterans Affairs Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System (VAGLAHS), and from local clinics and board and care facilities. Patients met criteria for schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Axis I Disorders (First et al., 1996). Most of the bipolar patients (n=28) had a history of psychotic symptoms and the majority of bipolar patients were euthymic at the time of the study (n=29). Following consensus nomenclature, participants were considered euthymic if they had a score of <7 on the Young Mania Rating Scale (Tohen et al., 2009). Patients were excluded if they had substance dependence in the past six months, substance abuse in the past month, or IQ < 70. All patients were clinically stable as defined by: no mood episodes in the past month, no hospitalizations in the past 3 months, no changes in living situation in the past 2 months, and no medication changes in the past 6 weeks. Thirty-three of the schizophrenia patients and twenty-three of the bipolar patients were taking antipsychotic medications. Twelve bipolar patients were taking lithium.

Control participants were recruited through advertisements posted on websites. Controls were excluded if they had a history of schizophrenia, other psychotic disorder, bipolar disorder, recurrent major depressive disorder, substance dependence disorder, or substance abuse in the past month based on the SCID. Controls were also administered portions of the SCID for Axis II Disorders (First et al., 1994) and excluded if they met criteria for avoidant, paranoid, schizoid, schizotypal, or borderline personality disorder. They were also excluded for family history of psychotic or bipolar disorders among first-degree relatives. Additional exclusion criteria for all participants were: history of loss of consciousness for more than one hour, significant neurological disorder, or insufficient fluency in English.

All interviewers were trained through the Treatment Unit of the VA VISN 22 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center. Interviewers were trained to a minimum kappa of 0.75 for key psychotic and mood items on the SCID and to a minimum kappa of 0.75–0.80 for other symptom measures (Ventura et al., 1998). All participants had the capacity to give informed consent and provided written informed consent after procedures were fully explained, in line with procedures approved by the institutional review board at VAGLAHS.

2.2 Measures

2.2.1 Perceived emotional intelligence

All participants completed the TMMS, a 30-item self-report measure comprised of three subscales. The Attention subscale includes 13 items that measure the amount of attention one pays to subjective feelings (e.g. “I often think about my feelings”). The Clarity subscale includes 11 items that measure one’s ability to understand and differentiate between feelings (e.g. “I am rarely confused about what my feelings are”). The Repair subscale includes six items that measure one’s ability to regulate mood by repairing negative feelings (e.g. “I try to think good thoughts no matter how badly I feel”). Each item is rated on a scale from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree). The TMMS has demonstrated good internal reliability and convergent and discriminant validity in healthy samples (Salovey et al., 1995). Although few studies have utilized the TMMS in clinical populations, the scale has demonstrated good reliability in samples of individuals at clinical and genetic high risk for schizophrenia (Lee et al., 2008). In the current study, the Total score and three subscale scores demonstrated acceptable to good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) in all three groups (Supplementary Table 1). Scores for each scale range from 30–150 (Total), 13–65 (Attention), 11–55 (Clarity), and 6–30 (Repair).

2.2.2 Symptom ratings

All three groups completed the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS; Kring et al., 2013), which is comprised of two subscales. The Motivation and Pleasure (MAP) subscale includes nine items based on motivation, interest, and reported engagement in relevant social, vocational, and recreational activities over the past week. The Expression (EXP) subscale includes four items based on interviewer ratings of affective and verbal expression. Raters completed didactic training and co-rated sessions with one of the scale developers (WPH) and achieved acceptable reliability (ICC < .80) using tapes with gold-standard ratings.

The two patient groups also received the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) for positive symptoms and general psychiatric symptoms (Total score) (Kopelowicz et al., 2008; Ventura et al., 1993). For the patient groups, mania and depression were assessed using the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS; Young et al., 1978) and the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D; Hamilton, 1960). The BPRS, YMRS, and HAM-D were administered by raters who were trained to inter-rater reliability criteria and who participated in a continuous quality assurance program (Ventura et al., 1993).

2.2.3 Community functioning ratings

To assess current functioning, we administered the Role Functioning Scale (RFS) (McPheeters, 1984), which includes separate ratings for different domains of functioning (rated on a scale from 1–7). As in prior studies (Rassovsky et al., 2011; Sergi et al., 2006), we included ratings for Working Productivity, Independent Living, and Social Network Relationships.

