Abstract
Rationale
Stress experiences have been shown to be a risk factor for alcohol abuse in humans; however, a reliable mouse model using episodic social stress has yet to be developed.
Objectives
The current studies investigated the effects of mild and moderate social defeat protocols on plasma corticosterone, voluntary alcohol drinking, and motivation to drink alcohol.
Methods
Outbred CFW mice were socially defeated for 10 days during which the intruder mouse underwent mild (15 bites: mean = 1.5 min), or moderate (30 bites: mean = 3.8 min) stress. Plasma corticosterone was measured on days 1 and 10 of the defeat. Ethanol drinking during continuous access to alcohol was measured 10 days following the defeat or 10 days prior to, during and 20 days after the defeat. Motivation to drink was determined using a PR operant conditioning schedule during intermittent access to ethanol.
Results
Plasma corticosterone was elevated in both stress groups on days 1 and 10. Ethanol consumption and preference following moderate social stress was higher than both the mild stress group and controls. Mice with previously acquired ethanol drinking showed decreased ethanol consumption during the moderate stress followed by an increase 20 days post-defeat. Moderately stressed mice also showed escalated ethanol intake (11g/kg/day) and ethanol self-administration during a schedule of intermittent access to alcohol.
Conclusion
Social defeat experiences of moderate intensity and duration led to increased ethanol drinking and preference in CFW mice. Ongoing work investigates the interaction between glucocorticoids and dopaminergic systems as neural mechanisms for stress-escalated alcohol consumption.
Keywords: Stress, social defeat, drug abuse, corticosterone, mice, self-administration, animal models, ethanol
INTRODUCTION
Ostensibly aversive stress experiences can increase the pursuit and use of alcohol and stimulants in humans and animals (Becker et al. 2011; Koob 2008; Koob and Le Moal 1997; Leventhal and Cleary 1980; Tomkins 1966). Murine models promise to enhance our understanding of the genetic and neurobiological mechanisms underlying escalated intake and increased motivation for alcohol after stressful episodes. It has been challenging to reliably and adequately characterize the stress-alcohol relationship in animal models (Becker et al. 2011; Noori et al. 2014). Along with its companion paper on cocaine (Han et al. 2014 revision under review), the current report identifies several key social determinants linking episodic social defeat stress and its effect on alcohol drinking behaviors.
Mild episodes of social defeat constitute an ethologically relevant stressor that triggers sympathetic activation and the release of glucocorticoids (Koolhaas et al. 1997; Marti-Carbonell et al. 1992; Meerlo et al. 1996; Miczek and Tidey 1990) as indicated by elevated plasma concentrations of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and corticosterone. Social defeat stress is unique in that rodents do not habituate to repeated episodes of this stressor, evidenced by long-lasting enhancement of corticosterone and Fos expression in the hypothalamus and central amygdala (Martinez et al. 1998; Nikulina et al. 2004; Raab et al. 1986) and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and ventral tegmental area (VTA) (Berton et al. 2006). Episodic exposure even to brief, intermittent social interactions can have extensive long- and short-term cardiovascular, thermoregulatory, and behavioral changes in rodents (Tornatzky and Miczek 1993).
Glucocorticoid release resulting from social defeat stress may contribute to the reinforcing effects of alcohol (Deroche et al. 1993; Piazza et al. 1993). Alcohol consumption results in the release of corticosterone, suggesting that alcohol can be a stressor under some conditions (Koob et al. 1998). However, alcohol has also been shown to have anxiolytic effects in rodents as, for example, in the elevated plus maze task (Lister 1987). Different types of stress, such as chronic subordination in a social colony, social isolation, and episodic social defeat stress alter plasma corticosterone levels in rodents (Raab et al. 1986; Tornatzky and Miczek 1993; Uschold-Schmidt et al. 2012; Weiss et al. 2004). Systemic administration of corticosterone increases self-administration of ethanol in rodents (Deroche et al. 1993; Piazza et al. 1993). Here, we confirm previous findings that demonstrated social stressors induce a rise in plasma corticosterone levels as well as begin to investigate the precise, temporal relationship between corticosterone and episodic social stressors of different intensities..
Stress-induced glucocorticoids in blood plasma may mediate the activation of dopaminergic mesolimbic neurons (Rouge-Pont et al. 1998). Repeated exposure to stressors results in a long-term enhancement of dopamine release in the mesoaccumbens pathway in response to a stimulant challenge (Sorg and Kalivas 1991; Wilcox et al. 1986). Social defeat stress increases extracellular dopamine release in the shell of the NAc and affects the sensitivity of dopamine receptors in mice and rats (Han et al. 2014 Revision under review; Piazza and Le Moal 1998; Puglisi-Allegra et al. 1991; Tidey and Miczek 1997). These findings have led to the proposal that vulnerability to drug use may be exacerbated by neuroplastic changes in the HPA-axis and mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway as a result of social defeat stress. Changes in vulnerability have previously been measured using locomotor sensitization to acute drug challenges (Phillips et al. 1997). The present study attempted to predict a stress-induced escalation of ethanol intake by testing for sensitization of locomotor activity to an acute ethanol challenge (Fish et al. 2002). The exposure to a social stressor and the subsequent release of glucocorticoids may affect drug-taking behaviors by modifying dopamine release in the NAc(Han et al. 2014 Revision under review; Covington, III et al. 2005).
Various animal models of alcohol drinking can be implemented in order to study the effects of social defeat stress on voluntary ethanol drinking, including continuous or intermittent access to alcohol and drinking-in-the-dark (McBride & Li, 1998; Rhodes et al. 2005). Intermittent access (IA) to alcohol in a two-bottle free-choice paradigm leads to voluntary, preferential and dependence-inducing alcohol consumption (Hwa et al. 2011). We evaluated the reinforcing effects of ethanol in socially stressed rodents by measuring the rate of operant responding maintained by a progressive ratio schedule of ethanol reward (Czachowski and Samson 1999; Rodd-Henricks et al. 2003). Ethanol self-administration and voluntary drinking procedures provide a profile of the appetitive and consummatory aspects of regulated and escalated alcohol drinking. We implemented these models in order to study the effects of social defeat stress on voluntary ethanol consumption and motivation to drink in mice.