2.3 Statistical analysis

For demographics, symptoms, and functioning, group differences on continuous variables were evaluated with independent samples t-tests for measures administered to only the two patient groups, and one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for measures administered to all three groups. Group differences in sex, race, and ethnicity were evaluated with chi-square tests.

For the TMMS, group differences were evaluated with ANOVAs and followed up with post-hoc analyses with Bonferroni corrections. Exploratory analyses were then conducted to examine associations between the TMMS and key symptoms and functional variables. We consider these analyses exploratory due to the lack of directional hypotheses for this aspect of the analyses. Relationships between the TMMS, symptoms, and functioning were evaluated separately within the schizophrenia and bipolar groups using Pearson correlation coefficients. Finally, The TMMS subscales and symptoms that correlated significantly with functioning were entered into multiple regression analyses. These analyses examined whether subscales accounted for significant incremental variance (R2) in functioning after accounting for clinical symptoms.

3. Results

3.1 Demographics, symptoms, and functioning

The three groups did not differ on sex, age, race, ethnicity, or parental education (Table 1). There were group differences on personal education; schizophrenia patients had lower education levels compared to bipolar and control participants.

Table 1.

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Schizophrenia
(N = 35)
Bipolar
(N = 38)
Control
(N = 35)
Statistic
Sex (% male) 60 57.9 68.6 χ2(2, 108)= 0.97
Age (SD) 47.06 (9.79) 43.47 (11.38) 47.17 (6.53) F(2, 105)= 1.81
Race (%) χ2(6, 108)= 4.80
  White 60.0 68.4 77.1
  African American 17.1 15.8 14.3
  Asian 14.3 5.3 2.9
  Other 8.6 10.5 5.7
Ethnicity (% Hispanic) 20.0 18.4 14.3 χ2(4, 108)= 2.31
Education (SD) 12.97 (1.81)a 14.47 (2.05)b 14.83 (1.86)b F(2, 105)= 9.37**
Parental Education (SD) 13.57 (2.85) 15.25 (3.11) 14.32 (2.42) F(2, 97)= 2.97
Age of onset (SD) 21.34 (6.77) 20.87 (6.38) t(71)= −0.31
BPRS Positive 1.85 (0.73) 1.26 (0.26) t(70)= −4.53**
BPRS Total 39.31 (10.49) 32.70 (6.51) t(70)= −3.23**
YMRS Total 4.06 (4.32) 3.87 (4.84) t(71)= −0.18
HAM-D Total 8.83 (6.87) 6.16 (5.83) t(71)= −1.80
CAINS-MAP 14.29 (5.19)a 10.47 (6.78)b 4.91 (4.96)c F(2,101)=23.13**
CAINS-EXP 5.38 (3.65)a 2.11 (2.75)b 1.00 (1.72)b F(2,101)=22.24**
RFS Work 2.74 (1.60) 4.50 (1.77) t(69)= 4.40**
RFS Living 4.63 (1.29) 6.03 (1.16) t(69)= 4.82**
RFS Social 4.43 (1.67) 5.75 (1.70) t(69)= 3.31**

Notes: BPRS = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale; HAM-D = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; CAINS = Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MAP = Experiential negative symptoms; EXP = Expressive negative symptoms; RFS = Role Functioning Scale;

*

p < 0 .05,

**

p < 0.01;

for comparisons among all three groups, values with different superscripts are statistically different from each other.

On the CAINS, schizophrenia patients demonstrated higher MAP and EXP negative symptoms than the other two groups. Bipolar patients also demonstrated higher MAP symptoms compared to controls, though these groups did not differ on EXP symptoms. In addition, schizophrenia patients had higher BPRS total and positive symptoms compared to bipolar patients. The groups did not differ on ratings of mania or depression. For role functioning, schizophrenia patients demonstrated poorer functioning on all three components of the RFS compared to the bipolar group.

In addition, due to the heterogeneity of the bipolar participants, we conducted further between-group comparisons within this sample. We compared TMMS scores in the following three subgroups: 1) with (n=27) vs. without (n=10) history of psychosis, 2) euthymic (n=29) vs. not euthymic (n=8), and 3) on antipsychotic (n=23) vs. not on antipsychotic medication (n=14). There were no differences on perceived EI for bipolar patients based on history of psychosis or medication status. However, euthymic patients reported significantly higher scores on TMMS Total and Attention than patients who were not characterized as euthymic.

3.2 Group differences on perceived EI

Healthy controls reported higher scores on TMMS Total, Clarity, and Repair than both clinical groups (Table 2). On the Attention scale, control participants reported higher scores than schizophrenia patients only. The two patient groups did not differ significantly on any TMMS subscale. All of the significant between-group effect sizes were large (Table 2).

Table 2.

Group Differences on Perceived EI

TMMS Schizophrenia
(N = 35)
Bipolar
(N = 38)
Control
(N = 35)
F Post-hoc t-tests Effect sizes (Cohen’s d)

SZ v.
HC
SZ v.
BP
BP v.
HC
SZ v.
HC
SZ v.
BP
BP v.
HC
Total
(SD)
103.09
(14.58)
110.05
(15.66)
123.57
(11.70)
19.09** 6.48** 1.95 4.13** 1.55 0.46 0.98
Attention
(SD)
46.20
(6.85)
49.51
(8.01)
51.66
(6.49)
5.16** 3.42** 1.88 1.24 0.82 0.44 0.30
Clarity
(SD)
35.09
(7.05)
38.00
(7.31)
46.34
(5.86)
26.07** 7.27** 1.72 5.33** 1.74 0.41 1.26
Repair
(SD)
21.80
(3.81)
22.54
(4.30)
25.57
(3.39)
9.42** 4.38** 0.77 3.31** 1.05 0.18 0.78

Notes: TMMS = Trait Meta-Mood Scale;

**

p < .01;

SZ v. HC: a comparison of schizophrenia patients and healthy control participants, SZ v. BP: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar patients, BP v. HC: a comparison of bipolar patients and healthy control participants.

3.3 Correlations among perceived EI, clinical symptoms, and functioning

For schizophrenia patients (Table 3), higher Attention significantly correlated with higher psychiatric symptoms (BPRS Total). Higher Total and Clarity scores were related to greater RFS Independent Living. For bipolar patients (Table 4), higher TMMS Total and Clarity were associated with lower psychiatric symptoms (BPRS Total), and higher scores on all TMMS scales were related to lower symptoms of mania (YMRS). In terms of functioning, higher scores on all TMMS scales were associated with better RFS Social, and higher Repair was associated with greater RFS Independent Living.

Table 3.

Correlations among Perceived EI, Clinical Symptoms, and Functioning: Schizophrenia Patients

TMMS
Total
TMMS
Attention
TMMS
Clarity
TMMS
Repair
RFS
Work
RFS
Living
RFS
Social
BPRS Positive 0.11 0.23 −0.17 0.31 −0.26 −0.23 −0.29
BPRS Total 0.17 0.36* −0.15 0.29 −0.29 −0.14 −0.06
YMRS Total 0.06 0.25 −0.19 0.13 −0.28 −0.04 −0.26
HAM-D Total 0.15 0.24 −0.06 0.24 −0.30 −0.09 −0.26
CAINS-MAP 0.02 0.27 −0.17 −0.14 −0.24 −0.27 −0.50**
CAINS-EXP −0.33 −0.31 −0.25 −0.25 −0.18 −0.20 −0.10
RFS Work −0.05 −0.06 −0.01 −0.06
RFS Living 0.36* 0.18 0.41* 0.30
RFS Social 0.01 −0.13 0.19 −0.10

Notes: TMMS = Trait Meta-Mood Scale; RFS = Role Functioning Scale; BPRS = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale; HAM-D = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; CAINS = Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MAP = Experiential negative symptoms; EXP = Expressive negative symptoms;

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01

Table 4.