The present study used outbred Carworth Farms Webster (CFW) mice because of their genetic and phenotypic variation (Crabbe et al. 1994). Mice of this strain demonstrate a wide range of alcohol intake, and thus provide evidence that can be translated to human conditions. The current study and its companion (Han et al. 2014 under review) report closely similar elevated plasma corticosterone as well as dopamine release from the nucleus accumbens in response to two different types of social defeat stressors of distinct intensities and durations. Significantly, only those mice that were exposed to moderate, but not mild social defeat episodes showed escalated and persistent ethanol intake, preference, and self-administration.
METHODS
Animals
Adult male Carworth Farms Webster (CFW) mice (n=165; Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA, U.S.A) weighed 23-25g upon arrival. Experimental mice were group-housed for one week in groups of six in large polycarbonate cages (48 x 26cm x 16cm) with corn cob bedding and unlimited access to standard rodent chow (Purina LabDiet 5001) and tap water. This allowed mice to habituate to the constant temperature (21 ± 1C), 20% humidity, and light/dark photocycle (lights off at 0700 and lights on at 1900) conditions of the vivarium. The mice were then housed individually in polycarbonate cages (28 x 17 x 12cm) with ad libitum access to water and rodent chow. The guidelines of the “Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals” were followed for all procedures (2011), and were approved by the Institutional Care and Use Committee of Tufts University.
Social Defeat Stress
Upon arriving in the vivarium, each male ‘resident’ CFW mouse was pair-housed with a female CFW mouse in a polycarbonate cage (28 x 17 x 12cm). After at least two weeks, each male resident was assessed for aggression in confrontation with an intruder in the absence of the female cagemate for 5 minutes. . The number of attack bites by the resident mouse was recorded. This procedure was performed for 10 consecutive days. Mice that were determined to be reliably aggressive (greater than 30 bites in 5 min) were used as ‘resident’ mice to socially defeat the experimental mice.
Experimental mice were randomly assigned to be in the moderate (being attacked 30 times) stress group, the mild (being attacked 15 times) stress group, or the non-stressed control group. Mice in the control group were weighed daily, while the mice in the stress groups were weighed and then socially defeated for 10 consecutive days (Days 1-10) using the following procedure, which consisted of the pre-defeat threat, defeat, and post-defeat threat phases (Yap et al. 2005). This procedure began 2 – 3 hours after the beginning of the dark cycle every day. The female cagemate was removed before the pre-defeat phase and kept in a holding cage until the end of the threat phase. In the pre-defeat threat phase, an intruder mouse was placed into a perforated, protective cage (15cm x 7cm x 7cm) and placed into the home cage of an aggressive ‘resident’ mouse for 5 minutes. Intruders faced a different resident during each confrontation to prevent habituation of the resident to the intruder. During the defeat phase, the intruder mouse was removed from the perforated cage and placed into the resident's cage without protection. The defeat phase lasted until one of the following conditions was met: the intruder had received 30 or 15 bites, depending on experimental condition, 5 minutes had elapsed, or the intruder showed at least 3 consecutive seconds of defeat posture (Miczek et al. 1982). Mice that displayed defeat posture were removed from the resident's cage and immediately began the next phase (occurred in 3% of confrontations). In the threat phase the intruder was placed back into the perforated protective cage in the resident's cage for 5 minutes. Following the threat phase, the intruder was returned to its homecage for the remainder of the 24 hours.
Corticosterone Measurements
Blood samples were collected from the submandibular vein 20 minutes after the start of the defeat phase of the social stress procedures on days 1 and 10 using disposable sterile lancets (MEDIpoint Inc., NY). Blood collection took less than one minute per sample. Samples were centrifuged for 10 minutes at 4°C, 3,000 revolutions per minute and blood plasma was extracted. A corticosterone enzyme immunoassay kit (Arbor Assays, Ann Arbor, Michigan) was used to analyze the samples for corticosterone. Detection levels for corticosterone were 7.8125-1,000ng/ml.
Locomotor Sensitization
On Day 20, locomotor activity of each experimental mouse was recorded in an open field (53cm x 38cm x 46cm) using EthoVision tracking software (Version 2.4.19). Mice were given a daily injection of saline during the three days prior to this testing in order to habituate to the intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection. Experimental mice were first placed into the open field and allowed to habituate for 30 minutes. Mice were then given an injection (i.p.) of saline and locomotor activity was recorded for 15 minutes. In the final phase, mice were given an ethanol challenge (2g/kg, i.p.) and locomotor activity was recorded for 45 minutes. The ethanol dose and injection-test interval were chosen based on previous work (Fish et al. 2002; Middaugh et al. 1989).
Ethanol Drinking
20% ethanol solutions (w/v) were prepared by diluting 95% ethyl alcohol (Pharmaco-AAPER, Brookfield, CT) with tap water. At least two days before the ethanol challenge, mice were given access to two 50ml centrifuge tubes (Nalgene) with water. Centrifuge tubes, equipped with no. 5 rubber stoppers and stainless steel nozzles with ball bearings, were presented to the mice through the metal wire cage lid. The day following the ethanol challenge (Day 21), mice were given either continuous or intermittent access to a two-bottle choice of 20% ethanol and water, for 3 weeks, or for 5 weeks, respectively (Hwa et al. 2011). Bottles were weighed daily in order to determine the average daily fluid consumption. The ethanol and water bottles switched sides every day to control for side preference. Mice were weighed each morning to calculate ethanol intake (grams of ethanol per kilogram of body weight). Ethanol evaporation and spillage due to experimenter bottle weighing were determined by weighing a pair of ethanol and water bottles that were held on a cage without an animal. The fluid “drip” measurements were subtracted from the daily ethanol and water bottles’ weights to calculate the volume of fluid consumption.