Correlations among Perceived EI, Clinical Symptoms, and Functioning: Bipolar Patients

TMMS
Total
TMMS
Attention
TMMS
Clarity
TMMS
Repair
RFS
Work
RFS
Living
RFS
Social
BPRS Positive −0.12 −0.10 −0.18 0.04 0.29 0.15 −0.11
BPRS Total −0.32a −0.17 −0.37* −0.24 0.00 −0.06 −0.62**
YMRS Total −0.47** −0.39* −0.36* −0.37* 0.16 0.03 −0.32
HAM-D Total −0.14 0.05 −0.25 −0.17 −0.17 −0.04 −0.32
CAINS-MAP −0.27 −0.25 −0.16 −0.23 −0.54** −0.27 −0.55**
CAINS- EXP −0.08 −0.04 −0.15 0.06 −0.24 −0.01 −0.12
RFS Work 0.15 0.18 0.09 0.06
RFS Living 0.26 0.29 0.02 0.38*
RFS Social 0.61** 0.54** 0.45** 0.47**

Notes: TMMS = Trait Meta-Mood Scale; RFS = Role Functioning Scale; BPRS = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale; HAM-D = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; CAINS = Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MAP = Experiential negative symptoms; EXP = Expressive negative symptoms;

a

p = .05,

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01

We additionally examined whether the correlations in the schizophrenia patients and in the bipolar patients for TMMS and RFS subscales were significantly different from each other. The correlations between RFS Social and TMMS Total (z = −2.86, p < .01), Attention (z = −3.01, p < .01), and Repair (z = −2.50, p < .05) were all significantly larger in the bipolar group.

We additionally considered correlations between symptoms and functioning (Tables 34). In both groups, higher MAP negative symptoms correlated with poorer RFS Social. For bipolar patients, there were additional significant correlations: higher MAP symptoms correlated with poorer RFS Work and total psychiatric symptoms correlated with poorer RFS Social.

3.4 Regression analyses

In the schizophrenia group, the TMMS was related to RFS Independent Living. However, because none of the clinical symptoms correlated significantly with this outcome domain, we did not need to consider incremental validity beyond symptoms.

For the bipolar group, the strongest associations were between the TMMS and RFS Social. We conducted four hierarchical regressions (one for each TMMS index) to examine whether the TMMS accounted for unique variance in RFS Social, beyond the clinical symptoms that also correlated with this outcome. In all four models, BPRS Total, YMRS, and CAINS MAP scores were entered in Step 1, as these all showed bivariate correlations with RFS Social. In step 2, one of the four TMMS scores was added. Two TMMS scores accounted for significant incremental variance in RFS Social (Table 5). TMMS Total explained an additional 8%, and Attention explained an additional 6%, of the variance in social functioning. Other TMMS scales did not explain significant unique variance. We also observed a significant correlation between Repair and RFS Independent Living in the bipolar group, but this outcome domain did not have any significant clinical symptom correlates, so we did not conduct a regression analysis.

Table 5.

Regression analysis for incremental prediction of RFS Social, bipolar patients

Step 1 Step 2 R2 R2 change F change
Models 1–4:
BPRS Total
YMRS Total
CAINS-MAP
0.56 0.56 12.76**
Model 1: TMMS Total 0.64 0.08 6.25*
Model 2: TMMS Attention 0.62 0.06 4.74*
Model 3: TMMS Clarity 0.60 0.04 2.78
Model 4: TMMS Repair 0.54 0.04 2.61

Notes: RFS = Role Functioning Scale; CAINS = Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms; MAP = Experiential negative symptoms; TMMS = Trait Meta-Mood Scale; BPRS = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; YMRS = Young Mania Rating Scale;

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01

4. Discussion

In this study, schizophrenia patients reported lower perceived EI than healthy controls across all TMMS subscales. Bipolar patients also reported lower Clarity, Repair, and Total perceived EI compared to healthy controls, and the two clinical groups did not significantly differ on any TMMS subscales. Perceived EI showed significant relationships to different clinical symptoms and aspects of functioning within each clinical group, and correlations were robust between TMMS, manic symptoms, and social functioning in the bipolar group. These findings provide initial support for the clinical and functional relevance of the subjective beliefs that individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder have concerning their ability to identify, understand, and manage their emotions.

4.1 Group differences on perceived EI

The lower perceived EI reported by the schizophrenia group is consistent with prior research examining self-report measures of related constructs, including mindfulness, alexithymia, and emotion regulation (Chadwick et al., 2005; Kimhy et al., 2012; van’t Wout et al., 2007). The findings also converge with research on individuals at clinical high-risk for schizophrenia who report lower Clarity and Repair than healthy controls (Lee et al., 2008). Furthermore, the schizophrenia patients’ diminished perceived EI corresponds to prior findings of impairments on performance-based EI (Eack et al., 2010; Kee et al., 2009). Overall, this pattern of findings suggests that self-perceived emotion processing limitations match the emotion processing impairments displayed on performance measures.

Except for the Attention subscale, the bipolar group also reported lower perceived EI than controls, though differences were less pronounced than in schizophrenia. Despite showing relatively intact EI on performance measures (Burdick et al., 2011; Lee et al., 2013), bipolar patients report considerable difficulty attending to, understanding, and repairing emotions. These individuals may have persistent difficulty enacting these skills effectively due to repeatedly experiencing unregulated emotions in the course of their daily lives (Gruber et al., 2013). Hence, the reasons for the low perceived EI in schizophrenia may stem from emotion processing skill deficits, whereas the low perceived EI in bipolar disorder may accurately reflect difficulty in using these skills in an emotionally tumultuous life. In addition, while perceived EI was not affected by history of psychosis or medication status, euthymic bipolar patients reported higher scores on TMMS Total and Attention than patients with elevated mood. While the subgroups for these comparisons were small, results suggest that stable mood may be associated with greater perceived EI.

4.2 Correlations and regression analyses

There were several notable differences in the clinical and functional correlates of perceived EI in the two clinical groups. Higher Attention was associated with higher psychiatric symptoms among schizophrenia patients. This finding converges with prior research indicating that, paying greater attention to one’s feelings is sometimes associated with higher levels of depression, anxiety, positive schizotypy, and more severe delusions (Berenbaum et al., 2006; Kerns, 2005; Salovey et al., 1995; Serper and Berenbaum, 2008). Perhaps patients who are highly aware of emotions but do not fully differentiate or manage them effectively become overwhelmed by emotions and experience higher levels of psychiatric symptoms. Alternatively, schizophrenia patients with higher Attention scores may have better insight and be more accurate reporters of their symptoms and internal emotional experiences. In this view, greater Attention may actually be therapeutically beneficial because attending to feelings is a prerequisite for developing skills to clarify and repair them.

Regarding functional correlates, in the schizophrenia group, higher TMMS scores (Total and Clarity) were associated only with better community functioning for independent living. In the bipolar group, higher perceived EI on all scales was related to lower manic symptoms and better social functioning. In addition, all TMMS subscales showed significantly stronger correlations with social functioning in the bipolar group than in the schizophrenia group, and Total and Attention scores explained additional variance in social functioning, after accounting for symptom correlates. Hence, perceived EI shows a strong and distinctive association with social functioning in bipolar disorder.

This was the first study to examine negative symptoms in bipolar disorder and healthy control participants using the CAINS, a recently developed interview-based assessment of experiential and expressive negative symptoms. A few prior studies using other scales have suggested that overall negative symptoms are elevated in bipolar disorder (Ameen and Ram, 2007; Di Nicola et al., 2013; Toomey et al., 1998). The current study indicates that elevations primarily reflect disturbances in the experiential sub-domain (MAP subscale). MAP scores related to functioning significantly and at comparable levels in both patient groups. Although the TMMS showed only small to medium correlations with negative symptoms in the schizophrenia (Table 3) and bipolar (Table 4) groups, its relationships with negative symptoms were quite strong (>0.60 for several TMMS subscales) for healthy controls (Supplementary Table 2). The current findings suggest that negative symptoms, which are typically associated with schizophrenia, are elevated and related to functioning in bipolar disorder.