Ethanol reinforcement during fixed ratio and progressive ratio schedules
The present experiment studied ethanol reinforcement while using an operant conditioning panel that was inserted into the home cage (Miczek and de Almeida 2001). For operant responding under the control of schedules of ethanol reinforcement, all events were monitored and controlled by a computer using MED-PC for Windows (MedAssociates, St. Albans, Vermont).
Two different schedules of reinforcement were used: fixed ratio (FR-1) and progressive ratio (PR). The operant conditioning panel was affixed to the home cage at the start of the dark cycle and each response was reinforced with the presentation of 0.05 mL of 20% ethanol or tap water. After thirty minutes, the FR-1 session was terminated, the panel was removed, and the 2-bottle presentation was resumed. It was not possible to implement a continuous access protocol due to the confound of having water and alcohol concurrently present under independent PR contingencies. An IA procedure was used as it offered the advantage of preventing periods of alcohol deprivation. During the progressive ratio performance, the operant conditioning panels contained only one nose-poke receptacle. The side at which the removable receptacle was presented alternated every other session, matching the IA procedure. The type of fluid reinforcement alternated each session to match those presented in the IA protocol. The progression of the ratio of responses to 0.05 mL fluid reinforcement followed an exponential series (Richardson and Roberts 1996). Thus, the response requirement increased progressively according to the following schedule: 1, 2, 4, 6, 9, 12, 15, 20, 25, 32, 40, 50, 62, 77, and 95 for reinforcements 1 to 10, respectively. Breakpoint was defined as the maximum response requirement achieved before the session's termination (Hodos 1961). Sessions were terminated when an animal did not complete the following ratio requirement within 30 minutes. At the end of the sessions, the two-bottle IA protocol was resumed for the remainder of the 24 hours.
Experiment 1
The timeline for Experiment 1 is shown in Figure 1A. Mice were handled daily for the 3 days prior to the first day of social defeat stress. Mice then underwent 10 consecutive days of either moderate or mild social defeat stress. On the first and last days of stress (days 1 and 10), blood was collected 20 minutes after the beginning of the defeat phase. Non-stressed controls were weighed daily during the 10 day protocol. Blood plasma was later analyzed for corticosterone levels using the Corticosterone Enzyme Immunoassay Kit.
Experiment 2
The timeline for Experiment 2 is shown in Figure 1B. During Experiment 2, mice underwent 10 consecutive days of social defeat stress, as previously described. On day 20, mice were tested for locomotor activity following an ethanol challenge. For the next 20 days, mice were given continuous access to both a bottle of 20% ethanol and of water. After 20 days, mice were categorized as either ‘high drinking’ or ‘low drinking’ based on their ethanol intake. Controls drank approximately 5g/kg/24hr thus high drinking was considered to be twice that value. Mice that drank 10g/kg/24hr for 10 or more days during the 20 day period were designated ‘high drinkers’.
Experiment 3
The timeline for Experiment 3 is shown in Figure 1C. During Experiment 3, mice were given access to a 20% ethanol bottle and water bottle during the duration of the experiment. Baseline ethanol and water drinking were established during the first 10 days of the experiment. Mice were assigned to the moderate stress and non-stress groups and matched for their baseline ethanol drinking. In this experiment, mice were socially defeated on consecutive days during days 10 to 20. Fluid intake was measured daily during the social defeat procedure as well as 20 days afterward.
Experiment 4
The timeline for Experiment 4 is shown in Figure 1D. During Experiment 4, mice underwent the same social defeat and locomotor procedure as in Experiment 2. Immediately following the locomotor test on day 20, mice were exposed to the FR-1 schedule of ethanol reinforcement for one week. After one week of FR-1 performance, mice were subjected to the PR schedule for four weeks. Throughout both schedules, mice were given water and intermittent access (i.e. every other day) to 20% ethanol. Breakpoints and 24-hour fluid intake were measured each day.
Statistical Analysis
SigmaStat 11.0 software (Systat Software, San Jose, California) was used to analyze the data statistically. Two-way repeated measures (RM) analyses of variance (ANOVA) were performed to analyze the effect of the two stress procedures on weekly, voluntary ethanol consumption and preference during continuous access to ethanol and water in Experiment 2. This test was also used to determine the effect of time and stress experience on voluntary, daily ethanol consumption and self-administration, defined as breakpoint (Hodos 1961). One-way RM ANOVA was used to determine that stress had a similar effect on several cohorts of mice within the same group before data were merged. The effect of stress on blood plasma corticosterone on Day 1 and Day 10 of stress was analyzed using two-way RM ANOVA. Two-way RM ANOVA was used to determine the effect of the moderate stress group on previously acquired ethanol drinking behavior. Holm-Sidak post-hoc t-tests were used to identify significant treatment differences, as shown by p<0.05. A Wald Chi-Square test was used to analyze the proportions of high drinkers in the stress groups.
RESULTS
Effect of Moderate and Mild Social Defeat stress on Blood Plasma Corticosterone
The moderate social defeat procedure (mean = 227 ± 5.9sec; 23 ± 0.4 bites) was shown to be longer in duration than the mild social defeat procedure (mean = 90 ± 5.1sec; 15 ± 0.1 bites). Both socially defeat groups showed elevated corticosterone on day 1 and day 10 of the social defeat procedure (Figure 2). Two-way RM ANOVA revealed a main effect of stress on blood plasma corticosterone measurements [F(2, 29) =15.789, p<0.001]. The interaction between stress and time showed a trend [F(2, 29) =2.789, p=0.078]; however, there was no significant main effect of time.
Holm-Sidak post hoc analysis revealed that on day 1, both mild and moderate stress groups showed significantly increased corticosterone compared to the control group (p<0.05). Mice in the mild stress group had a significantly smaller rise in plasma corticosterone on day 10 when compared to day 1[t=2.052, p<0.05]. In contrast, plasma corticosterone concentration in the moderate stress group did not significantly change from day 1 to day 10.