4.3 Limitations and treatment implications

The current study had some limitations. First, we did not examine performance-based EI. While prior research indicates that the MSCEIT is not strongly related to perceived EI in healthy samples (Mayer et al., 2004), it would be important to examine this issue in patient groups. Second, we did not include other relevant measures for functioning, such as neurocognition or social cognition. Third, the correlational analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons and would not survive a stringent Bonferroni correction. It will therefore be important to replicate the current findings. In addition, correlations with functional outcome are cross-sectional. While impairments in perceived EI may lead to poorer functional outcome, it is also possible that poor functioning leads to more challenges in attending to, understanding, and repairing emotions. Finally, our sample included chronic outpatients who were all taking psychoactive medications at clinically-determined dosages. Our results may not generalize to patients who are unmedicated or during the early phases of illness.

Our results have somewhat different implications for recovery-oriented interventions for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Individuals with schizophrenia show impairments on performance-based EI and corresponding disturbances in perceived EI. Efforts to enhance functioning in individuals with schizophrenia would be most productively accomplished by initially focusing on skill development. Recent interventions designed to improve aspects of emotion processing (e.g., identification, differentiation) show promise (e.g., Horan et al., 2011; Penn et al., 2007). As skill development progresses, it will be important to address beliefs about one’s ability to use these skills in real-life situations. Interventions targeting beliefs associated with engaging in productive activities and bridging skill training to daily life activities are also emerging (Grant and Beck, 2009; Keefe et al., 2012).

For individuals with bipolar disorder who report perceived EI disturbances despite relatively intact EI skills, interventions may be most productively focused on developing new ways of understanding intensely fluctuating emotions. In addition to cognitive-behavioral interventions aimed at modifying emotional experiences (e.g., through emotion regulation strategies), newer mindfulness and acceptance-based approaches seek to modify one’s relationship to emotions (e.g., learning to observe and detach from emotions) so they are not so overwhelming (Hayes et al., 2011). These approaches have yielded encouraging initial results in bipolar disorder (Stange et al., 2011; Van Dijk et al., 2013), and schizophrenia (Khoury et al., 2013; Tai and Turkington, 2009).

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

Support for this study came from NIMH Grants MH091468 (William P. Horan, Ph.D.) and MH065707 and MH43292 (Michael F. Green, PhD). A postdoctoral fellowship for the first author was supported by an NIMH training grant in Cognitive and Affective Dysfunctions in the Psychoses at the University of California, Los Angeles (T32MH09668). The authors wish to thank Amanda Bender, Michelle Dolinsky, Crystal Gibson, Cory Tripp, and Katherine Weiner for assistance in data collection.

Role of funding source

Funding for the current study was provided by NIMH Grants MH091468 (William P. Horan, Ph.D.) and MH065707 and MH43292 (Michael F. Green, PhD).

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

All authors contributed to the conceptualization of the study. Statistical analyses and writing of the first draft of the manuscript were performed by Dr. Tabak. Dr. Horan assisted with the literature review and statistical analyses. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

Dr. Green reports consulting for AbbVie, DSP, Forum, Mnemosyne (scientific board), and Roche. He has received research support from Amgen. The rest of the authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Ameen S, Ram D. Negative symptoms in the remission phase of bipolar disorder. Ger. J. Psychiatry. 2007;10:1–7. [Google Scholar]
  2. Berenbaum H, Boden MT, Baker JP, Dizen M, Thompson RJ, Abramowitz A. Emotional correlates of the different dimensions of schizotypal personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2006;115:359–368. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Burdick KE, Goldberg TE, Cornblatt BA, Keefe RS, Gopin CB, Derosse P, Braga RJ, Malhotra AK. The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery in patients with bipolar I disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:1587–1592. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chadwick P, Hember M, Symes J, Peters E, Kuipers E, Dagnan D. Responding mindfully to unpleasant thoughts and images: Reliability and validity of the Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ) Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2008;47:451–455. doi: 10.1348/014466508X314891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chadwick P, Taylor KN, Abba N. Mindfulness groups for people with psychosis. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 2005;33:351–359. [Google Scholar]
  6. Di Nicola M, De Risio L, Battaglia C, Camardese G, Tedeschi D, Mazza M, Martinotti G, Pozzi G, Niolu C, Di Giannantonio M, Siracusano A, Janiri L. Reduced hedonic capacity in euthymic bipolar subjects: a trait-like feature? J. Affect. Disord. 2013;147:446–450. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eack SM, Greeno CG, Pogue-Geile MF, Newhill CE, Hogarty GE, Keshavan MS. Assessing social-cognitive deficits in schizophrenia with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test. Schizophr. Bull. 2010;36:370–380. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Extremera N, Fernández-Berrocal P. Perceived emotional intelligence and life satisfaction: Predictive and incremental validity using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2005;39:937–948. [Google Scholar]
  9. Fernandez Berrocal P, Extremera N. A review of trait meta-mood research. In: Columbus AM, editor. Advances in Psychology Research. Vol. 55. Nova Science Publishers; 2008. pp. 17–45. [Google Scholar]
  10. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J, Benjamin L. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (version 2.0) New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  11. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer R, Williams J. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. Patient Edition. New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  12. Grant PM, Beck AT. Defeatist beliefs as a mediator of cognitive impairment, negative symptoms, and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:798–806. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gruber J. A review and synthesis of positive emotion and reward disturbance in bipolar disorder. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2011;18:356–365. doi: 10.1002/cpp.776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gruber J, Harvey AG, Gross JJ. When trying is not enough: Emotion regulation and the effort-success gap in bipolar disorder. Emotion. 2012;12:997–1003. doi: 10.1037/a0026822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gruber J, Hay AC, Gross JJ. Rethinking emotion: cognitive reappraisal is an effective positive and negative emotion regulation strategy in bipolar disorder. Emotion. 2014;14:388–396. doi: 10.1037/a0035249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gruber J, Kogan A, Mennin D, Murray G. Real-world emotion? An experience sampling approach to emotion experience and regulation in bipolar I disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2013;122:971–983. doi: 10.1037/a0034425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hayes SC, Villatte M, Levin M, Hildebrandt M. Open, aware, and active: contextual approaches as an emerging trend in the behavioral and cognitive therapies. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2011;7:141–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Horan WP, Hajcak G, Wynn JK, Green MF. Impaired emotion regulation in schizophrenia: Evidence from event-related potentials. Psychol. Med. 2013;43:2377–2391. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Horan WP, Kern RS, Tripp C, Hellemann G, Wynn JK, Bell M, Marder SR, Green MF. Efficacy and specificity of social cognitive skills training for outpatients with psychotic disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2011;45:1113–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kee KS, Horan WP, Salovey P, Kern RS, Sergi MJ, Fiske AP, Lee J, Subotnik KL, Nuechterlein KH, Sugar Ca, Green MF. Emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2009;107:61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Keefe RSE, Vinogradov S, Medalia A, Buckley PF, Caroff SN, D’Souza DC, Harvey PD, Graham KA, Hamer RM, Marder SM, Miller DD, Olson SJ, Patel JK, Velligan D, Walker TM, Haim AJ, Stroup TS. Feasibility and pilot efficacy results from the multisite Cognitive Remediation in the Schizophrenia Trials Network (CRSTN) randomized controlled trial. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2012;73:1016–1022. doi: 10.4088/JCP.11m07100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kerns JG. Positive schizotypy and emotion processing. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2005;114:392–401. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Khoury B, Lecomte T, Gaudiano BA, Paquin K. Mindfulness interventions for psychosis: A meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 2013;150:176–184. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kimhy D, Vakhrusheva J, Jobson-Ahmed L, Tarrier N, Malaspina D, Gross JJ. Emotion awareness and regulation in individuals with schizophrenia: Implications for social functioning. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:193–201. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kopelowicz A, Ventura J, Liberman RP, Mintz J. Consistency of Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale factor structure across a broad spectrum of schizophrenia patients. Psychopathology. 2008;41:77–84. doi: 10.1159/000111551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kring AM, Elis O. Emotion deficits in people with schizophrenia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2013;9:409–433. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kring AM, Gur RE, Blanchard JJ, Horan WP, Reise SP. The Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS): Final development and validation. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2013;170:165–172. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lee J, Altshuler L, Glahn DC, Miklowitz DJ, Ochsner K, Green MF. Social and nonsocial cognition in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: Relative levels of impairment. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2013;170:334–341. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lee SJ, Yoo SY, Kang D-H, Lee KJ, Ha TH, Wee W, Lee A-R, Kim NS, Kwon JS. Potential vulnerability markers within the affective domain in subjects at genetic and clinical high risk for schizophrenia. Psychopathology. 2008;41:236–244. doi: 10.1159/000125557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lin Y-C, Wynn JK, Hellemann G, Green MF. Factor structure of emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2012;139:78–81. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR. Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) user’s manual. Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Toronto, Ontario; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  33. Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR. Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychol. Inq. 2004;15:197–215. [Google Scholar]
  34. Mayer JD, Salovey P, Caruso DR. Emotional intelligence: new ability or eclectic traits? Am. Psychol. 2008;63:503–517. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.6.503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Palmer B, Donaldson C, Stough C. Emotional intelligence and life satisfaction. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2002;33:1091–1100. [Google Scholar]
  36. Penn DL, Roberts DL, Combs D, Sterne A. The Development of the Social Cognition and Interaction Training Program for Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders. Psychiatr. Serv. 2007;58:449–451. doi: 10.1176/ps.2007.58.4.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Perich T, Manicavasagar V, Mitchell PB, Ball JR. Mindfulness, response styles and dysfunctional attitudes in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2011;134:126–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rassovsky Y, Horan WP, Lee J, Sergi MJ, Green MF. Pathways between early visual processing and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 2011;41:487–497. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Salguero JM, Palomera R, Fernández-berrocal P. Perceived emotional intelligence as predictor of psychological adjustment in adolescents?: a 1-year prospective study. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2012;27:21–34. [Google Scholar]
  40. Salovey P, Mayer JD, Goldman SL, Turvey C, Palfai TP. Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In: Pennebaker JW, editor. Emotion, Disclosure, and Health. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1995. pp. 125–154. [Google Scholar]
  41. Salovey P, Stroud LR, Woolery A, Epel ES. Perceived Emotional Intelligence, Stress Reactivity, and Symptom Reports: Further Explorations Using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Psychol. Health. 2002;17:611–627. [Google Scholar]
  42. Schutte NS, Malouff JM, Thorsteinsson EB, Bhullar N, Rooke SE. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2007;42:921–933. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sergi MJ, Rassovsky Y, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF. Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2006;163:448–454. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Serper M, Berenbaum H. The relation between emotional awareness and hallucinations and delusions in acute psychiatric inpatients. Schizophr. Res. 2008;101:195–200. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Stange JP, Eisner LR, Hölzel BK, Peckham AD, Dougherty DD, Rauch SL, Nierenberg AA, Lazar S, Deckersbach T. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for bipolar disorder: effects on cognitive functioning. J. Psychiatr. Pract. 2011;17:410–419. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000407964.34604.03. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Tai S, Turkington D. The evolution of cognitive behavior therapy for schizophrenia: current practice and recent developments. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:865–873. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tohen M, Frank E, Cl B, Colom F, Sn G, Ln Y. The International Society for Bipolar Disorders (ISBD) Task Force report on the nomenclature of course and outcome in bipolar disorders. 2009:453–473. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00726.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Toomey R, Faraone SV, Simpson JC, Tsuang MT. Negative, positive, and disorganized symptom dimensions in schizophrenia, major depression, and bipolar disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1998;186:470–476. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199808000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Van Dijk S, Jeffrey J, Katz MR. A randomized, controlled, pilot study of dialectical behavior therapy skills in a psychoeducational group for individuals with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2013;145:386–393. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. van’t Wout M, Aleman A, Bermond B, Kahn RS. No words for feelings: alexithymia in schizophrenia patients and first-degree relatives. Compr. Psychiatry. 2007;48:27–33. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ventura J, Liberman R, Green MF, Shaner A. Training and quality assurance with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. Psychiatry Res. 1998;79:163–173. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(98)00038-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ventura J, Lukoff D, Nuechterlein KH, Liberman R, Green MF, Shaner A. Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) expanded version: scales, anchor points, and administration manual. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1993;3:227–243. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wolkenstein L, Zwick JC, Hautzinger M, Joormann J. Cognitive emotion regulation in euthymic bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2014;160:92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Young R, Biggs J, Ziegler V, Meyer D. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. Br. J. Psychiatry. 1978;133:429–435. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

RESOURCES