Weekly Ethanol Intake after Moderate, or Mild Social Defeat
The distinct effects of moderate and mild social defeat stress were evident across several cohorts of mice, as revealed by one-way ANOVA; therefore, the data from the cohorts of each experimental group were merged (Figure 3, Figure 4). During the 20 days of continuous ethanol access, the moderate stress group drank more ethanol during 24 h access than either the mild stress group or the control group (moderate: mean = 11.3 ± 0.80 g/kg; mild: mean = 6.49 ± 1.1 g/kg; control: mean = 5.68± 0.82 g/kg). Two-way RM ANOVA revealed main effects of stress [F(2, 84) = 10.408, p< 0.001] and time [F(3,251) = 10.826, p<0.001]. Post-hoc tests showed that the ethanol drinking behavior of the moderate stress group was significantly elevated compared to both the mild stress and non-stressed control group over the course of the four 5-day periods of continuous access to ethanol. Animals in the moderate stress group also demonstrated elevated ethanol preference during the experiment, compared to the non-stressed control group. There was a main effect of time [F(3,251) = 5.088, p<0.01] and stress [F(2,84) = 8.764, p<0.001] on ethanol preference during the 20 days of two-bottle free choice access to 20% ethanol and water as demonstrated by a two-way RM ANOVA. The mild stress group demonstrated ethanol intake and preference similar to that of the non-stressed control group. A Wald Chi-Square test revealed that the proportion of high drinkers in the moderate stress group but not the mild stress group was significantly higher than controls (Figure 5). These data provide evidence for an initial and persistent increase in ethanol consumption in the moderate stress group.
Body weight may influence ethanol intake values that are measured as grams of ethanol per kilogram of body weight. Body weights of the stress groups and controls were analyzed during the social defeat procedure and no significant differences were found. During ethanol consumption there was a main effect of stress [F(2,84)=3.673, p<0.05] and time [F(2,252) =43.706, p<0.001]. Post-hoc tests revealed that the moderate stress group had a significantly increased body weight compared to controls. All groups had significantly increased body weight after 5 days of ethanol intake when compared to their initial weight on day 1.
Locomotor Sensitization to Ethanol
Mice in the moderate stress (pre-drug: 94 ± 5m post-drug: 78 ± 9m), mild stress (pre-drug: 59 ± 3m post-drug: 61 ± 8m), and control (pre-drug: 79 ± 5m post-drug: 61 ± 8m) groups did not show a significant change in locomotor activity in response to an i.p. ethanol injection.There was no significant difference in the percent of mice that showed locomotor sensitization to the ethanol challenge. There was no correlation between the average daily ethanol intake (g/kg) of the moderate stress, mild stress, or non-stressed control groups and the change in locomotor activity in response to an ethanol injection.
Effect of Moderate Social Defeat on Previously Acquired Ethanol Drinking
The baseline (pre) average for all mice during the five days before the start of social defeat stress was 6.50 g/kg (Figure 6). Two-way RM ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of time [F(7,129) = 8.951, p<0.001] and a significant interaction between stress and time [F(7,59) = 4.105, p<0.001]. The moderately stressed mice showed significantly decreased ethanol drinking during the social defeat stress procedure compared to their baseline ethanol intake. After 40 days of ethanol drinking, these mice showed a significantly increased ethanol intake compared to their baseline. Control mice demonstrated significantly increased ethanol intake relative to their baseline 25 and 35 days after the start ethanol drinking.
Effect of Moderate Social Defeat Stress on Motivation to Drink Ethanol
Moderately socially defeated mice showed elevated ethanol intake and self-administration during an IA schedule, compared to non-stressed controls. There was a main effect of moderate social defeat stress on daily voluntary ethanol consumption [F(1,96) = 4.198, p<0.05] (Figure 7) during IA. There was also an effect of moderate stress on the breakpoints (Figure 8) achieved in the PR operant responding condition [F(1, 96) = 8.530, p<0.01]. Holm-Sidak post-hoc tests revealed that these effects were maintained for the first 4 weeks of IA.
DISCUSSION
This report identifies salient features of episodic social stress conditions that result in reliably escalated alcohol drinking in mice. The current experimental procedures attempt to capture the epidemiologically established stress-alcohol link in a mouse model (Jose et al. 2000; Richman et al. 1996; Rospenda et al. 2000). Exposure to social defeat stress of moderate intensity and duration (i.e. being attacked 30 times in 5 min) engendered an increase in ethanol intake and preference 10 days later; however, there was no relationship between locomotor sensitization to alcohol and escalated alcohol consumption. In contrast, a mild social stressor had no effect on alcohol drinking compared to non-stressed controls despite similar corticosterone and dopamine responses in both stress procedures (Han et al. 2014 under review). Studies with cocaine have shown that a range of stress conditions escalate self-administration in rodent models (Boyson et al. 2011; Goeders and Guerin 1994; Haney et al. 1995; Tidey and Miczek 1996); however, increases in alcohol consumption are generated by more limited stress parameters (Funk et al. 2005; Van Erp et al. 2001). While other studies have investigated the stress-alcohol link in mice (Chester et al. 2008; Croft et al. 2005; Sillaber et al. 2002), this is the first to delineate conditions of social defeat stress that reliably lead to escalated voluntary ethanol consumption and self-administration in several cohorts of outbred mice.
In rodent models, social defeat stress protocols vary greatly in intensity and duration, defined respectively by the number of attack bites inflicted upon an intruder by a resident rodent. Many social defeat stress protocols combine lengthy episodes of severe stress with continuous exposure to sensory stimuli from a potential aggressor (Berton et al. 2006; Golden et al. 2011; Kudryavtseva et al. 1991). In some studies, mice were exposed to severe social stressors in which bouts ended only after an intruder received 100 attack bites and opioid-like antinociception became apparent (Miczek et al. 1982). It has been challenging to develop a social stress procedure which consistently escalates alcohol consumption in mouse strains (Croft et al. 2005; Sillaber et al. 2002). Here, we selected the conditions in which intruders received 15 or 30 attack bites to begin to identify the necessary and sufficient parameters of social defeat stress and stress hormones in the escalation of alcohol drinking. We have designated these two intensities as mild and moderate, respectively, in contrast to the more severe social stress procedures (Golden et al. 2011; Miczek et al. 1982).
Social defeat stress has been well characterized in terms of sympathetic and glucocorticoid activity in several mammalian species (Haller et al. 1997; Haller 2014). The present effects of social defeat stress on alcohol drinking appear to be paralleled with activation of the HPA axis; however, while mild and moderate social stress resulted in increased corticosterone, only the latter resulted in escalated alcohol drinking. This dissociation of ethanol intake and plasma corticosterone elevation may be due to adaption of mildly stressed mice to the aggressive confrontations as evidenced by the decreased rise in corticosterone on day 10 of the social defeat procedure. The moderate stress group had a similar rise in plasma corticosterone on day 1 as day 10 suggesting no such adaptation. The measured concentration of basal plasma corticosterone (180ng/mL) was consistent with some studies in DBA/2J and albino mouse strains (Kakihana and Moore 1976; Gibson et al. 1979); however, other studies have found much lower basal plasma corticosterone levels (Finn et al. 2004). Plasma corticosterone may be altered by many factors such as handling (Irwin et al. 1986) or time of day (Barriga et al. 2001) which could account for the variance from study to study.
In rat models of social defeat, treatment with stress-relevant levels of exogenous corticosterone increases alcohol intake above baseline values (Fahlke et al. 1995). Glucocorticoids can have intrinsic reinforcing effects, such that rodents will self-administer corticosterone to maintain physiologically high blood plasma concentrations (Piazza et al. 1993). Injections of the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone to alcohol-preferring rats reduce their alcohol consumption to the level of alcohol-nonpreferring animals (Fahlke et al. 1994). We hypothesize that stress-induced elevated corticosterone levels may interact with dopaminergic systems in the nucleus accumbens in order to engender these changes in alcohol drinking behaviors (Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra 1996; Rothschild et al. 1985); however, further investigation into the relationship between escalated alcohol intake and corticosterone is needed before a mechanistic explanation of this relationship may be proposed.
Here, we describe a social defeat stress procedure which escalates alcohol consumption in CFW mice under two different conditions of alcohol access, continuous and intermittent. While previous findings suggest that mice of this strain do not consume high levels of alcohol (Crabbe et al. 1999; Phillips and Crabbe 1991), exposure to bouts of moderate social defeat elevated alcohol consumption in these outbred mice to a level comparable to that of the ethanol-preferring mice (Hwa et al. 2011; Middaugh et al. 1999). While the moderate stress group demonstrated high levels of drinking, all stress groups, including controls, showed a gradual increase in ethanol intake over time. This effect is consistent with other studies that have found similar results in non-stressed mice (Hwa et al. 2011). The cause for the emerging drinking in control mice remains to be determined. We are currently determining how the high alcohol consumption of the inbred C57BL/6J mouse strain may be altered by our moderate social defeat stress procedure.
Subgroups of outbred CFW mice are highly motivated to seek out and consume high amounts of ethanol (high drinkers), while others consume very little (low drinkers). The average daily alcohol consumption of high drinkers was15 g/kg/24hr, while low drinkers consumed 5g/kg/24hr. These data suggest that within the CFW strain of mice, there are some subjects that are susceptible to stress-induced escalated alcohol consumption while others are resilient to this stress effect. The present study found that the proportion of high drinkers in a population of CFW mice increases with increasing stress intensity. Since preclinical and clinical studies implicate genetic variation, rather than environmental differences, as especially important in defining an individual's drug abuse liability, most ethanol drinking studies use inbred strains of mice (Crabbe et al. 1994; Middaugh et al. 1999). Indeed, two bottle choice procedures were introduced to highlight differences in alcohol consumption and preference as a function of strain (McClearn and Rodgers 1959).
Here, we began to investigate how social defeat stress interacts with previously established ethanol consumption. When exposed to 10 days of episodic social defeat stress, ethanol-drinking animals initially showed a decrease in alcohol consumption, consistent with previous studies in rats (Funk et al. 2005; Van Erp and Miczek 2001). Relative to the alcohol-naïve mice, the onset of drinking behaviors was delayed. Prior exposure to ethanol could possibly be blunting HPA activation in response to social defeat stress, leading to a suppression of systemic glucocorticoid release and its effect on dopaminergic reward pathways (Lee et al. 2000a; Lee et al. 2000b).
The present study demonstrates high levels of ethanol self-administration during a PR condition in socially stressed outbred mice without the use of fluid restriction; however, motivation to self-administer alcohol decreased over time (Figure 8) which is in contrast to the 24-hour ethanol intake that remained elevated (Figure 7). The basis for this dissociation of ethanol intake and the motivational effects of ethanol remains unknown and must be studied further. The PR schedule of reinforcement is particularly useful when measuring the relative strength of a reward (Hodos 1961). Rats that perceive alcohol as highly preferable will self-administer larger quantities of ethanol than alcohol non-preferring rats under a PR schedule of reinforcement (Samson et al. 1992; Waller et al. 1984). Though some inbred mouse strains will voluntarily drink significant quantities of ethanol, in CFW mice fluid restriction is usually required for reliable ethanol self-administration under the control of several schedules of reinforcement (Faccidomo et al. 2012; Newman et al. 2012).
Preclinical and clinical evidence implicates social stress as a major trigger in the initiation and maintenance of drug-taking behaviors (Goeders 2003; Sinha 2008). However, not all social stressors will similarly affect drug use in these populations. Even when physiological consequences appear closely similar, stressors of different controllability and predictability can dissimilarly affect underlying neurobiological reward pathways (Miczek et al. 2008).
In conclusion, we present a social defeat stress procedure which reliably escalates alcohol consumption and self-administration in outbred CFW mice. Despite similar initial elevations in glucocorticoid and dopamine response (Han et al. 2014 Revision under review), only the social defeat experience of moderate intensity and duration led to increased ethanol drinking. The interaction between glucocorticoids and dopaminergic systems in the brain can be further explored after this identification of the salient parameters of social defeat stress that lead to increased drug taking.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by NIH grants AA013983 and DA031734 (Klaus Miczek, Ph.D) and F31 AA021622 (Lara Hwa). The authors would like to thank Tom Sopko and Peter Andrew for their outstanding contributions.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- Ambroggi F, Turiault M, Milet A, Deroche-Gamonet V, Parnaudeau S, Balado E, Barik J, van der Veen R, Maroteaux G, Lemberger T, Schütz G, Lazar M, Marinelli M, Piazza PV, Tronche F. Stress and addiction: glucocorticoid receptor in dopaminoceptive neurons facilitates cocaine seeking. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:247–249. doi: 10.1038/nn.2282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker HC, Lopez MF, Doremus-Fitzwater TL. Effects of stress on alcohol drinking: a review of animal studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;218:131–156. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2443-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berton O, McClung CA, DiLeone RJ, Krishnan V, Renthal W, Russo SJ, Graham D, Tsankova NM, Bolanos CA, Rios M, Monteggia LM, Self DW, Nestler EJ. Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science. 2006;311:864–868. doi: 10.1126/science.1120972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyson CO, Miguel TT, Quadros IM, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Prevention of social stress-escalated cocaine self-administration by CRF-R1 antagonist in the rat VTA. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;218:257–269. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2266-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;128:331–342. doi: 10.1007/s002130050142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chester JA, Barrenha GD, Hughes ML, Keuneke KJ. Age- and sex-dependent effects of footshock stress on subsequent alcohol drinking and acoustic startle behavior in mice selectively bred for high-alcohol preference. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:1782–1794. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00763.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Covington HE, III, Kikusui T, Goodhue J, Nikulina EM, Hammer RP, Jr., Miczek KA. Brief social defeat stress: long lasting effects on cocaine taking during a binge and Zif268 mRNA expression in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:310–321. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crabbe JC, Belknap JK, Buck KJ. Genetic animal models of alcohol and drug abuse. Science. 1994;264:1715–1723. doi: 10.1126/science.8209252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crabbe JC, Phillips TJ, Buck KJ, Cunningham CL, Belknap JK. Identifying genes for alcohol and drug sensitivity: recent progress and future directions. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:173–179. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(99)01393-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croft AP, Brooks SP, Cole J, Little HJ. Social defeat increases alcohol preference of C57BL/10 strain mice; effect prevented by a CCKB antagonist. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:163–170. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0165-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czachowski CL, Samson HH. Breakpoint determination and ethanol self-administration using an across-session progressive ratio procedure in the rat. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1999;23:1580–1586. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deroche V, Piazza PV, Deminière JM, Le Moal M, Simon H. Rats orally self-administer corticosterone. Brain Res. 1993;622:315–320. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90837-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faccidomo S, Quadros IM, Takahashi A, Fish EW, Miczek KA. Infralimbic and dorsal raphé microinjection of the 5-HT1B receptor agonist CP-93,129: attenuation of aggressive behavior in CFW male mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;222:117–128. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2629-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahlke C, Engel JA, Eriksson CJ, Hard E, Soderpalm B. Involvement of corticosterone in the modulation of ethanol consumption in the rat. Alcohol. 1994;11:195–202. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(94)90031-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahlke C, Hård E, Eriksson CJ, Engel JA, Hansen S. Consequence of long-term exposure to corticosterone or dexamethasone on ethanol consumption in the adrenalectomized rat, and the effect of type I and type II corticosteroid receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;117:216–224. doi: 10.1007/BF02245190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn DA, Sinnott RS, Ford MM, Long SL, Tanchuck MA, Phillips TJ. Sex differences in the effect of ethanol injection and consumption on brain allopregnanolone levels in C57BL/6 mice. Neuroscience. 2004;123:813–819. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fish EW, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Repeated alcohol: behavioral sensitization and alcohol-heightened aggression in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;160:39–48. doi: 10.1007/s00213-001-0934-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funk D, Harding S, Juzytsch W, Lê AD. Effects of unconditioned and conditioned social defeat on alcohol self-administration and reinstatement of alcohol seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:341–349. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0194-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson A, Ginsburg M, Hall M, Hart SL. The effects of opiate receptor agonists and antagonists on the stress-induced secretion of corticosterone in mice. Br J Pharmacol. 1979;65:139–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1979.tb17342.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goeders NE. The impact of stress on addiction. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2003;13:435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goeders NE, Guerin GF. Non-contingent electric footshock facilitates the acquisition of intravenous cocain self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994;114:63–70. doi: 10.1007/BF02245445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golden SA, Covington HE, III, Berton O, Russo SJ. A standardized protocol for repeated social defeat stress in mice. Nat Protoc. 2011;6:1183–1191. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2011.361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haller J. The glucocorticoid/aggression relationship in animals and humans: an analysis sensitive to behavioral characteristics, glucocorticoid secretion patterns, and neural mechanisms. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014 doi: 10.1007/7854_2014_284. [ePub Feb 2014, doi 10.1007/7854_2014_284] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haller J, Albert I, Makara GB. The effects of the α2 adrenoceptor blocker idazoxan on defeat-induced immobility and plasma corticosterone in rats is antagonized by administration of adrenocorticotrophin-antiserum. Behav Pharmacol. 1997;8:269–273. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han X, Albrechet-Souza L, Doyle MR, Shimamoto A, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Social Stress and Escalated Drug Self-administration in Mice II. Cocaine and Dopamine in Nucleus Accumbens. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2014 doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3734-8. Submitted. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney M, Maccari S, Le Moal M, Simon H, Piazza PV. ocial stress increases the acquisition of cocaine self-administration in male and female rats. Brain Res. 1995;698:46–52. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00788-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Härfstrand A, Fuxe K, Cintra A, Agnati LF, Zini I, Wikström AC, Okret S, Yu ZY, Goldstein M, Steinbusch H. Glucocorticoid receptor immunoreactivity in monoaminergic neurons of rat brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1986;83:9779–9783. doi: 10.1073/pnas.83.24.9779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodos W. Progressive ratio as a measure of reward strength. Science. 1961;134:943–944. doi: 10.1126/science.134.3483.943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwa LS, Chu A, Levinson SA, Kayyali TM, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Persistent escalation of alcohol drinking in C57BL/6J mice with intermittent access to 20% ethanol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:1938–1947. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01545.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwa LS, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. Alcohol in excess: CRF1 receptors in the rat and mouse VTA and DRN. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013;225:313–327. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2820-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irwin J, Ahluwalia P, Zacharko RM, Anisman H. Central norepinephrine and plasma corticosterone following acute and chronic stressors: influence of social isolation and handling. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1986;24:1151–1154. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90471-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- José BS, van Oers HA, van de Mheen HD, Garretsen HF, Mackenbach JP. Stressors and alcohol consumption. Alcohol Alcohol. 2000;35:307–312. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/35.3.307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kakihana R, Moore JA. Circadian rhythm of corticosterone in mice: the effect of chronic consumption of alcohol. Psychopharmacologia. 1976;46:301–305. doi: 10.1007/BF00421118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JG, Jung HS, Kim KJ, Min SS, Yoon BJ. Basal blood corticosterone level is correlated with susceptibility to chronic restraint stress in mice. Neurosci Lett. 2013;555:137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.09.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF. A role for brain stress systems in addiction. Neuron. 2008;59:11–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 1997;278:52–59. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF, Sanna PP, Bloom FE. Neuroscience of addiction. Neuron. 1998;21:467–476. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80557-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koolhaas JM, Meerlo P, de Boer SF, Strubbe JH, Bohus B. The temporal dynamics of the stress response. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:775–782. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(96)00057-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudryavtseva NN, Bakshtanovskaya IV, Koryakina LA. Social model of depression in mice of C57BL/6J strain. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;38:315–320. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90284-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S, Schmidt D, Tilders F, Cole M, Smith A, Rivier C. Prolonged exposure to intermittent alcohol vapors blunts hypothalamic responsiveness to immune and non-immune signals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2000a;24:110–122. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S, Schmidt D, Tilders F, Rivier C. Increased activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of rats exposed to alcohol in utero: role of altered pituitary and hypothalamic function. Mol Cell Neurosci. 2000b;16:515–528. doi: 10.1006/mcne.2000.0890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal H, Cleary PD. The smoking problem: a review of the research and theory in behavioral risk modification. Psychol Bull. 1980;88:370–405. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.88.2.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987;92:180–185. doi: 10.1007/BF00177912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marti-Carbonell MA, Darbra S, Garau A, Balada F. [Hormones and aggression]. Arch Neurobiol (Madr ) 1992;55:162–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez M, Phillips PJ, Herbert J. Adaptation in patterns of c-fos expression in the brain associated with exposure to either single or repeated social stress in male rats. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:20–33. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClearn GE, Rodgers DA. Differences in alcohol preference among inbred strains of mice. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1959;20:691–695. [Google Scholar]
- Meerlo P, de Boer SF, Koolhaas JM, Daan S, van den Hoofdakker RH. Changes in daily rhythms of body temperature and activity after a single social defeat in rats. Physiol Behav. 1996;59:735–739. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)02182-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miczek KA, de Almeida RMM. Oral drug self-administration in the home cage of mice: alcohol-heightened aggression and inhibition by the 5-HT1B agonist anpirtoline. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;157:421–429. doi: 10.1007/s002130100831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miczek KA, Thompson ML, Shuster L. Opioid-like analgesia in defeated mice. Science. 1982;215:1520–1522. doi: 10.1126/science.7199758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miczek KA, Tidey JW. Asghar K, De Souza E, editors. Amphetamines: aggressive and social behavior. Pharmacology and Toxicology of Amphetamine and Related Designer Drugs. NIDA Res Monogr. 1989;94:68–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miczek KA, Yap JJ, Covington HE., III Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol Ther. 2008;120:102–128. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2008.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middaugh LD, Favara JP, Boggan WO. Ethanol stimulation after chronic exposure in C57 mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1989;34:331–335. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90321-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middaugh LD, Kelley BM, Bandy ALE, McGroarty KK. Ethanol consumption by C57BL/6 mice: influence of gender and procedural variables. Alcohol. 1999;17:175–183. doi: 10.1016/s0741-8329(98)00055-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman EL, Chu A, Bahamón B, Takahashi A, DeBold JF, Miczek KA. NMDA receptor antagonism: escalation of aggressive behavior in alcohol-drinking mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;224:167–177. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2734-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikulina EM, Covington HE, III, Ganschow L, Hammer RP, Jr., Miczek KA. Long-term behavioral and neuronal cross-sensitization to amphetamine induced by repeated brief social defeat stress: Fos in the ventral tegmental area and amygdala. Neuroscience. 2004;123:857–865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noori HR, Helinski S, Spanagel R. Cluster and meta-analyses on factors influencing stress-induced alcohol drinking and relapse in rodents. Addict Biol. 2014;19:225–232. doi: 10.1111/adb.12125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips TJ, Crabbe JC. Behavioral studies of genetic differences in alcohol action. In: Crabbe JC, editor. Genetic Basis of Alcohol and Drug Actions. New York. Plenum Press; 1991. pp. 25–104. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips TJ, Roberts AJ, Lessov CN. Behavioral sensitization to ethanol: Genetics and the effects of stress. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior. 1997;57(3):487–493. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00448-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza PV, Deroche V, Deminière JM, Maccari S, Le Moal M, Simon H. Corticosterone in the range of stress-induced levels possesses reinforcing properties: Implications for sensation-seeking behaviors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90:11738–11742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.24.11738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza PV, Le Moal M. The role of stress in drug self-administration. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1998;19:67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0165-6147(97)01115-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puglisi-Allegra S, Imperato A, Angelucci L, Cabib S. Acute stress induces time-dependent responses in dopamine mesolimbic system. Brain Res. 1991;554:217–222. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90192-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raab A, Dantzer R, MIchaud B, Mormede P, Taghzouti K, Simon H, Lemoal M. Behavioural, physiological and immunological consequences of social status and aggression in chronically coexisting resident-intruder dyads of male rats. Physiol Behav. 1986;36:223–228. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(86)90007-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson NR, Roberts DCS. Progressive ratio schedules in drug self-administration studies in rats: a method to evaluate reinforcing efficacy. J Neurosci Methods. 1996;66:1–11. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(95)00153-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richman JA, Flaherty JA, Rospenda KM. Perceived workplace harassment experiences and problem drinking among physicians: broadening the stress/alienation paradigm. Addiction. 1996;91:391–403. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimondini R, Agren G, Borjesson S, Sommer W, Heilig M. Persistent behavioral and autonomic supersensitivity to stress following prenatal stress exposure in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2003;140:75–80. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00273-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodd-Henricks ZA, McKinzie DL, Melendez RI, Berry N, Murphy JM, McBride WJ. Effects of serotonin-3 receptor antagonists on the intracranial self-administration of ethanol within the ventral tegmental area of Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003;165:252–259. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1300-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rospenda KM, Richman JA, Wislar JS, Flaherty JA. Chronicity of sexual harassment and generalized work-place abuse: effects on drinking outcomes. Addiction. 2000;95:1805–1820. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.9512180510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothschild AJ, Langlais PJ, Schatzberg AF, Miller MM, Saloman MS, Lerbinger JE, Cole JO, Bird ED. The effects of a single acute dose of dexamethasone on monoamine and metabolite levels in rat brain. Life Sci. 1985;36:2491–2501. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(85)90145-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rougé-Pont F, Deroche V, Le Moal M, Piazza PV. Individual differences in stress-induced dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens are influenced by corticosterone. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:3903–3907. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rougé-Pont F, Marinelli M, Le Moal M, Simon H, Piazza PV. Stress-induced sensitization and glucocorticoids. II. Sensitization of the increase in extracellular dopamine induced by cocaine depends on stress-induced corticosterone secretion. J Neurosci. 1995;15:7189–7195. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-11-07189.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabino V, Cottone P, Koob GF, Steardo L, Lee MJ, Rice KC, Zorrilla EP. Dissociation between opioid and CRF1 antagonist sensitive drinking in Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;189:175–186. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0546-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samson HH, Schwarzstevens K, Tolliver GA, Andrews CM, Files FJ. Ethanol drinking patterns in a continuous-access operant situation: effects of ethanol concentration and response requirements. Alcohol. 1992;9:409–414. doi: 10.1016/0741-8329(92)90040-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sillaber I, Rammes G, Zimmermann S, Mahal B, Zieglgänsberger W, Wurst W, Holsboer F, Spanagel R. Enhanced and delayed stress-induced alcohol drinking in mice lacking functional CRH1 receptors. Science. 2002;296:931–933. doi: 10.1126/science.1069836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R. Chronic stress, drug use, and vulnerability to addiction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1141:105–130. doi: 10.1196/annals.1441.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorg BA, Kalivas PW. Effects of cocaine and footshock stress on extracellular dopamine levels in the ventral striatum. Brain Res. 1991;559:29–36. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90283-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tidey JW, Miczek KA. Social defeat stress selectively alters mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 1996;721:140–149. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00159-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tidey JW, Miczek KA. Acquisition of cocaine self-administration after social stress: role of accumbens dopamine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1997;130:203–212. doi: 10.1007/s002130050230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomkins SS. Psychological model for smoking behavior. Am J Public Health Nations Health. 1966;56:Suppl–20. doi: 10.2105/ajph.56.12_suppl.17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tornatzky W, Miczek KA. Long-term impairment of autonomic circadian rhythms after brief intermittent social stress. Physiol Behav. 1993;53:983–993. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90278-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uschold-Schmidt N, Nyuyki KD, Füchsl AM, Neumann ID, Reber SO. Chronic psychosocial stress results in sensitization of the HPA axis to acute heterotypic stressors despite a reduction of adrenal in vitro ACTH responsiveness. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1676–1687. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Erp AM, Miczek KA. Persistent suppression of ethanol self-administration by brief social stress in rats and increased startle response as index of withdrawal. Physiol Behav. 2001;73:301–311. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00458-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Erp AM, Tachi N, Miczek KA. Short or continuous social stress: suppression of continuously available ethanol intake in subordinate rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2001;12:335–342. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200109000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller MB, McBride WJ, Gatto GJ, Lumeng L, Li TK. Intragastric self-infusion of ethanol by ethanol-preferring and -nonpreferring lines of rats. Science. 1984;225:78–80. doi: 10.1126/science.6539502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss IC, Pryce CR, Jongen-Rêlo AL, Nanz-Bahr NI, Feldon J. Effect of social isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2004;152:279–295. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilcox RA, Robinson TE, Becker JB. Enduring enhancement in amphetamine-stimulated striatal dopamine release in vitro produced by prior exposure to amphetamine or stress in vivo. Eur J Pharmacol. 1986;124:375–376. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(86)90244-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang H, Wang JR, Didion JP, Buus RJ, Bell TA, Welsh CE, Bonhomme F, Yu AH, Nachman MW, Pialek J, Tucker P, Boursot P, McMillan L, Churchill GA, de Villena FP. Subspecific origin and haplotype diversity in the laboratory mouse. Nat Genet. 2011;43:648–655. doi: 10.1038/ng.847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yap JJ, Covington HE, III, Gale MC, Datta R, Miczek KA. Behavioral sensitization due to social defeat stress in mice: antagonism at mGluR5 and NMDA receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;179:230–239. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2023-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]